Abstract
Circadian clocks are centrally involved in the regulation of daily behavioural and physiological processes. These clocks are synchronized to the 24 h day by external cues (Zeitgeber), the most important of which is the light–dark cycle. In polar environments, however, the strength of the Zeitgeber is greatly reduced around the summer and winter solstices (continuous daylight or continuous darkness). How animals time their behaviour under such conditions has rarely been studied in the wild. Using a radio-telemetry-based system, we investigated daily activity rhythms under continuous daylight in Barrow, Alaska, throughout the breeding season in four bird species that differ in mating system and parental behaviour. We found substantial diversity in daily activity rhythms depending on species, sex and breeding stage. Individuals exhibited either robust, entrained 24 h activity cycles, were continuously active (arrhythmic) or showed ‘free-running’ activity cycles. In semipalmated sandpipers, a shorebird with biparental incubation, we show that the free-running rhythm is synchronized between pair mates. The diversity of diel time-keeping under continuous daylight emphasizes the plasticity of the circadian system, and the importance of the social and life-history context. Our results support the idea that circadian behaviour can be adaptively modified to enable species-specific time-keeping under polar conditions.
Keywords: circadian clock, continuous daylight, mating system, arrhythmic activity, Arctic, Calidris
1. Introduction
It's a cruel season that makes you get ready for bed while it's light out.
Bill Watterson, Calvin and Hobbes, 1995
Correct timing is essential in many biological processes. To time daily behavioural and physiological processes, most organisms use an endogenous circadian clock, which is synchronized to the 24 h day by one or more external timing cues, known as Zeitgebers [1–3]. The most powerful Zeitgeber is the daily light–dark cycle [3], to which most organisms are constantly entrained. However, in polar environments, the strength of this Zeitgeber is greatly reduced around the summer and winter solstices when the sun never sets (hereafter referred to as ‘continuous daylight’) or never rises. How do animals keep time under weak environmental rhythmicity? Polar organisms could (i) become arrhythmic, (ii) entrain to weaker Zeitgebers (e.g. diel changes in light intensity, polarization patterns or sun azimuth [4,5]) or (iii) rely on endogenous rhythms so that they ‘free-run’ with respect to the 24 h day. Only a few studies have investigated activity patterns under polar conditions in the wild and the existing findings are inconsistent [6–14]. In some polar residents, a seasonal absence or reduction of behavioural rhythmicity has been observed [6–8], but in various other species, rhythmicity persisted [9–13], or a free-running rhythm was observed [15–19]. In the arctic ground squirrel Urocitellus parryii, body temperatures were arrhythmic during hibernation, but showed robust 24 h cycles after emergence from the hibernacula and throughout the arctic summer [20,21]. In the autumn, after entering the hibernacula, circadian rhythms were free-running as long as the animals were euthermic [20,21].
The diversity of behavioural responses, even within the limited number of species tested, is surprising and suggests that a variety of factors may be involved in regulating circadian plasticity. It has been proposed that circadian clocks can be adaptively modified to enable species-specific time-keeping under polar conditions [6,12,22–24]. Thereby, clocks would be ‘fitted’ to specific aspects of the ecology and behaviour of an organism. For example, it has been hypothesized that for resident herbivores, the evident weakness of the biological clock is an adaptation that enables the animals to feed around the clock [24]. By contrast, insectivorous migrant birds showed continued rhythmicity of long activity bouts during the day and short rest phases at night [9,11,25,26], coinciding with activity and eclosion times of insects [10,13]. Adaptive modification of the circadian system was further supported by parallel findings on diel profiles of melatonin, the most important hormone involved in the regulation of the avian circadian system [12,27–29]. Although plausible, species-specific diel patterns have not yet been rigorously tested by comparative data from free-living animals.
Owing to earlier technical limitations of observing behaviour of wild animals over long time periods, most studies have investigated activity–rest rhythms in captive animals, often under laboratory conditions. However, there is increasing evidence that activity patterns described in captive animals (e.g. wheel running in hamsters) can differ substantially from those of wild animals [30,31].
In this study, we recorded activity–rest cycles in free-living birds belonging to four species that sympatrically and simultaneously breed in similar tundra habitat. We used a standardized, non-invasive, radio-telemetry-based method (see also [32]) that allowed us to follow individuals over several weeks on the arctic breeding grounds. Aiming to substantiate ideas that circadian clocks in the Arctic have evolved in response to the animals' life history, we chose species that represent diversity in mating systems and parental care patterns (summarized in table 1).
Table 1.
species | social mating systema | parental care patterna | sex | activity pattern |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
arrhythmic | entrained | free-running | ||||
semipalmated sandpiper | monogamous | biparental | male | pre-incubation | — | incubation |
female | pre-incubation | — | incubation | |||
pectoral sandpiper | polygynous | female only | male | entire season | — | — |
female | pre-incubation | incubation | — | |||
red phalarope | polyandrous, sex-role reversed | male only | male | pre-incubation | incubation | — |
female | entire season | — | — | |||
Lapland longspur | monogamous, occasional polygyny | biparental (female-only incubation) | male | — | entire season | — |
female | — | entire season | — |
We examined the timing of activity of males and females in four long-distance migrants: three closely related shorebirds (family Scolopacidae) and one songbird (family Emberizidae). Shorebirds are particularly attractive for chronobiological studies, because diverse activity patterns have been observed but rarely studied in detail [29,37–39]. Songbirds provide a useful comparison because their circadian systems have been studied extensively [27,28,40]. In the two polygamous shorebirds and the socially monogamous songbird, only one sex incubates, whereas incubation is shared in the semipalmated sandpiper, a monogamous shorebird species. In the two polygamous shorebirds, the non-incubating sex predominantly competes for mating opportunities. We generally hypothesized that seasonal constraints and variation in life histories will affect activity rhythms. Specifically, the narrow annual window of opportunity for breeding, combined with continuous light availability during the short arctic summer, should heighten pressure on birds to adjust activity patterns for maximal reproductive benefits (e.g. increase the duration of activity to pursue additional mating opportunities, or time activity in order to maximize foraging efficiency). Hence, the different mating systems and parental care patterns should relate to different selective advantages of maintaining internal clocks under almost constant light conditions. If so, activity rhythms should be species- and sex-specific, reflecting not only foraging opportunities, but also mating opportunities and parental responsibilities. A recent study on birds reported that circadian period length is heritable and suggested that properties of circadian clocks can affect performance in sexually selected traits [41]. Hence, the aim of this study is to describe the activity rhythms of the four arctic-breeding species, and assess variation in relation to mating system, sex and breeding stage.
In addition, we assessed the amplitude of the circadian system in the field by examining diel melatonin profiles in the semipalmated sandpiper because of its biparental care. It has previously been suggested that entrainment to subtle Zeitgebers would be facilitated by a low-amplitude melatonin cycle. We therefore tested the idea that in a species where breeding partners need close temporal coordination of activity, melatonin cycles had particularly low amplitudes [38,42].
Our results show that a diversity of activity patterns (24 h entrained rhythm, ‘free-running’-like rhythm and arrhythmicity) can occur under the same environmental conditions, depending on the life history of the species, the sex of the individual and the breeding stage. This indicates that the circadian system may be more plastic than previously thought, and that arrhythmicity is not exclusively found in polar residents. Furthermore, plasma melatonin rhythmicity was undetectable in semipalmated sandpipers, in contrast to melatonin profiles reported for arctic songbirds [12,27] and for shorebirds kept under natural central European day length conditions [29]. Our study highlights the value of studying arctic animals for understanding timing strategies and for testing the malleability of the circadian system under natural conditions.
2. Methods
(a). General field procedures and radio-transmitter use
We studied semipalmated (SESA) and pectoral sandpipers (PESA), red phalaropes (REPH) and Lapland longspurs (LALO) in June–July 2007–2008 in a 2 km2 study area near Barrow, Alaska (71°32′ N, 156°65′ W); (for more details on the study site, see [14,43]). The site consists of wet coastal–plain tundra vegetation. The birds arrive in late May to mid-June and experience continuous light throughout their breeding season. Despite continuous daylight, ambient temperatures and light intensity varied on a 24 h basis (figure 1). We captured birds with mistnets or in walk-in traps on the nest between 4 June and 13 July. From each individual, we sampled blood (50–200 µl), measured wing, tarsus, culmen and total head length (to the nearest mm), and took the weight (to the nearest g). Each bird was banded with a unique combination of colour bands, a green flag (shorebirds only) and an aluminium band from the Bird Banding Laboratory of the US Geological Survey Patuxent Wildlife Research Center. We determined sex based on morphology, behaviour and molecular markers [44,45].
We searched nests throughout the breeding season by observing the behaviour of birds, either when flushed during laying or incubation, or on incubation breaks, and monitored their fate. If shorebird nests were found after clutch completion, then egg development was assessed through flotation [46]. The start of the incubation period was estimated as date of hatch minus 22, 20, 19 and 12 days for PESA, SESA, REPH and LALO, respectively.
We equipped 142 birds (21 LALO, 42 PESA, 47 REPH, 32 SESA) with glue-on, light-weight (less than 5% of body mass) radio-transmitters (2007: BD-2, 1.05 g, Holohil Systems Ltd, Canada; 2008: A2420, 1.3 g, Advanced Telemetry Systems, USA) with a frequency range of 164–167 MHz. After release, all radio-tagged birds behaved normally.
(b). Automated activity recording and behavioural observations
We used two to four automated receiving units (ARUs), each connected to a directional four-element Yagi-antenna and powered by a car battery, to simultaneously and continuously monitor the activity of the free-living, radio-tagged birds [32,47]. The ARUs scanned through all programmed frequencies (deployed transmitters) in 1 min intervals, and recorded background noise and signal strength (in dB). Radio-signals were detected up to distances of approximately 1 km. Raw data were saved in receiver memory, and subsequently saved on a PC for further analysis.
We applied a minimum signal threshold of −129.89 to −119.87 dB, depending on the ARU and antenna set-up, above which the signal exceeded background noise. Signal strength did not change much when an animal (i.e. the transmitter) was stationary, but varied when the animal was moving or changed posture. Therefore, we used the change (Δ) in signal strength between subsequent 1 min intervals to quantify whether an animal was active or not. We determined threshold values of Δ signal strength to distinguish between activity and inactivity through field calibration by direct observation of the birds' activity. Using synchronized clocks, we monitored 36 tagged birds (nine LALO, six PESA, six REPH, 15 SESA; both sexes) for 1.5–6 h (average: 130 ± 17 min) and at 1 min intervals recorded behaviour (active: foraging, flying, preening, fighting, displaying; inactive: sitting, resting, sleeping, incubating).
We analysed Δ signal strength data with a general linear mixed-effect model (GLMM) with binomial error distribution. Models included activity (1/0; as determined from behavioural observations) as dependent variable, species and sex as fixed factors, and individual as random factor. Δ signal strength values differed significantly between active and inactive birds (see the electronic supplementary material, figure S1; GLMM, F1,1762 = 32.94, p < 0.001) but not between species (F3,32 = 1.56, p = 0.21). Based on this and similar analyses of data from other species (www.sparrowsystems.biz, ARTS activity manual), we used a Δ signal strength of 3.8 dB as threshold value to characterize an animal as ‘active’ (Δ ≥ 3.8 dB) or ‘inactive’ (Δ < 3.8 dB). This value equals the upper end of the 99% CI of the mean for inactive animals. Note that using thresholds of 3 and 5 dB gave qualitatively similar results. Missing data points (i.e. values below the background noise level) were caused, for example, by animals venturing outside the system's recording range, by brief system shutdown for data retrieval, by the antenna of the radiotransmitter being under water (e.g. red phalaropes are often found on water), or by the antenna being in contact with the ground or masked by the micro-relief features. The mean number of recordings per individual within a 5 min interval was 2.62 (95% CI: 2.52, 2.73). The mean number of recordings did not differ between species (likelihood ratio test of a mixed-effect model with ID as random intercept, χ² = 3.27, d.f. = 3, p = 0.35; number of observations: 340 980; number of individuals: 117).
(c). Melatonin measurements
In 2004, we captured 108 SESAs during the breeding season at different times throughout the 24 h cycle (see figure S2 in the electronic supplementary material for detailed sampling regime), either with walk-in traps on the nest (n = 78) or with mistnets (n = 30). Thirteen individuals were caught and bled twice, on different days (mean interval: 10 ± 1.8 days, range: 3–21 days) and at different times.
Blood samples were centrifuged for 10 min at 1500g to separate the plasma, which was stored immediately at –20°C for subsequent analysis. Plasma samples (40–100 µl) were transported to Germany on dry ice and kept at −80°C. Melatonin was assayed in duplicates by radioimmunoassay after extraction on diatomaceous earth columns (for details, see [48]). We conducted three extractions and two assays. Samples were arranged so that every extraction and assay contained samples from each 2 h interval. Average detection limit was 18.9 ± 0.1 pg ml−1, average recovery was 50.1 ± 9.7 per cent (recovery extraction 1 = 69%; recovery extraction 2 = 42%; recovery extraction 3 = 39%), intra-assay variation was 3.7 ± 2.2 per cent and inter-assay variation was 4.3 per cent.
(d). Data analysis and statistics
We performed all analyses with the R v. 2.9.0 software [49]. For rhythmic analysis of activity patterns, we created time series from the ARU as described above. These continuous activity records were used to assess significant circadian periodicity and to calculate period length using the Lomb–Scargle periodogram analysis implemented in ChronoShop (software developed by K.S. can be downloaded from https://www.nioo.knaw.nl/users/kspoelstra). This method can detect periodic components in datasets with missing values, and is therefore ideally suited for telemetry-based time series obtained from free-living animals [50]. The analyses were performed for periodicities ranging from 10 to 30 h with 4 min increments. The program calculates a Lomb–Scargle PN value, which is the normalized power as a function of angular frequency (ω = 2π/P) for all periods (P) tested, and tests whether this value exceeds the significance threshold. Period length was considered significantly different from 24 h if the average plus or minus the standard error did not overlap with 24 h.
For the analyses of activity cycles, we distinguished between three breeding stages: pre-incubation (before completion of the clutch), incubation (after clutch completion, before hatching) and post-incubation (after hatching). We conducted periodogram analysis only for individuals with more than 6 days of continuous activity records per breeding stage. We used the results of the periodogram analyses to distinguish between arrhythmic (i.e. non-significant periodicity), entrained (i.e. significant periodicity near 24 h) and free-running (i.e. significant periodicity deviating from 24 h) activity rhythms (table 1). Free-running birds typically showed fast cycles at first, followed by a marked increase in period length. In these birds, we estimated period length separately for a free-run before and after a change in direction (i.e. from period length shorter than 24 h to those longer than 24 h). Because we collected few data from birds during the post-incubation period, this period was excluded for detailed analysis.
3. Results
Actograms revealed substantial variation in daily activity rhythms, depending on the species and sex (figure 2). In the three sandpiper species, marked seasonal changes in daily activity patterns occurred even within individuals, in particular in association with the onset of incubation (figures 2 and 3, table 1).
The periodogram analysis clearly shows the absence of rhythmic activity patterns in the non-incubating sexes of the two polygamous species (figure 2c,f, table 1). Male pectoral sandpipers (n = 9) and female red phalaropes (n = 6) were continuously active over the 24 h period, without long rest phases. Lomb–Scargle periodogram analysis did not detect any significant periodicities during the entire recorded presence of these individuals. Similarly, no significant periodicity could be detected for any of the sandpipers in the pre-incubation period (female pectoral sandpipers, n = 2; male red phalaropes, n = 5; male and female semipalmated sandpiper, n = 4 and 1, respectively).
In contrast to the shorebirds, Lapland longspurs generally displayed a typical diurnal pattern of activity, which remained more or less constant throughout the breeding season (figure 2g,h). Rest times were approximately between midnight and 4.00 (figures 2g,h and 3g,h) when light intensity and temperatures were at their lowest (figure 1). Both males and females showed a robust activity rhythm with a 24 h period length (table 2). We also observed a diel rhythm with a 24 h period in the male red phalarope and female pectoral sandpiper, at least during the incubation period (figures 2d,e and 3d,e, table 2).
Table 2.
species | sex | period length |
n | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
mean | s.e. | range | |||
semipalmated sandpiper | male | 25.90 | 0.71 | 21.73–28.60 | 9 |
female | 24.68 | 1.97 | 21.20–28.80 | 4 | |
pectoral sandpiper | female | 24.05 | 0.02 | 24.00–24.07 | 3 |
red phalarope | male | 24.01 | 0.08 | 23.80–24.40 | 6 |
Lapland longspur | male | 23.98 | 0.04 | 23.60–24.20 | 9 |
female | 23.96 | 0.03 | 23.87–24.07 | 8 |
During the incubation period, males and females of the biparental semipalmated sandpiper showed remarkable activity patterns (figure 2a,b and table 2). The birds were clearly rhythmic, but period lengths were significantly different from 24 h, suggestive of a free-running rhythm. However, many individuals showed dramatic shifts in period length, and the overall range of periodicities was large (roughly 21–29 h). Thus, activity patterns differed from the consistent rhythmicity expected from endogenous, circadian clocks, and may have arisen from different origins or from weak entrainment. Although period length varied among individuals, there was no difference between the sexes (table 2).
In all samples, melatonin levels were below 18.9 pg ml−1 (detection limit), independent of time of day (see the electronic supplementary material, figure S2). Although in two out of three extractions recovery rates were low, we are confident that increased recoveries would not have changed our results. Even in extraction 1 with 69 per cent recovery, plasma melatonin was below the detection limit in all cases. Furthermore, plasma melatonin was not detectable in eight birds sampled during periods of low light intensity (21.00–03.00) despite large plasma volumes (more than 80 µl) and good recoveries (more than 70%).
4. Discussion
Despite similar environmental conditions with continuous daylight, we find substantial diversity in daily activity rhythms of free-living birds, depending on species, sex and breeding stage. All species are migratory and—except for the short arctic summer—experience typical light–dark regimes. Individuals exhibited either robust 24 h activity cycles, were continuously active (arrhythmic) or showed activity cycles suggestive of ‘free-running’ circadian rhythms. We now discuss each of these patterns in turn.
(a). Entrained 24 h rhythms under continuous daylight
Lapland longspurs showed a robust 24 h rhythm of activity during the day and rest during the (short) ‘night’ (figure 2g,h). These findings are consistent with observations that rhythmicity persisted under continuous natural daylight in other species, including birds [4,5,9,12,23], arctic ground squirrels [20,42] and bumble-bees [13]. Likewise, melatonin, a hormone that plays an important role in the circadian system, was shown to maintain diel rhythmicity under continuous polar daylight in longspurs [27,28] and in another passerine (willow warbler Phylloscopus trochilus [12]). Although plasma melatonin concentrations were drastically reduced compared with those of songbirds at lower latitudes, levels were still elevated at night (see figure S2 in the electronic supplementary material and [12,28]). Together, this suggests that the circadian system of the Lapland longspur remained fully rhythmic and entrained throughout the breeding season under continuous natural daylight conditions. A recent study suggests that daily fluctuations in ambient temperature and in light intensity or quality (figure 1) can act as synchronizers [28].
During the incubation period, we also observed a rhythm with a clear 24 h period in the care-giving sex of the uniparental shorebirds (figure 2e,d). These birds were also more active during the day than at night, which is consistent with studies showing higher nest attendance at night [51]. Synchronization to the polar day may be adaptive because of daily temperature fluctuations (figure 1). Diel changes in temperature are substantial at ground level (figure 1a), and cause variation in both the need for continuous incubation (eggs cool off faster at ‘night’) and in foraging efficiency (invertebrate food is harder to access at ‘night’ [11,52]).
(b). Behavioural arrhythmicity and reproductive sleeplessness
Our data show the absence of rhythmic activity patterns in all sandpipers in the pre-incubation period (figure 2a,c,d–f), and in the competitive sex of the two polygamous species throughout the season (figure 2c,f). A similar seasonal absence or reduction of rhythmicity has been observed in two polar residents: the Svalbard ptarmigan Lagopus mutus hyperboreus [6,7] and reindeer Rangifer tarandus platyrhynchus [8]. In herbivores, in particular, the evident weakness of the biological clock has been interpreted as an adaptation enabling individuals to feed around the clock [24]. Our data show that a seasonal absence of rhythmicity is not unique to resident polar vertebrates [24]. However, in sandpipers, which feed on invertebrates, arrhythmicity was breeding-stage-specific and sex-role-specific. In the two polygamous species, the incubating sex was rhythmic only during incubation, whereas the non-incubating sex was active around the clock. In early June, ground temperatures often dropped below freezing level during the ‘night’ (figure 1a), substantially reducing arthropod availability [52]. This makes foraging around the clock an unlikely explanation for arrhythmicity. Instead, we suggest that around-the-clock activity of arctic shorebirds represents ‘reproductive sleeplessness’ (i.e. around-the-clock competition for access to mates) [14]. In pectoral sandpipers, we showed earlier that a radio-telemetry-based estimate of activity coincides with high electromyogram activity and wakefulness [14], and that males that were the most active sired the most offspring. This suggests that arrhythmicity with nearly constant activity has evolved in a continuous daylight environment in response to intense sexual selection (competition for mates). In general, we would then expect to find arrhythmicity in arctic polygynous or lekking species with a strong mating skew.
(c). Free-running, sociable rhythms in biparental shorebirds
During the incubation period, male and female semipalmated sandpipers showed activity patterns with variable period lengths that significantly differed from 24 h. Our data provide preliminary evidence for social synchronization among breeding partners that ‘free-ran’ with respect to the 24 h day. For three pairs for which sufficient data were available, period lengths (male–female) were estimated as 27.9–28.8, 27.0–27.3 and 21.7–21.4 h, respectively. This suggests that the daily rhythms of pair members were synchronized with complementary—but not necessarily equal—activity and incubation patterns (figure 2a,b).
The basis of this rhythmicity and possible social synchronization is still unclear. Close synchronization with 24 h Zeitgebers (figure 1) can be excluded because of the observed range of period lengths. Tidal rhythms can also be excluded because they provide signals that are 24.8 h in length. Consequently, our results suggest that social cues directly related to reproductive activity may act as a Zeitgeber which overrules the 24 h Zeitgebers and which allows coordination of daily activity rhythms (i.e. incubation bouts). Although there is some evidence for entrainment by conspecifics [53], social cues are generally believed to be a relatively weak Zeitgeber and to affect timing mainly in the absence of other, stronger Zeitgebers. Thus, continuous daylight in the arctic summer might facilitate synchronization by social Zeitgebers [27,40,54]. Furthermore, the more weakly self-sustained a circadian system is, the more readily it is entrained by subtle Zeitgebers [29,40]. A recent study on two other wader species reported very low amplitudes in the daily cycle of the circadian hormone melatonin [29]. During the polar summer, melatonin is expected to be further suppressed by continuous daylight. In support of this idea, and in contrast to reports from songbirds [12,27,28], we found evidence that plasma melatonin rhythmicity was undetectable in free-living semipalmated sandpipers. Elevated levels of plasma melatonin were absent in birds caught during day and night (see the electronic supplementary material, figure S2). At present, we cannot rule out that there is a very low amplitude melatonin peak below the detection limit of our assay. We also lack comparative data from other shorebird species to determine whether melatonin was particularly low in the biparental semipalmated sandpiper. Nonetheless, the low amplitude of the birds' melatonin cycle may facilitate entrainment to social cues.
In support of social synchronization, observations of nest attendance [44] (figure 2) and direct observations of changeovers between incubating birds (S.S.T., M.V., K.S., B.H., M.W. & B.K. 2011, 2012, unpublished data) indicate that changeovers at the nest were mostly instantaneous. If mates initially differ in temporal preference, then social synchronization implies adjustment of individual schedules [55]. For example, the dominant bird might impose its own rhythm on the partner, or the observed period length might reflect a value intermediate between the period lengths of the initial rhythms (mutual synchronization [53]). Behavioural observations suggest that the timing of the incubation shift is determined by the incubating bird [43], but this is based on a small sample size. Variation in the length of incubation bouts may also reflect body condition of the parents, differences in the microclimate around the nest, or experience or age of the parents [44,56].
Surprisingly, we observed intra-individual shifts in period length during the incubation period in six out of 11 individuals (figure 2a,b). In all such cases, birds shifted from a short (less than 24 h) to a long (more than 24 h) period, and thereby increased overall length of incubation bouts. Thus, a potential interpretation of these observations is that individuals adjusted incubation bout length over the season, for example, owing to improving environmental conditions. Longer incubation bouts may be advantageous to decrease the risk of predation, assuming that nest detection by predators is highest during the changeover. This is not unlikely for the most common avian nest predators, the glaucous gull (Larus hyperboreus) and different species of skua (Stercorarius sp.), which hunt visually and are very common in the study area. Likewise, incubation bout length may be limited by the need of the incubating bird to replenish its energy reserves [44,57]. As it becomes warmer with the progress of the season, incubation bouts could lengthen, because energetic costs should decline while foraging efficiency increases. Previous work suggested that female semipalmated sandpipers incubate more during the night than males [44]. However, our data contradict this. The observed free-running rhythm implies that for each pair member the timing of the incubation bout shifts over consecutive days, such that neither sex will have to incubate or forage exclusively during the (energetically) worst part of the day. Thus, the fact that these birds do not seem to use weak Zeitgebers to synchronize their activity with the 24 h day may be an adaption to biparental incubation.
True free-running rhythms—based on an internal clock—have rarely been observed in nature and are usually much closer in period length to 24 h than the rhythms we detected [15–19]. We propose that the incubation/activity rhythms observed in semipalmated sandpipers are not true free-running rhythms reflecting an individual's internal clock, but rather rhythms that are the outcome of selection on incubation bout length and/or of processes of social negotiation. This suggests that such activity rhythms may be more generally found in biparentally incubating birds that breed under polar light conditions.
5. Conclusions
We here provide the first compelling evidence based on behaviour of free-living, arctic-breeding birds for the existence of marked plasticity and substantial diversity in daily activity rhythms among species, between males and females of the same species, and between individuals in different stages of reproduction, from a single site and a single, brief observation period. Our study highlights the diversity of diel time-keeping strategies, and emphasizes the plasticity of the circadian system and the importance of the social context. We provide descriptive data on timing strategies ranging from sociable schedules that ‘free-run’ with respect to the environment, to arrhythmic, continuous activity during the mating season, to shifts to entrained rhythmicity, all at one location during a single, polar summer.
Our results indicate that the circadian system may be more plastic than previously thought [58]. The results show that arrhythmicity is not exclusively found in polar residents, but occurs just as well in migratory species that live in a typical day–night environment outside of the short breeding season. Rather than being strictly determined by residence, our preliminary data suggest that circadian systems are fitted to the social system of a given species, at least under arctic conditions. The remarkable temporal plasticity in waders could be facilitated by the low amplitude of melatonin, in contrast to the situation in songbirds [12,27,28].
We suggest several avenues for further work. First, comparative analyses would allow formal testing of the relationship between daily activity patterns and life-history traits (e.g. mating system, migratory behaviour). To this end, activity data on a larger number of species breeding in polar regions need to be gathered. Second, the adaptive value of various activity patterns is still poorly understood. This can be addressed by studying the fitness consequences of variation in activity patterns observed within species (as in [14]). Finally, the free-running-like rhythm observed in a biparental shorebird suggests that rhythms with periodicities that are close to 24 h could arise from physiological processes other than circadian rhythms, although the mechanistic basis of the observed daily activity cycles remains unknown. Our study underpins the intriguing potential of studying arctic species for an advanced understanding of both animal behaviour and circadian biology. Under released temporal constraints, contributions of specific ecological factors to timing strategies can become evident, and properties of the circadian system, such as evolutionary malleability and its relationship to species biology and social context, come into view.
Acknowledgements
This study was inspired by discussions with the late Ebo Gwinner. We are grateful to Raimund Barth, Elisabeth Bolund, Wolfgang Forstmeier, Alexandra Hoffmann, Sylvia Kuhn, Peter Loes, Christina Muck, Ariane Mutzel, Holger Schielzeth, Kim Teltscher, Katja Temnow and Andrea Wittenzellner for help in the field, and to Ingrid Schwabl and Karin Hamann for help in the laboratory. We thank Richard Lanctot from the US Fish and Wildlife Service (USFWS) in Anchorage and Glenn Sheehan from the Barrow Arctic Science Consortium for logistical and general support, Bill and Jim Cochran from Sparrow Systems for technical help and support, David Longenecker and Robert Stone from the Barrow Observatory of the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) for information about local temperatures and light intensities, Manfred Gahr and Wolfgang Goymann for support and access to the hormone laboratory, Michaela Hau for discussions, and Gary Carvalho, Richard Lanctot, Dustin Rubenstein and two anonymous reviewers for comments on the manuscript.
Data accessibility
The data can be found at Dryad doi:10.5061/dryad.4g1g0.
Funding statement
This work was supported by the Max Planck Society and by the European Social Fund in Baden-Württemberg. The study was conducted under a USFWS permit.
References
- 1.Dunlap JC, Loros J, DeCoursey P. 2004. Chronobiology: biological timekeeping. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pittendrigh CS. 1993. Temporal organization: reflections of a Darwinian clock-watcher. Ann. Rev. Physiol. 55, 16–54 10.1146/annurev.ph.55.030193.000313 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ph.55.030193.000313) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Binkley S. 1997. Biological clocks: your owner‘s manual. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Harwood Academic Publishers [Google Scholar]
- 4.Krüll F. 1976. Zeitgebers for animals in the continuous daylight of high arctic summer. Oecologia 24, 149–157 10.1007/BF00572756 (doi:10.1007/BF00572756) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pohl H. 1999. Spectral composition of light as a Zeitgeber for birds living in the high arctic summer. Physiol. Behav. 67, 327–337 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00070-0 (doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00070-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Reierth E, Stokkan KA. 2002. Biological rhythms in arctic animals. In Biological rhythms (ed. Kumar V.), pp. 216–223 New Delhi, India: Narosa Publishing House [Google Scholar]
- 7.Stokkan KA, Mortensen A, Blix AS. 1986. Food intake, feeding rhythm, and body-mass regulation in Svalbard rock ptarmigan. Am. J. Physiol. 251, R264–R267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.van Oort BEH, Tyler NJC, Gerkema MP, Folkow L, Blix AS, Stokkan K-A. 2005. Circadian organization in reindeer. Nature 438, 1095–1096 10.1038/4381095a (doi:10.1038/4381095a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cockrem JF. 1991. Plasma melatonin in the Adelie penguin (Pygoscelis adeliae) under continuous daylight in Antarctica. J. Pineal Res. 10, 2–8 10.1111/j.1600-079X.1991.tb00002.x (doi:10.1111/j.1600-079X.1991.tb00002.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kureck A. 1966. Schlüpfrhythmus von Diamesa arctica (Diptera Chironomidae) auf Spitzbergen. Oikos 17, 276–277 10.2307/3564950 (doi:10.2307/3564950) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Remmert H. 1965. Über den Tagesrhythmus arktischer Tiere. Z. Morph. Ökol. Tiere 55, 142–160 10.1007/BF00399511 (doi:10.1007/BF00399511) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Silverin B, Gwinner E, Van't Hof TJ, Schwabl I, Fusani L, Hau M, Helm B. 2009. Persistent diel melatonin rhythmicity during the Arctic summer in free-living willow warblers. Horm. Behav. 56, 163–168 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.04.002 (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.04.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stelzer RJ, Chittka L. 2010. Bumblebee foraging rhythms under the midnight sun measured with radiofrequency identification. BMC Biol. 8, 93. 10.1186/1741-7007-8-93 (doi:10.1186/1741-7007-8-93) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lesku JA, Rattenborg NC, Valcu M, Vyssotski AL, Kuhn S, Kuemmeth F, Heidrich W, Kempenaers B. 2012. Adaptive sleep loss in polygynous pectoral sandpipers. Science 337, 1654–1658 10.1126/science.1220939 (doi:10.1126/science.1220939) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bovet J, Oertli EF. 1974. Free-running circadian activity rhythms in free-living beaver (Castor canadensis). J. Comp. Physiol. 92, 1–10 10.1007/BF00696522 (doi:10.1007/BF00696522) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Korslund L. 2006. Activity of root voles (Microtus oeconomus) under snow: social encounters synchronize individual activity rhythms. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 255–263 10.1007/s00265-006-0256-3 (doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0256-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Müller K. 1968. Free circadian period of Phoxinus phoxinus at polar circle. Naturwissenschaften 55, 140. 10.1007/BF00624264 (doi:10.1007/BF00624264) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Erkinaro E. 1969. Free-running circadian rhythm in wood mouse (Apodemus flavicollis Melch) under natural light–dark-cycle. Experientia 25, 649. 10.1007/BF01896571 (doi:10.1007/BF01896571) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Johnsson A, Englemann W, Klemke W, Ekse AT. 1979. Free-running human circadian rhythms in Svalbard. Z. Naturforsch. 34, 470–473 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Williams CT, Barnes BM, Buck CL. 2012. Daily body temperature rhythms persist under the midnight sun but are absent during hibernation in free-living arctic ground squirrels. Biol. Lett. 8, 31–34 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0435 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.0435) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Williams CT, Barnes BM, Richter M, Buck CL. 2012. Hibernation and circadian rhythms of body temperature in free-living Arctic ground squirrels. Phys. Biochem. Zool. 85, 397–404 10.1086/666509 (doi:10.1086/666509) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cockrem JF. 1990. Circadian rhythms in Antarctic penguins. In Penguin biology (eds Davis LS, Darby JT.), pp. 319–344 San Diego, CA: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- 23.Daan S, Aschoff J. 1975. Circadian rhythms of locomotor activity in captive birds and mammals: their variations with season and latitude. Oecologia 18, 269–316 10.1007/BF00345851 (doi:10.1007/BF00345851) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.van Oort BEH, Tyler NJC, Gerkema MP, Folkow L, Stokkan K-A. 2007. Where clocks are redundant: weak circadian mechanisms in reindeer living under polar photic conditions. Naturwissenschaften 94, 183–194 10.1007/s00114-006-0174-2 (doi:10.1007/s00114-006-0174-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Karplus M. 1952. Bird activity in the continuous daylight of arctic summer. Ecology 33, 129–134 10.2307/1931264 (doi:10.2307/1931264) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Palmgren P. 1935. Über den Tagesrhythmus der Vögel im arktischen Sommer. Ornis Fennica 12, 107–121 [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hau M, Romero LM, Brawn JD, Van't Hof TJ. 2002. Effect of polar day on plasma profiles of melatonin, testosterone, and estradiol in high-arctic Lapland longspurs. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 126, 101–112 10.1006/gcen.2002.7776 (doi:10.1006/gcen.2002.7776) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ashley NT, Schwabl I, Goymann W, Buck CL. 2013. Keeping time under the midnight sun: behavioral and plasma melatonin profiles of free-living Lapland longspurs (Calcarius lapponicus) during the arctic summer. J. Exp. Zool. A Ecol. Genet. Physiol. 319, 10–22 10.1002/jez.1768 (doi:10.1002/jez.1768) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Helm B, Gwinner E, Koolhaas A, Battley P, Schwabl I, Dekinga A, Piersma T. 2012. Avian migration: temporal multitasking and a case study of melatonin cycles in waders. Prog. Brain Res. 199, 457–479 10.1016/B978-0-444-59427-3.00026-5 (doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-59427-3.00026-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gattermann R, et al. 2008. Golden hamsters are nocturnal in captivity but diurnal in nature. Biol. Lett. 4, 253–255 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0066 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0066) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rattenborg NC, Voirin B, Vyssotski AL, Kays RW, Spoelstra K, Kuemmeth F, Heidrich W, Wikelski M. 2008. Sleeping outside the box: electroencephalographic measures of sleep in sloths inhabiting a rainforest. Biol. Lett. 4, 402–405 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0203 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0203) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Adelman JS, Córdoba- Córdoba S, Spoelstra K, Wikelski M, Hau M. 2010. Radiotelemetry reveals variation in fever and sickness behaviours with latitude in a free-living passerine. Funct. Ecol. 24, 813–823 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2010.01702.x (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2010.01702.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hicklin P, Gratto-Trevor CL. 2010. Semipalmated sandpiper (Calidris pusilla). In The birds of North America (ed. Poole A.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell Lab of Ornithology; http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/006 (doi:10.2173/bna.6) [Google Scholar]
- 34.Holmes RT, Pitelka FA. 1998. Pectoral sandpiper (Calidris melanotos). In The birds of North America (ed. Poole A.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell Lab of Ornithology; http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/348 (doi:10.2173/bna.348) [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tracy DM, Douglas S, Dale J. 2002. Red phalarope (Phalaropus fulicarius). In The birds of North America (ed. Poole A.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell Lab of Ornithology; http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/698 (doi:10.2173/bna.698) [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hussell DJ, Montgomerie R. 2002. Lapland longspur (Calcarius lapponicus). In The birds of North America (ed. Poole A.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell Lab of Ornithology; http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/656 (doi:10.2173/bna.656) [Google Scholar]
- 37.Daan S, Koene P. 1981. On the timing of foraging flights by oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, on tidal mudflats. Netherlands J. Sea Res. 15, 1–22 10.1016/0077-7579(81)90002-8 (doi:10.1016/0077-7579(81)90002-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Leyrer J, Spaans B, Camara M, Piersma T. 2006. Small home ranges and high site fidelity in red knots (Calidris c. canutus) wintering on the Banc d'Arguin, Mauritania. J. Orn. 147, 376–384 10.1007/s10336-005-0030-8 (doi:10.1007/s10336-005-0030-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Martin GR. 1990. Birds by night. London, UK: T. & A. D. Poyser [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gwinner E, Hau M, Heigl S. 1997. Melatonin: generation and modulation of avian circadian rhythms. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 439–444 10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00224-4 (doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00224-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Helm B, Visser ME. 2010. Heritable circadian period length in a wild bird population. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 3335–3342 10.1098/rspb.2010.0871 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0871) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Long RA, Martin TJ, Barnes BM. 2005. Body temperature and activity patterns in free-living arctic ground squirrels. J. Mammal. 86, 314–322 10.1644/BRG-224.1 (doi:10.1644/BRG-224.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ashkenazie S, Safriel UN. 1979. Breeding cycle and behavior of the semipalmated sandpiper at Barrow, Alaska. Auk 96, 56–67 [Google Scholar]
- 44.Creswell W, Holt S, Reid JM, Whitfield DP, Mellanby RJ. 2003. Do energetic demands constrain incubation scheduling in a biparental species? Behav. Ecol. 14, 97–102 10.1093/beheco/14.1.97 (doi:10.1093/beheco/14.1.97) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Griffiths R, Double MC, Orr K, Dawson RJG. 1998. A DNA test to sex most birds. Mol. Ecol. 7, 1071–1075 10.1046/j.1365-294x.1998.00389.x (doi:10.1046/j.1365-294x.1998.00389.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Liebezeit JR, et al. 2007. Assessing the development of shorebird eggs using the flotation method: species-specific and generalized regression models. Condor 109, 32–47 10.1650/0010-5422(2007)109[32:ATDOSE]2.0.CO;2 (doi:10.1650/0010-5422(2007)109[32:ATDOSE]2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lambert TD, Kays RW, Jansen PA, Aliaga-Rossel E, Wikelski M. 2009. Nocturnal activity by the primarily diurnal Central American agouti (Dasyprocta punctata) in relation to environmental conditions, resource abundance and predation risk. J. Trop. Ecol. 25, 211–215 10.1017/S0266467408005804 (doi:10.1017/S0266467408005804) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fusani L, Gwinner E. 2004. Simulation of migratory flight and stopover affects night levels of melatonin in a nocturnal migrant. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 205–211 10.1098/rspb.2003.2561 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2561) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.R Development Core Team 2009. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ruf T. 1999. The Lomb–Scargle periodogram in biological rhythm research: analysis of incomplete and unequally spaced time-series. Biol. Rhythm Res. 30, 178–201 10.1076/brhm.30.2.178.1422 (doi:10.1076/brhm.30.2.178.1422) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Norton DW. 1972. Incubation schedules of four species of Calidridine sandpipers at Barrow, Alaska. Condor 74, 164–176 10.2307/1366280 (doi:10.2307/1366280) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Schekkerman H, Tulp I, Piersma T, Visser GH. 2003. Mechanisms promoting higher growth rate in arctic than in temperate shorebirds. Oecologia 134, 332–342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Favreau A, Richard-Yris MA, Bertin A, Houdelier C, Lumineau S. 2009. Social influences on circadian behavioural rhythms in vertebrates. Anim. Behav. 77, 983–989 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.004 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Reierth E, Stokkan K-A. 1998. Dual entrainment by light and food in the Svalbard ptarmigan (Lagopus mutus hyperboreus). J. Biol. Rhythms 13, 393–402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Helm B, Piersma T, Van der Jeugd H. 2006. Sociable schedules: interplay between avian seasonal and social behaviour. Anim. Behav. 72, 245–262 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.12.007 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.12.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pittendrigh C, Daan S. 1974. Circadian oscillations in rodents: systematic increase of their frequency with age. Science 186, 548–550 10.1126/science.186.4163.548 (doi:10.1126/science.186.4163.548) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Tulp I, Schekkerman H, Bruinzeel LW, Jukema J, Visser GH, Piersma T. 2009. Energetic demands during incubation and chick rearing in a uniparental and a biparental shorebird breeding in the high Arctic. Auk 126, 155–164 10.1525/auk.2009.07181 (doi:10.1525/auk.2009.07181) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bloch G, Barnes BM, Gerkema MP, Helm B. 2013. Animal activity around-the-clock with no overt circadian rhythms: patterns, mechanisms and adaptive value. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20130019. 10.1098/rspb.2013.0019 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0019) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data can be found at Dryad doi:10.5061/dryad.4g1g0.