Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Apr 3.
Published in final edited form as: J Atten Disord. 2010 Jul 8;15(7):593–603. doi: 10.1177/1087054710374576

Adverse Effects of Heavy Prenatal Maternal Smoking on Attentional Control in Children With ADHD

Maria G Motlagh 1, Denis G Sukhodolsky 1, Angeli Landeros-Weisenberger 1, Liliya Katsovich 1, Nancy Thompson 1, Lawrence Scahill 1, Robert A King 1, Bradley S Peterson 2,3, Robert T Schultz 4, James F Leckman 1
PMCID: PMC3974616  NIHMSID: NIHMS557269  PMID: 20616372

Abstract

Objective:

Exposure to heavy maternal cigarette smoking in pregnancy and severe maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy appear to be important risk factors for the development of ADHD. This study aimed to determine whether these perinatal risk factors were associated with neuropsychological deficits commonly seen in ADHD.

Method:

We examined the effect of these two risk factors on measures of attentional control, motor inhibition, visual-motor integration, and fine motor coordination in a group of 81 children with ADHD, aged from 8 to 18 years. The neuropsychological battery included the Connors’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT), the Stroop Color-Word Interference Test, the Beery Visual-Motor Integration Test, and the Purdue Pegboard Test.

Results:

Heavy maternal smoking during pregnancy was associated with slower reaction times (p < .002), and reaction time variability (p < .007) on the CPT.

Conclusions:

This study suggests a persistent negative effect of heavy prenatal maternal smoking on attentional control in children with ADHD. Future studies should examine the neurobiological basis and determine the degree to which inherited genetic susceptibility factors contribute to this finding.

Keywords: ADHD, smoking, pregnancy, attention, fine motor control

Introduction

ADHD is one of the most prevalent childhood disorders, occurring in 3% to 5% of school-age children (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, 2003). The core symptoms of inattention and impulsivity/hyperactivity can adversely affect a child’s academic and social emotional development (Pliszka & AACAP Work Group on Quality Issues, 2007). Because higher levels of motor activity and poorer performance on attention tests also characterize younger children, a developmental lag of cognitive functions has long been hypothesized to underlie ADHD (Barkley, 1998; Doehnert, Brandeis, Imhof, Drechsler, & Steinhausen, 2010; Kinsbourne, 1973). Deficits in attentional control, reflected in increased reaction times and an increased variability in reaction times measured during Continuous Performance Tests (CPT), are commonly observed in children with ADHD (Altink et al., 2009; Castellanos, Sonuga-Barke, Milham, & Tannock, 2006; Geurts, van der Oord, & Crone, 2006; Oosterlaan, Logan, & Sergeant, 1998; Schachar, Mota, Logan, Tannock, & Klim, 2000; Slaats-Willemse, Swaab-Barneveld, De, & Buitelaar, 2005; Soreni, Crosbie, Ickowicz, & Schachar, 2009; Stins et al., 2005; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). Individuals with ADHD are also frequently impaired on behavioral inhibition tasks as well as on measures of visuo, motor integration and fine motor control (Alderson, Rapport, & Kofler, 2007; Barkley et al., 1990; Flapper, Houwen, & Schoemaker, 2006; Hartsough & Lambert, 1985; Jakobson & Kikas, 2007; Lijfijt, Kenemans, Verbaten, & Van Engeland, 2005; Mourik et al., 2009; Oosterlaan et al., 1998; Piek, Pitcher, & Hay, 1999; Pitcher, Piek, & Hay, 2003).

The potential role of environmental factors in ADHD has been a major focus of research. A strong scientific literature documents that maternal smoking and increased maternal stress during pregnancy, as well as pregnancy and delivery complications (i.e., toxemia, eclampsia, poor maternal health, advanced maternal age, long duration of labor, fetal distress, antepartum hemorrhage, and low birth weight [LBW]), are risk factors for ADHD (Grizenko, Shayan, Polotskaia, Ter-Stepanian, & Joober, 2008; Huizink, de Medina, Mulder, Visser, & Buitelaar, 2002; Langley, Rice, van den Bree, & Thapar, 2005; McIntosh, Mulkins, & Dean, 1995; Mick, Biederman, Faraone, Sayer, & Kleinman, 2002a, Mick, Biederman, Prince, Fischer, & Faraone 2002b; Milberger, Biederman, Faraone, Chen, & Jones, 1998; Motlagh et al., 2010; Phares et al., 2004; Rodriguez & Bohlin, 2005; Ward, 1991).

For example, maternal smoking during pregnancy has been consistently associated with a two- to four-fold increased risk for ADHD in both case-control and cohort studies (Altink et al., 2009; Langley et al., 2005; Linnet et al., 2003). Prenatal exposure to smoking has also been shown to be associated with deficits in cognitive function and academic performance including selective and sustained attention, problem solving, and memory (Batstra, Hadders-Algra, & Neeleman, 2003; Batty, Der, & Deary, 2006; Breslau et al., 2007; Martin, Dombrowski, Mullis, Wisenbaker, & Huttunen, 2006; Milberger, Biederman, Faraone, Chen, & Jones, 1996; Trasti, Vik, Jacobsen, & Bakketeig, 1999), as well as problems with eye–hand co-ordination (Cornelius, Ryan, Day, Goldschmidt, & Willford, 2001; Julvez et al., 2007). However, few of these findings have been replicated in population-based studies, particularly after controlling for maternal education (Kafouri et al., 2009).

In the current study we tested for associations between measures of attentional control, motor inhibition, visual-motor integration, and fine motor coordination and prenatal exposure to heavy maternal smoking and severe maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy in a large group of children with well-characterized ADHD.

Method

Participants

Participants included 81 children and adolescents (64 boys, 17 girls, mean age = 11.7 years; SD = 2.6) who participated in one or more studies of childhood neuropsychiatric disorders conducted at the Yale Child Study Center (Bloch et al., 2006, 2009; Motlagh et al., 2010; Peterson, 2003; Schultz et al., 1998; Sukhodolsky, Landeros-Weisenberger, Scahill, Leckman, & Schultz, 2009). Participants with a primary diagnosis of ADHD were recruited either through the Yale outpatient clinic or through a local chapter of Children and Adults with Attention Deficit Disorder (CHADD). Children with a history of neurological illness, past seizures or history of head trauma with loss of consciousness, severe learning disabilities, pervasive developmental disorder, psychosis, or severe major depression were excluded. Socioeconomic status was estimated with the Hollingshead four-factor index (Hollingshead, 1975) and the majority of families were middle to upper-middle class. After complete description of the study to the participants, written informed consent was obtained from the parents and assent was obtained from children.

Procedures

The main sources of information for establishing a diagnosis of ADHD were in-person structured interviews, administered by an experienced master-level clinician with each child and parent and the participant’s medical record. Comorbid psychiatric diagnoses were established though comprehensive clinical evaluation and administration of the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Present and Lifetime Version (Kaufman et al., 1997). Following a review of all available information, two senior clinicians independently assigned Diagnostics and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) diagnoses using the best-estimate procedures (Leckman, Sholomskas, Thompson, Belanger, & Weissman, 1982). Children’s ADHD symptoms during the past 3 months were assessed with the parent-rated DuPaul ADHD scale (DuPaul, 1991; Thapar et al., 2003). Each item was rated on a 4-point ordinal scale (range: 0-3) and the scores for each item were summed for the total score on the DuPaul ADHD scale (range 0-53).

To evaluate prenatal exposure to smoking, all mothers were interviewed directly using the Modified Schedule for Risk and Protective Factors in Early Development (MSRPFED; Leckman et al., 1990; Santangelo et al., 1994; Vasconcelos et al., 2007; Walkup & Leckman, 1988). Information regarding pregnancy was collected blindly with regard to the diagnostic status and psychiatric symptom severity of the child. The mothers were asked if they smoked on average more than 10 cigarettes per day during their pregnancy. Based on their YES/NO response, we created a binary variable to test the association of heavy prenatal smoking and neuropsychological performance. The MSRPFED interview also included questions concerning the family’s life circumstances during the pregnancy. Detailed questions were asked concerning potential psychosocial stressors across a range of areas including the home environment, the marital relationship, the availability of emotional supports, parental employment and family’s financial status, the parents’ physical health, and any legal issues. After completing the MSRPFED interviews, two experienced raters (LK and NT) using five-point ordinal scales (range: 1-5) were asked to rate, independently and blind to the participant’s diagnoses, the severity of the life stressors using the Level of Stress Severity scale (LSS; Leckman et al., 1990). Given the distribution of this variable and the small number of participants with severe scores (LSS = 5), we created a binary variable (LSS scores < 4 vs. ≥ 4) to test the association of prenatal severe maternal stress and neuropsychological performance.

The child birth weight was obtained by maternal reporting at the time of interview. A birth weight of less than 2,500 grams was defined as LBW and this binary variable was incorporated into the initial models as a covariate.

Neuropsychological Battery

All participants also completed a battery of neuropsychological measures. Research assistants administering these measures were blind to the diagnostic status of the participants. During these evaluation procedures, medicated children were on their usual dose and type of medication, with the exception of psychostimulants. In the case of psychostimulants, parents were asked to refrain from giving their child their daily dose(s) of psychostimulants on the day of neuropsychological testing. The administration of neuropsychological tests was counterbalanced to rule out possible effects of fatigue on the tests.

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991)

The Vocabulary and Block Design subtests of the WISC-III were used to estimate the child’s verbal and non-verbal intelligence.

The Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT, version 3.0; Conners, 1995)

It is a measure of sustained attention and inhibitory control, which requires the participant to respond to the target letters by a button press and to withhold the response to the nontarget letter “X.” There is a total of 360 trials of which 36 (10%) are the nontargets. Thus, the motor system is primed to respond, and the participant must inhibit that prepotent response in the nontarget trials. The 360 trials are presented in 6 blocks, with three 20-trial sub-blocks which have different interstimulus intervals (1, 2, or 4 seconds). Each letter is displayed for 250 milliseconds. Average administration time is 14 minutes. Percent of omission and commission errors, the mean and SD reaction time (RT) for hits, and between blocks RT variability were used as dependent variables (Conners, Epstein, Angold, & Klaric, 2003).

The Stroop Color-Word Interference Test (Stroop, 1935)

It is a widely used test of cognitive inhibition. It consists of three conditions: (a) color naming, (b) word reading, and (c) color-word interference. In the color naming condition participants were asked to name, as quickly as possible, the color (red, green, or blue) of 126 dots, arrayed randomly in nine columns and 14 rows on a sheet of white paper (8.5×11 inches) and scanned left to right and then top to bottom. In the word reading condition participants were asked to read, as quickly as possible, an equal number of similarly arrayed words (“red,” “green,” or “blue”) printed in black ink. In the color-word reading condition, participants were asked to name a similar array of words written in incongruent colors as quickly as possible. The time to completion of each task was recorded in seconds. Stroop interference score was calculated as C − [(A × B)/(A + C)] (Golden, 1978).

Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration (VMI, 4th ed., Beery, 1997)

It is a paper-and-pencil test that consists of 24 geometric designs, arranged in order of increasing difficulty, that the child copies. It assesses visual-graphic ability or the degree to which visual perception and finger–hand movements are coordinated. The Beery standard score was used as the dependent variable.

Purdue Pegboard Task (PPT; Tiffen, 1968)

It is a measure of fine motor control and designed to measure finger and hand dexterity. This instrument includes a 12×18-inch board, 50 one-inch pins, 20 collars, and 40 washers that are located in four cups at the top of the board (Lafayette Instrument Company, 1985). The participant must place pins in one of the two columns, each of which have 25 slots. The number of correctly placed pins in each of the test’s 30 second conditions (dominant hand, nondominant hand, and bimanual) were used as dependent variables. Reported reliabilities range between 0.60 to 0.76 for single trial scores and 0.82 to 0.91 for three trial scores (Tiffin, 1968).

Data Analysis

Separate MANCOVAs were used to examine the association of each of the two risk factors evaluated in this study with the Conners’ CPT and the PPT. ANCOVAs were used for the Stroop and the VMI. Pearson’s correlations were used to examine the associations among normally distributed continuous variables, and Chi-square statistics were used to test the differences between categorical variables.

The initial models were adjusted for the child’s age (years), gender (male or female), their IQ estimate, LBW (birth weight less than 2,500 grams), maternal education, and the family’s socioeconomic status. All covariates were run in the initial models. If they significantly contributed to the variance in the initial model they were retained in a final General Linear Models (GLMs).

Results

Maternal Smoking During Pregnancy

During pregnancy 12 (14.8 %) of the mothers reported that they smoked on average more than 10 cigarettes per day during their pregnancy. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the ADHD patients with or without prenatal exposure to maternal smoking are presented in Table 1. Patients who had been exposed to heavy prenatal maternal smoking did not differ from those who had not been regarding their age, gender, mother or father’s level of education, or the frequency of LBW or comorbid conditions with other neuropsychiatric disorders such as Tourette syndrome. However, at trend level, a significant difference was observed for socioeconomic status. The children exposed to maternal heavy smoking during pregnancy, at a trend level, also had higher Inattentive, Hyperactivity, and Total scores on the parent-rated DuPaul scale (Table 1).

Table 1.

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics in Children With ADHD by Heavy Maternal Smoking Exposure During Pregnancy

Exposed to Prenatal Maternal
Smoking (N = 12)
Not Exposed to
Prenatal Smoking (N = 69)
p
Mean age (SD) 12.08 (2.6) 11.71 (2.6) .63
Gender (% male) 8 (66.6) 56 (81.15) .25
Race (% White) 12 (100) 62 (89.8) .24
Socioeconomic statusa (SD) 40.68 (12.20) 48.57 (11.94) .06
Maternal level of education (SD) 4.73 (1.27) 5.45 (1.33) .10
Paternal level of education (SD) 5.00 (1.09) 5.56 (1.09) .17
LBWb (N, %) 1 (8.3) 3 (4.3) .55
IQ estimate (SD) 112.80 (19.82) 109.07 (16.57) .74
ADHD (N, %)
 Combined type 9 (75) 49 (71) .77
 Hyperactive-impulsive type 0 (0) 3 (4.3) .46
 Inattentive type 3 (25) 17 (24.6) .97
Total ADHD severity scorec (SD) 32.30 (8.5) 27.83 (10.78) .07
 Inattentive score 18.40 (5.4) 15.79 (6.3) .08
 Hyperactivity score 13.90 (4.7) 12.05 (6.4) .07
Medication statusd (N, %)
 Psychostimulants 6 (50) 26 (37.6) .42
 Other psychotropic medications 4 (33.3) 28 (40.5) .63
Comorbid diagnosese (N, %)
 TS 4 (33.3) 34 (49.2) .30
 OCD 4 (33.3) 14 (20.2) .31
 Oppositional defiant 5 (41.6) 19 (27.5) .32
 Conduct disorder 0 (0) 2 (2.8) .55
 Depression 3 (25) 23 (33.3) .56
 Anxiety disorder (except OCD) 2 (16.6) 10 (14.4) .84

TS = Tourette syndrome; OCD = obsessive compulsive disorder; LBW = low birth weight.

a

Measured with the Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975).

b

LBW = birth weight < 2,500 grams.

c

Based on the Du Paul’s parent ratings of ADHD symptoms. The presented scores are the current ADHD symptom severity at the time of the neuropsychological evaluation.

d

Psychostimulants (N = 32), alpha2 agonists (N = 13), selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (N = 9), antipsychotics (N = 7), mood stabilizers (N = 2), and tricyclic antidepressants (N = 1).

e

Lifetime diagnosis based on structured interview and available clinical data (Leckman, Sholomskas, Thompson, Belanger, & Weissman, 1982).

In the initial models, the effect of heavy maternal smoking during pregnancy reached significance for the three outcome measures of Connors’ CPT, including errors of omission (F = 4.94, p = .028), reaction time (F = 7.45, p = .007), and reaction time variability (F = 10.22, p = .002). ADHD children exposed to heavy prenatal smoking performed worse compared to children who were not exposed (Table 2). These analyses controlled for the child’s age, gender, IQ, LBW, level of maternal education, and socioeconomic status. In the initial model only the child’s chronological age was significant. In the final model the child’s chronological age remained a significant covariate for errors of omission (F = 13.58, p < .001), reaction time (F = 14.7, p < .001), and reaction time variability (F = 3.87, p = .006).

Table 2.

Neuropsychological Performance of Children With ADHD With and Without Heavy Maternal Smoking During Pregnancy

Exposed to Prenatal
Smoking M (SD)
Not Exposed to Prenatal
Smoking M (SD)
F p
Connors CPT a
 Errors of omission (%) 7.9 (8.07) 6.60 (6.23) 4.94 .028
 Errors of commission (%) 54.39 (20.63) 57.85 (20.55) 0.29 .59
 Reaction time (ms) 509.42 (135.46) 431.81 (103.26) 7.45 .007
 RT variability (SE) 18.93 (10.31) 17.35 (12.51) 10.22 .002
Stroop b
 Golden Interference Score 185.52 (86.35) 186.43 (78.33) 0.58 .94
VMI c
 Beery standard score 84.75 (10.07) 92.40 (15.47) 1.63 .26
Purdue d
 Dominant raw score 13.04 (2.07) 13.33 (1.85) 0.61 .43
 Nondominant raw score 12.37 (2.52) 12.25 (1.92) 1.31 .25
 Bimanual raw score 9.37 (1.59) 10.38 (1.68) 4.99 .03

Heavy maternal smoking =3 10 cigarettes in 24 hours at any point in the pregnancy.

CPT = Continuous Performance Test; RT = reaction time; SE = standard error; VMI = Beery Visual Motor Integration Test.

a

Child age was the significant covariate errors of omission (F = 13.58, p < .001); reaction time (F = 14.7, p < .001) and reaction time variability (F = 3.87, p = .006).

b

Child age was the significant covariate (F = 27.94, p < .001).

c

Child age was the significant covariate (F = 22.27, p < .001).

d

Child age was the significant covariate for dominant raw score (F = 7.83, p = .008) and nondominant raw score (F = 13.20, p = .001), Child IQ was the significant covariate for nondominant raw score (F = 4.80, p = .034).

In the model of heavy maternal smoking and fine motor ability, the effect of smoking was not significant either for performance with the dominant (F = 0.61, p = .43) or the nondominant hand (F = 1.31, p = .25) raw scores on the PPT. However, the child’s exposure to heavy prenatal smoking during pregnancy was significantly associated with lower scores on the bimanual performance of Purdue (F = 4.99, p = .028). These analyses controlled for the child’s age, gender, IQ, LBW, level of maternal education, and socioeconomic status. The covariate effect of LBW, level of maternal education, and socioeconomic status were not significant and these covariates were excluded in the final GLM.

Inhibition control measured by the Stroop and visuomotor integration measured by the VMI were not affected by prenatal maternal smoking. Child’s age remained a significant covariate on the Stroop (F = 42.93, p < .001) and IQ remained a significant covariate for VMI (F = 18.55, p ≤ .001).

Severe Maternal Stress During Pregnancy

Severe maternal psychosocial stress during pregnancy was judged to be present in 19 (23.5%) of the cases. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the ADHD patients with and without prenatal exposure to severe psychosocial stress during pregnancy are presented in Supplemental Table 1.

Multivariate ANCOVAs revealed nonsignificant effects of severe maternal stress on the attentional control measured by the CPT and fine motor ability measured by the PPT (Supplemental Table 2). Likewise, no significant effects of severe maternal stress were seen in the separate ANOVA analysis of the Stroop and VMI tasks (Supplemental Table 2).

Discussion

This study examined the effect of relatively high exposure to maternal smoking during pregnancy (≥10 cigarettes per day) on neuropsychiatric functioning of children with ADHD while controlling for potential covariates. The effect of heavy maternal smoking on attentional control was evident in measures of both reaction time (p = .007) and reaction time variability (p = .002) on the CPT.

Nicotine exposure during pregnancy is a risk factor for ADHD-related disorders among children, and literature supporting an association between exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and subsequent symptoms of ADHD is consistent and robust (Cornelius & Day, 2009; Herrmann, King, & Weitzman, 2008). The potential neurotoxic effects of tobacco have a strong foundation in both animal and human studies, and reasonable evidence supports potential biologic pathways mediating the observed effects. Systematic meta-analyses have shown a dose-relationship with the number of cigarettes smoked during pregnancy, and several biologically plausible mechanisms could account for this association (Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, 2007; Langley et al., 2005; LeSage, Gustaf, Dufek, & Pentel, 2006; Winzer-Serhan, 2008).

Although we included a range of variables that are known to be linked with smoking (age, gender, IQ, birth weight, maternal education, and SES), fetal exposure to prenatal risk factors, especially those linked to offspring mental health, is not random (Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2008). In our subjects there was no association between maternal smoking and Hollingshead scores, nor between Hollingshead scores and ADHD severity which suggests that socioeconomic status or maternal education are not likely to be risk factors for increased symptom severity in our subjects.

An alternative explanation for the observed association of maternal smoking and attentional control in children with ADHD is that it is a proxy for ADHD and/or smoking risk genes (Altink et al., 2009; Thapar et al., 2009). For example, Thapar et al. (2009) using a novel naturalistic design, found that the magnitude of association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD was significantly greater in genetically related mother–offspring pairs than in the unrelated pairs. This finding suggests that this well-established link between maternal smoking in pregnancy and offspring ADHD symptoms might represent an inherited rather than a true environmental risk effect. Similarly, Altink et al. (2009) found a relationship between paternal smoking and the number of genetic risk factors in fathers including risk alleles of the dopamine D4 receptor and the dopamine transporter genes.

The retrospective nature of this study, the relatively small number of subjects, and the absence of information about paternal smoking are fundamental limitations of this study. Our findings are solely based on the recollections of mothers. Previous studies have suggested that there is a high degree of concordance between the data recorded in hospital records and the memories of mothers regarding prenatal events (Buka, Goldstein, Spartos, & Tsuang, 2004; Leckman et al., 1990; Rice et al., 2007; Tomeo et al., 1999). Studies in adults also show close agreement between self-reported cigarette smoking and its biochemical marker, plasma cotinine (Patrick et al., 1994). In addition, even distant (≥30-year) recall of cigarette consumption during pregnancy demonstrates a high level of agreement with archived data on smoking collected during pregnancy (Batty et al, 2006; Tomeo et al., 1999). Fewer data are available concerning the degree of agreement for more qualitative measures such as the level of maternal stress or coping ability. Some degree of recall bias is likely to be present. This may be particularly true for mothers who have a child with a disorder. In addition, compared to many recent ADHD studies, the sample size of the current study is small so that we had limited power to detect even large differences between two groups.

Other important limitations of this study include the use of a binary risk variable to assess maternal smoking and the failure to assess the presence of ADHD in the mothers. Future studies are needed to determine if there is a linear relationship between the number of cigarettes smoked during the pregnancy and the severity of the attentional problems. Likewise, information on parental ADHD would have allowed us to explore, in a preliminary fashion, the contribution of family-genetic risk to these findings.

Another limitation is the absence of a control population of typically developing children. Although we have comparable perinatal and neuropsychological data on a matched group of 65 typically developing children, the rate of maternal smoking during pregnancy was too low to provide sufficient power to perform these analyses (Motlagh et al., 2010). Nevertheless, if replicated, the specificity of the associations seen in this study indicates that maternal smoking may be linked specifically to the neural mechanisms underlying attentional control and not other neuropsychological deficits.

Identification of risk factors that affect the cognitive deficits seen in ADHD can improve our understanding of the etiology of ADHD. A possible shared genetic predisposition between neuropsychological components of ADHD and smoking provides more information about the underlying neural mechanisms. For example, the involvement of dopaminergic and cholinergic systems and gene–gene interactions may lead to novel therapeutic approaches for ADHD. The rate of heavy maternal smoking during pregnancy in this sample of ADHD cases was 14.8% which is higher than that typically seen in population-based samples. In most samples the rate of heavy maternal smoking in the general population is under 5% (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC], 2004). For example in a recent German study, an overall rate of any maternal smoking was 11.8% compared with a rate of 4.6% for women who were heavy smokers (>10 cigarettes/day; Meyer et al., 2009).

Public health efforts are needed to reduce the frequency of maternal smoking during pregnancy, specifically in high-risk populations (CDC, 2004; Phares et al., 2004; Pomerleau, Pomerleau, Snedecor, Gaulrapp, & Kardia, 2004; Wilens & Dodson, 2004). Thus far, prevention programs that address this issue have been shown to have beneficial results (Lumley et al., 2009).

Supplementary Material

01

Acknowledgment

The authors wish to thank Virginia Eicher for her invaluable assistance in completing this study.

Financial Disclosure/Funding

This research was funded in part by the Echlin Foundation, and the Kaiser Family. This research was also supported by NIH Grants MH066187, P01MH049351 (JFL), K05 MH076273 (JFL), K02 MH074677-01 (BSP), R01 MH59139 (BSP) and RR00125.

Biographies

Maria G. Motlagh, MD, is a child psychiatrist working as a visiting research scientist at Yale Child Study Center. Her focus of interest is on childhood onset neurodevelopmental disorders like Tourette syndrome (TS), Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD), and ADHD. She has been working on the results of a longitudinal study (completed over the course of 10 years—1995-2005) regarding the risk and protective factors in the prenatal and perinatal periods on the development of TS, OCD, and ADHD as well as understanding the role of environmental factors on the clinical manifestation of these disorders.

Denis G. Sukhodolsky, PhD, is an associate research scientist at the Yale Child Study Center. His research concerns efficacy and mechanisms of behavioral treatments for children with neuropsychiatric disorders, including TS, autism, and disruptive behavior disorders. His last work on the neuropsychiatric assessment of children with TS and/or ADHD has been submitted for Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

Angeli Landeros-Weisenberger, MD, is an associate postdoctoral fellow at Yale Child Study Center involved in many clinical studies on children with OCD, TS, or ADHD.

Liliya Katsovich, MBA, is a biostatistician involved in several studies on neurodevelopmental disorders since 11 years ago at Yale Child Study Center.

Nancy Thompson, BA, is an expert research associate who has been working more than ten years evaluation children with neuropsychiatric disorders at Yale Child Study Center.

Lawrence Scahill, MSN, PhD, is the Professor of Nursing and Child Psychiatry at Yale. For the past 9 years, he has been involved in psychopharmacological and behavioral interventions for children and adults with TS. He serves on the Medical Advisory Board of the TSsyndrome Association and is a principal investigator on two multisite studies evaluating the efficacy of a behavioral intervention for tics in children and adults with TS.

Robert A. King, MD, is a professor of Child Psychiatry and Medical Director of Tourette’s/OCD Clinic, Yale Child Study Center. His research interest is more on diagnosis, pathophysiology, and treatment of tic disorders, OCD, and related disorders.

Bradley S. Peterson, MD, directs Child and Adolescent Psychiatry as well as MRI Research including serious childhood-onset neuropsychiatric disorders, such as TS, OCD, and ADHD.

Robert T. Schultz, PhD, is the Harris associate professor of Psychology and Child Psychiatry and the director of the Yale Developmental Imaging Program. His research focuses on the biological bases of autism spectrum disorders and the genetic forms of mental retardation, as well as a variety of other childhood psychiatric disorders.

James F. Leckman, MD, is the Neison Harris professor of Child Psychiatry, Psychiatry, Psychology and Pediatrics at Yale. He serves as the Director of Research for the Yale Child Study Center. He is a well known child psychiatrist and patient-oriented clinical investigator. His peers have regularly selected him as one of the Best Doctors in America. He is the author or co-author of over 300 original articles published in peer-reviewed journals, seven books, and 120 book chapters. In 2002, he was identified by American Society for Information, Science and Technology as a “Highly Cited Researcher”—one of the world’s most cited authors in Psychology and Psychiatry—in the top half of the top one percent of all publishing researchers.

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. Alderson RM, Rapport MD, Kofler MJ. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioral inhibition: A meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35:745–758. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9131-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Altink ME, Slaats-Willemse DI, Rommelse NN, Buschgens CJ, Fliers EA, Arias-Vásquez A, Buitelaar JK. Effects of maternal and paternal smoking on attentional control in children with and without ADHD. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;8:465–475. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0001-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th Author; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barkley RA. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Scientific American. 1998;279:66–71. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0998-66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkley RA, DuPaul GJ, McMurray MB. Comprehensive evaluation of attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity as defined by research criteria. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1990;58:775–789. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.58.6.775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Edelbrock CS, Smallish L. The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1990;29:546–557. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199007000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Batstra L, Hadders-Algra M, Neeleman J. Effect of antenatal exposure to maternal smoking on behavioural problems and academic achievement in childhood: Prospective evidence from a Dutch birth cohort. Early Human Development. 2003;75:21–23. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Batty GD, Der G, Deary IJ. Effect of maternal smoking during pregnancy on offspring’s cognitive ability: Empirical evidence for complete confounding in the US national longitudinal survey of youth. Pediatrics. 2006;118:943–950. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beery KE. The Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration. 4th Modern Curriculum Press; New Jersey: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bloch MH, Craiglow BG, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Dombrowski PA, Panza KE, Peterson BS, Leckman JF. Predictors of early adult outcome in pediatric-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1085–1093. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bloch MH, Peterson BS, Scahill L, Otka J, Katsovich L, Zhang H, Leckman JF. Clinical predictors of future tic and OCD severity in children with Tourette syndrome. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2006;160:65–69. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.1.65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Breslau N. Commentary: Maternal smoking during pregnancy: Hazard for what? International Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;36:832–833. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Buka SL, Goldstein JM, Spartos E, Tsuang MT. The retrospective measurement of prenatal and perinatal events: Accuracy of maternal recall. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;71:417–426. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Smoking during pregnancy–United States, 1990-2002. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2004;53:911–915. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Conners CK. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test. Multi-Health Systems; North Tonawanda, NY: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  17. Conners CK, Epstein JN, Angold A, Klaric J. Continuous performance test performance in a normative epidemiological sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2003;31:555–562. doi: 10.1023/a:1025457300409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cornelius MD, Day NL. Developmental consequences of prenatal tobacco exposure. Current Opinion in Neurology. 2009;22:121–125. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328326f6dc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cornelius MD, Ryan CM, Day NL, Goldschmidt L, Willford JA. Prenatal tobacco effects on neuropsychological outcomes among preadolescents. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2001;22:217–225. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200108000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Costello EJ, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:837–844. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Doehnert M, Brandeis D, Imhof K, Drechsler R, Steinhausen HC. Mapping attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder from childhood to adolescence—no neurophysiologic evidence for a developmental lag of attention but some for inhibition. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:608–616. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. DuPaul GJ. Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community-based sample. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 1991;20:245–253. [Google Scholar]
  23. Flapper BC, Houwen S, Schoemaker MM. Fine motor skills and effects of methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2006;48:165–169. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206000375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gatzke-Kopp LM, Beauchaine TP. Direct and passive prenatal nicotine exposure and the development of externalizing psychopathology. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2007;38:255–269. doi: 10.1007/s10578-007-0059-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Geurts HM, van der Oord S, Crone EA. Hot and cool aspects of cognitive control in children with ADHD: Decisionmaking and inhibition. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:813–824. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9059-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Golden CJ. Stroop Color and Word Test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Stoelting; Wood Dale, ILL: 1978. [Google Scholar]
  27. Grizenko N, Shayan YR, Polotskaia A, Ter-Stepanian M, Joober R. Relation of maternal stress during pregnancy to symptom severity and response to treatment in children with ADHD. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience. 2008;33:10–16. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hartsough CS, Lambert NM. Medical factors in hyperactive and normal children: Prenatal, development, and health history findings. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1985;55:190–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1985.tb03433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Herrmann M, King K, Weitzman M. Prenatal tobacco smoke and postnatal secondhand smoke exposure and child neurodevelopment. Current Opinion in Pediatrics. 2008;20:184–190. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0b013e3282f56165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. Yale University; New Haven, CT: 1975. [Google Scholar]
  31. Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK. Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:1078–1085. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200209000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jakobson A, Kikas E. Cognitive functioning in children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with and without comorbid learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2007;40:194–202. doi: 10.1177/00222194070400030101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Julvez J, Ribas-Fito N, Torrent M, Forns M, Garcia-Esteban R, Sunyer J. Maternal smoking habits and cognitive development of children at age 4 years in a population-based birth cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;36:825–832. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kafouri S, Leonard G, Perron M, Richer L, Séguin JR, Veillette S, Paus T. Maternal cigarette smoking during pregnancy and cognitive performance in adolescence. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;38:158–172. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyn250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Ryan N. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kinsbourne M. Minimal brain dysfunction as a neurodevelopmental disorder symptoms in offspring. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1973;160:1985–1989. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lafayette Instrument Company . Instructions and normative data for Model 32020, Purdue Pegboard. LIC; Lafayette, IN: 1985. [Google Scholar]
  38. Langley K, Rice F, van den Bree MB, Thapar A. Maternal smoking during pregnancy as an environmental risk factor for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder behaviour. A review. Minerva Pediatrica. 2005;57:359–371. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Leckman JF, Sholomskas D, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Weissman MM. Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: A methodological study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1982;39:879–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290080001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leckman JF, Dolnansky ES, Hardin MT, Clubb M, Walkup JT, Stevenson J, Pauls DL. Perinatal factors in the expression of Tourette’s syndrome: An exploratory study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1990;29:220–226. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199003000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. LeSage MG, Gustaf E, Dufek MB, Pentel PR. Effects of maternal intravenous nicotine administration on locomotor behaviour in preweanling rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2006;85:575–583. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lijfijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, Van Engeland H. A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Deficient inhibitory motor control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:216–222. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Linnet KM, Dalsgaard S, Obel C, Wisborg K, Henriksen TB, Rodriguez A, Jarvelin MR. Maternal lifestyle factors in pregnancy risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated behaviors: Review of the current evidence. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1028–1040. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lumley J, Chamberlain C, Dowswell T, Oliver S, Oakley L, Watson L. Interventions for promoting smoking cessation during pregnancy. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009;8:CD001055. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001055.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Martin RP, Dombrowski SC, Mullis C, Wisenbaker J, Huttunen MO. Smoking during pregnancy: Association with childhood temperament, behavior, and academic performance. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2006;31:490–500. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McIntosh DE, Mulkins RS, Dean RS. Utilization of maternal perinatal risk indicators in the differential diagnosis of ADHD and UADD children. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;81:35–46. doi: 10.3109/00207459509015297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Meyer S, Raisig A, Gortner L, Ong MF, Bücheler M, Tutdibi E. In utero tobacco exposure: The effects of heavy and very heavy smoking on the rate of SGA infants in the Federal State of Saarland, Germany. European Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology and Reproductive Biology. 2009;146:37–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ejogrb.2009.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mick E, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Sayer J, Kleinman S. Case-control study of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and maternal smoking, alcohol use, and drug use during pregnancy. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002a;41:378–385. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200204000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mick E, Biederman J, Prince J, Fischer MJ, Faraone SV. Impact of low birth weight on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2002b;23:16–22. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200202000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Milberger S, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Chen L, Jones J. Is maternal smoking during pregnancy a risk factor for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children? American Journal of Psychiatry. 1996;153:1138–1142. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.9.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Milberger S, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Chen L, Jones J. Further evidence of an association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Findings from a high-risk sample of siblings. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 1998;27:352–358. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2703_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Motlagh MG, Katsovich L, Thompson N, Lin H, Kim YS, Scahill L, Leckman JF. Severe psychosocial stress and heavy cigarette smoking during pregnancy: an examination of the pre- and perinatal risk factors associated with ADHD and Tourette syndrome. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010 doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0115-7. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mourik R, Papanikolau A, van Gellicum-Bijlhout J, van Oostenbruggen J, Veugelers D, Post-Uiterweer A, Oosterlaan J. Interference control in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37:293–303. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9277-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Oosterlaan J, Logan GD, Sergeant JA. Respons inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD + CD, anxious, and control children: A meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:411–425. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Patrick DL, Cheadle A, Thompson DC, Diehr P, Koepsell T, Kinne S. The validity of self-reported smoking: A review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 1994;84:1086–1093. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.7.1086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Peterson BS. Brain imaging studies of the anatomical and functional consequences of preterm birth for human brain development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003;1008:219–237. doi: 10.1196/annals.1301.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Phares TM, Morrow B, Lansky A, Barfield WD, Prince CB, Marchi KS, Kinniburgh B. Surveillance for disparities in maternal health-related behaviors–selected states, Pregnancy Risk Assessment Monitoring System (PRAMS), 2000-2001. MMWR. Surveillance Summaries. 2004;53:1–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Piek JP, Pitcher TM, Hay DA. Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in males with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 1999;41:159–165. doi: 10.1017/s0012162299000341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pitcher TM, Piek JP, Hay DA. Fine and gross motor ability in males with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2003;45:525–535. doi: 10.1017/s0012162203000975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pliszka SR, AACAP Work Group on Quality Issues Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46:894–921. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318054e724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P. Behavioural genetics. 5th Worth; New York: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  62. Pomerleau CS, Pomerleau OF, Snedecor SM, Gaulrapp S, Kardia SL. Heterogeneity in phenotypes based on smoking status in the Great Lakes Smoker Sibling Registry. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:1851–1855. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rice F, Lewis A, Harold G, van den Bree M, Boivin J, Hay DF, Thapar A. Agreement between maternal report and antenatal records for a range of pre and peri-natal factors: The influence of maternal and child characteristics. Early Human Development. 2007;83:497–504. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rodriguez A, Bohlin G. Are maternal smoking and stress during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:246–254. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00359.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Santangelo SL, Pauls DL, Goldstein JM, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT, Leckman JF. Tourette’s syndrome: What are the influences of gender and comorbid obsessive-compulsive disorder? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1994;33:795–804. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199407000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Schachar R, Mota VL, Logan GD, Tannock R, Klim P. Confirmation of an inhibitory control deficit in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2000;28:227–235. doi: 10.1023/a:1005140103162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Schultz RT, Carter AS, Gladstone M, Scahill L, Leckman JF, Peterson BS, Pauls D. Visual-motor integration functioning in children with Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:134–145. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.1.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Slaats-Willemse D, Swaab-Barneveld H, De SL, Buitelaar J. Familial clustering of executive functioning in affected sibling pair families with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44:385–391. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000153227.34473.c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Soreni N, Crosbie J, Ickowicz A, Schachar R. Stop signal and Conners’ continuous performance tasks: Test-retest reliability of two inhibition measures in ADHD children. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2009;13:137–143. doi: 10.1177/1087054708326110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Stins JF, Tollenaar MS, Slaats-Willemse DI, Buitelaar JK, Swaab-Barneveld H, Verhulst FC, Boomsma DI. Sustained attention and executive functioning performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology. 2005;11:285–294. doi: 10.1080/09297040490916938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  72. Sukhodolsky DG, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Scahill L, Leckman JF, Schultz RA. Neuropsychological functioning in children with Tourette syndrome with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. 2009. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  73. Thapar A, Fowler T, Rice F, Scourfield J, van den Bree M, Thomas H, Hay D. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and attention deficit hyperactivity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1985–1989. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Thapar A, Rice F, Hay D, Boivin J, Langley K, van den Bree M, Harold G. Prenatal smoking might not cause attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from a novel design. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:722–727. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tiffen J. Purdue Pegboard Test. Scientific Research Associates; Chicago, IL: 1968. [Google Scholar]
  76. Tomeo CA, Rich-Edwards JW, Michels KB, Berkey CS, Hunter DJ, Frazier AL, Buka SL. Reproducibility and validity of maternal recall of pregnancy-related events. Epidemiology. 1999;10:774–777. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Trasti N, Vik T, Jacobsen G, Bakketeig LS. Smoking in pregnancy and children’s mental and motor development at age 1 and 5 years. Early Human Development. 1999;55:137–147. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(99)00017-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. van Mourik R, Papanikolau A, van Gellicum-Bijlhout J, van Oostenbruggen J, Veugelers D, Post-Uiterweer A , , , , , , …. [DOI] [PubMed]
  79. Vasconcelos MS, Sampaio AS, Hounie AG, Akkerman F, Curi M, Lopes AC, Miguel EC. Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal risk factors in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Walkup JT, Leckman JF. Modified schedule for risk and protective factors early in development. Yale University; New Haven, CT: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  81. Ward AJ. Prenatal stress and childhood psychopathology. Child Psychiatry Human Development. 1991;22:97–110. doi: 10.1007/BF00707788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd San Antonio, TX; Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  83. Wilens TE, Dodson W. A clinical perspective of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adulthood. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004;65:1301–1313. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v65n1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Winzer-Serhan UH. Long-term consequences of maternal smoking and developmental chronic nicotine exposure. Frontiers in Bioscience. 2008;13:636–649. doi: 10.2741/2708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES