Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2013 Aug 5;110(34):13774–13779. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306440110

Understanding metropolitan patterns of daily encounters

Lijun Sun a,b, Kay W Axhausen a,c,1, Der-Horng Lee b, Xianfeng Huang a,d
PMCID: PMC3752247  PMID: 23918373

Abstract

Understanding of the mechanisms driving our daily face-to-face encounters is still limited; the field lacks large-scale datasets describing both individual behaviors and their collective interactions. However, here, with the help of travel smart card data, we uncover such encounter mechanisms and structures by constructing a time-resolved in-vehicle social encounter network on public buses in a city (about 5 million residents). Using a population scale dataset, we find physical encounters display reproducible temporal patterns, indicating that repeated encounters are regular and identical. On an individual scale, we find that collective regularities dominate distinct encounters’ bounded nature. An individual’s encounter capability is rooted in his/her daily behavioral regularity, explaining the emergence of “familiar strangers” in daily life. Strikingly, we find individuals with repeated encounters are not grouped into small communities, but become strongly connected over time, resulting in a large, but imperceptible, small-world contact network or “structure of co-presence” across the whole metropolitan area. Revealing the encounter pattern and identifying this large-scale contact network are crucial to understanding the dynamics in patterns of social acquaintances, collective human behaviors, and—particularly—disclosing the impact of human behavior on various diffusion/spreading processes.

Keywords: human mobility, behavioral rhythms, social networks, social sciences


Highlighting their importance in various temporal spreading processes (13), recent studies of human contact networks demonstrate an increasing interest in physical encounters (49). Contrary to nonphysical social contacts initiated by mobile phone calls, e-mails, and online social networks (1014), human subjects’ physical encounters take place with heterogeneous prior knowledge of each other, from acquaintances to unknowns, linking two individuals by their copresence in both spatial and temporal dimensions (15). On the other hand, with increasing human interactions, communities may also emerge from social contagion enabled by physical proximity: from not noticing each other, to unintentionally interacting, to intentional communicating, to mutual trust (9). We are tracing—for a large population—in this case for all of Singapore’s bus users, how these encounters are structured by both individual behavior and institutional structures. Bus use is a small slice of urban life, but one where “familiar strangers” will emerge (16, 17)—strangers who have been encountered frequently in daily life, but might never have been addressed. This context is one of many, which, in their totality, give residents the social background against which they construct their more intense relationships. We believe the joint encounter pattern is influenced by individual regularity (18, 19). A previous study based on the dispersal of bank notes suggested that human trajectories follow continuous-time random walks (20); however, considering the inherent regularity in individual behaviors, recent analyses of large-scale trajectories from mobile phone data and travel diaries indicate, on the contrary, that individual mobility patterns display significant regularity and remarkable predictability (18, 19, 21).

With the help of sensors and online networks, data describing close proximity in real-world situations sheds light on encounter patterns and spreading dynamics in contact networks other than diary-based surveys (4). However, these data collection systems are generally embedded in limited samples in spatially small-scale settings such as schools (6), conferences and exhibitions (5, 7), and even in prostitution (8). On a large scale, we still lack empirical data describing examples of both individual regularity and joint encounter patterns (other than simulating mobility and behavior patterns individually, relying on computational and agent-based models) (2224). Thus, given data limitations, studies on individual mobility regularity and collective interactions are traditionally conducted separately: the mechanisms driving our daily encounters remain unclear.

Therefore, with the increasing quantity and range of human mobility, a central task is to explore social interaction patterns along with mobility regularity. However, previous data collection techniques fail to offer high-resolution information on collective interactions on a large scale (across the population). In this context, individual-based passive data collections embedded in our daily life, such as credit cards and smart cards transactions, can be advantageous. At present, transit use might be the best proxy to capture the patterns of both individual mobility and collective interactions in an urban environment (25). Here, we use public transit transaction records to uncover encounter patterns (see SI Appendix, section I for a detailed description). This dataset consists of more than 20 million bus trips from 2,895,750 anonymous users over 1 wk (Fig. 1A) (about 55% of the resident population) in Singapore. The high spatial-temporal resolution of this dataset allows us to extract time-resolved in-vehicle encounters, defined as two individuals occupying the same vehicle simultaneously (Fig. 1B). Using these, a city-scale empirical temporal contact network is created.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Characteristics of transit behaviors. (A) Departure rate of city bus trips. Demand shows similar shapes from Monday to Friday. Two prominent peaks can be identified every day, indicating collective commuting behavior. (B) Time-resolved encounter network on one vehicle service (214 passengers) created using transaction records. Size of each node indicates number of encountered people; color and thickness of each edge indicate start time and duration, respectively, of each encounter activity. This figure explains use of smart card data to create an empirical encounter network in city-scale. (C) Probability density function Inline graphic of trip frequency f and number of encountered people n of all individuals. Symmetric pear-shaped pattern suggests linear relationship of Inline graphic. (D) Probability density function Inline graphic of trip frequency f and interevent interval τ between consecutive bus trips. Although diversity of individual transit use obscures the distribution, a remarkable area with Inline graphic can be identified through similarity in regular commuters’ transit behavior.

Results

Use of transit service in general, and buses in particular, is differentiated by ethnicity, sex, age, and income, meaning that daily transit use might exhibit social segregation as well. To address dependency and segregation of bus use on social demographic attributes, we incorporated two additional datasets aside from smart card transactions: population census and national household interview travel survey (HITS). For our case, although transit use in Singapore shows dependency on age and income, public transit is still the most important transport means for daily commuting trips across all Singaporeans (SI Appendix, section II). By studying transit use, we found that both trip duration and trip frequency can be accurately characterized by exponentially decaying tails, showing that people’s transit activities are limited in number and duration during 1 wk (SI Appendix, sections III.2 and IV.1). To understand individuals’ transit use patterns, we first measured the number of encountered people n against trip frequencies f for each individual. We find that the joint distribution Inline graphic has a symmetric pattern against Inline graphic, indicating the substantial number of people encountered in each trip (Fig. 1C). We then measured the interevent time τ between consecutive bus trips for the population, finding that Inline graphic shows clear temporal patterns with prominent peaks (SI Appendix, section IV.3), which is contrary to the non-Poissonian nature of Inline graphic in communication activities observed from digital networks (1014), suggesting the periodicity of transit use on the population scale. To explore the pattern of τ at the individual level, we grouped people according to their trip frequencies. In Fig. 1D, we added f to each τ as an attribute and measured the joint probability distribution Inline graphic. Observing the heterogeneity rooted in individual behaviors helps us distinguish regular travelers from other passengers. Therefore, given the transit use variation (and the analysis in SI Appendix, sections III and IV), we suggest that transit use is a good example for capturing both individual mobility patterns and collective encounter patterns, certainly for our case study.

As mentioned, an important phenomenon triggered by collective regularities is the familiar stranger, which is also crucial in explaining how likely it is that the same persons will be encountered again (Fig. 2A). To explore the pattern of repeated encounters on the population scale, we created an aggregated encounter network over weekdays and measured the interevent time Inline graphic between consecutive encounters of paired individuals, capturing 27,892,055 intervals from 18,724,388 pairs. We found that the distribution Inline graphic is characterized by prominent peaks at 24 h, 48 h, 72 h, and 96 h, displaying a strong tendency of periodic encounters covering about 75% of all cases, suggesting that the joint regularity also displays significant temporal periodicity (Fig. 2B). We also observed a decreasing pattern of Inline graphic, as the observed numbers decrease when interval Inline graphic is longer. To avoid bias, we extracted and grouped the timings of current and the next encounters Inline graphic over all pairs, so that for one pair with three encounters, two records are created [Inline graphic and Inline graphic]. We then measured the joint distribution Inline graphic (Fig. 2C). Strikingly, we found that the joint distribution presents reproducible patterns on the same diagonal with Inline graphic, respectively, where Inline graphic and Inline graphic represent the days of Inline graphic and Inline graphic, respectively, suggesting the homogeneity of daily encounters. To measure reproducibility, we summed the probability for each day-of-the-week pair, excluding the diagonal cases with Inline graphic, obtaining the density matrix:

graphic file with name pnas.1306440110uneq1.jpg

Considering the values in Inline graphic, we found that repeated encounters over the population can be modeled well as a Bernoulli process with probability of success Inline graphic, which is another factor behind the decrease of Inline graphic.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Temporal patterns of repeated encounters. (A) A typical temporal contact network of one individual with his/her familiar strangers (encountered more than once). Edge colors indicate start timings during the week. As observed, neighbors are also highly connected over time, suggesting network’s strong social connection and small-world property. (B) Probability density function Inline graphic of interevent time between two consecutive encounters of paired individuals. The prominent peaks suggest that paired individuals tend to meet each other daily. A general decreasing trend is observed as well. On average, pairs of individuals with repeated encounters can meet each other about Inline graphic times during the week. (C) Joint probability density Inline graphic of timings of two consecutive encounters. (Inset) Zoom-in of one density square. (D) Distribution Inline graphic of encounter time for four groups with Inline graphic, respectively. Inset of figures shows the distribution Inline graphic of intervals between rescaled timing of paired encounters Inline graphic. The prominent peak at Inline graphic suggests most repeated encounters happen at the same time.

To reveal the homogeneity of day-to-day encounters, we further studied Inline graphic. As the Inset of Fig. 2C shows, we found a strong diagonal on Inline graphic, which covers 85% of the cases, suggesting that most recurring encounters happen at about the same time of day. Despite this, we also observed two areas representing cross-period encounters, such as the first in the afternoon and the following in the morning on the next day. To compare the distributions of different day-of-the-week pairs Inline graphic, we grouped the pairs according to Inline graphic, respectively, and rescaled both Inline graphic and Inline graphic to time of day Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Then, we measured the probability density Inline graphic of encounter time by merging Inline graphic and Inline graphic (Fig. 2D) and the distribution of interevent intervals Inline graphic (Fig. 2D, Inset). Taken together, we found both Inline graphic and Inline graphic for different groups share indistinguishable shapes, indicating that the daily encounters can be characterized by a general temporal pattern. Furthermore, although afternoon peaks are longer and higher than morning peaks in daily transit use (Fig. 1A and SI Appendix, Fig. S6B), we confirmed that repeated encounters tend to happen more often in the morning, suggesting that collective regularity is more pronounced in the morning than in the afternoon (21). In this contact network, it is also implied that spreading via repeated interactions is more likely in the morning than afternoon. In addition, the prominent peaks at Inline graphic indicate that the most probable time for a recurring encounter is the same as the previous one.

Still, until now, the field lacked analyses to uncover mechanisms that drive encounters on an individual level. Therefore, we began to measure the encounter frequency of paired individuals, finding that the distribution Inline graphic of encounter frequencies can be well characterized by a heavy-tailed distribution, even though the network is very dense, explaining the emergence of familiar strangers (SI Appendix, section VI.3). Although Inline graphic is a good approximation of connection strength, it fails to capture the actual overlapping of collective behaviors when considering other external factors such as transfers. To avoid misinterpreting the data, we used total encounter duration Inline graphic to better quantify the connection strength of individual pairs Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the duration of their kth encounter. In Fig. 3A, we show the distributions Inline graphic (for all individual pairs) and Inline graphic (over all trips) in both log–log scale and semilog scale. We found that Inline graphic can be well captured by an exponentially decaying tail, whereas Inline graphic displays a power-law tail Inline graphic with exponent Inline graphic. The significant degree of heterogeneity indicates that collective d has overtaken the exponential bounded Inline graphic. To summarize the observation of pairs of travelers, collective regularities do occur, suggesting that encounter patterns are influenced by paired individuals’ behavior patterns.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Individual’s impact on collective encounters. (A) Probability density functions Inline graphic of sum of durations of repeated encounters and Inline graphic of duration of individual encounters on weekdays; the same distributions are shown in semilog scale as Inset. Solid line shows power-law with exponent Inline graphic, suggesting that although distinct encounters exhibit exponentially bounded nature, paired total encounter duration presents scale-free property. Maximum values of d and Inline graphic are Inline graphic (25.6 h) and Inline graphic (2.2 h), respectively. (B) Distributions Inline graphic of personal weight and Inline graphic of number of familiar strangers (Inset) of 1,626,040 individuals with Inline graphic. Solid lines show power-laws and dashed curve shows distribution Inline graphic of number of total encounters Inline graphic as guides. (C) Probability density function Inline graphic of absolute trip difference m for grouped individuals with different r (colored lines). (Inset) Mean value of Inline graphic for people with certain r, suggesting a direct correlation: the higher one’s encounter capability, the more stable one’s behavior. (D) Evolution of time-aggregated network of 783,247 individuals with personal weight Inline graphic. (Top) The fraction of presented nodes and size of largest component. We found these two curves indistinguishable, suggesting high network connectivity. (Middle) Evolutions of average number of encounters Inline graphic and average number of encountered people Inline graphic. (Bottom) Evolution of average clustering coefficient c during the week, suggesting small-world property of this aggregated network.

Next, on an individual level (see SI Appendix, section VII for modeling details), we propose a personal weight Inline graphic of individual i proportional to their paired encounter frequency:

graphic file with name pnas.1306440110uneq2.jpg

where Inline graphic is the set of encountered people and Inline graphic is the frequency of encounters between individual i and j observed. Thus, Inline graphic captures the likelihood of encountering familiar strangers. In Fig. 3B, we chose individuals who had recurring encounters Inline graphic and plotted the probability density functions Inline graphic of personal weight and Inline graphic of the number of familiar strangers, respectively. We find that both distributions can be approximated well by power-laws with high cutoffs, with the same exponent Inline graphic. It is important to note the great variation in person weights—that is, encounter likelihoods—suggesting that encounter patterns might be influenced by individual behavior patterns.

To explore how individual behavioral regularity impacts collective encounter patterns, we tried to quantify both individual encounter capability and transit use. First, to better measure individual encounter likelihood beyond travel time influence, we rescaled Inline graphic to Inline graphic for each individual, where Inline graphic is the total travel time in hours. Inspired by the k-means clustering method—for individual i—we used the absolute difference Inline graphic of morning and evening trips to quantify behavioral regularity:

graphic file with name pnas.1306440110uneq3.jpg

where Inline graphic is the mean of start and end times of the jth Inline graphic trip and Inline graphic is the mean of Inline graphic. Therefore, Inline graphic captures the time variation of daily transit use (the lower Inline graphic is, the more repetitive the individual will be; SI Appendix, section VII.2). To reveal the relation between behavioral regularity and encounter likelihood, we grouped the individuals according to Inline graphic and measured the distribution Inline graphic for each group. As Fig. 3C shows, individuals with higher Inline graphic tend to have less skewed Inline graphic, whereas those with low Inline graphic display a more skewed distribution. In addition, we took the average of Inline graphic over people with certain r, where Inline graphic is the 95th percentile of their m, finding that the average absolute difference reaches about 50 min for Inline graphic, whereas for those with Inline graphic, the value decreases to less than 15 min, significantly shorter considering that the expected headway (i.e., service interval) of public buses is around 10 min (Fig. 3C, Inset). In summary, we found that a larger encounter likelihood of an individual is strongly rooted in his/her behavioral regularity.

With these common daily physical interactions, the resulting regularity-rooted encounter network plays an important role in various urban environment dynamics like epidemic spreading. Given that most contact-network–based spreading models still focus on network topology (26, 27) or small-scale contact processes (58), identifying this real-world physical contact network is potentially important in studying encounter patterns and diffusion/spreading dynamics in large populations. To model the dynamical evolution of this contact network, we extracted time-aggregated networks of people with Inline graphic. At the top of Fig. 3D, we plotted the fraction of those individuals over the population, finding a rapid increase from 0% to about 90% on Monday, followed by slower growth afterward. We next checked the variation of average number of encounters Inline graphic and average number of encountered people Inline graphic, finding linear increases of both Inline graphic and Inline graphic without saturation, indicating that the people one may encounter differ from day to day, result in weak and random connections compared with social relations (Fig. 3D, Middle). However, we note that Inline graphic increases faster than Inline graphic, suggesting that “stronger” connections with familiar strangers are formed gradually through random encounters. At the bottom of the figure, we plotted the evolution of average clustering coefficient c, finding that the encounter network displays strong small-world property network with characteristic path length Inline graphic and Inline graphic (28) (Inline graphic, Inline graphic and diameter Inline graphic; Inline graphic and Inline graphic). Viewed as a whole, the empirical encounter network we illustrate here is a well-connected small-world graph, in which individuals are no longer confined to local encounters in one vehicle, but interact strongly with increasing number of people across the whole city from day to day.

Discussion

It has been assumed that human behavior and social interaction/contagion were connected for a long time; however, it is difficult to identify the link between them in observational studies (29, 30). Taking advantage of the availability of metropolitan data collection offered by smart cards in Singapore (this exercise could be very useful for other cities around the world), we tie together thinking on individual mobility with collective interaction patterns. Although the specific results are certainly embedded within the social profile and data of our study, questions remain: how can smart card data give such insight on social interactions, and how much do these patterns vary from context to context? As a result of various preferences and constraints on individual behavior, spatial-temporal patterns and collective regularity can be found in daily life, such as morning/evening peaks in transportation, degree of crowding in shopping malls and supermarkets at weekends and in restaurants during dining hours, and so forth. Transit use is only one of these social activities with a limited time allocation and specific locations. This study scrutinized only one of a whole spectrum of metropolitan patterns, although it is a critical one. We took transit users as our subjects and the definition of encounter in our study is limited to physical proximity or copresence, which is characterized by individuals occupying the same vehicle simultaneously. Considering vehicle configuration and loading profile (SI Appendix, section I), physical proximity does not necessarily indicate a more intense social contact, such as talking to each other, but does imply diverse interactions, from not noticing each other, to fleeting eye contact and close observation. As a core of social psychology, social contagion deals with thoughts and behaviors of others by innovation adoption, rumor spreading, and decision making. Although the similarity between social contagion and epidemic spreading was recognized a long time ago (31), in the context of physical proximity, social contagion depends more on familiarity than epidemic spreading. Thus, future questions are raised: how to measure the familiarity in the passive familiar strangers networks, and how to define the threshold of familiarity on social diffusion processes. Nevertheless, we know that social communities emerge from the increasing familiarity of individuals into collective forms: from unintentional and passive interactions to intentional and active communication, from mere physical proximity to mutual trust (9). We have shown the existence of city-wide structures of copresence in Singapore; the extent to which the bus users are aware of these structures has yet to be determined.

Unlike other social networks, where people interact within a circle of friends and acquaintances, we show an often-ignored type of social link: weak, passive, and indirectly enabled by daily encounters. Moreover, the structuring of physical encounters is demonstrated on a metropolitan scale. As a result of deep-rooted individual behavior patterns, our results also present the collective regularity of people with their recurring encounters as evidence, explaining the familiar strangers phenomenon in daily life. With the rapid development of information and communication technologies, richer data will be generated throughout our daily life (32). Although the role of such data is limited by an inherent tradeoff, it does not portray everything in detail. However, the emergence of such data provides us with considerable opportunities to enhance our understanding of the social world and its processes. Our work should serve as a base to better understand collective human behaviors, dynamical evolution of social networks (33, 34), and especially the impact of collective regularity on various diffusion/spreading processes (4, 35, 36).

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

We thank Peng Gong and Song Liang for discussion and comments on the manuscript, Karen Ettlin for copyediting the paper, Margaret Grieco for giving us important comments on the text, and Singapore's Land Transport Authority for providing the smart card data. This study was supported by the National Research Foundation of Singapore, the funding authority of the Future Cities Laboratory.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1306440110/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Holme P, Saramäki J. Temporal networks. Phys Rep. 2012;519(3):97–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Perra N, Gonçalves B, Pastor-Satorras R, Vespignani A. Activity driven modeling of time varying networks. Sci Rep. 2012;2:469. doi: 10.1038/srep00469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Krings G, Karsai M, Bernhardsson S, Blondel VD, Saramäki J. Effects of time window size and placement on the structure of an aggregated communication network. EPJ Data Sci. 2012;1(1):1–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Read JM, Eames KTD, Edmunds WJ. Dynamic social networks and the implications for the spread of infectious disease. J R Soc Interface. 2008;5(26):1001–1007. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2008.0013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stehlé J, et al. Simulation of an SEIR infectious disease model on the dynamic contact network of conference attendees. BMC Med. 2011;9(1):87. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Salathé M, et al. A high-resolution human contact network for infectious disease transmission. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(51):22020–22025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009094108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Isella L, et al. What’s in a crowd? Analysis of face-to-face behavioral networks. J Theor Biol. 2011;271(1):166–180. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2010.11.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rocha LEC, Liljeros F, Holme P. Simulated epidemics in an empirical spatiotemporal network of 50,185 sexual contacts. PLOS Comput Biol. 2011;7(3):e1001109. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Grannis R. From the Ground Up: Translating Geography into Community Through Neighbor Networks. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Eckmann JP, Moses E, Sergi D. Entropy of dialogues creates coherent structures in e-mail traffic. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101(40):14333–14337. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0405728101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Barabási A-L. The origin of bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics. Nature. 2005;435(7039):207–211. doi: 10.1038/nature03459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Onnela JP, et al. Structure and tie strengths in mobile communication networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104(18):7332–7336. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610245104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Golder SA, Macy MW. Diurnal and seasonal mood vary with work, sleep, and daylength across diverse cultures. Science. 2011;333(6051):1878–1881. doi: 10.1126/science.1202775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rybski D, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Liljeros F, Makse HA. Communication activity in a social network: Relation between long-term correlations and inter-event clustering. Sci Rep. 2012;2:560. doi: 10.1038/srep00560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Crandall DJ, et al. Inferring social ties from geographic coincidences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(52):22436–22441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006155107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Milgram S. The frozen world of the familiar stranger. Psychol Today. 1974;17:70–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Paulos E, Goodman E. CHI Conf Proc. New York: ACM Press; 2004. The familiar stranger: Anxiety, comfort, and play in public places; pp. 223–230. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.González MC, Hidalgo CA, Barabási A-L. Understanding individual human mobility patterns. Nature. 2008;453(7196):779–782. doi: 10.1038/nature06958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Song C, Qu Z, Blumm N, Barabási A-L. Limits of predictability in human mobility. Science. 2010;327(5968):1018–1021. doi: 10.1126/science.1177170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brockmann D, Hufnagel L, Geisel T. The scaling laws of human travel. Nature. 2006;439(7075):462–465. doi: 10.1038/nature04292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Schönfelder S, Axhausen KW (2010) Urban Rhythms and Travel Behaviour: Spatial and Temporal Phenomena of Daily Travel (Ashgate, Farnham, UK)
  • 22.Eubank S, et al. Modelling disease outbreaks in realistic urban social networks. Nature. 2004;429(6988):180–184. doi: 10.1038/nature02541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wang P, González MC, Hidalgo CA, Barabási A-L. Understanding the spreading patterns of mobile phone viruses. Science. 2009;324(5930):1071–1076. doi: 10.1126/science.1167053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Smieszek T, et al. Reconstructing the 2003/2004 H3N2 influenza epidemic in Switzerland with a spatially explicit, individual-based model. BMC Infect Dis. 2011;11:115. doi: 10.1186/1471-2334-11-115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pelletier MP, Trépanier M, Morency C. Smart card data use in public transit: A literature review. Transp Res, Part C Emerg Technol. 2011;19:557–568. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rohani P, Zhong X, King AA. Contact network structure explains the changing epidemiology of pertussis. Science. 2010;330(6006):982–985. doi: 10.1126/science.1194134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pastor-Satorras R, Vespignani A. Epidemic spreading in scale-free networks. Phys Rev Lett. 2001;86(14):3200–3203. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.86.3200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature. 1998;393(6684):440–442. doi: 10.1038/30918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Christakis NA, Fowler JH. Social contagion theory: Examining dynamic social networks and human behavior. Stat Med. 2013;32(4):556–577. doi: 10.1002/sim.5408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Aral S, Muchnik L, Sundararajan A. Distinguishing influence-based contagion from homophily-driven diffusion in dynamic networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(51):21544–21549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908800106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Goffman W. Mathematical approach to the spread of scientific ideas—The history of mast cell research. Nature. 1966;212(5061):449–452. doi: 10.1038/212449a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Vespignani A. Predicting the behavior of techno-social systems. Science. 2009;325(5939):425–428. doi: 10.1126/science.1171990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Nowak MA. Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science. 2006;314(5805):1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1133755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Palla G, Barabási A-L, Vicsek T. Quantifying social group evolution. Nature. 2007;446(7136):664–667. doi: 10.1038/nature05670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ancel Meyers L, Newman MEJ, Martin M, Schrag S. Applying network theory to epidemics: Control measures for Mycoplasma pneumoniae outbreaks. Emerg Infect Dis. 2003;9(2):204–210. doi: 10.3201/eid0902.020188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Christakis NA, Fowler JH. The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. N Engl J Med. 2007;357(4):370–379. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa066082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information
1306440110_sapp.pdf (944.3KB, pdf)

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES