Abstract
The human connectome will provide a detailed mapping of the brain’s connectivity, with fundamental insights for health and disease. However, further understanding of brain function and dysfunction will require an integrated framework that links brain connectivity with brain dynamics, as well as the biological details that relates this connectivity more directly to function. In this Perspective, we describe such a framework for studying the brain’s “dynome” and its relationship to cognition.
It may seem ill-timed to be discussing post-connectome science just when connectomics has become a major initiative within neuroscience. Understanding the connectome presents many technical and theoretical challenges, which will deliver novel insights into brain function and dysfunction. However, it is already clear what some of the limitations of connectomics will be. Furthermore, the connectome can be - and indeed needs to be - studied at a very wide range of spatial scales, making any endpoint seem very far in the future. We will argue here that the neuroscience community needs to be thinking now about how to extend the insights that will emerge from the kinds of work highlighted in this special issue to incorporate additional features of brain dynamics and physiology; this is needed to address function and dysfunction of cognition. A shape for such a research program is already emerging in the study of fast time scale (~millisecond) brain processes, which is especially important when considering rapid changes in brain activity (e.g., during cognition) and to supplement the static observations and slower times scales available by other measurements (e.g., fMRI).
In general, connectomics refers to a comprehensive structural description of the human brain, rendered as a network (Sporns, 2013). These networks consist of two fundamental components: nodes and edges. A node is usually identified with a region of the brain, often taken in principled ways from knowledge of brain anatomy and function, and can vary in size and specificity, from the scale of a microscopic single neuron to a macroscopic brain region; in general, how to best define a node remains an active research topic (Stanley et al., 2013). An edge represents a connection between two nodes. In a structural network, an edge represents an anatomical connection between two brain elements. In a functional network, an edge represents the statistical association between activities recorded from separate brain elements (Park & Friston, 2013). The description of the anatomical connections (in some versions) is often called the “connectome”, while the dynamic networks associated with brain activity during a particular brain state (such as attention or rest; Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Bullmore & Sporns, 2009; Bressler & Menon, 2010) represents a “functional connectome”.
In this essay, we emphasize that connections, even functional connections, do not provide information critical to understanding how the brain produces cognition. What is needed is not only what is connected, but how and in what directions regions of the brain are connected: what signals they convey and how those signals are acted upon as part of a neural computational process. As we describe below, the “how” is important for understanding the ways in which various parts of the brain combine their particular computations to support cognitive function. Indeed, we argue that how the brain generates temporal structure is critical to the ways in which signals are routed, combined and coordinated. We note that this viewpoint overlaps with the philosophy of (Bargmann & Marder, 2013), although we focus here more directly on observations from the vertebrate brain and rhythms.
Brain dynamics are hugely complicated, in ways that we are just beginning to chart. Measurements from EEG, MEG, ECoG, local field potentials, and single-unit recordings (both intra- and extra-cellular) are documenting complex temporal structures that are far from random and are both reproducible and specific to classes of cognitive activities (Engel et al., 1997; Wang, 2010). Some of this structure is usually called “brain rhythms”, which typically are broadly distributed across a frequency band. In the analysis of brain electric and magnetic field activity, standard peak frequencies range from somewhat below 1 Hz to well over 100 Hz (Buzsaki & Draguhn, 2004). Many sophisticated tools exist to characterize rhythms; however care must be taken to distinguish true brain rhythms from analysis artifacts (Kass et al., 2005; R.E. Kass et al., 2014). Individual neurons can fire (somewhat) coherently with the temporal structure of neuronal population activity (Fries et al., 2007; Womelsdorf et al., 2007), or not (Senior et al., 2008; Manning et al., 2009), in ways that can depend on the behavioral condition and the neuron type.
The existence of this specific and reproducible temporal structure motivates the search for a kind of functional connectome that relates directly to it. Statistical associations between the spatially averaged “activity” in separate brain areas typically do not provide insight into the mechanisms that support these associations. That is, functional connectomics can provide very insightful summary statistics describing how large-scale brain networks correspond to cognitive states and how they change with learning or disease processes, but this framework is typically not suited to describing or explaining the intricate cellular processes that take part in producing what we call cognition, or even suggesting mechanisms by which regions are coordinated.
More specifically, the description of the brain as nodes and edges de-emphasizes the questions of what signals get sent over the edges and how those signals are processed. Also, distinctions that are clear in such a network framework (nodes versus edges) can be unclear in brain tissue. For example, the local field potential (LFP), measured at a point in space, depends both on “node” activity that is local to the measurement and also nonlocal synaptic input along “edges” targeting that point in space; the LFP measurements do not distinguish these components, though clues can be obtained by current source density measurements or other techniques. We contend that, to understand the nature of a functional connection requires a more detailed look at the local dynamics of the nodes (that is, not considering them as points with “activity”, but acknowledging more detailed physiological and dynamical structure), to understand how local processing is done, how it is regulated by neuromodulators, and how the language of signals coordinates multiple parts of the brain in cognitive tasks.
This expanded description of brain activity is what we call the “dynome”. The dynome is the collection of experimental and modeling observations having to do with dynamical structure (and its physiological and pathophysiological implementation) in the brain and its relationship to cognition. It includes what is usually known as the functional connectome, but expands the notion to go beyond statistical associations to the mechanisms involved in producing and processing signals within the brain. In the dynome context, “understanding” brain activity means uncovering the functions and dysfunctions provided by the brain’s temporal dynamics. Like the connectome, the dynome proposes a framework for a broad research program. Yet, the dynome does not have to be constructed de novo: there is already a body of work on which further efforts can be based, and in the next part of this essay we describe some of it. However, we note that, though much cognitively important dynamical structure has been uncovered, the field is still in its infancy.
What constitutes the dynome?
In this section we discuss a framework for constructing the dynome, along with examples of such work. Dynomics involves charting the dynamical structure of local and global networks, studied mainly in vitro and in vivo respectively, and connecting those dynamics to biophysical mechanisms and cognitively important computations via modeling of detailed neuron and network biophysics. This complements other approaches, including dynamical causal modeling, which tends to focus on more abstract, neural mass models (e.g., Kiebel et al., 2009; Moran et al., 2013), which may not reflect the biophysical properties critical to some neural computations. We focus, as invited, on brain rhythms, which reflect and influence spiking activity; however, dynamic processes can also happen in the absence of rhythmic brain activity (Ecker et al., 2010; Renart et al., 2010; Ainsworth et al., 2012; Histed & Maunsell, 2014) and the dynome includes other temporal structures as well (Larson-Prior et al., 2013).
Local temporal structure
By “local”, we mean a network whose components are physically close and which participate in similar computations, as described by functional anatomy. This could be a single cortical column or a larger but related group of columns (e.g., hypercolumns in visual cortex). This is similar to the idea of a “node”, but encompasses all the anatomical and physiological structure within the local network, including cortical layers, distinct neuronal populations, intrinsic currents, local synaptic connectivity, and responses to neuromodulation.
The dynamics within such a local network can be extremely complex. In vitro and in vivo preparations reveal the intrinsic properties of these local networks. The amount of reproducible temporal structure is astonishing, and the task of charting such structure is by no means finished. Structure found in vitro includes: Multiple mechanistically different versions of a rhythm in the same frequency band (Roopun et al., 2010); Multiple mechanistically different rhythms in the same cortical region (Ainsworth et al., 2011); Different rhythms appearing simultaneously in different cortical layers (Oke et al., 2010; Ainsworth et al., 2012); Different effects of neuromodulators on rhythms in different brain areas (Middleton et al., 2008; Roopun et al., 2008a); Switches in temporal structure with changes in activation (Roopun et al., 2008b); Fast rhythms nested inside slower rhythms (Gloveli et al., 2005; Carracedo et al., 2013); Faster intrinsic rhythms suppressed by slower ones (Pietersen et al., 2014). Some of this structure observed in vitro has also been found in vivo. For example, the properties of gamma rhythms as interactions of excitation and inhibition (Atallah & Scanziani, 2009; Cardin et al., 2009), and laminar differences in rhythms (Buffalo et al., 2011). Understanding the substrates and mechanisms that support these rhythms, their interactions, and their function, is one goal of dynomics.
In vivo, brain rhythms are rarely seen in isolation. Indeed, a widespread motif is that faster rhythms are nested in slower rhythms (Chrobak & Buzsaki, 1998; Lakatos et al., 2005; Palva & Palva, 2007; Colgin et al., 2009). Experiments and modeling have begun to illuminate the possible mechanisms of cross-frequency coupling, including an important role for inhibition (Wulff et al., 2009). However, major challenges remain in detecting and understanding cross-frequency coupling. Quantitative characterization of cross-frequency coupling from in vivo recordings is fraught with difficulties (e.g., due to complicated, non-sinusoidal nature of brain activity (Kramer et al., 2008b). Moreover, a meaningful understanding of cross-frequency coupling – beyond a biomarker of brain dynamics – requires knowledge of the biological mechanisms that support the observed activity in the different frequency bands expressed.
It remains to establish which aspects of in vitro dynamics manifest meaningfully in vivo, especially in the context of cognitive tasks. This is difficult, partly because in vivo recordings with behavior require the entire brain, which necessarily introduces uncertainty regarding important features that shape the observed dynamics (e.g., the neuromodulatory state of the area of interest, the nature of the inputs to the area, etc). In vitro experiments, on the other hand, sacrifice a direct link with behavior to allow controlled observations of the dynamics produced by a physically isolated area of interest. Though all spectral bands that are seen in vitro appear with remarkable consistency in vivo, we generally still do not know the underlying mechanisms of the in vivo rhythms. Moreover, the variety of mechanisms that support the same rhythm in vitro means that we cannot assume from the frequency of an observed rhythm the underlying physiology without, for example, local pharmacological manipulation or carefully designed, neuron subtype-specific optogenetic/pharmacogenetic interference.
In order to address the issues of how dynamic structure affects cognitive computations, it is necessary to understand how the physiology of local regions gives rise to local rhythms and their interactions. The most thoroughly studied rhythms are the class of gamma (30–90 Hz) rhythms (Whittington et al., 2000; Buzsaki & Wang, 2012). Though there are many subtleties to the underlying mechanisms and their consequences for spatiotemporal interactions of this set of rhythms, the basic phenomenon involves feedback inhibition from fast-spiking cells, notably parvalbumin-positive (PV+) cells, to pyramidal cells (in Pyramidal Interneuron Network Gamma or “PING”) or to the inhibitory cells themselves (Interneuron Network Gamma or “ING”): The decaying feedback inhibition provides a window of opportunity for cells to fire, and the decay time of the inhibition is central in determining the period of the rhythms. Other slower brain rhythms appear to depend more on voltage-dependent intrinsic currents, especially M-currents (outward currents suppressed by activation of muscarinic receptors) (Roopun et al., 2006) and h-currents (inward currents activated by hyperpolarization) (Luthi & McCormick, 1998). Both of these currents are sensitive to inhibition on fast time scales, so feedback inhibition from fast-spiking PV+ cells (with fast decay times) and somatostatin-positive (SOM+) cells (with synaptic decay kinetics several times slower than that of PV+ cells), can affect multiple intrinsic currents, leading to complex local dynamics in which the intrinsic time scales of the activation and deactivation of the currents - combined with synaptic input - shape the period. At still lower frequencies (< 4 Hz), even slower inhibition (GABAB receptor-mediated) and metabolic/metabotropic effects can support the slower timescales. Modeling has been done for many of the above frequencies to illuminate the roles of the various currents and the effects of neuromodulators in supporting and disrupting neuronal rhythms (Whittington et al., 2000; Destexhe & Sejnowski, 2003; Rotstein et al., 2005; Tort et al., 2007; Kramer et al., 2008a; Vierling-Claassen et al., 2010; Skinner, 2012; Lee et al., 2013; Cannon et al., 2014).
These observations only partially illustrate the tremendous complexity in local brain dynamics. However, this complexity is not arbitrary. There is structure to the complexity induced by neurobiology, as there is in the connectome, with much of that structure left to be uncovered.
Temporal structure and cognition
There is now a large and growing literature documenting the different electrophysiological rhythms associated with distinct cognitive operations, occurring across different spatiotemporal scales, and within broadly and narrowly defined anatomical regions (for reviews see Engel et al., 2001; Womelsdorf & Fries, 2006; Siegel et al., 2012). Studies over the last two decades have revealed that rhythms can support a variety of cognitively-relevant functions from enhancing thalamocortical inputs locally (Le Masson et al., 2002; Lakatos et al., 2008) to enabling two or more different regions to be bound together through coherence, thus enhancing feature discrimination or memory encoding (Fries, 2005; Sejnowski & Paulsen, 2006; Buffalo et al., 2011; Igarashi et al., 2014). Cognitive tasks often encompass a multitude of discrete processes that co-occur simultaneously in different brain regions. Dynomics focuses on understanding these processes, how they are coordinated, and the consequences of their disruption. Despite the complexity of these questions, some progress has been made, revealing insight into the dynamics of cognition, as we describe in the examples below.
Cognitive processes often begin with the presentation of multimodal task-relevant stimuli, and rhythms seem to play a key role in the integration of these signals (Senkowski et al., 2008; Hipp et al., 2011). This process is dependent on specialized regions being actively engaged during task performance, and studies from humans have revealed that large-scale networks are recruited and synchronized at a particular frequency band(s) in a region specific manner (Senkowski et al., 2008). The brain rhythm proposed to be most instrumental to task-based functional connections between neuronal populations in different brain areas is the set of beta (12–30 Hz) rhythms (Donner & Siegel, 2011). This set is also the most mysterious: there appear to be a large number of mechanistically different versions of these rhythms, produced in different parts of the nervous system (Cannon et al., 2014). These rhythms may involve different classes of cells, and use synaptic excitation and/or inhibition, as well as intrinsic currents (Roopun et al., 2006; Kopell et al., 2011). Other slower frequency ranges have also been found to be important for macroscale interactions. Most notably, low frequency delta rhythms (1–4 Hz) are known to coordinate large portions of the brain (Fujisawa & Buzsaki, 2011; Nacher et al., 2013). Some of this coordination is done through subcortical structures, notably the thalamus and the basal ganglia (Amzica & Steriade, 1998; Lopez-Azcarate et al., 2013; Antzoulatos & Miller, 2014).
Faster rhythms are generally thought to play a prominent role in localized processing, usually within a particular region or even within a cortical column; again, the most well studied oscillations are gamma rhythms associated with attention. Gamma oscillations play a prominent role in stimulus detection locally by modulating spike timing relative to a specific phase of the local field potential (Fries et al., 2001; Bichot et al., 2005). This process is thought to improve signal discriminability by elevating firing rates to near saturation levels and by decreasing spike-count variability (Masuda & Doiron, 2007; Mitchell et al., 2007). In addition to local processing, gamma rhythms support cross-regional coupling, particularly as it relates to attentional and working memory networks. For example, as demands on attention increase, different association areas also show strongest coupling between one another at gamma frequencies (Gregoriou et al., 2009) and demands on attention can also recruit other frequency bands that interact with gamma oscillations through cross-frequency coupling (Lakatos et al., 2008).
Other frequency bands are often coordinated or coupled between regions during cognitive tasks and the strength of coupling at a given frequency band between two regions can be modulated by task conditions (Buschman & Miller, 2007). In this sense, multiple networks, all associated with different frequency bands, may contribute to the same task at task-relevant time points through different rhythms (Palva et al., 2010). Brain rhythms also appear in cortical and subcortical structures whose coordination is essential for certain types of sensory perception and learning. Multiple frequency bands can contribute to this process, and the level of synchronization across large scale networks can be predictive of both sensory perception and task performance (Hipp et al., 2011). Functional connectivity between the striatum and prefrontal cortex, for example, has been shown to strengthen as rules associated with categories are acquired in a category-specific manner (Antzoulatos & Miller, 2011; 2014). This coupling, particularly in the beta frequency range, seems to be important for selecting task rules and dissociating ensembles associated with rule relevant behavior from overlapping neuronal populations (Buschman et al., 2012). These examples suggest that rhythms are dynamically modulated by task demands and they can change over the course of learning. Furthermore, cross-frequency coupling across structures important for different aspects of task performance may set the stage for cooperation among neuronal ensembles that are recruited depending on task conditions (Tort et al., 2008).
While some rhythms may be better suited to enable change during learning, other rhythms may be important for stability once learning has occurred. Studies from hippocampal slices reveal that calcium entry through NMDA receptors or voltage-gated calcium channels provide the basis for both LTP and LTD depending on the frequency with which the input arrives (Bear & Malenka, 1994), and recent studies have revealed that the nesting of gamma rhythms in hippocampal theta rhythms support memory encoding and retrieval depending on the phase of the theta cycle (Colgin et al., 2009; Tort et al., 2009; Igarashi et al., 2014). Modeling work has suggested that the interaction of gamma and theta rhythms are important for promoting spike-timing dependent plasticity through NMDA receptors (Lee et al., 2009). Just as importantly, other rhythms have been suggested to promote stabilization and the continuation of on-going processes, the most prominent being beta oscillations in theories where beta is important for maintaining the status quo (Engel & Fries, 2010).
The ubiquity of brain rhythms, their specificity, and their dynamic nature strongly suggest their importance in cognition and behavioral outcome. The question then remains: through what mechanism can rhythms be regulated with the specificity to support and coordinate discrete aspects of cognitive operations both temporally and spatially? We suggest that part of the answer to this question must involve the primary neuromodulator systems. Neuromodulators can act locally or globally in ways that have profound influences on overall network function. Widespread regulation by neuromodulators is most evident in conditions of sleep onset or sleep transitions, where specific rhythms come to characterize these states (McGinty & Harper, 1976; Kayama et al., 1992; Carter et al., 2012). Neuromodulators, however, can also function to convey information in a very discrete and targeted way by communicating information about task relevant stimuli to some regions of neocortex and not others (Parikh et al., 2007; Howe et al., 2013) and by promoting oscillations at specific frequency bands in a region specific manner (Roopun et al., 2010).
As described above, by changing physiology, neuromodulators change dynamics which, in turn, changes the processing of inputs. Hence, an important function of neuromodulators may be to change what regions are “on-line”; emerging evidence suggests that neuromodulators effectively regulate what inputs a region can “hear “ (Disney et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2013). Slice physiology experiments combined with modeling have elucidated a mechanism by which cholinergic mediated changes in signaling can support different cognitive functions through rhythm modulation; for example, pharmacology experiments in visual cortex first noted that cholinergic signaling has the potential to regulate the direction of information flow within cortical columns based on differences in muscarinic and nicotinic receptor expression across two classes of inhibitory (LTS and FS) interneurons (Xiang et al., 1998). Modeling built upon this finding provides a functional mechanism through which the emergence of deep layer beta-oscillations, associated with periods of top-down attention, could be explained by the enhanced excitability of slow-inhibitory interneurons in the presence of acetylcholine (Lee et al., 2013). Other modeling studies have also offered insight into the details of stimulus competition where neuromodulators are essential to recruiting interneuron networks to promote gamma rhythmicity (Borgers et al., 2008) or to promote synaptic weakening (Lee et al., 2009). Future insight into cognitive function will depend on understanding the mechanisms by which neuromodulators change physiological processes in a way that recruits or alters rhythms during cognition.
Bridging the scales: physiology and modeling
Most of the work that has been done so far on fast temporal structure (such as brain rhythms) has focused on two categories: either finding the biophysical bases of brain rhythms or charting the association of cognitive activity with rhythms. By contrast, there has been much less work attempting to understand how, or even if, the physiological properties underlying fast dynamics are used in cognitive computations. This section describes some of that work and the kinds of questions that need to be addressed.
A central question concerns how the signals that are transmitted along the anatomy of the connectome are heard (or not), and how these transmitted signals interact with local dynamics to transform and coordinate local activity. The investigation of that question is often done via modeling. For example, we know that unpatterned input can give rise to gamma rhythms (Borgers & Kopell, 2005) and that gamma rhythms are ideal for the creation of cell assemblies (Harris et al., 2003) and their protection against distractors (Olufsen et al., 2003; Borgers et al., 2005; 2008). The ability of gamma rhythms to facilitate such a computation through competition comes directly from the physiological properties of the gamma rhythm: it is the feedback inhibition underlying its formation that allows the most activated cells to fire in unison and suppress activity of other cells via the feedback inhibition. Another rhythm whose physiology is important to transmission and coordination is a form of the beta 1 rhythm: In rodent association cortex in vitro, the superficial layers produce a gamma rhythm in the presence of the glutamate receptor agonist kainate, and the deep layers produce a beta 2 (25 Hz) rhythm; when the kainate is partially removed by an antagonist after a period in which plasticity takes place, the gamma and beta 2 rhythms are replaced in all layers by a beta 1 oscillation (15 Hz) (Roopun et al., 2008b). In this rhythm, the activation is passed back and forth between the superficial and deep layers via inhibitory rebound. Modeling has shown that such a temporal pattern of activity has the ability to maintain a representation of an input beyond the duration of a stimulus, and coordinate cells assemblies from earlier and later inputs (Kramer et al., 2008a; Kopell et al., 2011); this maintenance is not possible in computational models when gamma-mediated cell assemblies are coordinated only by common inhibition in the superficial layers (Borgers et al., 2005; 2008). Furthermore, relationships exist between the gamma and beta frequency bands; for example, signals that are transmitted at beta frequency can be transformed to produce higher power in the gamma frequency, leading to gain control of input. (Lee et al., 2013). In general, the physiology of brain rhythms, especially connected with feedback inhibition, is believed to be important for creating the right phase relationship for coordination (Fries, 2005; Cannon et al., 2014) and therefore supportive of cognitive computations (Roopun et al., 2010).
The above question is centrally involved in the relationship between a network’s structural connectivity and the dynamic functional connectivity associated with a cognitive process (Honey et al., 2010; Woolrich & Stephan, 2013). In addressing that relationship, a natural approach is to simplify, for example, by examining simple oscillator models embedded in a network. However, an oscillator model typically consists of only one degree of freedom for the oscillator, its phase, which is manipulated by temporal input. For oscillations produced by the brain’s networks, there are myriad internal degrees of freedom, including participation of any given cell on a given cycle and the state of all the conductances of each cell at any given time. Thus, the literature on responses of simple oscillators to temporal input can give some direction, but not a complete picture. Therefore, to understand the relationship between the brain’s functional and structural networks, more biophysically realistic models are required. In that direction, one approach is to simulate neural population activity on a static anatomical network. This modeling approach has been used, for example, to suggest important contributions of general features (such as signal transmission delays and noise) to the organization of dynamic resting state functional networks (Deco et al., 2011). An even more complex modeling approach is to utilize detailed biophysical models of neural activity, embedded in an anatomical network. This approach requires much greater computational effort but may be essential to examine the effects on functional connectivity that arise from the actual biological dynamics of cognitive function.
The available modeling and physiology is just the beginning of investigations under the framework of the dynome. For example, it is not known why there are so many different forms of beta rhythm, but reasonable conjectures include that a) regions that produce - or resonate to - a given frequency can respond in a stronger way to input from a similar frequency (Lee et al., 2013); b) the kinds of computations done in the various regions are facilitated best by different biological implementations of the same rhythm; c) the various rhythms can be independently modulated, leading to flexibility in computation (Somogyi et al., 2003) and d) different mechanisms impart different phase sensitivities to input, so a set of beta rhythm generators may all have statistically identical frequencies, but respond very differently to a shared spectral profile of input. To understand if this is correct requires knowledge of how each region responds to its temporally patterned input. A critical feature is that the impact of an input on a brain region is not generic; the signal traveling along axons to some region can have effects on the target that would not occur if the same input went to a different region. To understand the impact of a neuronal input, we need to know features of the targeted region, including: what classes of cells are targeted, the time scales and nature (excitatory or inhibitory) of the synaptic currents, the intrinsic currents of the target cells, the state of the extracellular environment at the target, and the neuromodulators present.
Interdependence of dynamics and connectomics
We have emphasized the importance of dynamics - and the physiological implementation of these dynamics - in understanding the cognitive computations performed by the brain. The signals created and routed throughout the brain are carried by physical pathways that are studied by connectomics. However, the consideration of the more extensive notion of “functional connection” provided by dynomics – a notion that includes an understanding of the physiology that supports dynamic coordination - helps to clarify what sorts of connectomics information might be most useful. A very detailed anatomical description of a piece of tissue that does not specify the kind of information needed to understand the effect of neuronal input (see end of last section) cannot be used to address the kinds of questions posed in the dynome. We need to be able to add physiological knowledge and functional significance to the anatomical results. This situation is complicated by the interrelationship between physical connectivity and network dynamics. For example, as highlighted above, some dynamics are closely related to neuronal plasticity underlying memory (Tort et al., 2009; Igarashi et al., 2014), and both the frequency and timing of neuronal events is critical for expression of this (e.g., Bear & Malenka, 1994; Bi & Poo, 1998). As such, plasticity induces structural changes in neuronal connectivity (Bailey & Kandel, 1993), so changes in temporal structure are very likely to change the connectome.
In ‘non-plastic’ model systems the close interrelationship between connectivity and dynamics can also be readily observed. Even random connectivity graphs have discrete dynamic signatures associated with activity propagating within them (Traub et al., 2001) and different dynamic signatures appear to be correlated with different conduction delays (Kopell et al., 2000; Tort et al., 2007; Deco et al., 2009). This is one reason why the study of the dynome needs to be engaged in parallel with that of the connectome. Like connectomics, this program involves a daunting amount of work, but that work is well specified, and any addition to our knowledge driven by investigator initiated research consistent with this program is likely to have immediate implications for understanding how coordination happens within the nervous system.
As new technology has supported construction of the connectome, so will new technology facilitate continuing study of the dynome. Emerging technologies for the observation of the brain’s dynamic activity include high-density electrode recordings (Viventi et al., 2011), optogenetic tools (Chow et al., 2010; Klapoetke et al., 2014), and large-scale three-dimensional imaging of single neuron activity (Prevedel et al., 2014). These technologies make now an opportune time to study the dynome. To do so will also require the development and application of data analysis tools to characterize activity (R.E. Kass et al., 2014) including interacting rhythms across temporal and spatial scales (Tort et al., 2010), as well as principled approaches to link neuronal data with computational models (Huys & Paninski, 2009; Meng et al., 2014).
Dynamics and diseases
Finally, many neurological diseases involve dysregulation of brain rhythms (Whittington et al., 2011; McCarthy et al., 2012; Uhlhaas & Singer, 2012). For some of these, there are also pathological changes in structural and functional connectivity that come under the purview of connectomics (Kramer & Cash, 2012; McCarthy et al., 2012; Anticevic et al., 2013). However, the relationship between the anatomical changes and the cognitive changes in neurological disease remains unclear. By contrast, dynomics provides a path to explanation that may engender new interventions driven by the neurobiology. The path has the following form: 1) changes in physical and anatomical properties (via genetically-related neurodevelopmental changes, post-birth insults, or neurodegenration later in life) produce changes in local brain dynamics; 2) changes in local dynamics change the profile of interactions between brain regions; 3) such changes are pathological for producing the kinds of computations important for cognitive functioning. A working example of this approach can be found in attempts to link primary pathology with cognitive deficit in schizophrenia: Many markers for function of fast spiking interneurons are dysregulated in this disorder (Lewis et al., 2012). As a consequence local network dynamics – particularly gamma rhythms - are disrupted in specific brain regions (Cunningham et al., 2006; Pafundo et al., 2013); Gamma rhythms are mediators of mainly local functional interactions (see above) and their disruption in schizophrenia is associated with selective loss of short-range functional connections in patients with cognitive deficit (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2013).
The promise of such a view of neurological disease is that it can suggest ways to change the dynamics even when underlying disease etiology is not currently understood or able to be changed. In this case, therapy directly affects the dynamical brain pathology. A very promising technology for such interventions is deep brain stimulation, which has become a standard treatment option for medication resistant Parkinson’s disease symptoms. Brain stimulation is also being investigated for a variety of other diseases such as depression (Holtzheimer & Mayberg, 2011; Holtzheimer et al., 2012), obsessive compulsive disorder (de Koning et al., 2011), and epilepsy (Leuchter et al., 2012). The understanding of how such interventions could work will depend on a description of how the dynamics are being perturbed in these technologies. In particular, if the region of the brain being stimulated is a hub, the stimulation is apt to effect regions of the brain in ways that depend on hub dynamics and connectivity.
Can we “understand” the brain without studying the mechanisms of its (fast) dynamics?
It might be argued that the sorts of details we are describing above are important only for the implementation of principles supported by the brain, and these principles can be described in terms of networks of nodes and edges. The above examples suggest that this is unlikely. First of all, in understanding what might support a computation involved in cognition, we need to know what the “wetware” is capable of. Secondly an immersion in the physiology supporting temporal dynamics suggests mechanisms that would not be obvious if one were thinking abstractly about “computation” and rhythms; as discussed above, different mechanisms may support the same rhythm, and therefore respond to input, or changes in neuromodulation, in different ways.
Indeed, the functional connectome (as described in a graph) is known to be dynamic (Bassett et al., 2011; Chu et al., 2012). However, it largely remains to be understood how the rich diversity of observed functional network dynamics are regulated. Here fast temporal dynamics can provide essential cues. As discussed above, the details of the local dynamics can be essential to how a signal is “heard” and processed, both locally at a node, and non-locally throughout the network. Thus, all modulations that change fast time scale dynamics impact not only the statistically related activities captured in functional networks, but also details of how signals are routed, combined and coordinated over the brain’s “wires”. Since cognitive outcomes depend on stages of processing that can happen in tens to hundreds of milliseconds, we need a framework that allows assessment of information at this timescale to be considered. The dynome is exactly such a framework.
We are not advocating implementation of the dynome framework as a mega-project to be addressed on a highly condensed time scale. Rather, we propose continued efforts to balance the research activity of the neuroscience community, in which brain dynamics (including fast dynamics such as rhythms, as well as other non-oscillatory dynamics) are studied along with connectivity to reveal how such dynamics facilitate the flexible and dynamic coordination of brain regions. Further understanding the brain’s “dynome” would benefit from larger-scale consortium projects, and individual investigator-driven projects to advance our knowledge and understanding of brain processing. We have given some details about what research already exists and what areas could benefit from more attention. To understand the brain’s dynome, knowledge of the biological details that support the brain’s dynamics remains critical. However, we do not propose solely a bottom-up approach focused only on these biological details. Instead, we envision a feedback loop between: (i) observations of large-scale neurological phenomena in vivo, (ii) implementation of the basic elements of these phenomena in experimental laboratory models in vitro, and (iii) analytical and predictive computational models that feed back into experimental models to assess their validity against the large-scale observations. Like connectomics (and unlike the original aims of genomics), the goals are open-ended and the progress from many specific research projects will be of use.
There is another aspect of dynomics that makes this framework different from that of connectomics: the study of brain dynamics has the potential to bridge insights across levels of function. Studying how genes affect anatomy and physiology leads naturally to the study of small network dynamics; any information we have about small network activity provides the basis for further understanding how networks interact to produce meso- and macro-level behavior. Interactions of large networks are critical to understanding cognition and pathologies of cognition. We do not think this work needs to (or can) be finished through a linear progression of stages, from molecules to behavior; rather, work must occur at multiple levels simultaneously. We have employed the “omics” label here to emphasize the inclusive nature of this framework to understand brain dynamics. We propose that coordination of efforts to understand the mechanisms of brain dynamics across spatial and temporal scales will drive new understanding of brain function and dysfunction in cognition.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Ainsworth M, Lee S, Cunningham MO, Roopun AK, Traub RD, Kopell NJ, Whittington MA. Dual gamma rhythm generators control interlaminar synchrony in auditory cortex. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:17040–17051. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2209-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth M, Lee S, Cunningham MO, Traub RD, Kopell NJ, Whittington MA. Rates and rhythms: a synergistic view of frequency and temporal coding in neuronal networks. Neuron. 2012;75:572–583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander-Bloch AF, Vertes PE, Stidd R, Lalonde F, Clasen L, Rapoport J, Giedd J, Bullmore ET, Gogtay N. The anatomical distance of functional connections predicts brain network topology in health and schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:127–138. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amzica F, Steriade M. Electrophysiological correlates of sleep delta waves. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. 1998;107:69–83. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(98)00051-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anticevic A, Cole MW, Repovs G, Savic A, Driesen NR, Yang G, Cho YT, Murray JD, Glahn DC, Wang XJ, Krystal JH. Connectivity, pharmacology, and computation: toward a mechanistic understanding of neural system dysfunction in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:169. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antzoulatos EG, Miller EK. Differences between neural activity in prefrontal cortex and striatum during learning of novel abstract categories. Neuron. 2011;71:243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antzoulatos EG, Miller EK. Increases in Functional Connectivity between Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Category Learning. Neuron. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Instantaneous modulation of gamma oscillation frequency by balancing excitation with inhibition. Neuron. 2009;62:566–577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey CH, Kandel ER. Structural changes accompanying memory storage. Annual review of physiology. 1993;55:397–426. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.55.030193.002145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bargmann CI, Marder E. From the connectome to brain function. Nat Methods. 2013;10:483–490. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassett DS, Wymbs NF, Porter MA, Mucha PJ, Carlson JM, Grafton ST. Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:7641–7646. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018985108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bear MF, Malenka RC. Synaptic plasticity: LTP and LTD. Current opinion in neurobiology. 1994;4:389–399. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90101-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bi GQ, Poo MM. Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1998;18:10464–10472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-24-10464.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bichot NP, Rossi AF, Desimone R. Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. Science. 2005;308:529–534. doi: 10.1126/science.1109676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgers C, Epstein S, Kopell NJ. Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: a computational study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102:7002–7007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502366102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgers C, Epstein S, Kopell NJ. Gamma oscillations mediate stimulus competition and attentional selection in a cortical network model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:18023–18028. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809511105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borgers C, Kopell N. Effects of noisy drive on rhythms in networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Neural computation. 2005;17:557–608. doi: 10.1162/0899766053019908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2010;14:277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, Desimone R. Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:11262–11267. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011284108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschman TJ, Denovellis EL, Diogo C, Bullock D, Miller EK. Synchronous oscillatory neural ensembles for rules in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2012;76:838–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007;315:1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Wang XJ. Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:203–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cannon J, McCarthy MM, Lee S, Lee J, Borgers C, Whittington MA, Kopell N. Neurosystems: brain rhythms and cognitive processing. The European journal of neuroscience. 2014;39:705–719. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardin JA, Carlen M, Meletis K, Knoblich U, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Tsai LH, Moore CI. Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature. 2009;459:663–667. doi: 10.1038/nature08002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carracedo LM, Kjeldsen H, Cunnington L, Jenkins A, Schofield I, Cunningham MO, Davies CH, Traub RD, Whittington MA. A neocortical delta rhythm facilitates reciprocal interlaminar interactions via nested theta rhythms. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2013;33:10750–10761. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0735-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter ME, Brill J, Bonnavion P, Huguenard JR, Huerta R, de Lecea L. Mechanism for Hypocretin-mediated sleep-to-wake transitions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109:E2635–2644. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202526109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chow BY, Han X, Dobry AS, Qian X, Chuong AS, Li M, Henninger MA, Belfort GM, Lin Y, Monahan PE, Boyden ES. High-performance genetically targetable optical neural silencing by light-driven proton pumps. Nature. 2010;463:98–102. doi: 10.1038/nature08652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chrobak JJ, Buzsaki G. Gamma oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of the freely behaving rat. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1998;18:388–398. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00388.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu CJ, Kramer MA, Pathmanathan J, Bianchi MT, Westover MB, Wizon L, Cash SS. Emergence of stable functional networks in long-term human electroencephalography. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2012;32:2703–2713. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5669-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, Moser MB, Moser EI. Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;462:353–357. doi: 10.1038/nature08573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham MO, Hunt J, Middleton S, LeBeau FE, Gillies MJ, Davies CH, Maycox PR, Whittington MA, Racca C. Region-specific reduction in entorhinal gamma oscillations and parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in animal models of psychiatric illness. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2006;26:2767–2776. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5054-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Koning PP, Figee M, van den Munckhof P, Schuurman PR, Denys D. Current status of deep brain stimulation for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a clinical review of different targets. Current psychiatry reports. 2011;13:274–282. doi: 10.1007/s11920-011-0200-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Jirsa V, McIntosh AR, Sporns O, Kotter R. Key role of coupling, delay, and noise in resting brain fluctuations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:10302–10307. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901831106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Destexhe A, Sejnowski TJ. Interactions between membrane conductances underlying thalamocortical slow-wave oscillations. Physiol Rev. 2003;83:1401–1453. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00012.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disney AA, Aoki C, Hawken MJ. Gain modulation by nicotine in macaque v1. Neuron. 2007;56:701–713. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donner TH, Siegel M. A framework for local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2011;15:191–199. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ecker AS, Berens P, Keliris GA, Bethge M, Logothetis NK, Tolias AS. Decorrelated neuronal firing in cortical microcircuits. Science. 2010;327:584–587. doi: 10.1126/science.1179867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations--signalling the status quo? Current opinion in neurobiology. 2010;20:156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2001;2:704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Roelfsema PR, Fries P, Brecht M, Singer W. Role of the temporal domain for response selection and perceptual binding. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:571–582. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.6.571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102:9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P, Nikolic D, Singer W. The gamma cycle. Trends in neurosciences. 2007;30:309–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujisawa S, Buzsaki G. A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal, VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron. 2011;72:153–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gloveli T, Dugladze T, Rotstein HG, Traub RD, Monyer H, Heinemann U, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Orthogonal arrangement of rhythm-generating microcircuits in the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2005;102:13295–13300. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506259102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009;324:1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Dragoi G, Buzsaki G. Organization of cell assemblies in the hippocampus. Nature. 2003;424:552–556. doi: 10.1038/nature01834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hipp JF, Engel AK, Siegel M. Oscillatory synchronization in large-scale cortical networks predicts perception. Neuron. 2011;69:387–396. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Histed MH, Maunsell JH. Cortical neural populations can guide behavior by integrating inputs linearly, independent of synchrony. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111:E178–187. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1318750111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtzheimer PE, Kelley ME, Gross RE, Filkowski MM, Garlow SJ, Barrocas A, Wint D, Craighead MC, Kozarsky J, Chismar R, Moreines JL, Mewes K, Posse PR, Gutman DA, Mayberg HS. Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant unipolar and bipolar depression. Archives of general psychiatry. 2012;69:150–158. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtzheimer PE, Mayberg HS. Deep brain stimulation for psychiatric disorders. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:289–307. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey CJ, Thivierge JP, Sporns O. Can structure predict function in the human brain? NeuroImage. 2010;52:766–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howe WM, Berry AS, Francois J, Gilmour G, Carp JM, Tricklebank M, Lustig C, Sarter M. Prefrontal cholinergic mechanisms instigating shifts from monitoring for cues to cue-guided performance: converging electrochemical and fMRI evidence from rats and humans. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2013;33:8742–8752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5809-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huys QJ, Paninski L. Smoothing of, and parameter estimation from, noisy biophysical recordings. PLoS computational biology. 2009;5:e1000379. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Igarashi KM, Lu L, Colgin LL, Moser MB, Moser EI. Coordination of entorhinal-hippocampal ensemble activity during associative learning. Nature. 2014;510:143–147. doi: 10.1038/nature13162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kass RE, Ventura V, Brown EN. Statistical issues in the analysis of neuronal data. Journal of neurophysiology. 2005;94:8–25. doi: 10.1152/jn.00648.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayama Y, Ohta M, Jodo E. Firing of ‘possibly’ cholinergic neurons in the rat laterodorsal tegmental nucleus during sleep and wakefulness. Brain research. 1992;569:210–220. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90632-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiebel SJ, Garrido MI, Moran R, Chen CC, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modeling for EEG and MEG. Human brain mapping. 2009;30:1866–1876. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nat Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97:1867–1872. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopell N, Whittington MA, Kramer MA. Neuronal assembly dynamics in the beta1 frequency range permits short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:3779–3784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019676108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer MA, Cash SS. Epilepsy as a disorder of cortical network organization. Neuroscientist. 2012;18:360–372. doi: 10.1177/1073858411422754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer MA, Roopun AK, Carracedo LM, Traub RD, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Rhythm generation through period concatenation in rat somatosensory cortex. PLoS computational biology. 2008a;4:e1000169. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer MA, Tort AB, Kopell NJ. Sharp edge artifacts and spurious coupling in EEG frequency comodulation measures. Journal of neuroscience methods. 2008b;170:352–357. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakatos P, Karmos G, Mehta AD, Ulbert I, Schroeder CE. Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science. 2008;320:110–113. doi: 10.1126/science.1154735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakatos P, Shah AS, Knuth KH, Ulbert I, Karmos G, Schroeder CE. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. Journal of neurophysiology. 2005;94:1904–1911. doi: 10.1152/jn.00263.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larson-Prior LJ, Oostenveld R, Della Penna S, Michalareas G, Prior F, Babajani-Feremi A, Schoffelen JM, Marzetti L, de Pasquale F, Di Pompeo F, Stout J, Woolrich M, Luo Q, Bucholz R, Fries P, Pizzella V, Romani GL, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Consortium WUMH. Adding dynamics to the Human Connectome Project with MEG. NeuroImage. 2013;80:190–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Masson G, Renaud-Le Masson S, Debay D, Bal T. Feedback inhibition controls spike transfer in hybrid thalamic circuits. Nature. 2002;417:854–858. doi: 10.1038/nature00825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JH, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Top-down beta rhythms support selective attention via interlaminar interaction: a model. PLoS computational biology. 2013;9:e1003164. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S, Sen K, Kopell N. Cortical gamma rhythms modulate NMDAR-mediated spike timing dependent plasticity in a biophysical model. PLoS computational biology. 2009;5:e1000602. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leuchter B, Pedley TA, Lisanby SH, Mayberg HS, Schiff ND. Brain stimulation in neurology and psychiatry: perspectives on an evolving field. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2012;1265:vii–x. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06740.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis DA, Curley AA, Glausier JR, Volk DW. Cortical parvalbumin interneurons and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Trends in neurosciences. 2012;35:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Azcarate J, Nicolas MJ, Cordon I, Alegre M, Valencia M, Artieda J. Delta-mediated cross-frequency coupling organizes oscillatory activity across the rat cortico-basal ganglia network. Frontiers in neural circuits. 2013;7:155. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthi A, McCormick DA. H-current: properties of a neuronal and network pacemaker. Neuron. 1998;21:9–12. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80509-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning JR, Jacobs J, Fried I, Kahana MJ. Broadband shifts in local field potential power spectra are correlated with single-neuron spiking in humans. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2009;29:13613–13620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2041-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masuda N, Doiron B. Gamma oscillations of spiking neural populations enhance signal discrimination. PLoS computational biology. 2007;3:e236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy MM, Ching S, Whittington MA, Kopell N. Dynamical changes in neurological diseases and anesthesia. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2012;22:693–703. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinty DJ, Harper RM. Dorsal raphe neurons: depression of firing during sleep in cats. Brain research. 1976;101:569–575. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90480-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng L, Kramer MA, Middleton SJ, Whittington MA, Eden UT. A unified approach to linking experimental, statistical and computational analysis of spike train data. PloS one. 2014;9:e85269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton S, Jalics J, Kispersky T, Lebeau FE, Roopun AK, Kopell NJ, Whittington MA, Cunningham MO. NMDA receptor-dependent switching between different gamma rhythm-generating microcircuits in entorhinal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:18572–18577. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809302105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH. Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area V4. Neuron. 2007;55:131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran R, Pinotsis DA, Friston K. Neural masses and fields in dynamic causal modeling. Front Comput Neurosci. 2013;7:57. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nacher V, Ledberg A, Deco G, Romo R. Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas correlate with decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110:15085–15090. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314681110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oke OO, Magony A, Anver H, Ward PD, Jiruska P, Jefferys JG, Vreugdenhil M. High-frequency gamma oscillations coexist with low-frequency gamma oscillations in the rat visual cortex in vitro. The European journal of neuroscience. 2010;31:1435–1445. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07171.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olufsen MS, Whittington MA, Camperi M, Kopell N. New roles for the gamma rhythm: population tuning and preprocessing for the Beta rhythm. Journal of computational neuroscience. 2003;14:33–54. doi: 10.1023/a:1021124317706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pafundo DE, Miyamae T, Lewis DA, Gonzalez-Burgos G. Cholinergic modulation of neuronal excitability and recurrent excitation-inhibition in prefrontal cortex circuits: implications for gamma oscillations. The Journal of physiology. 2013;591:4725–4748. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.253823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva JM, Monto S, Kulashekhar S, Palva S. Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:7580–7585. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913113107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva S, Palva JM. New vistas for alpha-frequency band oscillations. Trends in neurosciences. 2007;30:150–158. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parikh V, Kozak R, Martinez V, Sarter M. Prefrontal acetylcholine release controls cue detection on multiple timescales. Neuron. 2007;56:141–154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park HJ, Friston K. Structural and functional brain networks: from connections to cognition. Science. 2013;342:1238411. doi: 10.1126/science.1238411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pietersen AN, Ward PD, Hagger-Vaughan N, Wiggins J, Jefferys JG, Vreugdenhil M. Transition between fast and slow gamma modes in rat hippocampus area CA1 in vitro is modulated by slow CA3 gamma oscillations. The Journal of physiology. 2014;592:605–620. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.263889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prevedel R, Yoon YG, Hoffmann M, Pak N, Wetzstein G, Kato S, Schrodel T, Raskar R, Zimmer M, Boyden ES, Vaziri A. Simultaneous whole-animal 3D imaging of neuronal activity using light-field microscopy. Nat Methods. 2014 doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kass RE, Eden U, Brown EN. Analysis of Neural Data. Springer; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Renart A, de la Rocha J, Bartho P, Hollender L, Parga N, Reyes A, Harris KD. The asynchronous state in cortical circuits. Science. 2010;327:587–590. doi: 10.1126/science.1179850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roopun AK, Cunningham MO, Racca C, Alter K, Traub RD, Whittington MA. Region-specific changes in gamma and beta2 rhythms in NMDA receptor dysfunction models of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008a;34:962–973. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roopun AK, Kramer MA, Carracedo LM, Kaiser M, Davies CH, Traub RD, Kopell NJ, Whittington MA. Period Concatenation Underlies Interactions between Gamma and Beta Rhythms in Neocortex. Frontiers in cellular neuroscience. 2008b;2:1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.03.001.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roopun AK, Lebeau FE, Rammell J, Cunningham MO, Traub RD, Whittington MA. Cholinergic neuromodulation controls directed temporal communication in neocortex in vitro. Frontiers in neural circuits. 2010;4:8. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2010.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roopun AK, Middleton SJ, Cunningham MO, Lebeau FE, Bibbig A, Whittington MA, Traub RD. A beta2-frequency (20–30 Hz) oscillation in nonsynaptic networks of somatosensory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:15646–15650. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607443103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rotstein HG, Pervouchine DD, Acker CD, Gillies MJ, White JA, Buhl EH, Whittington MA, Kopell N. Slow and fast inhibition and an H-current interact to create a theta rhythm in a model of CA1 interneuron network. Journal of neurophysiology. 2005;94:1509–1518. doi: 10.1152/jn.00957.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sejnowski TJ, Paulsen O. Network oscillations: emerging computational principles. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2006;26:1673–1676. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3737-05d.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senior TJ, Huxter JR, Allen K, O’Neill J, Csicsvari J. Gamma oscillatory firing reveals distinct populations of pyramidal cells in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2008;28:2274–2286. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4669-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senkowski D, Schneider TR, Foxe JJ, Engel AK. Crossmodal binding through neural coherence: implications for multisensory processing. Trends in neurosciences. 2008;31:401–409. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel M, Donner TH, Engel AK. Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner FK. Cellular-based modeling of oscillatory dynamics in brain networks. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2012;22:660–669. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somogyi P, Dalezios Y, Lujan R, Roberts JD, Watanabe M, Shigemoto R. High level of mGluR7 in the presynaptic active zones of select populations of GABAergic terminals innervating interneurons in the rat hippocampus. The European journal of neuroscience. 2003;17:2503–2520. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02697.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O. The human connectome: origins and challenges. NeuroImage. 2013;80:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley ML, Moussa MN, Paolini BM, Lyday RG, Burdette JH, Laurienti PJ. Defining nodes in complex brain networks. Front Comput Neurosci. 2013;7:169. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort AB, Komorowski R, Eichenbaum H, Kopell N. Measuring phase-amplitude coupling between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. Journal of neurophysiology. 2010;104:1195–1210. doi: 10.1152/jn.00106.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort AB, Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Kopell NJ, Eichenbaum H. Theta-gamma coupling increases during the learning of item-context associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:20942–20947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911331106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort AB, Kramer MA, Thorn C, Gibson DJ, Kubota Y, Graybiel AM, Kopell NJ. Dynamic cross-frequency couplings of local field potential oscillations in rat striatum and hippocampus during performance of a T-maze task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:20517–20522. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810524105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tort AB, Rotstein HG, Dugladze T, Gloveli T, Kopell NJ. On the formation of gamma-coherent cell assemblies by oriens lacunosum-moleculare interneurons in the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:13490–13495. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705708104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traub RD, Whittington MA, Buhl EH, LeBeau FE, Bibbig A, Boyd S, Cross H, Baldeweg T. A possible role for gap junctions in generation of very fast EEG oscillations preceding the onset of, and perhaps initiating, seizures. Epilepsia. 2001;42:153–170. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2001.26900.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks. Neuron. 2012;75:963–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vierling-Claassen D, Cardin JA, Moore CI, Jones SR. Computational modeling of distinct neocortical oscillations driven by cell-type selective optogenetic drive: separable resonant circuits controlled by low-threshold spiking and fast-spiking interneurons. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2010;4:198. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viventi J, Kim DH, Vigeland L, Frechette ES, Blanco JA, Kim YS, Avrin AE, Tiruvadi VR, Hwang SW, Vanleer AC, Wulsin DF, Davis K, Gelber CE, Palmer L, Van der Spiegel J, Wu J, Xiao J, Huang Y, Contreras D, Rogers JA, Litt B. Flexible, foldable, actively multiplexed, high-density electrode array for mapping brain activity in vivo. Nature neuroscience. 2011;14:1599–1605. doi: 10.1038/nn.2973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ. Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol Rev. 2010;90:1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittington MA, Roopun AK, Traub RD, Davies CH. Circuits and brain rhythms in schizophrenia: a wealth of convergent targets. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2011;11:508–514. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2011.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittington MA, Traub RD, Kopell N, Ermentrout B, Buhl EH. Inhibition-based rhythms: experimental and mathematical observations on network dynamics. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000;38:315–336. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00173-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womelsdorf T, Fries P. Neuronal coherence during selective attentional processing and sensory-motor integration. Journal of physiology, Paris. 2006;100:182–193. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Womelsdorf T, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Singer W, Desimone R, Engel AK, Fries P. Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science. 2007;316:1609–1612. doi: 10.1126/science.1139597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Stephan KE. Biophysical network models and the human connectome. NeuroImage. 2013;80:330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wulff P, Ponomarenko AA, Bartos M, Korotkova TM, Fuchs EC, Bahner F, Both M, Tort AB, Kopell NJ, Wisden W, Monyer H. Hippocampal theta rhythm and its coupling with gamma oscillations require fast inhibition onto parvalbumin-positive interneurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:3561–3566. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0813176106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiang Z, Huguenard JR, Prince DA. Cholinergic switching within neocortical inhibitory networks. Science. 1998;281:985–988. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5379.985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]