Abstract
The neocortex is the part of the mammalian brain that is involved in perception, cognition, and volitional motor control. It is a highly dynamic structure that is dramatically altered within the lifetime of an animal and in different lineages throughout the course of evolution. These alterations account for the remarkable variations in behavior that species exhibit. Of particular interest is how these cortical phenotypes change within the lifetime of the individual and eventually evolve in species over time. Because we cannot study the evolution of the neocortex directly we use comparative analysis to appreciate the types of changes that have been made to the neocortex and the similarities that exist across taxa. Developmental studies inform us about how these phenotypic transitions may arise by alterations in developmental cascades or changes in the physical environment in which the brain develops. Both genes and the sensory environment contribute to aspects of the phenotype and similar features, such as the size of a cortical field, can be altered in a variety of ways. Although both genes and the laws of physics place constraints on the evolution of the neocortex, mammals have evolved a number of mechanisms that allow them to loosen these constraints and often alter the course of their own evolution.
Keywords: epigenetic, comparative neuroanatomy, cortical development, evolution, Evo-Devo
“We certainly need to remember that between the genotype and phenotype, and connecting them to each other, there lies a whole complex of developmental processes.”
Waddington, 1942.
Evolution and development of the nervous system are inextricably intertwined. Studies that link these two biological processes have recently re-emerged from the older foundations of comparative neuroanatomy and descriptive neurodevelopment as the flashy new discipline often referred to as “Evo-Devo.” This re-awakening was made possible by two events. First, descriptive neurodevelopment transformed into an experimental discipline with the advent of molecular and genetic techniques that allowed scientists to figuratively “poke the frog.” The ability to make targeted changes, via genetic manipulations that differentially affect specific aspects of development, allowed us to appreciate the contingencies inherent in the developmental process and to understand the role these genetic cascades play in the construction of specific features of the nervous system. Importantly, it is becoming increasingly clear that the cortical field is not a static entity, but transforms continually at all stages of development. The second event was the emergence of new technologies in genetics that allowed scientists to decode and compare entire genomes of selected species. The prospect that this would ultimately uncover the fundamental differences between species propelled the somewhat aging field of evolutionary neurobiology to the forefront of neuroscience.
Our laboratory has long been interested in the evolution of the neocortex and has used comparative studies to formulate testable hypotheses regarding neurodevelopment. Specifically we are interested in the developmental mechanisms that give rise to aspects of neocortical organization that have changed significantly in species over the course of evolution. We focus on the neocortex for two important reasons. The first is that the neocortex is the portion of the brain involved in complex behaviors including perception, cognition, language, and temporal planning of events. Second, it is the portion of the brain that has changed most dramatically in mammals compared to other parts of the brain (Krubitzer, 2007). The neocortex has expanded tremendously in human and non-human primates, and has expanded independently in several other orders of mammals including cetaceans, proboscidea, and rodentia. However, it is not just an increase in the size that distinguishes some large-brained mammals from others, but also an increase in the number of functional subdivisions, and importantly, alterations in their patterns of connectivity. Studies of endocasts of the skulls of early mammals (Luo et al., 2001) as well as comparative studies (Meredith et al., 2011; O'Leary et al., 2013) suggest that the first mammals that roamed the earth some 200 million years ago had a small neocortex with perhaps 10–15 cortical fields, and a relatively large pyriform cortex and olfactory bulbs (Rowe et al., 2011; Dooley et al., 2013; see Kaas, 2011 for review). This early mammaliform and its descendants evolved to produce some extant species with a neocortex that dominates the rest of the nervous system and contains billions of cells with hundreds of cortical fields. The question is how did this occur, and what factors contribute to this increased complexity of form, function and behavior.
One obstacle in addressing this question is that cortical evolution in mammals cannot be studied directly. The types of changes that brains have evolved occur over multiple generations and often take tens of thousands to millions of years to emerge. However, there are two ways to circumvent this problem. The first is to examine the products of evolution, extant animal brains and bodies, to determine what changes have occurred. This comparative approach has been used to good effect to appreciate common features of the neocortex that all species share as well as derivations that have been made to the basic plan of organization. Unfortunately, comparative studies do not provide information on how phenotypic transformations occur, or the rate at which changes can happen. To appreciate how changes occurred we study the developmental mechanisms that are proposed to give rise to some aspect of cortical organization. Thus, it is critical to appreciate how processes such as neurogenesis, cell migration, neuronal differentiation, and axon guidance are altered in mammals with different cortical phenotypes. These alterations give rise to some feature of organization that we study in our comparative analysis such as cortical sheet size, cortical field size, and connectivity. For these reasons developmental studies tell us how phenotypic changes occur.
It is important to stress that any theory of brain evolution, cortical function or cortical plasticity should not consider the neocortex in isolation, but must recognize that the neocortex is only one component of the entire nervous system. Further, the nervous system is embedded in a body, which interacts with other organisms and the environment. This group of organisms and their environment generates a complex and highly dynamic “collective biomass” that itself has emergent properties which differ from, and in some instances exceed, the individual elements of which it is composed (Krubitzer, 2009). Further, it is critical to appreciate that the relationship between genes, the brain, the body, and the target of natural selection (behavior) is often highly convoluted and indirect (see Krubitzer and Seelke, 2012 for review).
In the following review we first provide an overview from comparative studies that outlines common features of cortical organization that have been identified in all species examined and how aspects of this common plan have been modified. Second, we address the question of how these phenotypic transformations have occurred, including a review of studies that examine how genes contribute to neurogenesis, cortical sheet size, and aspects of cortical arealization across development. We underscore the importance of examining not only genes intrinsic to the neocortex, but also genes that regulate the body plan and limb and effector morphology. Next, we discuss activity-driven alterations to the cortical phenotype. To appreciate the gene/environment interactions we look to natural examples of extreme morphological/behavioral specialization that is accompanied by exaggerated aspects of cortical organization, and describe our developmental studies in which we try to mimic these changes to the neocortex by radically altering sensory inputs. Finally, we describe more subtle examples in which animals of the same species, reared in different sensory environments, develop alterations to the cortical phenotype. We discuss potential epigenetic mechanisms that construct context dependent alterations to the phenotype.
What is the plan and how has it changed?
Comparative studies use multiple criteria to define a cortical field including functional techniques (e.g., electrophysiological recording, imaging, intracortical microstimulation), combined with architectonic and neuroanatomical techniques. In our experiments we survey a large extent of the neocortex by recording neural activity from hundreds of sites while successively presenting visual, auditory, and tactile stimulation to determine sensory domain allocation (the amount of cortex devoted to a particular sensory system). These techniques also allow us to determine the number and overall organization of different cortical fields within a sensory domain. These data can be combined with architectonic techniques in which the region of interest is stained for particular cell types, myelinated axons, enzymatic activity, or any number of other histochemical markers that illuminate cortical field boundaries, which are then directly related to functional techniques. Cortical regions can also be divided using neuroanatomical techniques to examine subcortical, cortical and interhemispheric connections of the field in question.
Using such techniques in a number of different species, our own and other laboratories have generated schemes of cortical organization composed of architectonically, connectionally, and functionally distinct maps of the sensory receptor arrays associated with visual, somatosensory, and auditory processing. These comparative studies indicate that there is a constellation of cortical fields that all mammals possess that can be defined using multiple criteria. These include primary visual, somatosensory, and auditory cortical fields (V1, S1, and A1 respectively) as well as one or two additional sensory areas (Figure 1; e.g., V2, S2/PV, R) (Dooley et al., 2013; see Kaas, 2011 for review).
Table 1.
A1—Primary auditory cortex |
Emx2—Empty spiracles homeobox 2—transcription factor expressed in a caudal (high) rostral (low) gradient |
FGF8—Fibroblast growth factor 8—morphogen important for generating the rostral-caudal axis |
GR—Glucocorticoid receptor |
HPA—Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal |
IPC—Intermediate progenitor cells |
ISVZ—Inner subventricular zone |
LG—Licking and grooming |
M1—Primary motor cortex |
oRG—Outer radial glial cells |
OSVZ—Outer subventricular zone |
Pax6—Paired box protein 6—transcription factor expressed in a rostral (high) caudal (low) gradient |
PV—Parietal ventral area |
R—Rostral somatosensory field |
RG—Radial glial cells |
S1—Primary somatosensory cortex |
S2—Second somatosensory area |
SVZ—Subventricular zone |
V1—Primary visual cortex |
V2—Second visual area |
VZ—Ventricular zone |
Interestingly, these fields are present even in the absence of apparent use in animals showing extreme specialization such as blind mole rats (Cooper et al., 1993; Bronchti et al., 2002). Moreover, independently evolved modifications to this plan take a similar form in different lineages (Krubitzer and Kaas, 2005). Systems-level evolved changes in cortical organization include:
The absolute and relative size of the cortical sheet
Sensory domain allocation
Response properties and stimulus preference of neurons within a cortical field
Relative size of cortical fields
Magnification of behaviorally relevant body parts
Addition of modules to cortical fields
Number of cortical fields
Connections of cortical fields
The persistence of the shared cortical field plan across all mammals and the similarities in its modifications suggest that there are large constraints on how cortical fields evolve. For further discussion of constraints and variability see Krubitzer and Seelke, 2012.
What factors contribute to these changes?
As noted above, while comparative studies allow us to appreciate the types of changes that have been made to the neocortex, developmental studies provide insights into how these changes occur. Thus, the next question that arises from our comparative analysis is what factors contribute to within-species variability of the features of cortical organization listed above. This is a question that has been posed for decades, commonly presented as a nature vs. nurture debate. Recently, advances in comparative genomics and epigenetics confirm the contributions of both genetic and context-dependent factors to different aspects of the cortical phenotype and within-species variability. Still contentious, however, is the extent to which each factor shapes or constructs any given phenotype.
Traditionally, context-dependent changes to cell phenotypes during development had been referred to as “epigenetic” (Waddington, 1942). Waddington coined the term epigenetics to explain how cells in the developing organism can have the same genotype, but gradually differentiate into different tissue. This phenomenon underscores that there is not a one-to-one correspondence between genotype and phenotype, and that there must be something beyond the genotype that generates this diversity. We now appreciate that this same ability to alter a cellular, systems, or behavioral phenotype occurs in mature, non-dividing cells in the central nervous system (Day and Sweatt, 2010), and this phenomenon has also been termed epigenetics. While early in development, context-dependent changes may be as simple as folic acid availability or location of a particular cell on a developing blastocyst, as development progresses, the context (and thus its potential for change) becomes more complicated. This is particularly true for the mammalian neocortex, where environmental context routinely molds the phenotype. A particular cortical phenotype may persist for multiple generations if the context in which it develops is static, but these features of cortical organization are not inherited and thus do not evolve. However, new studies, which we will discuss below, have overturned some assumptions about heritability and have begun to uncover the mechanisms that generate contextually dependent phenotypes that can be expressed in multiple generations, and in some instances become incorporated into the germ line and evolve.
Cortical sheet size
One of the well-defined systems-level changes to the brain has been an expansion of the cortical sheet. Throughout the course of mammalian evolution, this expansion has taken two different forms: (1) Absolute increase in size (direct scaling), and (2) Relative increases in size (non-linear scaling). Direct scaling consistently occurs with an increase in body size. The brain scales directly with the body, and every structure, including the neocortex and constituent fields, expand roughly equally. This is exemplified by the comparison of two closely-related rodents: The guinea pig (700 g) and the South American capybara, the largest rodent on earth which weighs up to 91 kg (200 lbs; Figure 2). The neocortex of the guinea pig is much smaller than that of the capybara, but relative to body size, the size of the neocortex and the primary sensory fields are comparable (Campos and Welker, 1976).
The second type of increase in the size of the cortical sheet is non-linear and is related to a different type of cortical organization. The California ground squirrel is similar in overall body size to the guinea pig (700 g), but its neocortex is substantially larger both absolutely and relative to the body or rest of the brain (Campi and Krubitzer, 2010). This non-linear increase in the size of the cortical sheet is accompanied by a decrease in the overall percentage of neocortex occupied by primary sensory areas along with an increase in the absolute number of cortical fields on the cortical sheet (Figure 2), a pattern even better exemplified in non-human primates such as squirrel monkeys. Squirrel monkeys have about the same body mass as both California ground squirrels and guinea pigs (750 g), but have an extraordinarily large neocortex compared to the body and the rest of the brain, and a dramatic increase in the number of cortical fields. Thus, an absolute (linear) increase in the size of the neocortex is not sufficient to yield an increase in its complexity (i.e., more cortical fields/changes in connections). Conversely, a relative (non-linear) increase in the size of the neocortex does appear to be necessary to increase number of cortical fields, but may not be sufficient to induce this change.
Some questions that emerge are: (1) How is an increase in the size of the cortical sheet accomplished? (2) Are the underlying mechanisms of direct and non-linear scaling of the cortex different? (3) What is the link between changes in brain and body size? and (4) Are the underlying mechanisms that give rise to increases in cortical sheet size similar in species that have independently increased the size of the cortical sheet (e.g., primates and cetaceans)?
The evolution of neurogenesis
Recent studies of neurogenesis have made important inroads into understanding, at least in some species, the mechanisms that contribute to tangential increases in the size of cortical sheet during development and how these mechanisms may be altered in different species to produce differences in the size of the cortical sheet. Historically, researchers investigating neurogenesis have hypothesized that animals with larger (usually gyrencephalic) brains have an increased duration of neurogenesis and modified cell cycle kinetics, and that such alterations are not present in small (lissencephalic) brained animals. This notion is supported by comparative studies in mice and macaque monkeys that demonstrate that more rounds of cell division occur over a longer period of time in the macaque compared to the mouse (Takahashi et al., 1995; Kornack and Rakic, 1998; Kornack, 2000). Subsequent studies described additional changes in neurogenesis that could account for an expanded cortical sheet in some lineages. One important alteration, first described in the macaque monkey, was the presence of an outer subventricular zone (OSVZ, Smart et al., 2002; Figure 3). Additionally, within the OSVZ are proliferative radial glia-like cells termed outer radial glial cells (oRG) that generate neurons that will compose the cerebral cortex (Fietz et al., 2010; Hansen et al., 2010; Reillo et al., 2011; Shitamukai et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2011; Martínez-Cerdeño et al., 2012). This large OSVZ and the oRG proliferative cells, at least in part, account for the exponential expansion of the cerebral cortex in some orders such as primates.
Initially, this expanded OSVZ and the corresponding oRG cells were considered an adaptation limited to large-brained, gyrencephalic mammals, but recently a much smaller OSVZ has been described in rats. This OSVZ shares many of the same features found in larger brained ferrets and macaques, such as the presence of oRG proliferative cells (Martínez-Cerdeño et al., 2012). There is also evidence for an OSVZ in the marmoset (a dwarfed, nearly lissencephalic primate) and the agouti (a gyrencephalic rodent; García-Moreno et al., 2012). Thus, small-brained mammals from multiple orders possess the basic ventricular compartments (OSVZ) and proliferative cells (oRG) that can generate expansions in the cortical sheet.
If not the presence of OSVZ and oRG, what differentiates large and small brains? It appears that large-brained animals have an increased generation of intermediate progenitor cells (Wang et al., 2011), a greater number of oRGs present across development (Hevner and Haydar, 2012), and a thicker OSVZ (Bystron et al., 2008; Martínez-Cerdeño et al., 2012), all of which can lead to a larger neocortex with a greater number of neurons (Figure 3). Additionally, while oRGs have been shown to produce neurons and intermediate progenitor cells in primates, which further divide into post-mitotic neurons (Hansen et al., 2010), in mice they have only been shown to divide directly into neurons (Wang et al., 2011), although this is not the case for all rodents (Martínez-Cerdeño et al., 2012). While more comparative studies need to be done, mounting evidence suggests that changes in the size of the cortical sheet are due to expansions of existing populations of cells and cell cycle kinetics, rather than the creation of novel mechanisms.
Exciting research published in the last year has identified proteins which appear to regulate the population of oRG cells (Trnp1; Stahl et al., 2013) and intermediate progenitor cells (BAF170; Tuoc et al., 2013), such that both over expression and under expression of these proteins in the neocortex alter the number of these progenitor cells and ultimately alter cortical sheet size. Further, research by Nonaka-Kinoshita et al. (2013) shows that increasing the pool of basal progenitors in the lissencephalic mouse increases the size of the neocortical sheet, but is not sufficient to induce gyrencephally; however, the same manipulation in the naturally gyrecephalic ferret both increased the size of the cortical sheet and induced additional cortical sulci. Thus, while increasing the number of progenitor cells invariably leads to a larger cortical sheet, existing data suggests that a sufficient population of oRG cells must also be present to create sulci and gyri in a naturally lissencephalic cortex (Stahl et al., 2013; Tuoc et al., 2013).
It is important to note that epigenetic events can also regulate the size of the cortical sheet. These context-dependent alterations in cortical sheet size appear to be caused by a variety of factors. For example, it has been well documented that folic acid (and cholate) regulates neurogenesis and apoptosis in the developing fetal brain, and differences in intake can alter the number of progenitor cells undergoing mitosis by 33–54% in the neocortex of mice (Craciunescu et al., 2004, 2010). Although a number of studies have demonstrated that domestication also has a profound impact on the size of the cortical sheet (see Kruska, 2005), it is difficult to disambiguate the contribution of genes vs. environment on cortical sheet size.
Cortical field size and connectivity
Like cortical sheet size, both genetic and epigenetic factors contribute to aspects of cortical field size and connectivity. A plethora of studies demonstrate that intrinsic factors contribute to a number of features of cortical organization including relative position on the cortical sheet, relative size of the cortical field, and cortical field connections (e.g., Bishop et al., 2000; O'Leary and Sahara, 2008; Assimacopoulos et al., 2012). For example, ground-breaking studies from a number of laboratories demonstrated that morphogens such as FGF8 generate a rostral cortical identity, and that these early signaling centers set up genetic cascades which regulate position, size, and connectivity of cortical fields. The importance of these early signaling centers is clearly demonstrated in recent studies in which Fgf8 was electroporated into different regions of the developing mouse embryo and duplicate fields (with rostrocaudal axes) were observed (Assimacopoulos et al., 2012, Figure 4). Electroporating Fgf8 into a caudal (aberrant) location results in an almost complete duplication of cortical maps with a mirror reversal of V1 and S1 at mid cortex, with two distinct “rostral” poles and a shared caudal pole boundary (Figure 4B). This compelling result presents a possible mechanism for mirror reversal organization of cortical fields (such as in anterior parietal fields 3a, 3b, 1 and 2 in primates). These naturally occurring duplicate somatotopic maps could have originated as an alteration in location and strength of these early signaling centers in parietal cortex. While the connectivity of duplicated cortical maps is not known, they do appear to be functionally responsive and topographically organized (Assimacopoulos et al., 2012).
Generation of this rostral-caudal axis by FGF8 influences downstream transcription factors expressed early in development, such as Pax6 and Emx2, which themselves appear to be critical for establishing appropriate expression patterns of cell adhesion molecules (Bishop et al., 2000, 2002; Hamasaki et al., 2004; O'Leary and Sahara, 2008; Figure 5). These molecules in turn regulate a number of aspects of the cortical phenotype including the relative size of cortical fields and their connectivity (Suzuki et al., 1997; Inoue et al., 1998; Bishop et al., 2002; Terakawa et al., 2013). For example, over or under expression of Emx2 in the early developing mouse neocortex has been shown to alter expression of cell adhesion molecules (Stoykova et al., 1997; Bishop et al., 2000, 2002; Andrews and Mastick, 2003), ultimately resulting in an increase or decrease (respectively) in the size of cortical fields on the caudal pole of the neocortex including V1, and alterations in thalamocortical connections (Bishop et al., 2000; Hamasaki et al., 2004; Figure 5). Importantly, in the absence of thalamocortical afferents, the expression patterns of some of these early transcription factors and genes are maintained (Nakagawa et al., 1999) indicating that activity is not requisite for their expression and thus certain aspects of cortical organization are immutable, regardless of context.
Genes extrinsic to the developing neocortex contribute to cortical organization and connectivity
Most studies of cortical development focus almost exclusively on genes that are intrinsic to the developing neocortex. However, as noted in our introduction, brains do not develop or evolve in isolation, but in the context of the body, behavior, and a rich sensory environment generated by biological and non-biological sources. An excellent example of the interaction between genes that regulate body morphology and the effect of this on the brain and behavior comes from comparisons of limb development in species that have radically different forelimb phenotypes, such as the mouse and the short-tailed fruit bat (Figure 6). The early development of the mouse and bat forelimb is remarkably similar. However, at mid stages of limb development, the interdigit membranes in the mouse undergo apoptosis, which results in a separation of individual digits of the forepaw (Cretekos et al., 2008). Conversely, at this stage of limb development in the bat apoptosis does not occur. In addition, in the bat there is a lengthening of the forelimb and elongation of the digit phalanges. Together these alterations generate much of the phenotypic differences in these species, which in turn are related to radical differences in the use of the forelimb. Comparative studies of gene expression during limb development indicate that there are several key genetic alterations that account for these differences. In the bat, upregulation of Prx1 results in a lengthening of the distal forelimb (Cretekos et al., 2008; Behringer et al., 2009; Figure 6A) and a posterior shift in Hoxd13 expression reduces some skeletal elements. In the mouse, BMPs trigger apoptosis of interdigit membranes. In the bat BMPs are inhibited by Gremlin thus preserving interdigit membranes and this inhibition is accompanied by an increase in FGF8 in the apical ectodermal ridge, which extends the distal growth of the forelimb. Another important distinction of the bat forelimb is the presence of touch domes. These specialized receptor assemblies are found across the wing membranes and are beautifully sensitive to small changes in air pressure (Zook and Fowler, 1986; Sterbing-D'Angelo et al., 2011).
These changes to the forelimb are associated with differential use of the limb and the ability to make fine discriminations with the wing for self propelled flight and wing to mouth feeding behavior (Sterbing-D'Angelo et al., 2011). Studies of neocortical organization of the somatosensory cortex demonstrate an expansion of the forelimb and digit (wing) representation within S1 of the bat compared to the mouse (Woolsey, 1967; Wise et al., 1986; Cretekos et al., 2001, 2008, Figure 6C). Additionally differences in the interhemispheric connections of the forelimb in bats have been observed. In both large and small-brained mammals with discrete digits, the forepaw/hand representation within S1 is almost devoid of connections across hemispheres (for review, see Innocenti, 1986). These acallosal hand/paw representations in S1 and associated anterior parietal fields are particularly discrete in species like primates that use the glabrous digits as a major effector for object exploration. In bats digits 2–4 are fused by the wing membranes and tactile stimulation of the wings is used for fine control in self-propelled flight. The wing representation in primary somatosensory cortex and associated fields receives dense callosal inputs for rapid interhemispheric communication between centers that process incoming inputs and generate fine motor control of the wing during flight (Krubitzer et al., 1998).
There are numerous other model systems demonstrating the role peripheral body morphology can have on the development of the neocortex in the scientific literature. Perhaps the most extensively studied peripheral/central system is the vibrissae and their corresponding barrels in S1 (for review, see Erzurumlu and Gaspar, 2012). While a complete discussion of the experimentally-induced plasticity of this system is beyond the scope of this review, genetic manipulations have produced mice which possess additional whisker follicles (Welker and Van der Loos, 1986) or which lack several whisker follicles (North et al., 2010). Welker and Van der Loos generated six strains of mice with differing patterns of extra vibrissae and found that regardless of the peripheral patterns of vibrissae, all extra vibrissae were represented cortically with extra barrels (Welker and Van der Loos, 1986). Likewise, mice lacking particular vibrissae also lacked the corresponding barrels in S1, as the representations of these vibrissae in associated subcortical pathways (North et al., 2010).
It should be noted that environmental factors also contribute to features of body morphology and in turn brain organization. For instance, gravitational stress can affect craniomandibular morphology including bone density (Singh et al., 2005), and diet and associated mastication behavior affects craniofacial morphology (He, 2004; Koyabu and Endo, 2009). Environmental factors such as salinity, temperature and humidity also contribute to body morphology (Johnston and Gottlieb, 1990), and even sex determination (Matsumoto et al., 2013). Together these body morphology changes could radically affect a number of aspects of behavior including self-propelled flight and feeding, which in turn could alter aspects of sensorimotor cortex organization and connectivity.
What is not understood is the extent to which these changes to peripheral morphology and use can drive fundamental changes in cortical organization, connectivity, sensory mediated discriminations, perceptions, and higher level cognitive processes, and if or how these changes to the brain become genetically encoded and evolve.
Observations from the natural world
For decades comparative neurobiologists have examined mammals that have evolved extreme specializations in an attempt to uncover general rules of construction as well as constraints imposed on the evolving nervous system. These types of observations highlight features that may be more difficult to uncover when only subtle differences exist in some aspect of brain organization in different species. One of the most extraordinary examples of this comes from studies of the duck-billed platypus (Figure 7A). The platypus has evolved electrosensory receptors that form anteroposterior rows on the bill that interdigitate with rows of mechanosensory receptors (Scheich et al., 1986; Gregory et al., 1987, 1988; Iggo et al., 1992). Most activities of the platypus are performed in the water during which time its eyes, ears and nose are closed. Thus, inputs from the bill, and to a limited extent the body, are the brain's source of information about the animal's immediate environment. Examination of the organization of the neocortex indicates an enormous expansion of the bill representation with clear territories devoted to processing electrosensory vs. mechanosensory inputs. There are three separate representations of the bill, which together occupy about 50% of the cortical sheet. In S1 alone, this magnification of a behaviorally relevant body part is enormous; the bill representation occupies 95% of S1 (Krubitzer et al., 1995).
Additional examples of extreme magnification have been observed in the primary somatosensory area of a number of mammals including the naked mole rat and star-nosed mole (Figure 7). This expansion of cortical territory related to effector specific inputs and active use is also observed in other sensory systems including an expansion of central vision in diurnal primates, and an expansion of ultrasonic frequency representations in echolocating bats (Suga et al., 1987). These alterations are due to changes in peripheral morphology, use, and the physical environment in which the animal develops and ultimately lives.
To determine the extent to which sensory receptor arrays, as well as inputs from multiple sensory systems, contribute to aspects of the cortical phenotype, our lab bilaterally enucleated short-tailed opossums very early in development, before thalamocortical afferents reached the cortex and before retinal ganglion cell axons reached the thalamus (Taylor and Guillery, 1994; Molnár et al., 1998). We found that loss of visual input results in a massive reallocation of sensory cortex (cortical domain changes) in that “visual cortex” is functionally taken over by the auditory and somatosensory systems (Kahn and Krubitzer, 2002; Figure 8A). This early loss of visual input resulted in a decrease in the size of architectonically defined V1 as well as an increase in the size of S1 (Karlen and Krubitzer, 2009). Further, cortex in the expected location of V1 received aberrant inputs from somatosensory and auditory structures of the cortex and thalamus (Karlen et al., 2006; Figure 8B). Studies in anophthalmic mice have also demonstrated alterations in subcortical connections and large changes in functional organization of “visual cortex” (Godement et al., 1979; Chabot et al., 2007), and studies of congenitally deaf mice show that auditory cortex is taken over by the visual and somatosensory systems (Hunt et al., 2006). Work in experimentally deafened cats supports these data. Cats that are deafened early have superior peripheral visual localization and motion detection abilities, and these abilities can be abolished when specific areas of auditory cortex are deactivated (the posterior auditory field and the dorsal zone, respectively; Lomber et al., 2010). Recently it was shown that projections from extrastriate visual areas to the dorsal zone of auditory cortex provides the anatomical substrate for this behavioral plasticity (Kok et al., 2013).
Armed with knowledge of the types of alterations that occur with extreme changes in receptor array and major loss of sensory input, scientists can determine if these same types of alterations occur with changes in environmental context in which the animal develops. There are numerous examples in the auditory, visual, and somatosensory systems that demonstrate physical rearing conditions produce changes to the cortical phenotype. For example dark-rearing or stripe-rearing in cats and ferrets leads to a decrease in neural responses to visual stimuli in orientations in which the manipulated animals lack experience (e.g., Blasdel et al., 1977; Sengpiel et al., 1999; Li et al., 2006) and recent studies in rodents have corroborated these results, demonstrating that diverse visual experience is necessary for normal visual development (O'Hashi et al., 2007; Kreile et al., 2011). Similar findings have been found in the auditory system, in which repeated presentation of specific auditory stimuli early in development produces an expansion of the cortical representation of the tones presented (Zhang et al., 2001). Finally, an increase in the amount of cortex devoted to representing particular regions of the body have been generated either through extensive use of the animal's optimal effector (Recanzone et al., 1992), or in some cases, training using the non-optimal effector (Tennant et al., 2012).
Recently we examined the effects of lifestyle and exposure to radically different sensory environments on the size and cellular composition of cortical fields in different rodents for different sensory systems. First, we quantified relative cortical field size in diurnal vs. nocturnal rodents and terrestrial vs. arboreal rodents (Figure 9A). We found differential expansions and contractions of visual, auditory and somatosensory cortex that were related to lifestyle. For example, diurnal squirrels had a relatively larger V1 while nocturnal rats had a relatively larger S1 and A1 (expressed as a percentage of the entire cortical sheet, Campi and Krubitzer, 2010). Furthermore, arboreal squirrels, which live in a visually demanding environment, had a larger V1 and showed an expansion of visual cortex compared to terrestrial squirrels.
We also quantified and compared differences in cortical field size and cellular composition of primary visual cortex between wild-caught Norway rats and Norway rats reared in the laboratory. Obviously the sensory experience, motor demands and sensory mediated behaviors in a natural (and pervasive) environment are more dynamic and complex than the more limited demands of the laboratory environment. We found that there were significant differences in the size of primary sensory areas with laboratory rats having larger a S1 and A1 compared to wild caught animals (Campi and Krubitzer, 2010; Figure 9A). Conversely, V1 in wild caught rats had a larger percentage of neurons and a greater density of neurons compared to laboratory rats (Campi et al., 2011; Figures 9B,C). These studies indicate that the fundamental structure of neocortex can be modified through experience.
What are the mechanisms that generate epigenetic alterations to the neocortex?
Decades of studies on developmental and adult plasticity of the neocortex demonstrate that sensory experience can profoundly transform features of cortical organization that are known to be altered throughout the course of evolution, such as cortical field size, organization, cellular composition, neural response properties, and connectivity. Rather than a simple and small refinement of parameters initiated by genes, experience plays a critical role in the construction of the neocortex. This should not be surprising since the role of the neocortex appears to be that of a comparative predictor for the generation of adaptive behavior, and behavior is the target of natural selection. These behaviors are often tightly temporally correlated with the stimulus, or temporally uncorrelated in which a substantial amount of time may have elapsed between the stimulus and behavior. In any case, the predictive precision of the neocortex is built by accurate representations of the physical context or collective biomass in which the animal develops and behaves. Thus, it is not surprising that there are mechanisms that allow the animal, and the brain, which generates its behavior, to change substantially within a lifetime.
Although the field of epigenetics has had a recent resurgence, as noted above, the notion that a number of processes occur between the genotype and the ultimate phenotype has been appreciated since the last half of the previous century (Holliday, 2006 for review). The current review has attempted to provide a number of concrete examples in which aspects of the cortical phenotype can vary based on a number of different genetic and experience dependent factors. While describing the details of epigenetic mechanisms is beyond the scope of our laboratory's purview, it would be remiss not to discuss how environmental signals program the operation of the genome, and the mechanisms by which these effects endure beyond the period of exposure during development (Kappeler and Meaney, 2010).
Some of the best examples of these interactions come from studies of mother–offspring interactions in rats. Maternal licking and grooming (LG) of pups is a variable trait in Long-Evans rats (Champagne et al., 2003), and the frequency of the maternal LG is dictated by environmental factors such as stress levels and light/dark cycles (Champagne and Meaney, 2006; Toki et al., 2007). For example, natural variations in LG of pups during the early postnatal period affect the development of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Liu et al., 1997; Caldji et al., 1998; Menard et al., 2004). In adulthood, offspring of high LG mothers have lower circulating adrenocorticotropic hormone levels during stress. This blunted stress response is associated with changes in glucocorticoid receptor (GR) mRNA and protein expression in the hippocampus, which regulates glucocorticoid feedback sensitivity (Weaver et al., 2004). Importantly, these behavioral effects and changes in gene expression that regulate the HPA in adults are initiated by mother-infant interactions during the early postpartum period. It is proposed that during the early postnatal period variations in tactile stimulation during LG induce epigenetic modifications in the promoter region of the GR gene resulting in alterations of GR expression in the hippocampus that persist throughout life. Increased tactile stimulation received by offspring of high LG mothers results in an increase in neurotransmitter binding and subsequent intracellular signaling in the hippocampus, which activates GR gene transcription. Importantly, the pattern of increased GR transcription persists into adulthood because of a reduction in methylation of the GR gene. DNA methylation is typically associated with a repression of gene expression (Miranda and Jones, 2007; see Kappeler and Meaney, 2010 for review), therefore a reduction in methylation of the promoter region of the GR gene is associated with increased GR gene expression (Meaney and Szyf, 2005). This modification of the genome and the behaviors ultimately generated by these changes can be transmitted to the second generation offspring, but are reversed with cross-fostering (rearing low LG pups with high LG parents).
There are also examples of epigenetic mechanisms operating directly on the nervous system. Work by Putignano et al. (2007) demonstrates that during visual critical periods, sensory inputs can directly turn on and off regulatory factors which alter the accessibility of gene promoters. When these genes are made experimentally accessible in adulthood, much of the ocular dominance plasticity that is observed early in development is reinstated (Putignano et al., 2007). Further studies have identified a specific histone deacetylase (HDAC9) which has been shown to translocate from the nucleus to the cytoplasm following neural activity early in development. When HDAC9 was experimentally prevented from translocating, manipulated cells showed decreased dendritic branches, while knockdown of HDAC9 increased dendritic growth (Sugo et al., 2010).
Despite the constraints imposed by genes and the contingencies of genetic cascades, and the laws of physics that govern all forms of matter and energy, biological organisms have evolved mechanisms that allow them to loosen these constraints and dynamically adapt both within a lifetime and across generations. In a sense, the strength of this evolvability (Earl and Deem, 2004) and the evolution of a large, malleable comparative predictor (neocortex), rather than specific genes or gene products, may be one of the fundamental differences that distinguish humans from other animals.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Deepa Ramamurthy, Mary Baldwin, Adele Seelke, Dylan Cooke, and Danielle Stolzenberg for helpful comments on the manuscript. Leah Krubitzer is supported by NEI (R01 EY022987). James Dooley has been supported by NEI (T32-EY015387-05).
References
- Andrews G. L., Mastick G. S. (2003). R-cadherin is a Pax6-regulated, growth-promoting cue for pioneer axons. J. Neurosci. 23, 9873–9880 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assimacopoulos S., Kao T., Issa N. P., Grove E. A. (2012). Fibroblast growth factor 8 organizes the neocortical area map and regulates sensory map topography. J. Neurosci. 32, 7191–7201 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0071-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behringer R. R., Rasweiler J. J., 4th., Chen C.-H., Cretekos C. J. (2009). Genetic regulation of mammalian diversity. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 74, 297–302 10.1101/sqb.2009.74.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop K. M., Goudreau G., O'Leary D. D. (2000). Regulation of area identity in the mammalian neocortex by Emx2 and Pax6. Science 288, 344–349 10.1126/science.288.5464.344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop K. M., John L. R. R., O'Leary D. D. M. (2002). Distinct actions of Emx1, Emx2, and Pax6 in regulating the specification of areas in the developing neocortex. J. Neurosci. 22, 7627–7638 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasdel G. G., Mitchell D. E., Muir D. W., Pettigrew J. D. (1977). A physiological and behavioural study in cats of the effect of early visual experience with contours of a single orientation. J. Physiol. 265, 615–636 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bronchti G., Heil P., Sadka R., Hess A., Scheich H., Wollberg Z. (2002). Auditory activation of ‘visual’ cortical areas in the blind mole rat (Spalax ehrenbergi). Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 311–329 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02063.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bystron I., Blakemore C., Rakic P. (2008). Development of the human cerebral cortex: boulder Committee revisited. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 110–122 10.1038/nrn2252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldji C., Tannenbaum B., Sharma S., Francis D., Plotsky P. M., Meaney M. J. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 5335–5340 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campi K. L., Collins C. E., Todd W. D., Kaas J., Krubitzer L. (2011). Comparison of area 17 cellular composition in laboratory and wild-caught rats including diurnal and nocturnal species. Brain Behav. Evol. 77, 116–130 10.1159/000324862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campi K. L., Krubitzer L. (2010). Comparative studies of diurnal and nocturnal rodents: differences in lifestyle result in alterations in cortical field size and number. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 4491–4512 10.1002/cne.22466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campos G. B., Welker W. I. (1976). Comparisons between brains of a large and a small hystricomorph rodent: capybara, Hydrochoerus and guinea pig, Cavia; neocortical projection regions and measurements of brain subdivisions. Brain Behav. Evol. 13, 243–266 10.1159/000123814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catania K. C. (2011). The sense of touch in the star-nosed mole: from mechanoreceptors to the brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 366, 3016–3025 10.1098/rstb.2011.0128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chabot N., Robert S., Tremblay R., Miceli D., Boire D., Bronchti G. (2007). Audition differently activates the visual system in neonatally enucleated mice compared with anophthalmic mutants. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 2334–2348 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05854.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne F. A., Meaney M. J. (2006). Stress during gestation alters postpartum maternal care and the development of the offspring in a rodent model. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 1227–1235 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne F. A., Weaver I. C., Diorio J., Sharma S., Meaney M. J. (2003). Natural variations in maternal care are associated with estrogen receptor alpha expression and estrogen sensitivity in the medial preoptic area. Endocrinology 144, 4720–4724 10.1210/en.2003-0564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper H. M., Herbin H., Nevo E. (1993). Visual system of a naturally microphthalmic mammal: the blind mole rat, Spalax ehrenbergi. J. Comp. Neurol. 328, 313–350 10.1002/cne.903280302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craciunescu C. N., Brown E. C., Mar M. H., Albright C. D., Nadeau M. R., Zeisel S. H. (2004). Folic acid deficiency during late gestation decreases progenitor cell proliferation and increases apoptosis in fetal mouse brain. J. Nutr. 134, 162–166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craciunescu C. N., Johnson A. R., Zeisel S. H. (2010). Dietary choline reverses some, but not all, effects of folate deficiency on neurogenesis and apoptosis in fetal mouse brain. J. Nutr. 140, 1162–1166 10.3945/jn.110.122044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cretekos C. J., Rasweiler J. J., Behringer R. R. (2001). Comparative studies on limb morphogenesis in mice and bats: a functional genetic approach towards a molecular understanding of diversity in organ formation. Reprod. Fertil. Dev. 13, 691–695 10.1071/RD01115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cretekos C. J., Wang Y., Green E. D., Martin J. F., Rasweiler J. J., 4th., Behringer R. R. (2008). Regulatory divergence modifies limb length between mammals. Genes Dev. 22, 141–151 10.1101/gad.1620408 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day J. J., Sweatt J. D. (2010). DNA methylation and memory formation. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1319–1323 10.1038/nn.2666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dooley J. C., Franca J. G., Seelke A. M. H., Cooke D. F., Krubitzer L. A. (2013). A connection to the past: monodelphis domestica provides insight into the organization and connectivity of the brains of early mammals. J. Comp. Neurol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/cne.23383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Earl D. J., Deem M. W. (2004). Evolvability is a selectable trait. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 11531–11536 10.1073/pnas.0404656101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erzurumlu R. S., Gaspar P. (2012). Development and critical period plasticity of the barrel cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1540–1553 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08075.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fietz S. A., Kelava I., Vogt J., Wilsch-Bräuninger M., Stenzel D., Fish J. L., et al. (2010). OSVZ progenitors of human and ferret neocortex are epithelial-like and expand by integrin signaling. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 690–699 10.1038/nn.2553 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-Moreno F., Vasistha N. V., Trevia N., Bourne J. A., Molnár Z. (2012). Compartmentalization of cerebral cortical germinal zones in a lissencephalic primate and gyrencephalic rodent. Cereb. Cortex 22, 482–492 10.1093/cercor/bhr312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Godement P., Saillour P., Imbert M. (1979). Thalamic afferents to the visual cortex in congenitally anophthalmic mice. Neurosci. Lett. 13, 271–278 10.1016/0304-3940(79)91506-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory J. E., Iggo A., McIntyre A. K., Proske U. (1987). Electroreceptors in the platypus. Nature 326, 386–387 10.1038/326386a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory J. E., Iggo A., McIntyre A. K., Proske U. (1988). Receptors in the bill of the platypus. J. Physiol. 400, 349–366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamasaki T., Leingärtner A., Ringstedt T., O'Leary D. D. (2004). EMX2 regulates sizes and positioning of the primary sensory and motor areas in neocortex by direct specification of cortical progenitors. Neuron 43, 359–372 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen D. V., Lui J. H., Parker P. R., Kriegstein A. R. (2010). Neurogenic radial glia in the outer subventricular zone of human neocortex. Nature 464, 554–561 10.1038/nature08845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He T. (2004). Craniofacial morphology and growth in the ferret: effects from alteration of masticatory function. Swed. Dent. J. Suppl. 165, 1–72 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry E. C., Remple M. S., O'Riain M. J., Catania K. C. (2006). Organization of somatosensory cortical areas in the naked mole-rat (Heterocephalus glaber). J. Comp. Neurol. 495, 434–452 10.1002/cne.20883 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herron P. (1978). Somatotopic organization of mechanosensory projections to SII cerebral neocortex in the raccoon (Procyon lotor). J. Comp. Neurol. 181, 717–727 10.1002/cne.901810403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hevner R. F., Haydar T. F. (2012). The (not necessarily) convoluted role of basal radial glia in cortical neurogenesis. Cereb. Cortex 22, 465–468 10.1093/cercor/bhr336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holliday R. (2006). Dual inheritance. Curr. Top. Microbiol. Immunol. 301, 243–256 10.1007/3-540-31390-7_9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt D. L., Yamoah E. N., Krubitzer L. (2006). Multisensory plasticity in congenitally deaf mice: how are cortical areas functionally specified? Neuroscience 139, 1507–1524 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.01.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iggo A., Gregory J. E., Proske U. (1992). The central projection of electrosensory information in the platypus. J. Physiol. 447, 449–465 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Innocenti G. M. (1986). General organization of callosal connections in the cerebral cortex, in Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 5, eds Peters A., Jones E. G. (New York, NY: Plenum; ), 291–353 [Google Scholar]
- Inoue T., Tanaka T., Suzuki S. C., Takeichi M. (1998). Cadherin-6 in the developing mouse brain: expression along restricted connection systems and synaptic localization suggest a potential role in neuronal circuitry. Dev. Dyn. 211, 338–351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston T. D., Gottlieb G. (1990). Neophenogenesis: a developmental theory of phenotypic evolution. J. Theor. Biol. 147, 471–495 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80260-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas J. H. (2011). Reconstructing the areal organization of the neocortex of the first mammals. Brain Behav. Evol. 78, 7–21 10.1159/000327316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas J. H. (2012). Evolution of columns, modules, and domains in the neocortex of primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109Suppl. 1, 10655–10660 10.1073/pnas.1201892109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn D. M., Krubitzer L. (2002). Massive cross-modal cortical plasticity and the emergence of a new cortical area in developmentally blind mammals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 11429–11434 10.1073/pnas.162342799 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kappeler L., Meaney M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and parental effects. Bioessays 32, 818–827 10.1002/bies.201000015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlen S. J., Kahn D. M., Krubitzer L. (2006). Early blindness results in abnormal corticocortical and thalamocortical connections. Neuroscience 142, 843–858 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlen S. J., Krubitzer L. (2009). Effects of bilateral enucleation on the size of visual and nonvisual areas of the brain. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1360–1371 10.1093/cercor/bhn176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kok M. A., Chabot N., Lomber S. G. (2013). Crossmodal reorganization of cortical afferents to dorsal auditory cortex following early- and Late-onset deafness. J. Comp. Neurol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1002/cne.23439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornack D. R. (2000). Neurogenesis and the evolution of cortical diversity: mode, tempo, and partitioning during development and persistence in adulthood. Brain Behav. Evol. 55, 336–344 10.1159/000006668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornack D. R., Rakic P. (1998). Changes in cell-cycle kinetics during the development and evolution of primate neocortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 1242–1246 10.1073/pnas.95.3.1242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koyabu D. B., Endo H. (2009). Craniofacial variation and dietary adaptations of african colobines. J. Hum. Evol. 56, 525–536 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreile A. K., Bonhoeffer T., Hübener M. (2011). Altered visual experience induces instructive changes of orientation preference in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 13911–13920 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2143-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L. (2007). The magnificent compromise: cortical field evolution in mammals. Neuron 56, 201–208 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L. (2009). In search of a unifying theory of complex brain evolution. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 44–67 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04421.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L., Campi K. L., Cooke D. F. (2011). All rodents are not the same: A modern synthesis of cortical organization. Brain Behav. Evol. 78, 51–93 10.1159/000327320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L., Clarey J. C., Tweedale R., Calford M. B. (1998). Interhemispheric connections of somatosensory cortex in the flying fox. J. Comp. Neurol. 402, 538–559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L., Kaas J. (2005). The evolution of the neocortex in mammals: how is phenotypic diversity generated? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 444–453 10.1016/j.conb.2005.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L., Manger P., Pettigrew J., Calford M. (1995). Organization of somatosensory cortex in monotremes: in search of the prototypical plan. J. Comp. Neurol. 351, 261–306 10.1002/cne.903510206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer L. A., Seelke A. M. (2012). Cortical evolution in mammals: the bane and beauty of phenotypic variability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109Suppl. 1, 10647–10654 10.1073/pnas.1201891109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruska D. C. T. (2005). On the evolutionary significance of encephalization in some eutherian mammals: effects of adaptive radiation, domestication, and feralization. Brain Behav. Evol. 65, 73–108 10.1159/000082979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y., Fitzpatrick D., White L. E. (2006). The development of direction selectivity in ferret visual cortex requires early visual experience. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 676–681 10.1038/nn1684 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu D., Diorio J., Tannenbaum B., Caldji C., Francis D., Freedman A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science 277, 1659–1662 10.1126/science.277.5332.1659 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lomber S. G., Meredith M. A., Kral A. (2010). Cross-modal plasticity in specific auditory cortices underlies visual compensations in the deaf. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1421–1427 10.1038/nn.2653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo Z. X., Crompton A. W., Sun A. L. (2001). A new mammaliaform from the early Jurassic and evolution of mammalian characteristics. Science 292, 1535–1540 10.1126/science.1058476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Cerdeño V., Cunningham C. L., Camacho J., Antczak J. L., Prakash A. N., Cziep M. E., et al. (2012). Comparative analysis of the subventricular zone in rat, ferret and macaque: evidence for an outer subventricular zone in rodents. PLoS ONE 7:e30178 10.1371/journal.pone.0030178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto Y., Buemio A., Chu R., Vafaee M., Crews D. (2013). Epigenetic control of gonadal aromatase (cyp19a1) in temperature-dependent sex determination of red-eared slider turtles. PLoS ONE 8:e63599 10.1371/journal.pone.0063599 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meaney M. J., Szyf M. (2005). Maternal care as a model for experience-dependent chromatin plasticity? Trends Neurosci. 28, 456–463 10.1016/j.tins.2005.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menard J. L., Champagne D. L., Meaney M. J. (2004). Variations of maternal care differentially influence ‘fear’ reactivity and regional patterns of cFos immunoreactivity in response to the shock-probe burying test. Neuroscience 129, 297–308 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meredith R. W., Janečka J. E., Gatesy J., Ryder O. A., Fisher C. A., Teeling E. C., Goodbla A., et al. (2011). Impacts of the Cretaceous Terrestrial Revolution and KPg extinction on mammal diversification. Science 334, 521–524 10.1126/science.1211028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miranda T. B., Jones P. A. (2007). DNA methylation: the nuts and bolts of repression. J. Cell. Physiol. 213, 384–390 10.1002/jcp.21224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molnár Z. (2011). Evolution of cerebral cortical development. Brain Behav. Evol. 78, 94–107 10.1159/000327325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molnár Z., Clowry G. (2012). Cerebral cortical development in rodents and primates. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 45–70 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00003-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molnár Z., Knott G. W., Blakemore C., Saunders N. R. (1998). Development of thalamocortical projections in the South American gray short-tailed opossum (Monodelphis domestica). J. Comp. Neurol. 398, 491–514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakagawa Y., Johnson J. E., O'Leary D. D. (1999). Graded and areal expression patterns of regulatory genes and cadherins in embryonic neocortex independent of thalamocortical input. J. Neurosci. 19, 10877–10885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nonaka-Kinoshita M., Reillo I., Artegiani B., Martínez-Martínez M. A., Nelson M., Borrell V., et al. (2013). Regulation of cerebral cortex size and folding by expansion of basal progenitors. EMBO J. 32, 1817–1828 10.1038/emboj.2013.96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- North H. A., Karim A., Jacquin M. F., Donoghue M. J. (2010). EphA4 is necessary for spatially selective peripheral somatosensory topography. Dev. Dyn. 239, 630–638 10.1002/dvdy.22185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Hashi K., Miyashita M., Tanaka S. (2007). Experience-dependent orientation plasticity in the visual cortex of rats chronically exposed to a single orientation. Neurosci. Res. 58, 86–90 10.1016/j.neures.2007.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Leary D. D., Sahara S. (2008). Genetic regulation of arealization of the neocortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 90–100 10.1016/j.conb.2008.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Leary M. A., Bloch J. I., Flynn J. J., Gaudin T. J., Giallombardo A., Giannini N. P., Goldberg S. L., et al. (2013). The placental mammal ancestor and the post-K-Pg radiation of placentals. Science 339, 662–667 10.1126/science.1229237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Putignano E., Lonetti G., Cancedda L., Ratto G., Costa M., Maffei L., et al. (2007). Developmental downregulation of histone posttranslational modifications regulates visual cortical plasticity. Neuron 53, 747–759 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Recanzone G. H., Merzenich M. M., Jenkins W. M., Grajski K. A., Dinse H. R. (1992). Topographic reorganization of the hand representation in cortical area 3b owl monkeys trained in a frequency-discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 67, 1031–1056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reillo I., de Juan Romero C., García-Cabezas M. Á., Borrell V. (2011). A role for intermediate radial glia in the tangential expansion of the mammalian cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1674–1694 10.1093/cercor/bhq238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe T. B., Macrini T. E., Luo Z. X. (2011). Fossil evidence on origin of the mammalian brain. Science 332, 955–957 10.1126/science.1203117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheich H., Langner G., Tidemann C., Coles R. B., Guppy A. (1986). Electroreception and electrolocation in platypus. Nature 319, 401–402 10.1038/319401a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sengpiel F., Stawinski P., Bonhoeffer T. (1999). Influence of experience on orientation maps in cat visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 727–732 10.1038/11192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shitamukai A., Konno D., Matsuzaki F. (2011). Oblique radial glial divisions in the developing mouse neocortex induce self-renewing progenitors outside the germinal zone that resemble primate outer subventricular zone progenitors. J. Neurosci. 31, 3683–3695 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4773-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh R., Carvalho T., Gerstner G. E. (2005). Loading effects on rat craniomandibular morphology: a system for gravity studies. Acta Astronaut. 56, 357–366 10.1016/j.actaastro.2004.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smart I. H. M., Dehay C., Giroud P., Berland M., Kennedy H. (2002). Unique morphological features of the proliferative zones and postmitotic compartments of the neural epithelium giving rise to striate and extrastriate cortex in the monkey. Cereb. Cortex 12, 37–53 10.1093/cercor/12.1.37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stahl R., Walcher T., De Juan Romero C., Pilz G. A., Cappello S., Irmler M., et al. (2013). Trnp1 regulates expansion and folding of the mammalian cerebral cortex by control of radial glial fate. Cell 153, 535–549 10.1016/j.cell.2013.03.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterbing-D'Angelo S., Chadha M., Chiu C., Falk B., Xian W., Barcelo J., et al. (2011). Bat wing sensors support flight control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 11291–11296 10.1073/pnas.1018740108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoykova A., Götz M., Gruss P., Price J. (1997). Pax6-dependent regulation of adhesive patterning, R-cadherin expression and boundary formation in developing forebrain. Development 124, 3765–3777 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suga N., Niwa H., Taniguchi I., Margoliash D. (1987). The personalized auditory cortex of the mustached bat: adaptation for echolocation. J. Neurophysiol. 58, 643–654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugo N., Oshiro H., Takemura M., Kobayashi T., Kohno Y., Uesaka N., et al. (2010). Nucleocytoplasmic translocation of HDAC9 regulates gene expression and dendritic growth in developing cortical neurons. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1521–1532 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07218.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki S. C., Inoue T., Kimura Y., Tanaka T., Takeichi M. (1997). Neuronal circuits are subdivided by differential expression of type-II classic cadherins in postnatal mouse brains. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 9, 433–447 10.1006/mcne.1997.0626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T., Nowakowski R. S., Caviness V. S.Jr. (1995). The cell cycle of the pseudostratified ventricular epithelium of the embryonic murine cerebral wall. J. Neurosci. 15, 6046–6057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor J. S., Guillery R. W. (1994). Early development of the optic chiasm in the gray short-tailed opossum, Monodelphis domestica. J. Comp. Neurol. 350, 109–121 10.1002/cne.903500108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennant K. A., Adkins D. L., Scalco M. D., Donlan N. A., Asay A. L., Thomas N., et al. (2012). Skill learning induced plasticity of motor cortical representations is time and age-dependent. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 98, 291–302 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terakawa Y. W., Inoue Y. U., Asami J., Hoshino M., Inoue T. (2013). A sharp cadherin-6 gene expression boundary in the developing mouse cortical plate demarcates the future functional areal border. Cereb. Cortex. 23, 2293–2308 10.1093/cercor/bhs221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toki S., Morinobu S., Imanaka A., Yamamoto S., Yamawaki S., Honma K. (2007). Importance of early lighting conditions in maternal care by dam as well as anxiety and memory later in life of offspring. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 815–829 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05288.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuoc T. C., Boretius S., Sansom S. N., Pitulescu M.-E., Frahm J., Livesey F. J., et al. (2013). Chromatin regulation by BAF170 controls cerebral cortical size and thickness. Dev. Cell 25, 256–269 10.1016/j.devcel.2013.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waddington C. H. (1942). The Epigenotype. Endeavour 1, 18–20 [Google Scholar]
- Wang X., Tsai J. W., LaMonica B., Kriegstein A. R. (2011). A new subtype of progenitor cell in the mouse embryonic neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 555–561 10.1038/nn.2807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weaver I. C. G., Cervoni N., Champagne F. A., D'Alessio A. C., Sharma S., Seckl J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 847–854 10.1038/nn1276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welker E., Van der Loos H. (1986). Quantitative correlation between barrel-field size and the sensory innervation of the whiskerpad: a comparative study in six strains of mice bred for different patterns of mystacial vibrissae. J. Neurosci. 6, 3355–3373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welker W. I., Seidenstein S. (1959). Somatic sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of the racoon (Procyon Lotor). J. Comp. Neurol. 111, 469–501 10.1002/cne.901110306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise L. Z., Pettigrew J. D., Calford M. B. (1986). Somatosensory cortical representation in the Australian ghost bat, Macroderma gigas. J. Comp. Neurol. 248, 257–262 10.1002/cne.902480208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolsey T. A. (1967). Somatosensory, auditory and visual cortical areas of the mouse. Johns Hopkins Med. J. 121, 91–112 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang L. I., Bao S., Merzenich M. M. (2001). Persistent and specific influences of early acoustic environments on primary auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1123–1130 10.1038/nn745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zook J. M., Fowler B. C. (1986). A specialized mechanosensory array of the bat wing. Myotis 23–24, 31–36. [Google Scholar]