Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Mar 25.
Published in final edited form as: Cortex. 2012 Mar 30;48(9):1091–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.017

Linking Neurogenetics and Individual Differences in Language Learning: The Dopamine Hypothesis

Patrick C M Wong 1,2,4, Kara Morgan-Short 3, Marc Ettlinger 1, Jing Zheng 2,4
PMCID: PMC3965203  NIHMSID: NIHMS367704  PMID: 22565204

Abstract

Fundamental advances in neuroscience have come from investigations into neuroplasticity and learning. These investigations often focus on identifying universal principles across different individuals of the same species. Increasingly, individual differences in learning success have also been observed, such that any seemingly universal principle might only be applicable to a certain extent within a particular learner. One potential source of this variation is individuals’ genetic differences. Adult language learning provides a unique opportunity for understanding individual differences and genetic bases of neuroplasticity because of the large individual differences in learning success that have already been documented, and because of the body of empirical work connecting language learning and neurocognition. In this article, we review the literature on the genetic bases of neurocognition, especially studies examining polymorphisms of dopamine-related genes and procedural learning. This review leads us to hypothesize that there may be an association between dopamine-related genetic variation and language learning differences. If this hypothesis is supported by future empirical findings we suggest that it may point to neurogenetic markers that allow for language learning to be personalized.

Keywords: neurogenetics, procedural learning, dopamine

Introduction

Research on the neuroscience of learning has been dominated by investigations of neuroplasticity that consider questions such as what aspects of the brain can change, under what conditions can they change, and at what age is change still be possible (e.g., Hubel and Wiesel, 1970; Merzenich et al., 1984; Recanzone et al., 1992)? This research has informed our most fundamental understanding of learning and the brain. Increasingly, researchers are now paying close attention to the fact that large individual differences also exist in learning (e.g., Golestani and Zatorre, 2009; Wong et al., 2007). Therefore it is crucial that research on the neuroscience of learning should begin to examine the origins of these individual differences, including neurogenetic contributions. In this article, we will focus on a type of learning that shows large indvidual differences especially when learning begins in adulthood, namely language learning (e.g., Donyei, 2005; Johnson and Newport, 1989). Language is arguably a defining characteristic of humans (e.g., Donald, 1991; Jerison, 1973; Lewin, 1993), and its relationship with the brain has been extensively studied (e.g., Friederici et al., 2002; Hickok and Poeppel 2007; Ullman, 2004). Language learning is therefore ideally suited for examining the biological bases of individual differences in learning. We will focus on the learning of grammar, an aspect of language that has been shown to be very difficult to acquire to native-like proficiency (Abrahamsson and Hyltenstam, 2009; Weber-Fox and Neville, 1996) and that has a relatively clear neurophysiological basis.

Numerous factors have been found to relate to success in language learning, including environmental and neural factors such as musical experience (Wong & Perrachione, 2007; Slevc & Miyake, 2006), the type of training (Morgan-Short, Sanz, Steinhauer & Ullman, 2010; Morgan-Short, Steinhauer, Sanz & Ullman, 2012; Norris & Ortega, 2000, Peach & Wong, 2004), working memory (Miyake & Friedman, 1998), and neuroanatomy (e.g., Golestani et al., 2009 Wong et al., 2008, Warrier et al., 2009). Although these studies have identified some sources of variability in language learning success, none have focused on genetics. The complexity of both language and the genome makes it challenging to identify specific genes that contribute to language learning, especially genes related to our focus of normal variation in learning (see gene ASPM for the perception of lexical tone, Wong et al., in press; see genes ROBO1, Hannula-Jouppi et al., 2005; FOXP2, Lai et al., 2003; CNTNAP2, Whitehouse et al., 2011 for work on communicative impairments and developmental delay). Fortunately, several characteristics of the dopaminergic system have been established, including relevant genes, brain systems, domain-general (e.g., cognitive) functions, and language functions. These characteristics can form the basis for developing informed hypotheses concerning the genetic basis of grammar learning. For example, studies have attributed grammar learning (and non-linguistic rule learning) to domain-general functions such as the procedural (implicit) memory system (e.g., Ullman, 2004), as well as to brain systems such as structures within the frontostriatal pathway (especially Broca’s area) (e.g., Opitz and Friederici, 2003).

The dopaminergic system is tied to the frontostriatal pathway, among other brain structures (see Seamans & Yang, 2004 for a review), as well as to the procedural memory system (e.g., Shohamy & Adcock, 2010). In non-linguistic domains, the genes that encode dopamine (DA) receptors and transporters/catabolizers are tied to various types of procedural (rule) learning and brain responses (e.g., Karabanov et al., 2010). Based on the aforementioned facts about the dopaminergic system, we hypothesize that DA-related genes (and their interactions) may be associated with variation in grammar learning and functions of the frontostriatal pathway. This review provides our analysis and synthesis of the literature that led us to develop the above-stated hypothesis. It is our aim that this review and hypothesis will serve as a catalyst in generating new empirical research on the genetic bases of language learning.

Below, we will first discuss some basic facts about the DA system and its functions as they relate to procedural rule learning, reward, and cognition more broadly. We will specifically include studies that examine polymorphisms of DA-related genes and differences in performance on cognitive tasks. We will then focus the discussion on grammar learning, including the associated cognitive functions and brain systems. The relations among DA-related genes, procedural learning, grammar learning, and brain systems ultimately lead us to hypothesize that there may be a relationship between DA-genes and grammar learning.

The Dopaminergic System and DA-Related Genes

Major divisions of the dopaminergic system contain neurons from the substantia nigra pars compacta and ventral tegmental area projecting to divisions of the striatum, cingulate cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex (PFC), and other regions (see Seamans and Yang, 2004 for a review). Once released presynaptically, DA interacts with one of five DA receptors D1, D2, D3, D4, and D5, encoded by genes DRD1, DRD2, DRD3, DRD4, and DRD5 respectively. DA receptors are G protein-coupled receptors and are divided into two major classes: D1-like (D1 and D5) and D2-like (D2, D3 and D4). These receptors are distributed across regions of the central nervous system with different relative density levels. While very high densities of both classes of DA receptors are found in the striatum (across the caudate, putamen, and nucleus accumbens), a high density of D1, but not D2, receptors can be found in the frontal cortex (Camps et al., 1990; Khan et al., 2000; Little, Carroll, and Cassin, 1995). In addition to interacting with DA receptors, once released, DA is eliminated extracellularly by the DA transporter (encoded by gene DAT1) and the catabolizer enzyme Catechol-O-methyltransferase (encoded by gene COMT), primarily in the striatum and frontal cortex, respectively (e.g., Cass and Gerhardt, 1995; Cragg, Rice, and Greenfield, 1997; Sesack et al., 1998; Wayment et al., 2001). Thus, levels of DAT and COMT ultimately affect the impact of DA. DA and DA receptors modulate a number of different molecular and cellular processes, including AMPA, NMDA, and GABA responses, which can lead to short- and long-term synaptic changes across different regions of the brain (see Seamans and Yang, 2004 for a review). One of these processes involves the Dopamine-and-cAMP-regulated neuronal phosphoprotein (32 kDa) (encoded by gene DARPP-32, also known as PPP1R1B), which is found in the striatum (Ouimet et al., 1992) and affects functions and plasticity of DA receptors (e.g., Calabresi et al., 2000; Stipanovich et al., 2008). Over- and under-activation of DA receptors can lead to enhanced and/or impaired brain functions (e.g., Vijayraghavan et al., 2007; Zahrt et al., 1997).

It is worth noting that although our fundamental understanding of the dopaminergic system comes from animal research, and although substantial similarities exist between the non-human mammalian (especially primate) and human systems, there remain differences between the two systems. For example, a high density of D2 receptors can be found in the human hippocampal CA1/2 region but not in the monkey (Camps et al., 1990; Jiao et al., 2003; see Shohamy and Adcock, 2010 for a review). Therefore, when human functions (e.g., language) are examined, it is important to directly examine the human DA system. In our discussion below, we will focus on the human literature.

Different alleles of DA-related genes are consequential to the impact of DA on neurons. For the receptor genes, specific alleles have been linked to an increase/decrease of receptor binding. For example, the A1 allele of DRD2 Taq1A is tied to reduced D2 receptor binding in the striatum (Noble, 2003). For the transporter genes, specific alleles are related to different levels of expression of the genes so that DA clearance rates could be affected. For example, Heinz et al. (2000) found individuals with the 9-repeat (at 3’-UTR region) allele had up to a 22% reduction in DAT protein compared to those with the 10-repeat allele, potentially making more DA available for individuals with the 9-repeat allele. As reviewed below, polymorphisms of DA-related genes can lead to differences in a variety of brain functions in humans, including psychiatric conditions, cognitive performance, and learning. Other genes are certainly relevant but we will only focus on those that have been examined in humans and whose functions we hypothesize to be linked to the kind of linguistic learning discussed in this article.

Procedural Learning and the DA System

Decades of research have linked the dopaminergic system to a variety of learning paradigms that have been termed stimulus-reward learning, prediction error, reward learning, probabilistic (category) learning, reinforcement learning (learning the underlying rule of reinforcement), and trial-by-trial feedback learning (e.g., Frank et al., 2009; Karabanov et al., 2010; Klein et al., 2007). Both animal and human subjects have been included in this research, and many reviews have been published summarizing this line of work (e.g., Seamans and Yang, 2004; Shohamy and Adcock, 2010). There are inconsistencies in how learning paradigms are named across studies, and sometimes similar features are present in what are described as different learning paradigms. Nonetheless, a common feature of the different learning paradigms is procedural learning.

Procedural learning is a type of learning that is generally viewed as distinct from declarative learning. Declarative memory and learning is the knowledge that we have about facts and events related to our world. It is typically characterized as “knowledge that,” encompasses representations of both semantic and episodic memory, and is primarily subserved by the medial temporal lobe, particularly the hippocampus, as well as by temporal and parietal cortical areas and BA45/47 (Eichenbaum and Cohen, 2001; Squire and Knowlton, 2000; Ullman, 2001, 2004). Procedural memory and learning, which is often described as “knowledge how,” underlies not only our ability to learn and perform skills and habits, such as typing, driving a car, throwing a ball, etc. (Eichenbaum and Cohen, 2001), but also our ability to learn abstract rules. The procedural memory system is primarily associated with frontal/basal ganglia circuits and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), including BA44 (Ullman, 2001, 2004), and the PFC (e.g., Frank et al., 2004), where DA-associated neurons are found. Several cognitive tasks demonstrate procedural learning’s relationship with the dopamine system. One such task is the “weather prediction” task (Gluck and Bower, 1988), in which subjects learn to predict category outcomes (“rain” or “sunshine”) by the probabilistic linkage of a set of cards and these two categories. Knowlton et al. (1996) found that subjects with basal ganglia lesions have difficulty with such tasks. Another example of a procedural learning task is the learning of an artificial grammar. de Vries et al. (2010) found in healthy adults that performance in artificial grammar learning was improved after levodopa (a precursor of DA) intake. In our own work, we found that procedural memory is correlated with the learning of an artificial grammar, specifically, the learning of word and sound pattern combination rules (morpho-phonological grammar) (Ettlinger et al., 2009) and the learning of rules about how words are combined to form sentences (syntax) (Morgan-Short, Farreta-Stutenberg, Brill, Carpenter, Wong, submitted). Moreover, such learning is associated with activation in PFC (including Broca’s area) and basal ganglia (especially the striatum) (Ettlinger et al., 2010). Although it is often the case that procedural learning paradigms include learning via feedback in order for learners to learn the underlying pattern (e.g., Frank et al., 2004; Schultz et al., 1997), learning from trial-by-trial feedback is not always necessary to achieve procedural (more specifically probabilistic) learning. In their recent study, Li et al. (2011) found that both explicit instruction without trial-by-trial feedback and trial-by-trial feedback without explicit instruction resulted in successful probabilistic learning and activation in the striatum and prefrontal cortex, though to different degrees.

Procedural Learning, DA Receptors, and Genes

Research into the relationship between procedural memory/learning and the dopaminergic system has examined receptor binding and polymorphisms of DA-related genes more directly. In two Positron Emission Tomography (PET) studies, D2 receptor binding and receptor density have been found to be associated with (non-episodic) category fluency (Cervenka et al., 2008) and implicit sequence learning (Karabanov et al., 2010). In addition, Takahashi et al. (2010) found that healthy adults with lower striatal D1 receptor density show over- and under-estimations of low and high probabilities, respectively, in a version of the probabilistic category learning task. As discussed earlier, functions of DA-related genes, DRD1-5, DAT1, COMT and DARPP-32, can significantly affect DA’s impact. Polymorphisms of these genes change gene products’ biochemical properties and expression distribution. Therefore, it is not surprising that individuals with different genetic profiles may have different learning capabilities. For example, Jocham et al. (2009) and Klein et al. (2007) found that subjects with the DRD2 (Taq1A) A1 allele show impaired probabilistic learning (including reversal learning). A1 allele is known to be associated with reduction in dopamine receptor D2 expression in the human frontostriatal pathway (Ritchie and Noble, 2003). In terms of reward and feedback learning, Krugel et al. (2009), Camara et al. (2010), Yacubian et al. (2007), and Dreher et al. (2009) have found an association between the COMT gene and the learning of reward structures and rapid adaptation to reward contingencies. More interestingly, Yacubian et al. and Dreher et al. found an important dopamine gene-gene interaction between the COMT and DAT1. Subjects with the lowest DA clearance rate, i.e., the combination of COMT Val158Met and DAT1 9-repeat genotype, showed the highest activity in the striatum and/or PFC activities associated with reward learning. The fact that these two DA-related genes interact speaks to the importance of considering more than one DA gene within a single study. For example, reduced D2 receptor binding (DRD2 TaqA1 allele) could be offset by a decrease in extracellular DA transport (DAT1 9-repeat). In addition to COMT, Frank, et al. (2007) and Frank et al. (2009) found DRD2 (C957T) and DARPP-32 to be associated with different aspects of reinforcement learning. Both DRD2 (C957T) and DARPP-32 have been found to express in the striatum (Hirvonen et al., 2005; Hirvonen et al., 2004; Stipanovich et al., 2008). Although not typically a focus, DRD4 has also been examined in the reward learning literature and has been found to be linked to reward magnitude and activation in the anterior insula and cingulate cortex (Camara et al., 2010).

Other Cognitive Functions

In addition to procedural learning, DA and its related genes are associated with other cognitive functions such as working memory and attention. D1 receptor binding has not only been associated with working memory in animals (e.g., Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995), but also in humans (McNab et al., 2009). McNab et al. found that after 15 hours of working memory training, adults not only improved on working memory capacity, but their D1 receptor binding potential was also changed. In addition to working memory, a study by Karlsson et al. (2009) also found D1 receptor binding to be related to inhibitory control. Unlike D1 receptors, which are more clearly linked to working memory and the prefrontal cortex, the evidence for D2 receptors and working memory is mixed (Luciana et al., 1992; Muller et al., 1998). Furthermore, DA-related gene COMT has been found to be associated with working memory (Goldberg et al., 2003), executive function and inhibitory control (Savitz et al., 2006; Wilkosc et al., 2010 for a review), as well as attention deficits, which are associated with PFC deficits (Reuter et al., 2006). DAT1 has been associated with performance gain in working memory training (Brehmer et al., 2009). Likewise, DRD1, DRD2, DRD3, and DRD4 have been associated with executive control and inhibitory functions (Lane et al., 2008; Rodriguez-Jimenez et al., 2006; Rybakowski et al., 2005; Wilkosc et al., 2010). DARPP-32 is associated with better performance in a battery of cognitive tasks, including IQ, working memory, and procedural learning (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2007). Taken together, these studies suggest that DA and related genes are associated with a broad array of human cognitive and learning functions that are associated with the PFC and the basal ganglia.

Summary of DA-Related Genes, Brain, and Functions

Although investigations into single nucleotide polymorphisms and human behaviors have generated much attention, some results have failed to be replicated. Nonetheless, the majority of the DA-related genes we discussed here have been examined in multiple studies, some with converging multi-modal evidence. As far as these genes are concerned, we have every reason to believe that their associations with human cognitive functions are at least partially understood. Table 1 provides a summary of the DA-related genes that might be potentially relevant to language learning based on our literature review above. Subjects with the allele that results in increased impact of DA (what we marked “DA+”) have been found to show better procedural learning, working memory, and/or executive function, with brain activity often seen in the striatum and different aspects of the prefrontal cortex. It is worth noting that as in many genomic studies, opposite phenotypic effects of the same alleles or same effects of the alternate alleles have been observed for a variety of reasons such as culture-gene and gene-gene interactions (Battle et al., 2007; Chiao and Blizinsky, 2010; Kim et al., 2010). Alternatively, opposite phenotypic effects could be an indication of a Type-I error (false positive).

Table 1.

Genes/SNPs that have been implicated in procedural learning and other cognitive functions, some of which may potentially be relevant to language learning.

Gene Polymorphism (dB SNP#) DA+ Allele Allele Functions Relevant Brain Regions Relevant Behavioral Functions Relevant Studies
DRD1 A-48G (rs4532) A A allele increases DRD1 binding PFC Executive function, inhibitory control Lane, et al. (2008); Rybakowski, et al. (2005)
DRD2 Taq1A (rs1800497) A2 A1 allele reduces DRD2 density and DA binding MeFC, Striatum, OFC, Cingulate Probabilistic Learning Klein, et al. (2007); Jocham et al. (2009)
DRD2 C957T (rs6277) T T allele increases DRD2 binding Striatum Reward Learning, executive functions Rodriguez-Jimenez, et al., (2006); Frank, et al. (2009); Frank, et al., (2007); M. Hirvonen, et al., (2004); J. Hirvonen et al., (2005)
DAT1 (rs28363170) 9-R (“S”) 9-R lowers DAT expression PFC, OFC, striatum Reward Learning Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, and Posner (2005); Dreher, Kohnab, Kolachanab, Weinbergerb, and Berman (2009)
COMT Val158Met (rs4680) met Met allele decreases COMT enzymatic activity PFC, OFC, striatum Reward Learning, executive function Frank, Doll, Oas-Terpstra, and Moreno (2009); Yacubian, et al. (2007); Dreher, Kohnab, Kolachanab, Weinbergerb, and Berman (2009)

DA+ allele = dopamine-preferential allele, PFC=Prefrontal Cortex, MeFC=Medial Prefrontal Cortex, OFC=Orbital Frontal Cortex

Although we focused on describing DA-related genes and their contribution to procedural learning, it is worth noting that functional impacts of these genes are also influenced by environmental factors such as psychosocial stressors (White et al., 2009) and smoking (e.g., Wiebe et al., 2009). However, as far as we are aware, the gene-environment interaction studies conducted so far have yet to examine how such interaction might affect language learning.

Language Learning and Memory Systems

Thus far, we have discussed different aspects of the dopaminergic system, including genomic studies, and how it relates to procedural learning and other aspects of cognition including working memory and inhibitory control. Here, we discuss more specifically how language learning might be related to the dopaminergic system. These studies all point to successful grammar learning being tied to procedural memory and its associated brain structures. Furthermore, working memory may also be relevant.

Theoretical Models

After many years of learning, many adults still have difficulty mastering a foreign language/second language (L2). While the learning of certain aspects of foreign languages, such as vocabulary, can be acquired with nearly native-like proficiency by many adult learners (Weber-Fox and Neville, 1996), rules governing combinations of linguistic elements (grammar) are relatively difficult to acquire (Abrahamsson and Hyltenstam, 2009; Johnson and Newport, 1989; Weber-Fox and Neville, 1996). These grammatical rules include rules governing how sound elements or phonemes are combined (phonology), how words are created (morphology), and how words and their derivations are combined to form sentences (syntax). Research studies have provided evidence that the learning of vocabulary (lexicon) and grammar is tied to the declarative (explicit) (Damasio et al., 1996) and procedural (implicit) (Altmann, 2002; Gupta and Dell, 1999; Marslen-Wilson and Tyler, 2007) memory systems, respectively (Ullman 2001, 2004). The connection between the lexicon and declarative memory and between grammar and procedural memory is framed explicitly under the Declarative/Procedural (DP) model (Ullman, 2001, 2004) and other similar models (Paradis, 1994) (see Table 2 for a summary). According to this model, for the native/first language (L1), the mental lexicon (vocabulary) is expected to rely on declarative memory, while the mental grammar is expected to rely on procedural memory (Ullman 2001, 2004). Evidence implicating the importance of grammar and procedural memory in L1 acquisition comes from work in children with specific language impairment (SLI), a developmental language impairment that can often involve deficits in grammar usage (e.g., Rice & Oetting, 1993, Rice et al., 2009). Specifically, Evans et al. (2009) found that SLI children were also impaired in statistical learning, a type of learning that is viewed as implicit. If it is in fact the case that language (grammatical) impairment in childhood is linked to the procedural system, our DA-related gene-grammar linkage hypothesis might also apply to L1 acquisition and impairment. Furthermore, DA-related genes might interact with other genes that have been implicated in SLI, including Contactin-associated protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2) (Vernes et al., 2008), ATPase, Ca++ transporting, type 2C, member 2 (ATP2C2), and c-maf-inducing protein (CMIP) (Newbury et al., 2009).

Table 2.

Primary characteristics of the declarative and procedural memory systems and functions in first and second languages, based on Ullman (2001, 2004, 2005)

Declarative memory Procedural memory
Neural basis • Medial temporal lobe, particularly the hippocampus • Frontal/basal ganglia circuits
• Neocortical regions, including temporal and parietal cortex and BA 45/47 • Inferior frontal gyrus, particularly BA 44
Non-linguistic function • Semantic (facts) and episodic (events) memory • Motor and cognitive skills and habits
Primary linguistic function for L1 • L1 lexicon • L1 grammar
Primary linguistic function for L2 • L2 lexicon and grammar at low proficiency and exposure • No posited role for L2 lexicon or grammar at low proficiency and exposure
• L2 lexicon at high proficiency and exposure • L2 grammar at high proficiency and exposure

Note. BA = Brodmann area. L1 = First language. L2 = Second language.

Although some variation is expected in L1 acquisition, substantially more is expected in L2 acquisition in adulthood. The relationship between the memory systems and mental lexicon and grammar is arguably more complex for L2 acquisition (Ullman, 2001; Ullman et al., 2005). On one hand, as in the L1, declarative memory is expected to underlie the lexicon in L2 at all levels of proficiency and experience with the L2. On the other hand, L2 grammar, unlike L1 grammar, is expected to be dependent on declarative memory at lower levels of proficiency and experience, but may come to rely on procedural memory at higher levels of L2 proficiency and experience. In fact, it may be the case that one must come to rely on procedural memory for grammatical processing in order to reach the highest levels of L2 proficiency. Thus, from the standpoint of individual differences and cognitive indicators of learning success, L2 learners who reach higher levels of proficiency in an L2 may be the individuals that possess a better procedural learning ability. Evidence for the DP (and other similar) models includes behavioral (psycholinguistic) (Bowden et al., 2010; Neubauer and Clahsen, 2009; Silva and Clahsen, 2008) and neuroimaging studies of L1 and L2 learners (e.g., Golestani and Zatorre, 2004; Xue et al., 2004). For example, neuroimaging studies suggest that lexical processing in L1 and L2 relies on similar neural substrates (Chee et al., 1999; Klein et al., 1999; Xue et al., 2004), but that the neural substrates underlying L1 and L2 grammatical processing differ either qualitatively or quantitatively, with the area and level of activation becoming more L1-like (Golestani and Zatorre, 2004; Perani et al., 1998) and more specific to inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Abutalebi, 2008) at higher levels of proficiency.

It is worth noting that alternative models such as the Convergence Hypothesis (Green, 2003) and the Competition Model (Hernandez et al., 2005) exist to make predictions about L2 learning. However, the most important aspect of our discussion here pertains to the dopaminergic system which overlaps with the procedural memory system. These alternative models do not make explicit predictions regarding the role of the dopaminergic system in language learning, do not explain why procedural memory is often tied to grammar learning, and most importantly do not reject the notion of the dopaminergic system being intimately linked to grammar learning.

Besides procedural memory, working memory has also been found to be associated with L2 learning, presumably because it facilitates grammar and other kinds of learning by allowing relevant information to be kept active (Mackey et al., 2010; see Miyake and Friedman, 1998 for a review of classical studies). Aspects of working memory have been attributed to the PFC, where DA-related genes (especially DRD1 and COMT) are expressed. Thus, by examining these genes, it is also possible to examine the potential contribution of working memory in L2 learning.

Procedural System and Artificial Grammar Learning

The aforementioned studies mostly concern L2 learning in the classroom or other “naturalistic” learning environments. However, much research has examined artificial grammar learning in the laboratory. Clearly, the potential limitation of artificial language paradigms is their ecological validity. However, because of the challenge of establishing internal validity in natural language studies, the best method for investigating certain language-related issues may be studies that consider converging evidence from artificial and natural language studies. It is worth mentioning that in our own study designed to explicitly compare artificial grammar learning in the laboratory and L2 learning in the classroom, we found a close connection between the two (Ettlinger et al., 2011).

In addition to studies on actual (“naturalistic”) L2 learners (typically learners in the classroom), support for the DP model comes from the artificial grammar learning (short-term learning in the laboratory) literature. When presented with segmented words in a continuous speech stream, listeners can learn to rely on transitional probabilities of phonemes and syllables (e.g., Saffran et al., 1996), which have been found to be subserved by the PFC and basal ganglia, among other structures (McNealy et al., 2006). In addition to learning speech-sound transitional probabilities and word segmentation, language users need to learn how words are created. In the process of creating words, changes in sound structures may also take place. For example, English learners know that to create the plural forms of many nouns, the suffix /-s/ is added at the end of a word (e.g., cat-s = cat + s). However, sometimes the /-s/ has to be altered because of its surrounding phonological context. For example, to signal the plural form of ‘dog’, the /-s/ is changed to a /-z/ sound in spoken language (dogz) because the final sound /g/ in ‘dog’ is articulatorily more similar to a /z/ sound than an /s/. In a series of recent studies, we have found that the learning of some morpho-phonological rules in an artificial grammar is associated with procedural memory (Ettlinger et al., 2009) and the fronto-striatal pathway (Ettlinger et al., 2009). Perhaps the most frequently cited example of artificial grammar learning is the learning of syntax. For example, Opitz and Friederici (2003) found increasing levels of activation in Broca’s area during the learning of phrase structure rules via the artificial language BROCANTO. Using a modified version of this language, BROCANTO2, Morgan-Short et al. (2012) found a reliance on lexical/semantic processing for a syntactic structure (phrase structure) at low proficiency but native-like processing of the same structure at high levels of proficiency, supporting the DP model for a group of learners that received implicit training, i.e., exposure to the language without provision of grammatical explanation. Because artificial languages have been well documented in the literature as a source for insights into grammar learning, they can be used as starting points for investigations into the polymorphisms of DA-related genes and language learning. Although the question of interest here is DA-related genes and grammar learning, more direct evidence for the involvement of the DA system in grammar learning would involve direct targeted manipulation of the availability and impact of DA, as in pharmacological studies (e.g., de Vries et al., 2010).

Counter-Evidence and Other Challenges

We have discussed our hypothesized framework for understanding the neurogenetics of language learning by highlighting the relationships between genes and specific brain systems, and by describing how these brain systems control the domain-general cognitive functions that appear to be integral to language learning. However, we should note some important caveats. First, much of the evidence we have discussed on the relationships among the brain, domain-general cognitive processing, and language come from studies of native language ability. These relationships are less clear with respect to L2 learning. On the one hand, Abutalebi (2008) highlights evidence suggesting that L2 learning depends on the same sets of neural processes as L1 learning. Ullman (2005) however, suggests that L2 learning makes far greater use of the declarative memory system and corresponding neural substrates, namely the medial temporal lobe, at least during initial stages of learning.

Furthermore, the dissociation between procedural memory brain regions and declarative memory brain regions has been called into question by recent research. For example, Shohamy et al. (2009) and Newell et al. (2007) have found evidence that medial temporal regions play an important role in probabilistic learning. This may not be surprising given that dopamine receptors can be found in structures such as the hippocampus (see Shohamy and Adcock, 2010 for a review), and hippocampal structures are strongly associated with declarative memory (e.g., Squire and Knowlton, 2000).

Also, in a recent morpho-phonological grammar learning study, Ettlinger et al. (2009) found that procedural memory is most intimately involved in the learning of simple concatenation rules, but that acquiring a grammar based on analogy depends on declarative memory. Thus, it is important to note that although we hypothesize that there may be a relationship among procedural memory, DA, and grammar learning, the hypothesis may only speak to some aspects of grammar learning. Our goal in this review article is to posit potential links between grammar learning and certain DA-related genes in order to encourage future investigations that consider the specific types of grammar learning that we hypothesize to be tied to DA-related genes.

The impact of DA on our nervous system and neurocognitive functions is obviously extremely broad, and its effects reach far beyond language learning. The question of whether certain types of grammar learning are also associated with subtle variations of some other DA-related behaviors would require future studies to determine.

Other Considerations in Studying the Neurogenetics of Language Learning

Other Relevant Genes

Although this review primarily concerns the dopaminergic system and its relationship with normal variation of language learning, genes that have been implicated in communication disorders are undoubtedly relevant (see Newbury & Monaco, 2010 for a recent review). We will discuss several key areas of findings here. SLI has been linked to chromosome 16q and 19q (Bishop, 2001; Rice, 2007; SLI Consortium, 2004). More recently, several genes, such as CNTNAP2 (Vernes et al., 2008), and ATP2C2 and CMIP (Newbury et al., 2009), have been found to be associated with SLI. In developmental dyslexia, a disorder related to language, Roundabout homolog 1 (ROBO1) (Hannula-Jouppi et al., 2005) and KIAA0319 (Cope et al., 2005) have been implicated.

A number of studies have examined the genetic basis of stuttering (fluency disorders). One twin study involving over 4000 twins showed that genetic factors might explain a majority of the variance in stuttering (Gerber, 2001) and subsequent studies have linked this disorder to a number of chromosomes (e.g., 1q and 7q, Riaz et al., 2005). More recently, mutations of several genes have been proposed to play a role in a subset of cases of stuttering, including N-acetylglucosamine-1-phosphotransferase, gamma subunit (GNPTG), N-acetylglucosamine-1-phosphate transferase, alpha and beta subunits (GNTAB), and N-acetylglucosamine-1-phosphodiester alpha-N-acetylglucosaminidase (NAGPA) (Kang et al., 2010).

Perhaps the most frequently discussed genetic deficit of human communication is one that affects the KE family in which 15 individuals show developmental speech and language impairments (Hurst et al., 1990; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1995; Gopnik, 1990). Their impairments have later been linked to the FOXP2 (SPCH1) gene (Lai et al., 2001; Fisher et al., 1998; Lai et al., 2000), which is expressed in motor-related circuits, including the basal ganglia, thalamus, inferior olives, and cerebellum (Lai et al., 2003). FOXP2 belongs to a member of the large FOX (Forkhead box) family of transcription factors. Mutations of FOXP2 are associated with difficulties in the learning and production of coordinated sequences of orofacial movements (e.g., Vargha-Khadem et al., 1995; Shriberg et al., 2006). The involvement of FOXP2 in speech production function is confirmed by structural and functional neuroimaging studies. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) has shown significantly different amounts of grey matter in motor-related areas in affected members of KE family compared to unaffected members (Watkins et al., 2002), which corroborates with a positron emission tomography (PET) study showing anomalies in activation in the motor-related areas (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1998). An fMRI study comparing affected and unaffected members of the KE family in covert and overt verb generation tasks revealed that while the unaffected members showed a typical left-dominant distribution of activation including Broca’s area, the affected members showed more posterior and bilateral patterns (Liégeois et al., 2003; see Vargha-Khadem et al. (2005) and Fisher & Scharff (2009) for reviews of these neural findings related to FOXP2). Similarly, Pinel et al. (2012) found common polymorphisms of FOXP2 to be associated with variation in activation of language-related brain areas, including Broca’s area. More relevant to our hypothesis, Enard et al. (2009) further demonstrated in a mouse model that insertion of two human-specific amino acid substitutions into the mouse version of the Foxp2 protein resulted in decreased DA concentrations in cortico-basal ganglia circuits, altered plasticity of those circuits, and subtle changes in pup vocalization. Thus, our hypothesized association between DA and grammar learning could also be related to FOXP2. Future studies should consider how genes related to communication disorders (including FOXP2) might be involved in the DA system, and how the DA-related genes we reviewed might interact with the genes to ultimately give rise to successful language learning and impairment.

Developmental and Environmental Factors

Although the scope of this review is restricted to the potential genetic contributions to language learning in adulthood, we must acknowledge that developmental and environmental factors contribute substantially to language learning in general. Language input is a strong predictor of of measures of language development in children such as vocabulary size (Huttenlocher et al., 1991). Moreover, there is extensive evidence suggesting that by the end of the first year, infants’ sensitivity to speech sounds reduces from both native and non-native sounds to only sounds that occur in their native language (e.g., Werker & Tees, 1984; Polka & Werker, 1994), presumably due to the degree of exposure to their native language. Interestingly, with training, adults are able to learn foreign speech sounds (e.g., Pisoni et al., 1982; Jamieson & Morosan, 1989; Lively et al., 1993; Bradlow et al., 1997) albeit with great difficulty. As we discussed above, there is also a large degree of variability in how successfully adults learn new speech sounds (e.g., Wong & Perrachione, 2007) and grammar (Ettlinger et al., 2009)

Most interestingly, different training paradigms may result in variable degrees of success, depending on the learner (Perrachione et al., 2011), suggesting that environment (training in this case) plays an important role in influencing how learning success. In this Perrachione et al. study, English-speaking subjects learned a speech contrast that occurs in Mandarin Chinese but not English. Learners who showed reduced auditory perception, measured before training, were affected by the type of training the most. Specifically, training that introduced trial-by-trial variability in the training stimuli substantially impaired these learners, whereas learners who showed stronger auditory perceptual ability benefited more from trial-by-trial variability in training. These results suggest that it might be possible to personalize training for individual learners. Although, the evidence here is from using a behavioral measure (i.e. auditory perceptual ability) for personalizing learning, it is conceivable that genomic information can be used in the future.

GWAS and WGS

In considering future research on the neurogenetics of language learning, it is important to consider newer technological advances. Whole genome sequencing (WGS) and genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have become the norm for many studies of complex traits and have provided information about the molecular genetics of a number common diseases (e.g., McCarthy et al, 2008; Lyssenko et al., 2008; Lupski et al., 2010). Because of the substantial cost associated with performing a GWAS, which require a very large sample and often a replication sample, we believe it would be more cost effective to perform some initial hypothesis-driven experiments with a set of candidate genes (e.g., DA-related genes) for understanding language learning. These initial experiments not only allow for a demonstration that it is possible to examine the genetic bases of language learning, it can also inform us about the underlying molecular neurobiological mechanisms. If our hypothesis about DA-related genes and language learning is correct, a GWAS and/or WGS study can be informative beyond listing SNPs that show statistical significance. For example, GWAS can contribute to an understanding of potential gene-gene interactions that give rise to the impact of DA, and potentially other non-DA related neural pathways that are relevant to language learning for further hypothesis-driven experiments.

Conclusion

Independent lines of research have focused on the DA system and procedural learning on one hand, and language learning and cognitive and brain systems on the other. In this article, we argue that in fact these two lines of research should be considered in tandem in order to gain insight into the neurogenetics of language learning. Future research should begin to examine how genetic polymorphisms that have been linked to procedural learning might also be linked to language learning. With this linkage information, future research can then examine whether learners with different genotypic profiles can benefit from different kinds of training. The latter effort will be among the first to extend the concept of personalized medicine from pharmacogenetics (Eichelbaum et al., 2006; Evans and Johnson, 2001) to human learning. If successful, such a “personalized learning” approach will not only be useful for language learning, but many forms of skill learning that are relevant to neuropsychological and communication disorders, as well as physical and occupational medicine.

Although our focus was on the DA system and language learning, genes that are relevant to communication disorders are likely part of the genetic network that governs how language is learned as well. Future research is needed to examine the relative contributions of these genes and the type of language learning that they contribute.

Finally, it is important to note that the DA system also affords a very general impact on the nervous system beyond language learning and processing. Thus, future research should consider the degree to which the polymorphisms of the genes we discussed impact language and other cognitive capabilities in order to delineate their domain-specific and domain-general effects.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Mark Antoniou, Lionel Newman and Saaquib Bakhsh for their assistance in preparing for this manuscript. This work is supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01DC008333, R21DC009652, and ARRA R21DC009652-S1) awarded to P.W., and the National Institutes of Health (ARRA RC1DC010633) awarded to J.Z.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Abrahamsson N, Hyltenstam K. Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning. 2009;59:249–306. [Google Scholar]
  2. Abutalebi J. Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam) 2008;128(3):466–478. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Altmann GT. Statistical learning in infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2002;99(24):15250–15251. doi: 10.1073/pnas.262659399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Battle NC, Choudhry S, Tsai HJ, Eng C, Kumar G, Beckman KB, Naqvi M, Meade K, Watson HG, Lenoir M, Burchard EG. Ethnicity-specific gene-gene interaction between IL-13 and IL-4Ralpha among African Americans with asthma. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine. 2007;175(9):881–887. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200607-992OC. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bishop DVM. Genetic and environmental risks for specific language impairment in children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Biological Sciences. 2001;356(1407):369–380. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bowden HW, Gelfand MP, Sanz C, Ullman MT. Verbal inflectional morphology in L1 and L2 Spanish: A frequency effects study examining storage versus composition. Language Learning. 2010;60(1):44–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00551.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bradlow AR, Pisoni DB, Akahane-Yamada R, Tohkura Y. Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1997;101(4):2299–2310. doi: 10.1121/1.418276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brehmer Y, Westerberg H, Bellander M, Fuerth D, Karlsson S, Baeckman L. Working memory plasticity modultaed by dopamine transpoter genotype. Neuroscience Letters. 2009;467:117–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Calabresi P, Gubellini P, Centonze D, Picconi B, Bernardi G, Chergui K, Svenningsson P, Fienberg AA, Greengard P. Dopamine and cAMP-regulated phosphoprotein 32 kDa controls both striatal long-term depression and long-term potentiation, opposing forms of synaptic plasticity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20(22):8443–8451. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-22-08443.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Camara E, Kraemer U, Cunillera T, Marco-Pallares J, Cucurell D, Nager W, Mestres-Misse A, Bauer P, Schuele R, Schoels L, Tempelmann C, Rodriguez-Fornells A, Muente T. The effects of COMT (Val108/158Met) and DRD4 (SNP -521) dopamine genotypes on brain activations related to valence and magnitude of rewards. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:1985–1996. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Camps M, Kelly PH, Palacios JM. Autoradiographic localization of dopamine D 1 and D 2 receptors in the brain of several mammalian species. Journal of Neural Transmission-General Section. 1990;80(2):105–127. doi: 10.1007/BF01257077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cass WA, Gerhardt GA. In vivo assessment of dopamine uptake in rat medial prefrontal cortex: comparison with dorsal striatum and nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1995;65(1):201–207. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.65010201.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cervenka S, Baeckman L, Cselenyi Z, Halldin C, Farde L. Associations between dopamine D2-receptor binding and cognitive performance indiated functional compartmentalization of the human striatum. NeuroImage. 2008;40:1287–1295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chee MW, Tan EW, Thiel T. Mandarin and English single word processing studied with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1999;19(8):3050–3056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-08-03050.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chiao JY, Blizinsky KD. Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 2010;277(1681):529–537. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Consortium S. Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;74(6):1225–1238. doi: 10.1086/421529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cope N, Harold D, Hill G, Moskvina V, Stevenson J, Holmans P, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC, Williams J. Strong Evidence That KIAA0319 on Chromosome 6p Is a Susceptibility Gene of Developmental Dyslexia. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2005;76(4):581–591. doi: 10.1086/429131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cragg SJ, Rice ME, Greenfield SA. Heterogeneity of electrically evoked dopamine release and reuptake in substantia nigra, ventral tegmental area, and striatum. American Physiological Society. 1997;77:863–873. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.2.863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Damasio H, Grabowski TJ, Tranel D, Hichwa RD, Damasio AR. A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature. 1996;380(6574):499–505. doi: 10.1038/380499a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. de Vries MH, Ulte C, Zwitserlood P, Szymanski B, Knecht S. Increasing dopamine levels in the brain improves feedback-based procedural learning in healthy participants: an artificial-grammar-learning experiment. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(11):3193–3197. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Donald M. The Chronology of Anatomical and Cultural Change: In Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  22. Donyei Z. The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in Second Language Acquisition. Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dreher J, Kohnab P, Kolachanab B, Weinbergerb DR, Berman KF. Variation in dopamine genes influences responsivity of the human reward system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2009;106:617–622. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805517106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eichelbaum M, Ingelman-Sundberg M, Evans WE. Pharmacogenomics and individualized drug therapy. Annual Review of Medicine. 2006;57:119–137. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.56.082103.104724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Eichenbaum H, Cohen NJ. From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  26. Enard W, Gehre S, Hammerschmidt K, Hölter SM, Blass T, Somel M, Brückner MK, Schreiweis C, Winter C, Sohr R, Becker L, Wiebe V, Nickel B, Giger T, Müller U, Groszer M, Adler T, Aguilar A, Bolle I, Calzada-Wack J, Dalke C, Ehrhardt N, Favor J, Fuchs H, Gailus-Durner V, Hans W, Hölzlwimmer G, Javaheri A, Kalaydjiev S, Kallnik M, Kling E, Kunder S, Mossbrugger I, Naton B, Racz I, Rathkolb B, Rozman J, Schrewe A, Busch DH, Graw J, Ivandic B, Klingenspor M, Klopstock T, Ollert M, Quintanilla-Martinez L, Schulz H, Wolf E, Wurst W, Zimmer A, Fisher SE, Morgenstern R, Arendt T, de Angelis MH, Fischer J, Schwarz J, Pääbo S. A humanized version of Foxp2 affects cortico-basal ganglia circuits in mice. Cell. 2009;137(5):800–802. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ettlinger M, Bradlow AR, Wong PCM. The persistence and obliteration of opaque interactions. In: Bochnak R, editor. Proceedings of the 45th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society; Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ettlinger M, Morgan-Short K, Faretta-Stutenberg M, Newman LA, Wong PCM. The relationship between artificial and natural language learning. 85th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America; Pittsburgh, PA. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ettlinger M, Novis S, Wang L, Wong PCM. Neuroanatomical correlates of successful language learning. Organization for Human Brain Mapping Conference; Barcelona, Spain. 2010. [Google Scholar]
  30. Evans JL, Saffran JR, Robe-Torres K. Statistical Learning in Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52:321–335. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0189). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Evans WE, Johnson JA. Pharmacogenomics: The inherited basis for interindividual differences in drug response. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics. 2001;2:9–39. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genom.2.1.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fisher SE, Scharff CS. FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends in Genetics. 2009;25:166–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, Pembrey ME. Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature Genetics. 1998;18:168–170. doi: 10.1038/ng0298-168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Frank MJ, Doll BB, Oas-Terpstra J, Moreno F. Prefrontal and striatal dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in exploration and exploitation. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12(8):1062–1068. doi: 10.1038/nn.2342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Frank MJ, Moustafa AA, Haughey HM, Curran T, Hutchison KE. Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2007;104(41):16311–16316. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706111104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O’Reilly RC. By carrot or by stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science. 2004;306(5703):1940–1943. doi: 10.1126/science.1102941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Friederici AD, Steinhauer K, Pfeifer E. Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. National Academy of Sciences USA. 2002;99(1):529–534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012611199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gerber SE. The Handbook of Genetic Communicative Disorders. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  39. Gluck MA, Bower GH. From conditioning to category learning: An adaptive network model. Journal of Expemental Psychology-General. 1988;117(3):227–247. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.117.3.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Goldberg T, Egan MF, Gscheidle T, Coppola R, Weickert T, Kolachama BS, Goldman D, Weinberger DR. Executive subprocesses in working memory. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:889–896. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Golestani N, Zatorre RJ. Learning new sounds of speech: Reallocation of neural substrates. NeuroImage. 2004;21(2):494–506. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Golestani N, Zatorre RJ. Individual differences in the acquisition of second language phonology. Brain and Language. 2009;109(2-3):55–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gopnik M. Genetic basis of grammar defect. Nature. 1990;347:26. doi: 10.1038/347026a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Green DW. The neural basis of the lexicon and the grammar in L2 acquisition. In: van Hout R, Hulk A, Kuiken F, Towell R, editors. The Interface between Syntax and the Lexicon in Second Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  45. Gupta P, Dell GS. The emergence of language from serial order and procedural memory. In: MacWhinney B, editor. The Emergence of Language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999. pp. 447–481. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hannula-Jouppi K, Kaminen-Ahola N, Taipale M, Eklund R, Nopola-Hemmi J, Kaariainen H, Kere J. The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genetics. 2005;1(4):e50. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0010050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Heinz A, Goldman D, Jones DW, Palmour R, Hommer D, Gorey JG, Lee KS, Linnoila M, Weinberger DR. Genotype influences in vivo dopamine transporter availability in human striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22(2):133–139. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00099-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hernandez A, Li P, MacWhinney B. The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(5):220–225. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8(5):393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hirvonen J, van Erp TG, Huttunen J, Aalto S, Nagren K, Huttunen M, Lonnqvist J, Kaprio J, Hietala J, Cannon TD. Increased caudate dopamine D2 receptor availability as a genetic marker for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(4):371–378. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.4.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hirvonen M, Laasko A, Nagren K, Rinee JO, Pohjalainen T, Hieltala J. C957T polymorphism of the dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) gene affects striatal DRD2 availability in vivo. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9:1060–1066. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology. 1970;206(2):419–436. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1970.sp009022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hurst JA, Baraitser M, Auger E, Graham F, Norell S. An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1990;32(4):352–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1990.tb16948.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Seltzer M, Lyons T. Early Vocabulary Growth: Relation to Language Input and Gender. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27(2):236–248. [Google Scholar]
  55. Jamieson DG, Morosan DE. Training non-native speech contrasts in adults: Acquisition of the English /θ/-/đ/ by francophones. Perception and Psychophysics. 1989;40(4):205–215. doi: 10.3758/bf03211500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Jerison H. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  57. Jiao X, Pare WP, Tejani-Butt S. Strain differences in the distribution of dopamine transporter sites in rat brain. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2003;27(6):913–919. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00150-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jocham G, Klein TA, Neuman J, Cramon D, Reuter M, Ullsperger M. Dopamine DRD2 polymorphism alters reversal learning and associated neural activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:3695–3704. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5195-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Johnson JS, Newport EL. Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology. 1989;21(1):60–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kang C, Riazuddin S, Mundorff J, Krasnewich D, Friedman P, Mullikin JC, Drayna D. Mutations in the Lysosomal Enzyme-Targeting Pathway and Persistent Stuttering. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2010;362:677–685. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0902630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Karabanov A, Cervenka S, de Manzano O, Forssberg H, Farde L, Ullen F. Dopamine D2 receptor density in the limbic striatum is related to implicit but not explicit movement sequence learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2010;107(16):7574–7579. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911805107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Karlsson S, Nyberg L, Karlsson P, Fischer H, Thilers P, Macdonald S, Brehmer Y, Rieckmann A, Halldin C, Farde L, Backman L. Modulation of striatal dopamine D1 binding by cognitive processing. NeuroImage. 2009;48(2):398–404. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Khan ZU, Gutierrez A, Martin R, Penafiel A, Rivera A, de la Calle A. Dopamine D5 receptors of rat and human brain. Neuroscience. 2000;100(4):689–699. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00274-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kim HS, Sherman DK, Sasaki JY, Xu J, Chu TQ, Ryu C, Suh EM, Graham K, Taylor SE. Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2010;107(36):15717–15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Klein D, Milner B, Zatorre RJ, Zhao V, Nikelski J. Cerebral organization in bilinguals: A PET study of Chinese-English verb generation. Neuroreport. 1999;10(13):2841–2846. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199909090-00026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Klein TA, Neuman J, Reuter M, Hennig J, Cramon D, Ullsperger M. Genetically determined differences in learning from errors. Science. 2007;318:1642–1645. doi: 10.1126/science.1145044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Knowlton BJ, Mangels JA, Squire LR. A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science. 1996;273(5280):1399–1402. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5280.1399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Krugel LK, Biele G, Mohr P, Li S, Heekeren HR. Genetic variation in dopaminergic neuromodulation influences the ability to rapidly and flexibly adapt decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2009;106:17951–17956. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905191106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Levy ER, Hodgson S, Fox M, Jeremiah S, Povey S, Jamison DC, Green ED, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP. The SPCH1 region on human 7q31: genomic characterization of the critical interval and localization of translocations associated with speech and language disorder. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2000;67(2):357–368. doi: 10.1086/303011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP. A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature. 2001;413:519–523. doi: 10.1038/35097076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lai CS, Gerrelli D, Monaco AP, Fisher SE, Copp AJ. FOXP2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 11):2455–2462. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lane H, Liu Y, Huang C, Hsieh C, Chang Y, Chang L, Chang Y, Chang W. Prefrontal executive function and D1, D3, 5-HT2A and 5-HT6 receptor gene variation in healthy adults. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience. 2008;33:47–53. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Lewin R. Language and Modern Human Origins: In the Origin of Modern Humans. New York, NY: Scientific American Library; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  74. Li J, Delgado M, Phelps E. How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2011;108(1):55–60. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014938108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Liegeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F. Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:1230–1237. doi: 10.1038/nn1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Little KY, Carroll FI, Cassin BJ. Characterization and localization of [125I]RTI-121 binding sites in human striatum and medial temporal lobe. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 1995;274:1473–1483. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lively SE, Logan JS, Pisoni DB. Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/. II. The role of phonetic environment and talker variability in learning new perceptual categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1993;94(3):1242–1255. doi: 10.1121/1.408177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Luciana M, Depue RA, Arbisi P, Leon A. Facilitation of working memory in humans by a D2 dopamine receptor agonist. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1992;4:58–68. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.1.58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lupski JR, Reid JG, Gonzaga-Jauregui C, Deiros DR, Chen DCY, Nazareth L, Bainbridge M, Dinh H, Jing C, Wheeler DA, Yang C, Gehman C, Guo D, Irikat RK, Tom W, Fantin NJ, Muzny DM, Gibbs RA. Whole-Genome Sequencing in a Patient with Charcot-Marie-Tooth Neuropathy. New England Journal of Medicine. 2010;362:1181–1191. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0908094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lyssenko V, Nagorny CL, Erdos MR, Weirup N, Jonsson A, Spegel P, Bugliani M, Saxena R, Fex M, Pulizzi N, Isomaa B, Tuomi T, Nilsson P, Kuusisto J, Tuomilehto J, Boehnke M, Altshuler D, Sundler F, Eriksson JG, Jackson AU, Laakso M, Marchetti P, Watanabe RM, Mulder H, Groop L. Common Variant in MTNR1B associated with increased risk of type 2 diabetes and impaired early insulin secretion. Nature Genetics. 2008;41:82–88. doi: 10.1038/ng.288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mackey A, Adams R, Stafford C, Winke P. Exploring the relationship between modified output and working memory. Language Learning. 2010;60(3):501–533. [Google Scholar]
  82. Marslen-Wilson WD, Tyler LK. Dissociating types of mental computation. Nature. 1997;387(6633):592–594. doi: 10.1038/42456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Marslen-Wilson WD, Tyler LK. Morphology, language and the brain: The decompositional substrate for language comprehension. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences. 2007;362(1481):823–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. McCarthy MI, Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Goldstein DB, Little J, Ioanidis JPA, Hirschhorn JN. Genome-wide association studies for complex traits: consensus, uncertainty and challenges. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2008;9:356–369. doi: 10.1038/nrg2344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. McNab F, Varrone A, Farde L, Jucaite A, Bystritsky P, Forssberg H, Klingberg T. Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training. Science. 2009;323(5915):800–802. doi: 10.1126/science.1166102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. McNealy K, Mazziotta JC, Dapretto M. Cracking the language code: Neural mechanisms underlying speech parsing. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(29):7629–7639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5501-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Merzenich MM, Nelson RJ, Stryker MP, Cynader MS, Schoppmann A, Zook JM. Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1984;224(4):591–605. doi: 10.1002/cne.902240408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Straub RE, Lipska BK, Verchinski BA, Goldberg T, Callicott JH, Egan MF, Huffaker SS, Mattay VS, Kolachana B, Kleinman JE, Weinberger DR. Genetic evidence implicating DARPP-32 in human frontostriatal structure, function, and cognition. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2007;117(3):672–682. doi: 10.1172/JCI30413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Miyake A, Friedman N. Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies on training and retention. In: Healy AF, Bourne LE, editors. Foreign Language Learning: Psycholinguistic Studies on Training and Retention. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; 1998. pp. 339–364. [Google Scholar]
  90. Morgan-Short K, Faretta-Stutenberg M, Brill K, Carpenter H, Wong P. Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Submitted. [Google Scholar]
  91. Morgan-Short K, Sanz C, Steinhauer K, Ullman MT. Acquisition of Gender Agreement in Second Language Learners: An Event-Related Potential Study. Language Learning. 2010;60(1):154–193. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00554.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Morgan-Short K, Steinhauer K, Sanz C, Ullman MT. Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 24(4):933–947. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Muller U, von Cramon DY, Pollmann S. D1- versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18(7):2720–2728. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-07-02720.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Neubauer K, Clahsen H. Decomposition of inflected words in a second language: An experimental study of German participles. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 2009;31:403–435. [Google Scholar]
  95. Newbury DF, Monaco AP. Genetic Advances in the Study of Speech and Language Disorders. Neuron. 2010;68(2):309–320. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Newbury DF, Winchester L, Addis L, Paracchini S, Buckingham LL, Clark A, Cohen W, Cowie H, Dworzynski K, Everitt A, Goodyer IM, Hennessy E, Kindley AD, Miller LL, Nasir J, O’Hare A, Shaw D, Simkin Z, Simonoff E, Slonims V, Watson J, Ragoussis J, Fisher SE, Seckl JR, Helms PJ, Bolton PF, Pickles A, Conti-Ramsden G, Baird G, Bishop DV, Monaco AP. CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2009;85(2):264–272. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Newell BR, Lagnado DA, Shanks DR. Challenging the role of implicit processes in probabilistic category learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2007;14(3):505–511. doi: 10.3758/bf03194098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Noble EP. D2 dopamine receptor gene in psychiatric and neurologic disorders and its phenotypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2003;116B(1):103–125. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Norris JM, Ortega L. Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning. 2000;50(3):417–528. [Google Scholar]
  100. Opitz B, Friederici AD. Interactions of the hippocampal system and the prefrontal cortex in learning language-like rules. NeuroImage. 2003;19(4):1730–1737. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00170-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Ouimet CC, LaMantia AS, Goldman-Rakic P, Rakic P, Greengard P. Immunocytochemical localization of DARPP-32, a dopamine and cyclic-AMP-regulated phosphoprotein, in the primate brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1992;323(2):209–218. doi: 10.1002/cne.903230206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Paradis M. Neurolinguistic aspects of implicit and explicit memory: implications for bilingualism. In: Ellis N, editor. Implicit and Explicit Learning of Second Languages. London: Academic Press; 1994. pp. 393–419. [Google Scholar]
  103. Peach RK, Wong PCM. Integrating the message level into treatment for agrammatism using story retelling. Aphasiology. 2004;18:429–441. [Google Scholar]
  104. Perani D, Paulesu E, Galles NS, Dupoux E, Dehaene S, Bettinardi V, Cappa SF, Fazio F, Mehler J. The bilingual brain: Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain and Language. 1998;121(10):1841–1852. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.10.1841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Perrachione T, Lee J, Ha L, Wong PCM. Learning a novel phonological contrast depends on interactions between individual differences and training paradigm design. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2011;130(1):461–472. doi: 10.1121/1.3593366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Pinel P, Fauchereau F, Moreno A, Barbot A, et al. Genetic Variants of FOXP2 and KIAA0319/TTRAP/THEM2 Locus Are Associated with Altered Brain Activation in Distinct Language-Related Regions. 2012;32:817–825. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5996-10.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Pisoni DB, Aslin RN, Perey AJ, Hennessy BL. Some effects of laboratory training on identification and discrimination of voicing contrasts in stop consonants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1982;8(2):297–314. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.8.2.297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Polka L, Werker JF. Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 20(2):421–435. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.2.421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Recanzone GH, Merzenich MM, Dinse HR. Expansion of the cortical representation of a specific skin field in primary somatosensory cortex by intracortical microstimulation. Cereb Cortex. 1992;2(3):181–196. doi: 10.1093/cercor/2.3.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Reuter M, Kirsch P, Hennig J. Inferring candidate genes for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) assessed by the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) Journal Neural Transmission. 2006;113(7):929–938. doi: 10.1007/s00702-005-0366-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Riaz N, Steinberg S, Ahmad J, Pluzhnikov A, Riazuddin S, Cox NJ, Drayna D. Genomewide Significant Linkage to Stuttering on Chromosome 12. American Society of Human Genetics. 2005;76(4):647–651. doi: 10.1086/429226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Rice ML. Children with specific language impairment: Bridging the genetic and developmental perspectives. In: Hoff E, Shatz M, editors. Handbook of Language Development. Blackwell; 2007. pp. 411–437. [Google Scholar]
  113. Rice ML, Hoffman L, Wexler K. Judgements of Omitted BE and DO in Questions as Extended Finiteness Clinical Markers of Specific Language Impairment (SLI) to 15 Years: A Study of Growth and Asymptote. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52:1417–1433. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0171). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Rice ML, Oetting JB. Morphological Deficits of Children with SLI – Evaluation of Number Marking and Agreement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1993;36:1249–1257. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3606.1249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Ritchie T, Noble EP. Association of seven polymorphisms of the D2 dopamine receptor gene with brain receptor-binding characteristics. Neurochemical Research. 2003;28(1):73–82. doi: 10.1023/a:1021648128758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Hoenicka J, Jimenez-Arriero MA, Ponce G, Bagney A, Aragues M, Palomo T. Performance in the wisconsin card sorting test and the C957T polymorphism of the DRD2 gene in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychobiology. 2006;54:166–170. doi: 10.1159/000098652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Rueda MR, Rothbart MK, McCandliss BD, Saccomanno L, Posner MI. Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2005;102(41):14931–14936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506897102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Rybakowski JK, Borkowska A, Czerski PM, Kapelski P, Dmitrzak-Weglars M, Hauser J. An assocation study of dopamine receptors polymorphisms and the wisconsin card sorting test in schizophrenia. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2005;112:1575–1582. doi: 10.1007/s00702-005-0292-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Saffran JR, Newport EL, Aslin RN. Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996;35:606–621. [Google Scholar]
  120. Savitz J, Solms M, Ramesar R. The molecualar genetics of cognition: Dopamine, COMT, and BDNF genes. Brain and Behavior. 2006;5:311–328. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00163.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275(5306):1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Seamans J, Yang CR. The principal features and mechanisms of dopamine modulations in the prefrontal cortex. Progress in Neurobiology. 2004;74:1–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Sesack SR, Hawrylak VA, Guido MA, Levey AI. Cellular and subcellular localization of the dopamine transporter in rat cortex. Advances in Pharmacology. 1998;42:171–174. doi: 10.1016/s1054-3589(08)60720-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Shohamy D, Adcock RA. Dopamine and adaptive memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14(10):464–472. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Shohamy D, Myers CE, Hopkins RO, Sage J, Gluck MA. Distinct Hippocampal and Basal Ganglia Contributions to Probabilistic Learning and Reversal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21(9):1820–1832. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Shriberg LD, Ballard KJ, Tomblin JB, Duffy JR, Odell KH, Williams CA. Speech, prosody, and voice characteristics of a mother and daughter with a 7;13 translocation affecting FOXP2. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:500–525. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/038). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Silva R, Clahsen H. Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2008;11:245–260. [Google Scholar]
  128. Slevc LR, Miyake A. Individual Differences in Second-Language Proficiency: Does Musical Ability Matter. Psychological Sciences. 2006;17(8):675–681. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01765.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Squire LR, Knowlton BJ. The medial temporal lobe, the hippocampus, and the memory systems of the brain. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Vol. 253. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2000. pp. 765–780. [Google Scholar]
  130. Stipanovich A, Valjent E, Matamales M, Nishi A, Ahn JH, Maroteaux M, Bertran-Gonzalez J, Brami-Cherrier K, Enslen H, Corbille AG, Filhol O, Nairn AC, Greengard P, Herve D, Girault JA. A phosphatase cascade by which rewarding stimuli control nucleosomal response. Nature. 2008;453(7197):879–884. doi: 10.1038/nature06994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Takahashi H, Matsui H, Camerer C, Takano H, Kodaka F, Ideno T, Okubo S, Takemura K, Arakawa R, Eguchi Y, Murai T, Okubo Y, Kato M, Ito H, Suhara T. Dopamine D receptors and nonlinear probability weighting in risky choice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(49):16567–16572. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3933-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Ullman MT. The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2001;4(1):105–122. [Google Scholar]
  133. Ullman MT. Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition. 2004;92(1-2):231–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Ullman MT, Pancheva R, Love T, Yee E, Swinney D, Hickok G. Neural correlates of lexicon and grammar: Evidence from the production, reading, and judgment of inflection in aphasia. Brain Lang. 2005;93(2):185–238. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.001. discussion 239-142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M. FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:131–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn1605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins K, Alcock K, Fletcher P, Passingham R. Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1995;92:930–933. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.3.930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Vernes SC, Phil D, Newbury DF, Phil D, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, Alarcon M, Oliver PL, Davies KE, Phil D, Geschwind DH, Monaco AP, Fisher SE, Phil D. A Functional Genetic Link between Distinct Developmental Language Disorders. The New Englad Journal of Medicine. 2008;359:2337–2345. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0802828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Vijayraghavan S, Wang M, Birnbaum SG, Williams GV, Arnsten AF. Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10(3):376–384. doi: 10.1038/nn1846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Warrier CM, Wong PCM, Penhune VB, Zatorre RJ, Parrish TB, Abrams DA, Kraus N. Relating Structure to Function: Heschl’s Gyrus and Acoustic Processing. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:61–69. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3489-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Watkins KE, Vargha-Khadem F, Ashburner J, Passingham RE, Connelly A, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Mishkin M, Gadian DG. MRI analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: structural brain abnormalities. Brain. 2002;125(3):465–478. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Wayment HK, Schenk JO, Sorg BA. Characterization of extracellular dopamine clearance in the medial prefrontal cortex: role of monoamine uptake and monoamine oxidase inhibition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(1):35–44. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-01-00035.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Weber-Fox CM, Neville HJ. Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1996;8(3):231–256. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Werker RJT. Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross-language speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1984;77(6):1866–1878. doi: 10.1121/1.390988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. White MJ, Lawford BR, Morris CP, Young RM. Interaction Between DRD2 C957T Polymorphism and An Acute Psychosocial Stressor on Reward-Related Behavioral Impulsivity. Behavior Genetics. 2009;39:285–295. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9255-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Whitehouse AJ, Bishop DV, Ang QW, Pennell CE, Fisher SE. CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2011;10(4):451–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00684.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Wiebe SA, Espy KA, Stopp C, Respass J, Stewart P, Jameson TR, Gilbert DG, Huggenvik JI. Gene-environment interactions across development: Exploring DRD2 genotype and prenatal smoking effects on self-regulation. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(1):31–44. doi: 10.1037/a0014550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Wilkosc M, Hauser J, Tomaszewska M, Dmitrzak-Weglars M, Sabinska M, Szczpankiewicz A, Borkowska A. Influence of dopaminergic and sertoninergic genes on working memory in healthy subjects. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis. 2010;70:86–94. doi: 10.55782/ane-2010-1777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS. Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature. 1995;376(6541):572–575. doi: 10.1038/376572a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Wong PCM, Chandrasekaran B, Zheng J. The Derived Allele of ASPM is Associated with Lexical Tone Perception. PLoS ONE. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034243. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Wong PCM, Perrachione TK, Parrish TB. Neural characteristics of successful and less successful speech and word learning in adults. Human Brain Mapping. 2007;28(10):995–1006. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Wong PCM, Warrier CM, Penhune VB, Roy AK, Sadehh A, Parrish TB, Zatorre RJ. Volume of left Heschl’s Gyrus and linguistic pitch learning. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:828–836. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Xue G, Dong Q, Jin Z, Zhang L, Wang Y. An fMRI study with semantic access in low proficiency second language learners. Neuroreport. 2004;15(5):791–796. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200404090-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Yacubian J, Sommer T, Schroeder K, Glascher J, Kalisch R, Leuenberger B, Braus DF, Buchel C. Gene-gene interaction associated with neural reward sensitivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2007;104(19):8125–8130. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702029104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Zahrt J, Taylor JR, Mathew RG, Arnsten AF. Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17(21):8528–8535. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08528.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES