Summary
Local recurrent networks in neocortex are critical nodes for sensory processing, but their regulation by experience is much less understood than long-distance (translaminar or cross-columnar) projections. We studied local L2/3 recurrent networks in rat somatosensory cortex during deprivation-induced whisker map plasticity, by expressing channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in L2/3 pyramidal cells and measuring light-evoked synaptic currents in ex vivo S1 slices. In columns with intact whiskers, brief light impulses evoked recurrent excitation and supralinear inhibition. Deprived columns showed modestly reduced excitation and profoundly reduced inhibition, providing a circuit locus for disinhibition of whisker-evoked responses observed in L2/3 in vivo. Slower light ramps elicited sustained gamma frequency oscillations, which were nearly abolished in deprived columns. Reduction in gamma power was also observed in spontaneous LFP oscillations in L2/3 of deprived columns in vivo. Thus, L2/3 recurrent networks are a powerful site for homeostatic modulation of excitation-inhibition balance and regulation of gamma oscillations.
Keywords: Somatosensory cortex, map plasticity, deprivation, channelrhodopsin, feedback inhibition
Introduction
Experience regulates multiple components of cortical microcircuits to mediate sensory map plasticity. Plasticity in long-range excitatory circuits (thalamocortical, translaminar and cross-columnar) is well characterized and often follows Hebbian rules in which deprived inputs weaken or lose synapses, and spared inputs strengthen or add synapses (e.g., Antonini & Stryker, 1993; Allen et al., 2003; Trachtenberg & Stryker 2001; Broser et al., 2008; Yamahachi et al., 2009). In contrast, the contribution of local recurrent circuits to cortical plasticity is much less understood. Local recurrent circuits help generate sensory tuning (Schummers et al., 2002) and include both recurrent excitation and inhibition whose ratio regulates sensory gain and information flow (Adesnik & Scanziani, 2010; Atallah et al., 2012). Recurrent inhibition from fast-spiking (FS) interneurons generates gamma oscillations (30–90 Hz), which synchronize local spikes and may promote formation of cell assemblies and information transfer to higher areas (Fries, 2009; Buzsaki & Wang, 2012). Thus, local recurrent circuits may be a powerful nexus for regulation of cortical information processing. Experience can alter some components of recurrent circuits (Maffei et al., 2004; 2006; Cheetham et al., 2007; Maffei & Turrigiano 2008), but the net functional effect, and whether this plasticity is functionally distinct from plasticity in long-range circuits, is not known. Local circuits are intermixed with long-range axons and therefore cannot be studied selectively using classical extracellular stimulation. Here, we use optogenetics to selectively activate local recurrent circuits and measure the functional changes that accompany map plasticity.
We focus on L2/3 of somatosensory cortex, which is a major site of receptive field reorganization during whisker map plasticity (Glazewski & Fox, 1996). Rats have five rows of whiskers, termed A–E, represented in S1 by an isomorphic map of cortical columns. Thalamocortical input arrives principally in L4, which projects to L2/3. L2/3 pyramidal (PYR) cells make glutamatergic synapses on nearby PYR cells (recurrent excitation), and on L2/3 and L5 interneurons that inhibit L2/3 PYR cells (recurrent inhibition) (Reyes et al., 1998; Kapfer et al., 2007; Lefort et al., 2009). Plucking the D row of whiskers in juvenile animals causes whisker map plasticity in which spiking responses to deprived whiskers are depressed in L2/3 of deprived columns, while L4 remains relatively unaffected (Drew & Feldman, 2009; Jacob et al., 2012). Major loci of this Hebbian weakening are the L4→L2/3 feedforward projection (Allen et al., 2003; Shepherd et al., 2003; Bender et al., 2006) and L2/3 cross-columnar projections (Broser et al., 2008). How deprivation alters L2/3 recurrent circuit function is unknown.
We hypothesized that local recurrent L2/3 circuits are a major site of compensatory (homeostatic) plasticity that stabilizes, rather than weakens, cortical firing during whisker deprivation. This could occur if deprivation preferentially weakened recurrent inhibition relative to excitation. Such disinhibition could explain the reduction in whisker-evoked inhibition observed in L2/3 pyramidal cells in vivo (Gambino & Holtmaat, 2012). Homeostasis is critical for cortical function, but remains much less understood than Hebbian plasticity (Turrigiano, 2012), and identification of L2/3 recurrent circuits as a major site for homeostatic plasticity would be novel. If a preferential loss of inhibition occurs within recurrent L2/3 circuits, it may also have an effect on gamma oscillations. Plasticity of gamma oscillations is a predicted effect of synaptic plasticity in cortical fast-spiking networks (Paik & Glaser, 2010), but has not yet been directly observed.
RESULTS
We studied functional activation of L2/3 recurrent circuits by optogenetic activation of L2/3 pyramidal cells using channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2). (L2/3 recurrent circuits cannot be selectively activated by extracellular electrical stimulation due to intermixed feedforward and cross-columnar axons.) ChR2(H134R)-EYFP fusion protein (Nagel et al., 2005), cytosolic GFP and the red fluorescent protein DsRed were co-expressed (using 3 independent plasmids) in L2/3 pyramidal cells by in utero DNA electroporation of Long-Evans rat embryos at E18 (Saito & Nakatsuji, 2001; Tabata & Nakajima, 2001; Petreanu et al., 2007; Miyashita et al., 2010). The separate GFP plasmid was needed to drive sufficient fluorescence for transcranial identification of P1 pups with strong S1 expression. We confirmed histologically that fluorescence expression was confined to L2/3 pyramidal cells and their axons, with no expression in L4 or L5 neurons (Fig. 1A, B). As expected, 100% of fluorescent cells had pyramidal morphology (n = 399 neurons, 4 histological sections, 2 rats). Overall, 21.1 ± 0.9% of L2/3 neurons (identified by immunostaining for Neu-N) also expressed fluorescence in the EYFP/GFP emission band (Fig. 1C) (n = 10 rats, 37 columns), consistent with a prior study in mouse S1 (Adesnik & Scanziani, 2010). Linear spectral unmixing of GFP and EYFP signals (Zeiss 780 confocal with multispectral detector) confirmed strong EYFP expression in axonal and dendritic membranes (not shown).
Optogenetic activation of L2/3 recurrent circuits
Circuit function was studied in acute S1 slices prepared at P17-21. Slices were cut in the “across-row” plane that allows identification of columns corresponding to A–E whisker rows using transillumination (Finnerty et al., 1999). Whole-cell recordings were targeted to presumptive ChR2-expressing or –nonexpressing cells based on DsRed fluorescence. This approach avoids unwanted ChR2 activation by GFP excitation light. We confirmed in histological sections that 94 ± 1% of DsRed+ L2/3 pyramidal cells also exhibited fluorescence in the EYFP/GFP emission band (n = 1421 neurons, 37 columns, 10 rats). Moreover, all DsRed+ cells exhibited direct ChR2-mediated photocurrent when patched (see below). Thus, single cells expressed multiple plasmids, and DsRed fluorescence was a valid indicator for putative ChR2+ neurons.
Only slices with strong, uniform expression of DsRed across all S1 columns were used (Fig. 2A). A 443-nm blue laser beam (CrystaLaser DL-445-040) was shaped using a pinhole and focusing optics and routed through a 4x air objective to the slice. Beam diameter (2 s.d. of Gaussian profile) was either 312 microns (spike threshold experiment) or 238 microns (synaptic physiology experiments), both of which fit in a single column (diameter 375–500 microns, Wimmer et al., 2010).
We first measured ChR2-elicited spike thresholds to determine the light stimulation parameters that achieve single-column photostimulation in L2/3. We made whole-cell current clamp recordings from DsRed+ (putative ChR2+) neurons (n=13) in L2/3 of the C column, and applied 2-ms light pulses centered in L2/3 of either the C column or adjacent columns (Fig. 2B). Synaptic transmission was intact, so that both direct photocurrent and network synaptic activation contributed to spiking. At each stimulation site, we identified the threshold photostimulation intensity required to elicit a single spike from Vrest (Fig. 2C). In the home column, low-intensity light (0.2 ± .05 mW) evoked spikes in every DsRed+ neuron, with < 0.2 ms latency to initial (subthreshold) depolarization. This latency is too short to be mediated synaptically, and is indicative of ChR2 expression in the recorded neuron (Cruikshank et al., 2010). Higher photostimulation intensity (> 0.5 mW) was required to evoke spikes from stimulation sites in adjacent columns and at the L4–L5 border in the home column (Fig. 2D). Thus, 2-ms light impulses of < 0.5 mW in L2/3 drive spikes in L2/3 pyramidal cells only within a single S1 column. All subsequent experiments used light intensity within the column-specific range.
Synaptic responses from the L2/3 recurrent network
To measure the functional output of the L2/3 recurrent network, we made voltage-clamp recordings from DsRed-negative L2/3 pyramidal neurons, and measured EPSCs and IPSCs evoked by light centered in L2/3 of the neuron’s home column. D-APV (50 μM) and saclofen (100 μM) were bath applied to isolate AMPAR-EPSCs and GABAA-IPSCs. EPSCs and IPSCs were measured at −68 mV and 0 mV, respectively (ECl and EAMPA in our solutions). Unlike DsRed+ cells, which uniformly exhibited short-latency responses (<0.2 ms), most DsRed− cells exhibited long-latency responses (>1.5 ms) consistent with network-mediated synaptic currents (Fig. 3A). Synaptic responses were only analyzed in long-latency cells that lacked direct ChR2 photocurrent. We confirmed that ChR2-evoked EPSCs were blocked by NBQX (10 μM) but not gabazine (1 μM), while IPSCs were blocked by both NBQX and gabazine (applied independently), as expected for disynaptic GABA-A inhibition (n = 4 cells) (Fig. S1A). TTX (1 μM) completely blocked ChR2-evoked currents (n = 4 cells), indicating that synaptic currents were not due to direct ChR2-mediated release from terminals (Fig. S1B). Pharmacologically isolated EPSCs and IPSCs reversed at 0.4 ± 2.5 mV and −66.7 ± 0.8 mV (n = 5 and 6 cells) (Fig. S1C–D), confirming that recording at −68 and 0 mV isolated EPSCs and IPSCs, respectively.
Prior studies of synaptic activation within the local L2/3 recurrent network in S1 focused either on output of a single pyramidal cell (Reyes et al., 1998; Feldmeyer et al., 2006; Lefort et al., 2009), recruitment of inhibition by spiking of 1–2 pyramidal cells (Kapfer et al., 2007), or excitation-inhibition balance evoked by strong or sustained network activation (Adesnik & Scanziani, 2010; Mateo et al., 2011). How progressive activation of the L2/3 pyramidal cell population recruits recurrent excitation and inhibition is unknown. We therefore first characterized recruitment of recurrent excitation and inhibition in response to increasing photostimulation of the ChR2-expressing L2/3 pyramidal cell population. Experiments were performed in whisker-intact B columns from P17-21 rats that had the D row of whiskers plucked for plasticity experiments (see below).
For each neuron (n = 10), we identified the excitatory stimulation threshold (Ethresh), defined as the photostimulation intensity that reliably evoked a detectable EPSC. We used a 2-ms light pulse to mimic transient sensory-driven spiking (Simons, 1978; Jadhav et al., 2009). IPSCs were invariably absent at Ethresh. Increasing photostimulation intensity (in multiples from 1.0 to 2.0 x Ethresh) evoked steadily larger EPSCs (quantified as charge integrated over the first 50 ms of the response), but more steeply larger IPSCs (Fig. 3B–D). IPSC charge increased particularly steeply between 1.0 and 1.4 x Ethresh, consistent with supralinear recruitment of a subset of recurrent inhibition (Kapfer et al., 2007). To compare relative recruitment of excitation and inhibition, we calculated fractional excitation (defined as E/(E+I)) at each stimulus intensity. Fractional excitation varied inversely with stimulus intensity, reflecting preferential recruitment of inhibition over excitation as L2/3 network activation increased (Fig. 3C–E).
To determine the relationship between light intensity and spiking in the L2/3 network, we first measured how Ethresh for ChR2-negative cells compares with spike threshold for ChR2-positive cells in the same column (n = 7 columns, 21 ChR2− cells, 20 ChR2+ cells). All co-columnar ChR2− cells had similar Ethresh, but intermixed ChR2+ cells showed a range of spike thresholds from 1.0 to 2.0x mean Ethresh for the ChR2− cells in that column (Fig. S2A). This is expected because each pyramidal cell receives excitatory synapses from many neighbors, so Ethresh in ChR2− cells must approximate the lowest spike threshold of local ChR2+ cells. As photostimulus intensity was increased from 1.0 to 2.0x mean column Ethresh, the fraction of ChR2+ cells that spiked increased nearly linearly (Fig. S2A), but ChR2− cells were not recruited to spike (0/15 cells, 6 columns). Thus, ChR2 stimulation does not drive positive-feedback recruitment of the L2/3 pyramidal cell network. This was due to powerful disynaptic IPSPs (Fig. S2B) that prevented spiking in ChR2− pyramidal cells, as shown previously for ChR2 activation of the L2/3 network in vivo (Mateo et al., 2011). Both fast-spiking (FS) and non-FS L2/3 interneurons are known to mediate this recurrent inhibition (Mateo et al., 2011).
Effects of whisker deprivation
To drive whisker map plasticity, the D row of whiskers was plucked from P12 until the day of recording (P17-21). This manipulation drives weakening of principal whisker-evoked spiking responses in L2/3 of deprived columns, which is a major component of plasticity in juveniles (Glazewski & Fox, 1996; Feldman and Brecht, 2005; Drew & Feldman, 2009). To understand how L2/3 recurrent network function is altered by deprivation, we compared ChR2-evoked synaptic responses in deprived (D) columns with spared (B, C and E) columns. Example cells from the B and D columns of a single slice are shown in Fig. 4. In the B column, EPSCs grew slowly with photostimulation intensity, whereas IPSCs grew more steeply, so that inhibition dominated excitation for all photostimulation intensities > 1.2x Ethresh. In the D column, EPSCs were recruited similarly, but IPSCs were undetectable until 1.5x Ethresh, and inhibition did not exceed excitation until 1.8x Ethresh. To rule out differences in functional expression of ChR2 between columns, we recorded the local field potential evoked by photostimulation in the presence of TTX, kynurenic acid, picrotoxin, D-APV, and saclofen to block spikes and fast synaptic transmission. This ‘photocurrent-LFP’ reflects summed photocurrents from the population of pyramidal neurons near the field potential pipette. Photocurrent-LFP magnitude was identical in the two columns across a wide range of photostimulation intensities (Fig. 4C).
Across cells, IPSC charge was smaller in D columns than B columns across a wide range of photostimulation intensity (p < 1E-6, 2-factor ANOVA, B vs. D, n = 10 cells each). The largest reductions were observed for IPSC1.1 to IPSC1.4, with 96.7% smaller IPSCs, on average, in D columns (Fig. 5A). EPSC charge was also reduced (p < 0.001, 2-factor ANOVA, B vs. D), but by a smaller amount (EPSC1.1 – EPSC1.4 were 41.5% smaller in D than B columns). To take IPSC1.4 and EPSC1.4 as examples (Fig. 5B), IPSC1.4 was 92% smaller in D than B columns (D: 69 ± 29 pA peak, 1.4 ± 0.4 nA*ms charge; B: 880 ± 352 pA peak, 13.9 ± 5.4 nA*ms charge, p < 0.05, t-test). EPSC1.4 was 45% smaller in D than B columns (D: 139 ± 37 pA peak, 1.2 ± 0.27 nA*ms charge; B: 272 ± 53 pA peak, 2.2 ± 0.4 nA*ms charge, p < 0.05 and p = 0.054, respectively). In contrast, there was no difference in Vrest (B: −69.5 ± 1 mV D: −68.3 ± 2 mV, p=0.6, t-test) or Rinput (at −68 mV, B: 329 ± 50 MΩ; D : 279 ± 31 MΩ, p = 0.4) (Fig. 5B, inset). To determine whether excitation-inhibition ratio was changed, we calculated fractional excitation (E/(E+I)) for each cell and stimulation intensity. Fractional excitation was significantly higher in D than in B columns at 1.1–1.4x Ethresh(2 -factor ANOVA, deprivation effect p = 0.001, interaction p < 0.05, Tukey HSD post-hoc test, p < 0.05 for 1.1–1.4x Ethresh) (Fig. 5C).
These changes in network-mediated synaptic responses occurred without systematic differences in direct ChR2 photocurrent (as assayed by photocurrent-LFP amplitude at Ethresh), or in the ability of light stimulation to drive initial EPSCs (assayed by the laser intensity required to elicit EPSC1.0), indicating that ChR2 activation was equally effective in driving spikes in ChR2+ cells in D vs. B columns (Fig. 5D). To determine whether deprivation effects were confined to deprived D columns, we recorded neurons (n = 10) in spared C and E columns, using a subset of stimulus intensities (1.0, 1.2 and 1.3x Ethresh). EPSCs and IPSCs in C and E columns were identical to B columns (ANOVA, p > 0.05), and were larger than in deprived D columns (ANOVA, p < 0.05) (Fig. 5B, inset). Again, ChR2 activation was equally effective in C/E vs. D columns (Fig. 5D). Together, these results indicate that D-row deprivation powerfully reduced recurrent inhibition in L2/3 of deprived columns, and more modestly reduced recurrent excitation.
Effect of deprivation on network oscillations in vitro
A major function of local recurrent inhibition, particularly via fast-spiking (FS) interneurons, is to synchronize spiking activity in the gamma frequency band, generating oscillations that are measurable both intracellularly and in local field potentials (Cardin et al., 2009; Buzsaki & Wang, 2012). Gamma oscillations have been proposed to promote synaptic integration, to select or bind cell assemblies, and to enable efficient area-to-area coupling for information transfer (Fries, 2009; Wang, 2010). Gamma occurs in awake rodent S1 (Sirota et al., 2008; Pritchett et al., Soc. Neurosci. Abs. 2012), but its functional role there is not understood. We predicted that reduced recurrent inhibition may reduce the capacity of L2/3 to generate gamma oscillations.
To measure gamma oscillations within L2/3 recurrent circuits, we photostimulated with light ramps (1.2 s duration, linear ramp to 2.5, 5, or 10x Ethresh, 238 μm spot diameter centered in L2/3 of one column). Calibration experiments showed that these ramps evoked spikes largely in the home column (Fig. S3). We made voltage-clamp recordings from DsRed-negative pyramidal cells and measured ramp-evoked EPSCs and IPSCs. In spared B columns (n = 16 cells), evoked IPSCs (measured at 0 mV) showed prominent oscillations in the low gamma range (20–60 Hz), as shown previously in slices from whisker-intact mice (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010). Evoked EPSCs (measured at −68 mV) had substantially less gamma power (Fig. 6A). In deprived D columns (n = 16 cells), the sustained component of the IPSC was attenuated, and gamma oscillations were substantially reduced (Fig. 6B–F). This was true for all ramp intensities. For IPSC10, the sustained component (0.3 – 1.2 s after ramp onset) was 68% smaller in D columns (B: 142 ± 27 pA, D: 45 ± 17 pA), and had 70% less gamma power (mean in 20–60 Hz band; B: 43.4 ± 14 pA2, D: 12.9 ± 6.4 pA2 in D columns), while the early peak (0–0.3 s) was not much affected (B: 611 ± 73 pA, D: 497 ± 80 pA). 2-factor ANOVA (whisker experience x ramp intensity) showed a significant effect of experience on sustained IPSC amplitude (p < 5E-5) and gamma power (p < 0.005). In contrast, EPSC sustained amplitude was indistinguishable between spared and deprived columns (ANOVA, p > 0.05), and EPSC gamma power showed a modest decrease (Fig. 6C, E) (p < 0.05). Thus, deprivation powerfully reduced the ability of L2/3 local circuits to generate endogenous gamma oscillations.
Spontaneous network oscillations in vivo
To test whether reduced oscillations also occurred in vivo, we measured spontaneous LFPs in spared and deprived S1 cortical columns in urethane anesthetized rats. Rats (n=5) were deprived of D-row whiskers from P12, and recordings were made at P30–P37 after 14 d deprivation followed by 4–11 d of partial whisker regrowth. LFP recordings (500 ms duration) were made in L4 and L2/3 of D (deprived) and C or E (spared) columns in each animal (Fig. 7A–B). Spontaneous LFPs were dominated by low frequencies but included periods with enhanced gamma power (Fig. 7B, bold epochs), as observed previously (Cardin et al., 2009). We compared LFP spectra between spared and deprived columns within each animal by normalizing all spectra to the average peak gamma power (30–50 Hz band) measured in spared columns for that animal. For L2/3, the resulting normalized spectra (averaged across animals) show that spectral power was reduced in deprived columns across a broad range of frequencies, with maximal decrease of 26 ± 12% in the 30–40 Hz range (n = 3070 sweeps in 14 deprived columns, 2617 sweeps in 11 spared columns, 5 rats). Power was significantly decreased in all gamma frequencies (20–80 Hz, tested in 10 Hz bands, α=0.05, permutation test) (Fig. 7D). This effect was consistent in 4/5 animals (Fig. 7E). Reduced gamma power was observable in raw LFPs as reduced LFP amplitude during gamma-containing epochs (Fig. 7B). In contrast, in L4 of the same penetrations, deprivation had no effect on average gamma power (3162 sweeps in deprived columns, 2517 sweeps in spared columns, Fig. 7C–D). Thus, deprivation decreases spontaneous (resting) gamma in L2/3 in vivo. We did not analyze whisker deflection-evoked gamma because any reduction is trivially attributed to the weakened L4 input to L2/3 (Allen et al., 2003; House et al., 2011).
DISCUSSION
L2/3 is a major locus of whisker map plasticity in post-neonatal rodents (Fox, 2002; Feldman, 2009), but prior studies of cellular plasticity mechanisms in L2/3 have focused on excitatory circuits almost exclusively. Whisker deprivation alters cross-columnar excitation (Finnerty et al., 1999; Marik et al., 2010), L4-L2/3 feedforward excitation (Allen et al., 2003; Shepherd et al., 2003; Clem and Barth, 2006; Bender et al., 2006; Hardingham et al., 2008; House et al., 2011), and local recurrent excitation in single columns (Cheetham et al., 2007; 2008; Bruno et al., 2009; Wen and Barth, 2011). These excitatory circuit changes are mostly consistent with reduced feedforward and local excitation within deprived columns, increased feedforward excitation into spared columns, and increased cross-columnar excitation from spared to deprived columns. These are appropriate to explain basic Hebbian features of map reorganization, including the reduction in responses to deprived whiskers and the strengthening and expansion of spared whisker representations.
In contrast, the role of L2/3 inhibitory circuits in plasticity is not understood. We hypothesized that deprivation preferentially weakens local recurrent inhibition in L2/3 relative to excitation. This disinhibition would constitute a homeostatic (compensatory) response to whisker deprivation (Turrigiano, 2012), which would be a significant modification of classical Hebbian models of L2/3 plasticity (e.g., Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998). Recently, in vivo measurements have detected deprivation-induced disinhibition in whisker-evoked sensory responses in L2/3 of S1 (Li et al., Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 2010; Gambino and Holtmaat, 2012). However, whether L2/3 recurrent circuits are a site of this disinhibition was not known.
We used ChR2 activation of L2/3 pyramidal cells to measure AMPA-mediated excitation and GABA-A mediated inhibition in local L2/3 recurrent networks (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010; Mateo et al., 2011). Whisker deprivation moderately reduced L2/3 recurrent excitation (by ~40%, for EPSC1.1–1.4), consistent with the known reduction in PYR-PYR local connectivity (Cheetham et al., 2007). However, a much larger decrease (~95%) was observed for recurrent inhibition, resulting in a substantial increase in excitation-inhibition ratio. These effects were observed in deprived D columns relative to spared B, C and E columns, suggesting that disinhibition was column-specific. Increased excitation-inhibition ratio was most pronounced at stimulus levels of 1.1–1.4x Ethresh, corresponding to initial network recruitment (Fig. 5). This is likely to be the most physiologically relevant activity regime, since L2/3 neurons spike sparsely in vivo (Jadhav et al., 2009; O’Connor et al., 2010). Thus, L2/3 recurrent circuits are a major site of disinhibition following whisker deprivation, and of homeostatic plasticity within deprived columns. We do not know how NMDA- or GABAB-receptor mediated currents were affected by deprivation.
This is the first study, to our knowledge, to use optogenetic stimulation to examine changes in circuit function after experience-dependent plasticity. Optogenetic activation was critical to enable selective, single-column activation of the L2/3 recurrent network without contamination from fibers of passage, feedforward input, or direct stimulation of interneurons. Furthermore, graded stimulation by increasing light intensity allowed us to characterize the progressive recruitment of excitation and inhibition in the active recurrent network, which provides substantial additional information beyond unitary synaptic physiology using dual whole-cell recording. Light ramp stimulation confirmed that recurrent inhibition was reduced substantially more than excitation, and showed that the ability of deprived L2/3 networks to generate gamma oscillations was profoundly reduced (Fig. 6). This reduction was also observed in vivo, suggesting that L2/3 network changes are physiologically relevant (Fig. 7). In contrast, prior studies of recurrent circuit plasticity were restricted to selected unitary connections, or inferred recurrent network function from spontaneous activity, which is relatively nonselective and influenced by multiple circuit components (Cheetham et al., 2007; Maffei et al., 2004; 2006).
Reduced inhibition as a homeostatic mechanism during critical period plasticity
Deprivation is known to weaken or delay early development of inhibition in L4 (Chattopadhyaya et al., 2004; Maffei et al., 2004; Jiao et al., 2006; Chittajallu and Isaac, 2010), but its effects during the critical period for plasticity and in L2/3 have been unclear. Visual deprivation during the critical period potentiates recurrent inhibition in L4 of V1 (Maffei et al., 2006), but evidence for such potentiation in L2/3 or in S1 has been lacking. We performed deprivation during the P10-14 critical period for maximal L2/3 plasticity in S1 (Stern et al., 2001), and found that L2/3 recurrent inhibition was strongly reduced. This is consistent with several structural measures of plasticity in L2/3 interneurons (Marik et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2011; Keck et al., 2011; van Versendaal et al., 2012), and provides a circuit locus for the disinhibition observed in vivo for whisker-evoked sensory responses in L2/3 of deprived (Li and Feldman, Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 2010) and spared (Gambino and Holtmaat, 2012) columns. Thus, the current results show that L2/3 recurrent circuits are a major site of disinhibition for critical period plasticity. The preferential reduction in inhibition is unlike the L4-L2/3 feedforward projection, where deprivation drives a parallel (balanced) reduction in excitation and inhibition (House et al., 2011). Because local recurrent inhibition was not reduced in spared columns (Fig. 5), disinhibition to surround whiskers in spared columns in vivo (Gambino and Holtmaat, 2012) must reflect either reduced cross-columnar drive onto local L2/3 interneurons or reduced efficacy of cross-columnar inhibitory axons (Helmstaedter et al., 2009). Deprivation also weakens L4-L2/3 feedforward inhibition in V1 and increases L2/3 network excitability, but whether local recurrent circuits are affected is not known (Maffei and Turrigiano, 2008).
Preferential reduction of L2/3 inhibition will increase network excitability and promote whisker-evoked spiking, and therefore is a homeostatic response to deprivation that co-occurs with Hebbian weakening of excitatory input (Turrigiano, 2012). Reduced inhibition may also broaden sensory tuning, depending on which interneuron subtypes are affected (Atallah et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2012). Deprivation-induced disinhibition in L2/3 of S1 also promotes LTP of spared whisker responses (Gambino and Holtmaat, 2012), and thus may be a permissive gate for subsequent steps in whisker map plasticity (Gandhi et al., 2008; Yazaki-Sugiyama et al., 2009, House et al., 2011). Consistent with this idea, disinhibition precedes associative learning and receptive field plasticity in L2/3 of auditory cortex (Froemke et al., 2007; Letzkus et al., 2011), and reduction of inhibition restores ocular dominance plasticity in adult V1 (Sale et al., 2010).
Sites and mechanisms for inhibitory plasticity
Reduced recurrent excitation and inhibition may represent the functional outcome of known structural plasticity in L2/3 circuits. Deprivation reduces PYR-PYR unitary connection rate in deprived columns by reorganization of synaptic contacts and local axons (Cheetham et al., 2007; 2008; Bruno et al., 2009) without altering intrinsic excitability or unitary EPSP amplitude (Allen et al., 2003; Bender et al., 2006; Cheetham et al., 2007). This is likely to underlie the reduction in L2/3 recurrent excitation. In addition, deprivation drives rapid loss of inhibitory cell axons (in S1; Marik et al., 2010), dendritic and spine retraction by L2/3 interneurons, loss of inhibitory axonal boutons, and loss of inhibitory synapses on PYR dendrites (in V1; Chen et al., 2011; Keck et al., 2011; van Versendaal et al., 2012). These changes are likely to contribute to the loss of L2/3 recurrent inhibition observed here. Reduced inhibition may also reflect physiological weakening of excitatory synapses onto L2/3 interneurons (Lu et al., 2007).
Many inhibitory cell types exist in L2/3 that could mediate the loss of recurrent inhibition (Gentet, 2012). One likely candidate is FS basket cells, which contribute strongly to disynaptic inhibition elicited by brief ChR2 stimulation in L2/3 pyramidal cells (Mateo et al., 2012) and generate perisomatic recurrent inhibition. Deprivation increases FS→PYR unitary IPSPs and does not alter FS intrinsic excitability (House et al., 2011), suggesting that reduced recurrent inhibition to the 2-ms pulse may reflect a reduction in the strength or number of PYR->FS synapses. This is also consistent with the loss of gamma oscillations (see below). We speculate that recurrent inhibition evoked by ramp stimuli also involves somatostatin-positive Martinotti cells, which generate dendritic inhibition (Reyes et al., 1998; Kapfer et al., 2007). The reduction in ramp-evoked inhibition may reflect reduced activation or output of these cells, including structural loss of dendritic inhibitory synapses.
Plasticity of gamma oscillations
A major function of L2/3 FS inhibitory networks is to synchronize PYR cell spiking in the gamma frequency range (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009). Gamma oscillations are a characteristic feature of cortical processing, and have been proposed to promote synaptic integration, bind activity in cell assemblies, and regulate information transfer between neighboring neurons and distant cortical areas (Fries, 2009; Sohal et al., 2009; Wang, 2010). In rat S1, short bursts of tightly spatially localized gamma occur during exploration, though their behavioral relevance remains unclear (Sirota et al., 2008). Synaptic plasticity within PYR-FS circuits has been proposed to alter gamma oscillations (Paik and Glaser, 2010), but experimental evidence for plasticity of gamma (or other cortical rhythms) is lacking. We hypothesized that by reducing L2/3 inhibition, deprivation may reduce the capacity for gamma oscillations in deprived columns. Indeed, we observed a 70% reduction in ramp-evoked gamma power in L2/3 of S1 slices, and a 25% reduction in spontaneous gamma power in L2/3 of deprived columns in S1 in vivo (Figs 6 and 7). Thus, the capacity for gamma oscillations is plastic and depends on sensory experience, likely reflecting local plasticity in FS recurrent circuits.
We speculate that this reduction in gamma power may impair sensory computation and reduce the ability of deprived whisker input to be relayed to higher cortical areas. If so, this would act to reduce the effective perceptual impact of deprived whiskers beyond the Hebbian reduction in whisker-evoked spike count alone. Thus, modulation of gamma could be an additional mechanism by which sensory experience regulates sensory processing or perception.
Summary
We used optogenetics to investigate the functional recruitment of L2/3 recurrent circuits during deprivation-induced whisker map plasticity. Deprivation modestly reduced recurrent excitation, but powerfully reduced recurrent inhibition, suggesting that L2/3 recurrent circuits are a major site of homeostatic plasticity. At the same time, the capacity for gamma oscillations were powerfully reduced, which is likely to reduce the effective representation of deprived whiskers.
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES
Experiments used Long-Evans rats, both sexes. Procedures were approved by UC Berkeley Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and meet NIH guidelines.
In Utero Electroporation
Timed-pregnant rats [18 days post coitum] were anesthetized with isoflurane. The uterus was lifted from the abdominal cavity, and embryos were visualized and electroporated through the uterine wall. A glass pipette (30–40 μm tip diameter) was placed the left lateral cerebral ventricle, and ~1 μL of plasmid DNA solution was injected. The DNA solution contained: pCAG-ChR2(H134R)EYFP-WPRE plasmid (1 μg/μL), pCAG-DsRed plasmid (0.5 μg/μL), pCAG-GFP (0.5 μg/μL) plasmid, and 0.05% Fast Green. The capillary was removed, and electrode forceps (CUY650-5; NEPA GENE) were placed on either side of the head, outside the uterine wall. Five 50 V square pulses (50 ms duration, 950 ms interval) were delivered via an electroporator (BTX ECM830, Harvard Apparatus). The uterus was returned to the abdominal cavity, and the abdominal wall and skin were sutured. Buprenorphine was given for post-operative analgesia (0.05 mg/kg, twice at 8 hr interval). Pups were born by natural delivery.
The purpose of the GFP plasmid was to increase overall fluorescence intensity to enable identification of expressing pups by transcranial imaging at postnatal day (P) 1. Only pups showing strong fluorescence in the EYFP/GFP emission band in S1 on P1 were used in later experiments. The purpose of the DsRed plasmid was to enable fluorescently targeted patching of expressing cells during brain slice physiology (EYFP/GFP fluorescence was not imaged during physiology experiments, to avoid activating ChR2 with GFP excitation wavelengths). All DsRed cells expressed ChR2 (see Results), indicating that individual cells take up multiple plasmids.
Whisker deprivation
Starting at postnatal day (P) 12, D-row whiskers D1–D6 and γ were plucked from the right side of the face under transient isoflurane anesthesia (3.5% in O2). This is a standard manipulation to drive whisker map plasticity in L2/3 of S1 (Drew and Feldman, 2009). Plucking continued every other day (for 5–9 days) until recording.
Slice preparation
Acute S1 slices were prepared at P17-21 using standard techniques. Pups were anesthetized with isoflurane, the brain was isolated, and slices (0.4 mm) were cut in a semi-coronal plane (Feldmeyer et al., 2002) that allows identification of A–E whisker columns (Finnerty et al., 1999; Allen et al., 2003). Slices were cut in low-sodium cutting solution (mM: NaCl 85, sucrose 75, D-(+)-glucose 25, NaHCO3 25, KCL 2.5, NaH2PO4 1.25, ascorbic acid 0.5, MgCl2 4, CaCl2 0.5, bubbled with 95:5 O2:CO2, pH 7.2) and collected into standard Ringer’s solution (mM: 119 NaCl, 26.2 NaHCO3, 11 D-(+)-glucose, 1.3 MgSO4, 2.5 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 2.5 CaCl2). Slices were incubated 30 min at 32°C, and then stored 1–4 hrs at RT before use. Column boundaries were identified with transillumination.
In each slice, the spatial extent and level of expression were evaluated by visualizing DsRed fluorescence with low-power epifluorescence imaging using a Rolera XR camera (Q Imaging, Surrey, British Columbia). (DsRed was used to avoid GFP excitation, which activates ChR2.) Only slices with strong, spatially uniform DsRed expression over all 5 barrels of S1 (e.g., Fig. 2A) were used in experiments. Whole-cell recordings were targeted to presumptive ChR2-expressing or –nonexpressing cells based on DsRed fluorescence using a 40x objective. In histological sections (n=37 columns, 10 rats) we determined that 94 ± 1% of DsRed+ L2/3 pyramidal cells also showed EYFP/GFP fluorescence, and 100% of DsRed+ cells showed direct ChR2 photocurrents, indicating that cells took up multiple plasmids (see below). This validates use of DsRed fluorescence to identify putative ChR2+ neurons.
Electrophysiology
Whole-cell recordings were made at 31°C using 2.5–4 MΩ pipettes and a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale CA). Current clamp recordings to measure ChR2-evoked spiking were made using K gluconate internal (mM: 116 K gluconate, 20 HEPES, 6 KCl, 2 NaCl, 0.5 EGTA, 4 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, 5 Na2phosphocreatine, pH 7.2, 295 mOsm). Recordings were made in Ringer’s solution. Voltage clamp recordings to measure ChR2-evoked synaptic currents were made using Cs gluconate internal with QX-314 and BAPTA (mM: 108 D-gluconic acid, 108 CsOH, 20 HEPES, 5 tetraethylammonium-Cl, 2.8 NaCl, 0.4 EGTA, 4 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, 5 BAPTA, 5 QX-314 bromide, pH 7.2, 290 mOsm). Pipette capacitance was neutralized, and whole-cell capacitance and series resistance were compensated (prediction and correction, 80%). The bath solution was Ringer’s with D-APV (50 μM) and saclofen (100 μM). To measure the photo-LFP associated with direct ChR2 photocurrents, a 1.8–2.2 MΩ field potential pipette was used, and TTX citrate (50 μM), kynurenic acid (2 mM) and picrotoxin (100 μM) were added to the bath. All drugs were from Tocris.
Rinput and Rseries were monitored in each sweep in response to a 5-mV test pulse. Recordings were targeted to pyramidal-shaped somata. Cells were excluded if Vmat break-in was > −60 mV, Rseries > 20 MΩ or Rinput < 100 MΩ. Vm values for voltage clamp recordings were corrected for the measured liquid junction potential (12 mV). Data acquisition and analysis used custom software in IGOR Pro (Wavemetrics, Portland, OR) and Matlab.
ChR2 activation
A 443-nm blue laser (40 mW, CrystaLaser DL445-040) was coupled via a multimodal fiber to the microscope epifluorescence arm, and projected to the slice through a 4x air objective. Focusing optics and a pinhole set the beam diameter at the slice to 238 um (2 x s.d. of Gaussian profile) for synaptic physiology experiments or 312 microns for the spike threshold experiment. Both of these are smaller than the width of a single barrel column in rats (diameter 375–500 microns, Wimmer et al., 2010). Laser intensity and timing were controlled by analog voltage commands generated in IGOR Pro. Laser intensity at the sample was calibrated using a light meter (Newport 1918-C). The lowest possible light intensities were always used to avoid phototoxicity.
Synaptic responses
Synaptic responses were defined as light-evoked currents with latency > 1 ms in non-fluorescent L2/3 pyramidal neurons (see below). Synaptic responses were measured in D-APV (50 μM) and saclofen (100 μM), and thus primarily reflect AMPA and GABA-A currents. The light stimulus was centered in L2/3 of the home column for the recorded neuron. For each cell, we first determined the light intensity (using a 2-ms light pulse) required to elicit a reliable synaptic response, which was invariably an EPSC. This light intensity was defined as excitatory threshold (Ethresh).
For cells in the impulse experiment, we measured light-evoked currents at −68 mV (presumed EPSCs) and 0 mV (presumed IPSCs) at 1.0, 1.1, 1.2, 1.3, 1.4, 1.5, 1.6, 1.8, and 2.0 x Ethresh to define an input-output curve for synaptic currents (10–14 sweeps at each holding potential and stimulus intensity, 10 s isi). Note that because driving force is equal for EPSCs and IPSCs, reporting current or conductance is equivalent. PSC magnitude was measured as charge (integrated current) in the first 50 ms of the response, relative to a 2-ms baseline prior to the light stimulus. Fractional excitation (E/(E+I)) was calculated from PSC charge. Rseries compensation was checked and corrected several times during each recording, and cells were discarded if uncompensated Rseries exceeded 20 MΩ. For cells in the ramp experiment, we measured currents at −68 and 0 mV in response to a linear ramp of light intensity (1.2 s duration) from 0 mW to either 2.5, 5, or 10 x Ethresh (5 sweeps at each holding potential at each stimulus intensity, 30 sec isi). Oscillations were analyzed during the last 900 ms of the ramp stimulus, by calculating power spectra on individual sweeps, in both Igor and Matlab (Chronux toolbox). Gamma in slices was defined as power at 20–80 Hz, which is slightly lower than in vivo (Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010).
In vivo LFP recording
D-row whiskers were plucked from P12-26 under transient isoflurane anesthesia. At P30–P37, rats were anesthetized with urethane (1.5 g/kg i.p., plus 10% maintenance doses as needed). A craniotomy was made over S1 (2.5 mm caudal, 5.2 mm lateral from bregma). LFPs were recorded using a tungsten electrode (5 ± 1 MΩ, FHC), amplified 1000x, bandpass filtered (10–300 Hz), digitized at 44.1 kHz using custom routines in Igor Pro, and downsampled to 1 kHz offline. Data are from 500-ms spontaneous activity periods (no whisker stimulation) collected every 10 sec. A small whisker deflection was applied after each spontaneous period, but was not analyzed here. Recording depth was 491 ± 12 μm (n=24) for L2/3, and 772.8 ± 7 μm for L4 (n=24), as previously calibrated for Long-Evans rats at this age (Celikel et al., 2004). Penetrations were targeted to both deprived (D) and spared (C or E) columns in each animal, with randomized recording order. Approximately 240 sweeps were recorded in each layer per penetration (L2/3: 237 ± 4 sweeps, L4: 237 ± 9 sweeps). After all recordings were finished, electrolytic lesions were made in L4 (5 μA, 10 s, tip negative) and recording sites reconstructed relative to barrel boundaries, as revealed by cytochrome oxidase staining in flattened tangential sections (Li et al., 2009). At least one deprived and one spared column were recorded in each animal, enabling within-animal comparison of deprived vs. spared column LFPs.
The in vivo gamma experiments used longer deprivation (14 d) than the slice experiments (5–9 d). Deprivation from 7–20 days duration drives similar weakening of whisker responses in L2/3 in vivo (Glazewski and Fox, 1996). While network function and plasticity mechanisms can differ with age, gamma was reduced in both experiments, suggesting it is a conserved feature of plasticity across this age range.
In vivo LFP analysis
L2/3 and L4 spectra were analyzed separately. In 2/5 animals, slight 60 Hz recording noise was present, so LFP waveforms were notch filtered at 60 Hz (0.05 dB stopband attenuation, applied to all spared and all deprived recordings in those animals). LFPs were whitened using a 2nd order auto-regression algorithm (Schneider and Neumaier 2001, Minlebaev et al. 2011) whose coefficients were determined by fitting all LFPs as a group. Spectra were calculated from each whitened LFP segment using the Chronux toolbox in MATLAB (3 tapers, 4 Hz bandwidth, 500 ms windows). To compare spectra between deprived and spared columns, all spectra from a single animal were divided by the peak power in the 30–50 Hz band of the average spared spectrum for that animal. These normalized spectra were then averaged across animals to determine the mean normalized spectra in spared and deprived columns. Confidence intervals (99%) were determined by jack-knifing (Chronux). Statistical differences were tested for average power in 10-Hz bins using a random permutation test in which spared/deprived labels were randomized without replacement (5 × 105 permutations, α=0.05, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons). Representative LFP segments (Fig. 7B) were selected as those segments whose spectra showed the minimum least-squared error relative to the average spectrum. The highlighted 40-Hz epochs were identified by band-pass filtering the raw LFP (40 ± 7.5 Hz, Butterworth), and highlighting segments with band-passed amplitude > 1 SD above the mean for that layer and condition (spared or deprived).
Histology
Rats were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB), and the brain was removed. Brains were post-fixed for 2 hr, cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in 0.1 M PB, and sectioned (50 μm) coronally on a freezing microtome. To determine the fraction of electroporated cells, sections were co-stained for the pan-neuronal marker Neu-N by incubating free-floating sections for 40–48 hr at 4° C with mouse anti-NeuN antibody (1:400, Millipore, MAB377) in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing 0.5% Triton X-100 and 5% normal goat serum. Sections were rinsed with PBS and incubated for 90 min at room temperature in Alexa-594 conjugated goat anti-mouse secondary (1:200, Invitrogen). Sections were rinsed 3 times in PB and mounted in Vectashield. Images were obtained by confocal microscopy (Zeiss LSM 710 Axio Observer).
Statistics
Reported values are mean ± SEM unless otherwise noted. Medians and 95% confidence intervals were generated by bootstrapping (1000 resamplings) from the original distributions.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Prof. K. Deisseroth (Stanford University) for the ChR2-H134R-EYFP-WPRE construct. Supported by NIH 1R01 NS073912 and NSF grant # SMA 1041755 to the Temporal Dynamics of Learning Center, an NSF Science of Learning Center. BRI was supported by National Science Foundation predoctoral fellowship DGE 1106400.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Adesnik H, Scanziani M. Lateral competition for cortical space by layer-specific horizontal circuits. Nature. 2010;464:1155–60. doi: 10.1038/nature08935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen CB, Celikel T, Feldman DE. Long-term depression induced by sensory deprivation during cortical map plasticity in vivo. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:291–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antonini A, Stryker MP. Rapid remodeling of axonal arbors in the visual cortex. Science. 1993;260:1819–21. doi: 10.1126/science.8511592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atallah BV, Bruns W, Carandini M, Scanziani M. Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly transform cortical responses to visual stimuli. Neuron. 2012;73:159–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bender KJ, Allen CB, Bender VA, Feldman DE. Synaptic basis for whisker deprivation-induced synaptic depression in rat somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4155–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0175-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broser P, Grinevich V, Osten P, Sakmann B, Wallace DJ. Critical period plasticity of axonal arbors of layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons in rat somatosensory cortex: layer-specific reduction of projections into deprived cortical columns. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:1588–603. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruno RM, Hahn TT, Wallace DJ, de Kock CP, Sakmann B. Sensory experience alters specific branches of individual corticocortical axons during development. J Neurosci. 2009;29:3172–81. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5911-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonomano DV, Merzenich MM. Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1998;21:149–86. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Wang X-J. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:203–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Carlén M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai LH, Moore CI. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–7. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celikel T, Szostak VA, Feldman DE. Modulation of spike timing by sensory deprivation during induction of cortical map plasticity. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:534–41. doi: 10.1038/nn1222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chattopadhyaya B, Di Cristo G, Higashiyama H, Knott GW, Kuhlman SJ, Welker E, Huang ZJ. Experience and activity-dependent maturation of perisomatic GABAergic innervation in primary visual cortex during a postnatal critical period. J Neurosci. 2004;24:9598–611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1851-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheetham CE, Hammond MS, Edwards CE, Finnerty GT. Sensory experience alters cortical connectivity and synaptic function site specifically. J Neurosci. 2007;27:3456–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5143-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheetham CE, Hammond MS, McFarlane R, Finnerty GT. Altered sensory experience induces targeted rewiring of local excitatory connections in mature neocortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:9249–60. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2974-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chittajallu R, Isaac JT. Emergence of cortical inhibition by coordinated sensory-driven plasticity at distinct synaptic loci. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:1240–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.2639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen JL, Lin WC, Cha JW, So PT, Kubota Y, Nedivi E. Structural basis for the role of inhibition in facilitating adult brain plasticity. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(5):587–94. doi: 10.1038/nn.2799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clem RL, Barth A. Pathway-specific trafficking of native AMPARs by in vivo experience. Neuron. 2006;49:663–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruikshank SJ, Urabe H, Nurmikko AV, Connors BW. Pathway-specific feedforward circuits between thalamus and neocortex revealed by selective optical stimulation of axons. Neuron. 2010;65:230–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drew PJ, Feldman DE. Intrinsic signal imaging of deprivation-induced contraction of whisker representations in rat somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:331–48. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman DE, Brecht M. Map plasticity in somatosensory cortex. Science. 2005;310:810–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1115807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer D, Lübke J, Silver RA, Sakmann B. Synaptic connections between layer 4 spiny neurone-layer 2/3 pyramidal cell pairs in juvenile rat barrel cortex: physiology and anatomy of interlaminar signalling within a cortical column. J Physiol. 2002;538:803–22. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.012959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer D, Lübke J, Sakmann B. Efficacy and connectivity of intracolumnar pairs of layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the barrel cortex of juvenile rats. J Physiol. 2006;575:583–602. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.105106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finnerty GT, Roberts LS, Connors BW. Sensory experience modifies the short-term dynamics of neocortical synapses. Nature. 1999;400:367–71. doi: 10.1038/22553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox K. Anatomical pathways and molecular mechanisms for plasticity in the barrel cortex. Neuroscience. 2002;111:799–814. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00027-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P. Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:209–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froemke RC, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. A synaptic memory trace for cortical receptive field plasticity. Nature. 2007;450:425–9. doi: 10.1038/nature06289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gambino F, Holtmaat A. Spike-timing-dependent potentiation of sensory surround in the somatosensory cortex is facilitated by deprivation-mediated disinhibition. Neuron. 2012;75:490–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gandhi SP, Yanagawa Y, Stryker MP. Delayed plasticity of inhibitory neurons in developing visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:16797–802. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806159105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentet LJ. Functional diversity of supragranular GABAergic neurons in the barrel cortex. Front Neural Circuits. 2012;6:52. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glazewski S, Fox K. Time course of experience-dependent synaptic potentiation and depression in barrel cortex of adolescent rats. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1714–29. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.4.1714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardingham N, Wright N, Dachtler J, Fox K. Sensory deprivation unmasks a PKA-dependent synaptic plasticity mechanism that operates in parallel with CaMKII. Neuron. 2008;60:861–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmstaedter M, Sakmann B, Feldmeyer D. Neuronal correlates of local, lateral, and translaminar inhibition with reference to cortical columns. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:926–37. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- House DR, Elstrott J, Koh E, Chung J, Feldman DE. Parallel regulation of feedforward inhibition and excitation during whisker map plasticity. Neuron. 2011;72:819–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacob V, Petreanu L, Wright N, Svoboda K, Fox K. Regular spiking and intrinsic bursting pyramidal cells show orthogonal forms of experience-dependent plasticity in layer V of barrel cortex. Neuron. 2012;73:391–404. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadhav SP, Wolfe J, Feldman DE. Sparse temporal coding of elementary tactile features during active whisker sensation. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:792–800. doi: 10.1038/nn.2328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiao Y, Zhang C, Yanagawa Y, Sun QQ. Major effects of sensory experiences on the neocortical inhibitory circuits. J Neurosci. 2006;26:8691–701. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2478-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapfer C, Glickfeld LL, Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Supralinear increase of recurrent inhibition during sparse activity in the somatosensory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:743–53. doi: 10.1038/nn1909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keck T, Scheuss V, Jacobsen RI, Wierenga CJ, Eysel UT, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Loss of sensory input causes rapid structural changes of inhibitory neurons in adult mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2011;71:869–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefort S, Tomm C, Floyd Sarria JC, Petersen CC. The excitatory neuronal network of the C2 barrel column in mouse primary somatosensory cortex. Neuron. 2009;61:301–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letzkus JJ, Wolff SB, Meyer EM, Tovote P, Courtin J, Herry C, Lüthi A. A disinhibitory microcircuit for associative fear learning in the auditory cortex. Nature. 2011;480:331–5. doi: 10.1038/nature10674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L, Bender KJ, Drew PJ, Jadhav SP, Sylwestrak E, Feldman DE. Endocannabinoid signaling is required for development and critical period plasticity of the whisker map in somatosensory cortex. Neuron. 2009;64:537–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L, Feldman DE. A novel and rapid component of whisker response plasticity in rat somatosensory (S1) barrel cortex. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts. 2010:284.1. [Google Scholar]
- Lu JT, Li CY, Zhao JP, Poo MM, Zhang XH. Spike-timing-dependent plasticity of neocortical excitatory synapses on inhibitory interneurons depends on target cell type. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9711–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2513-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maffei A, Nataraj K, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG. Potentiation of cortical inhibition by visual deprivation. Nature. 2006;443:81–4. doi: 10.1038/nature05079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maffei A, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG. Selective reconfiguration of layer 4 visual cortical circuitry by visual deprivation. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1353–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maffei A, Turrigiano GG. Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4377–84. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5298-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mateo C, Avermann M, Gentet LJ, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Petersen CC. In vivo optogenetic stimulation of neocortical excitatory neurons drives brain-state-dependent inhibition. Curr Biol. 2011;21:1593–602. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marik SA, Yamahachi H, McManus JN, Szabo G, Gilbert CD. Axonal dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in somatosensory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2010;8(6):e1000395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minlebaev M, Colonnese M, Tsintsadze T, Sirota A, Khazipov R. Early gamma oscillations synchronize developing thalamus and cortex. Science. 2011;334:226–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1210574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyashita T, Wintzer M, Kurotani T, Konishi T, Ichinohe N, Rockland KS. Neurotrophin-3 is involved in the formation of apical dendritic bundles in cortical layer 2 of the rat. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:229–40. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagel G, Brauner M, Liewald JF, Adeishvili N, Bamberg E, Gottschalk A. Light activation of channelrhodopsin-2 in excitable cells of Caenorhabditis elegans triggers rapid behavioral responses. Current Biology. 2005;15:2279–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor DH, Peron SP, Huber D, Svoboda K. Neural activity in barrel cortex underlying vibrissa-based object localization in mice. Neuron. 2010;67:1048–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paik SB, Glaser DA. Synaptic plasticity controls sensory responses through frequency-dependent gamma oscillation resonance. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6(9):e1000927. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petreanu L, Huber D, Sobczyk A, Svoboda K. Channelrhodopsin-2-assisted circuit mapping of long-range callosal projections. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:663–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reyes A, Lujan R, Rozov A, Burnashev N, Somogyi P, Sakmann B. Target-cell-specific facilitation and depression in neocortical circuits. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:279–85. doi: 10.1038/1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sale A, Berardi N, Spolidoro M, Baroncelli L, Maffei L. GABAergic inhibition in visual cortical plasticity. Front Cell Neurosci. 2010;4:10. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2010.00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saito T, Nakatsuji N. Efficient gene transfer into the embryonic mouse brain using in vivo electroporation. Dev Biol. 2001;240:237–246. doi: 10.1006/dbio.2001.0439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider T, Neumaier A. Algorithm 808: ARfit - A Matlab package for the estimation of parameters and eigenmodes of multivariate autoregressive models. ACM Trans Math Softw. 2001;27:58–65. [Google Scholar]
- Schummers J, Mariño J, Sur M. Synaptic integration by V1 neurons depends on location within the orientation map. Neuron. 2002;36:969–78. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01012-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd GM, Pologruto TA, Svoboda K. Circuit analysis of experience-dependent plasticity in the developing rat barrel cortex. Neuron. 2003;38:277–89. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00152-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons DJ. Response properties of vibrissa units in rat SI somatosensory neocortex. J Neurophysiol. 1978;41:798–820. doi: 10.1152/jn.1978.41.3.798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzsáki G. Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron. 2008;60:683–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohal VS, Zhang F, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature. 2009;459:698–702. doi: 10.1038/nature07991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern EA, Maravall M, Svoboda K. Rapid development and plasticity of layer 2/3 maps in rat barrel cortex in vivo. Neuron. 2001;31:305–15. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00360-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabata H, Nakajima K. Efficient in utero gene transfer system to the developing mouse brain using electroporation: visualization of neuronal migration in the developing cortex. Neuroscience. 2001;103:865–872. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00016-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trachtenberg JT, Stryker MP. Rapid anatomical plasticity of horizontal connections in the developing visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2001;21:3476–82. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-10-03476.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turrigiano G. Homeostatic synaptic plasticity: local and global mechanisms for stabilizing neuronal function. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2012;4:a005736. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a005736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Versendaal D, Rajendran R, Saiepour MH, Klooster J, Smit-Rigter L, Sommeijer JP, De Zeeuw CI, Hofer SB, Heimel JA, Levelt CN. Elimination of inhibitory synapses is a major component of adult ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron. 2012;74:374–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 2010;90:1195–268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wen JA, Barth AL. Input-specific critical periods for experience-dependent plasticity in layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2011;31:4456–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6042-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson NR, Runyan CA, Wang FL, Sur M. Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitory networks in vivo. Nature. 2012;488:343–8. doi: 10.1038/nature11347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wimmer VC, Bruno RM, de Kock CP, Kuner T, Sakmann B. Dimensions of a projection column and architecture of VPM and POm axons in rat vibrissal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:2265–76. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamahachi H, Marik SA, McManus JN, Denk W, Gilbert CD. Rapid axonal sprouting and pruning accompany functional reorganization in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2009;64:719–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yazaki-Sugiyama Y, Kang S, Câteau H, Fukai T, Hensch TK. Bidirectional plasticity in fast-spiking GABA circuits by visual experience. Nature. 2009;462:218–21. doi: 10.1038/nature08485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.