Abstract
Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a learning disability affecting 5–17% of children. Although researchers agree that DD is characterized by deficient phonological processing (PP), its cause is debated. It has been suggested that altered rapid auditory processing (RAP) may lead to deficient PP in DD and studies have shown deficient RAP in individuals with DD. Functional neuroimaging (fMRI) studies have implicated hypoactivations in left prefrontal brain regions during RAP in individuals with DD. When and how these neuronal alterations evolve remains unknown. In this article, we investigate functional networks during RAP in 28 children with (n = 14) and without (n = 14) a familial risk for DD before reading onset (mean: 5.6 years). Results reveal functional alterations in left-hemispheric prefrontal regions during RAP in prereading children at risk for DD, similar to findings in individuals with DD. Furthermore, activation during RAP in left prefrontal regions positively correlates with prereading measures of PP and with neuronal activation during PP in posterior dorsal and ventral brain areas. Our results suggest that neuronal differences during RAP predate reading instruction and thus are not due to experience-dependent brain changes resulting from DD itself and that there is a functional relationship between neuronal networks for RAP and PP within the prereading brain.
Keywords: developmental disorder, functional MRI, learning disability, pediatric neuroimaging, reading disability
Introduction
Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a language-based learning disability with known neurological origin (Galaburda et al. 2006), affecting ∼5–17% of all children (Shaywitz 1998). It is a specific reading disability characterized by difficulties with speed and accuracy of word decoding, language comprehension, and spelling (Siegel 2006). Cognitive difficulties may further include speech perception, the accurate representation and manipulation of speech sounds, problems with language, memory or letter sound knowledge. In some cases, it is further characterized by difficulties with rapid automatized naming (RAN; Siegel 2006; O'Brien et al. 2012). These difficulties are not due to lack of exposure to reading instruction (Lyon et al. 2003) and are independent from individual's intelligence quotient (IQ) (Seigel 1989; Ferrer et al. 2010). Epidemiologic longitudinal studies indicate that DD constitutes a persistent condition which cannot be attributed to a transient developmental delay (Shaywitz and Shaywitz 2005). To date, the earliest that DD can reliably be diagnosed is in second or third grade (British Dyslexia Association 2012) and most children who receive a diagnosis exhibit enduring reading impairments throughout adolescence (Flowers 1994; Lyon 1995) and into adulthood (Felton et al. 1990; Vogel and Adelman 1992).
Genetic and family studies strongly suggest a genetic basis for DD (e.g., Childs and Finucci 1983; Pennington 1991); however, no single-gene model can account for the phenotype observed. Longitudinal and crosssectional studies from several research projects indicate that up to 50% of all children with a familial risk for DD will also develop reading problems (Elbro et al. 1998; Scarborough 1998; Gallagher et al. 2000; Pennington and Lefly 2001; Snowling et al. 2007; Eklund et al. 2013). Similarly, there is a smaller portion (∼5–10%) of children that will develop reading problems despite a low or absent familial risk factors (e.g., Scarborough 1998). As many as 10 regions of chromosomes have been implicated in DD (e.g., DYX1C1 on 15q, KIAA0319 and DCDC2 on 6p22, and ROBO1 on 13q; for a review, see Gibson and Gruen 2008). The majority of these genes (e.g., KIAA0319, DCDC2, and DYX1C1) have shown to be crucial for neuronal migration and development of the cerebral neocortex (Wang et al. 2006). Rodent studies support the notion that neuronal migration issues may be a causal factor in the deficits observed in DD, including rapid spectrotemporal auditory processing problems (e.g., Fitch et al. 1994). Inducing neuronal migration anomalies in rats leads to significant auditory processing impairments (Fitch et al. 1994), comparable to those seen in children with language disabilities (Tallal and Piercy 1973a, 1973b). Only a few studies, have yet investigated the preliterate brain of typical or atypical developing children (e.g., Pugh et al. 2012). However, studying young children with a familial risk for dyslexia, offers an unique chance to investigate early neural, behavioral, and genetic determinants of DD.
Across languages consensus exists that DD is a specific language disorder with a characterized weakness in phonological processing (PP; e.g., Vellutino 1979; Goswami 2000). Pure PP theories, however, fail to explain widespread evidence of associated deficits in the visual (e.g., Eden, VanMeter, Rumsey, Maisog et al. 1996; Eden, VanMeter, Rumsey, Zeffiro 1996; Grinter et al. 2010; Lipowska et al. 2011), auditory (e.g., Hari and Renvall 2001; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007; Hornickel et al. 2009, 2012; Wright and Conlon 2009; Goswami, Wang et al. 2011; Stefanics et al. 2011), and motor domains (Ramus 2003; Yang and Hong-Yan 2011). Up to 50% of all individuals with DD are reported to also be affected by core sensory processing impairments (Ramus 2003). Some researchers have therefore argued that the perceptual and phonological difficulties observed in DD may be secondary to a more fundamental perceptual deficit (e.g., Ramus et al. 2003; Tallal 2004; Goswami 2011) such as difficulties in rapid auditory or spectrotemporal processing (McArthur and Bishop 2001; Tallal 2004; Goswami 2011; Diaz et al. 2012).
Rapid auditory processing (RAP) difficulties have been reported in up to 63% of all individuals with DD (Ramus et al. 2003). These difficulties can be observed during tasks of rapid temporal processing, gap detection, recognition of frequency and amplitude modulations, elevated frequency discrimination, and auditory stream segregation (Tallal 1980, 2004; Ramus 2003; Abrams et al. 2006; Tallal and Gaab 2006; Wright and Conlon 2009; Goswami, Fosker et al. 2011; Goswami, Wang et al. 2011; Hornickel et al. 2012). For example, compared with typical reading controls, individuals with dyslexia commonly show difficulties discriminating between consonant–vowel pairs (e.g., ba/da) that mainly differ in the first 40 ms, but not between syllables incorporating longer duration acoustic differences (Tallal and Piercy 1974; Reed 1989). Furthermore, children with DD are challenged when presented with amplitude modulations similar to those seen at the syllable level of speech (Talcott et al. 2000; Goswami et al. 2002; Goswami, Fosker et al. 2011). Goswami et al. (2002) used multiple regression analyses on results from 72 children and observed a significant relation between beat detection and phonological awareness. Auditory processing abilities not only differentiate children with DD from typical reading controls, but also distinguish between children with superior and inferior reading abilities (Goswami et al. 2002). The relationship between reading ability and the timing of subcortical auditory processing has previously been described to represent a continuum, with poor readers having delayed and good readers having early subcortical auditory timing (Banai et al. 2009). However, it is to note that even though ample evidence for sensorimotor deficits in DD exists, some studies, have failed to replicate findings of auditory processing difficulties in DD or found these only in some individuals with DD (France et al. 2002; Breier et al. 2003; Ramus 2003; Gibson et al. 2006) or could not find evidence for a link between rapid auditory and PP deficits (Georgiou et al. 2010; Willburger and Landerl 2010).
Early language difficulties have been firmly associated with later reading disorders (Beitchman et al. 1986; Scarborough 1990, 1998; Stanovich and Siegel 1994). Beyond various linguistic impairments (e.g., syntactic awareness, language comprehension, or speech perception), RAP, and phonological abilities in infants and young children have shown to predict later reading ability (e.g., Benaisch and Tallal 2002; Lyytinen et al. 2004; Tsao et al. 2004; Benasich et al. 2006; Rvachew and Grawburg 2006). Longitudinal work comparing infants and young children with familial risk for DD to typically developing controls shows that differences in categorizing speech sounds already exists in infancy (6 months) and persists until adulthood (Richardson et al. 2003). For example, in a 3-year-long longitudinal study, Huss et al. (2010) show that accurate perception of amplitude envelope rise time predicts phonological awareness and reading development in children ages 8–13 years. Accounting for up to 60% of the variance in reading ability, these findings connect metrical and basic auditory rise time processing, providing a link between primary sensory impairments in auditory processing and development of literacy skills (Huss et al. 2010).
Most neuroimaging research in DD has focused on the investigation of reading and reading-related variables fundamental for the characteristics seen in DD (e.g., PP skills). Numerous functional neuroimaging (fMRI) studies in typical children and adults have implicated left-hemispheric brain network during reading and reading-related tasks, such as PP. One of the most consistent and well-replicated findings in DD is a hypoactivation of this left-hemispheric network during reading, including temporoparietal, occipitotemporal, and inferior frontal brain regions (e.g., for reviews, see Temple 2002; Gabrieli 2009). Neuronal differences have furthermore been supported, by reports of structural atypicalities in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions (Eckert et al. 2005; Kronbichler et al. 2008; Pernet et al. 2009; Linkersdorfer et al. 2012) and reduced functional connectivity (Horwitz et al. 1998; Hampson et al. 2004). Additionally, early studies in prereading and young children at risk for reading failure reiterate the importance of left-hemispheric posterior networks, which later become crucial for skilled reading (Maurer et al. 2007; Specht et al. 2009; Brem et al. 2010; Raschle et al. 2011; Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a; Raschle, Zuk, Ortiz-Mantilla et al. 2012b). However, the interplay between observed sensory deficits in some individuals with DD and the well-replicated phonological impairments has not been investigated in the brain.
Ultimately, although neurological impairments have been repeatedly linked to DD, the nature of the precise neural phenotype remains debated (Ramus 2003; Demonet et al. 2004). A second line of neuroimaging research has focused on basic sensory and sensorimotor processing difficulties observed in DD (e.g., McArthur and Bishop 2001; Ramus et al. 2003; Goswami 2011). For example, research studies using fMRI have reported altered brain activation in individuals with DD in left prefrontal brain regions during experimental modulations of speech rate (Ruff et al. 2002) or RAP (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). The left prefrontal cortex has furthermore been associated with rapid but not slow auditory processing abilities in 2 fMRI studies assessing children (Gaab et al. 2007) and adults (Temple et al. 2000) with and without a diagnosis of DD. Both studies performed whole-brain fMRI while participants listened to nonlinguistic acoustic stimuli, incorporating initial rapid or slowed frequency transitions (mirroring the spectrotemporal structure of consonant–vowel–consonant speech syllables). In both studies, typical developing children (average age 10.5 years) and adults, left-hemispheric prefrontal brain regions were activated when comparing rapid with slowed transitions, while the same activation pattern is absent in individuals with DD (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch, et al. 2007). Additionally, preliminary evidence points towards a possible remediation effect, implied by an increase of left prefrontal activity after training (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). Functional MRI studies about auditory processing deficits in DD have furthermore been complemented by electrophysiological evidence. Using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography, differences in spectotemporal auditory processing have been found in children and adults with a diagnosis of DD (Heim et al. 2003a, 2003b).
The extent to which structural and functional brain differences seen in DD are related to the cause or the consequence of the disability itself is uncertain since most previous research has focused on children and adults with years of reading instruction. However, structural and fMRI results from preliterate and young children at familial risk for DD have provided first evidence for structural and functional brain alterations associated with reading and language development, similar to those seen in older children and adults with DD (e.g., Maurer et al. 2009; Specht et al. 2009; Raschle et al. 2011; Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a; Raschle, Zuk, Ortiz-Mantilla et al. 2012b). In the current study, we aim to assess the neuronal basis of RAP in prereading children with a familial risk for DD. We will employ the same nonlinguistic auditory stimuli as previously described in 2 studies of school-aged children and adults with DD (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). We hypothesize that children with a familial risk for DD compared with typically developing controls, already show alterations in left prefrontal brain regions during the processing of rapid compared with slow changes in sounds. Furthermore, we aim to connect previous findings of reduced neuronal activation in preliterate children at familial risk for DD during PP (Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a; Raschle, Zuk, Ortiz-Mantilla et al. 2012b) to the neuronal correlates of RAP.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Twenty-eight healthy, native English-speaking children with a familial risk for DD (FHD+/n = 14; mean age = 69.05 ± 4.98 months) and without a familial risk for DD (FHD−/n = 14; mean age = 67.71 ± 7.04 months) took part in the current study. Twenty-two children are right handed (9 FHD+/13 FHD−), 4 children have not indicated a preference yet (ambidextrous; 3 FHD+/1 FHD−), and 2 children (2 FHD−) are left handed. fMRI analyses were performed with and without inclusion of the 2 left-handed children. However, no difference in outcome was observed. Consequent analyses were thus based on the whole group. Children with a familial risk for DD (FHD+) have at least one first-degree relative with a clinical diagnosis of DD. Those in the control group (no familial risk; FHD−) have no first-degree relative with a clinical diagnosis of DD or reading disability. All participants are part of an ongoing longitudinal study at Boston Children's Hospital which aims to examine behavioral and neural premarkers of DD in preschoolers and beginning readers with and without a familial risk for dyslexia (Boston Longitudinal Study of Dyslexia, BOLD). Participating families are invited each year for 2 visits, 1 behavioral standardized testing session and 1 neuroimaging session for 4 consecutive years starting in preschool. Subjects included in the current article are drawn from year 1 of this longitudinal dataset. None of the children enrolled in this study have a history of neurological or psychological disorder, head injury, poor vision, and poor hearing.
During an initial screening (telephone or email), parents were asked about their child's prereading status. Only children who were not yet reading and whose caregivers planned to have them enter kindergarten in the same year were invited to take part in the study. To further ensure prereading status, the word ID subtest of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Test (Woodcock 1987) was administered to all children. Twenty-one children (11 FHD+/10 FHD−) did not recognize any isolated sight words, 3 children (2 FHD+/1 FHD−) recognized 1 or 2, 3 children (1 FHD+/2 FHD−) recognized between 3 and 5 words and one child recognized 9 words (1 FHD−). All children were tested between May and November of their kindergarten entry year. Group characteristics are in line with similar longitudinal studies on early childhood development showing, for example, that by kindergarten entry only 2% of children in the United States of America are able to identify sight words and only 1% recognizes words in context (Denton et al. 2000; Morris and Bloodgood 2003). This study was approved by the local ethics committee at Boston Children's Hospital. Verbal assent and informed consent were obtained from each child and guardian, respectively.
Behavioral Group Characteristics and Demographics
Children completed standardized assessments examining language and prereading skills such as expressive and receptive vocabulary (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF Preschool 2nd edition; (Semel et al. 1986)), PP (Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP (Wagner et al. 1999)), the Verb Agreement and Tense Test (VATT; (van der Lely 2000)), and RAN (Rapid Automatized Naming Test; (Wolf and Denckla 2005)). Additionally, participating families were given a socioeconomic background questionnaire (questions adapted from the MacArthur Research Network: http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/Default.htm; for a complete overview of socioeconomic status questions, see Supplementary Material 1) and a home literacy questionnaire (based on Denney et al. 2001; as cited in Katzir et al. 2009; see Supplementary Material 2). The 2 groups of children do not significantly differ in gender (FHD+: 3 females/11 males and FHD−: 7 females/7 males), age (mean age during neuroimaging; FHD+: 70.7 months/FHD−: 69.2 months; P = 0.490), nonverbal IQ (KBIT-2; FHD+ mean score: 100.7/FHD− mean score: 100.2; P = 0.893), and socioeconomic background (e.g., parental education or total family income; P < 0.05). However, even though FHD+ and FHD− children do not significantly differ in gender, there are more boys than girls in the group of children at familial risk for DD. Post hoc analyses have been performed to rule out an effect of gender on brain activation.
fMRI—Task Procedure
Prior to neuroimaging, a 45-min preparation session was conducted in a mock scanner area (see also Raschle et al. 2009 and Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a). This session involved extensive training to familiarize each child with the task instructions and stimuli prior to the experiment. The neuroimaging session included a total of 3 fMRI tasks as well as structural image acquisition. Two fMRI experiments are part of the present analysis and further described here. The neuroimaging session lasted about 1.5 h including breaks, however total scan time per child was no more than 40 min maxima. Whole-brain imaging was performed on 28 children during a RAP task. Twenty-three children also completed a PP task (first sound matching). Due to the participants' age, all tasks were divided into 2 runs with a total duration of 5–6 min per run. The order of experiments and runs were pseudo-randomized across participants.
RAP Task
The stimuli and task were adapted from Temple et al. (2000) and have been described previously (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). Experimental stimuli lasting 600 ms were nonlinguistic with a spectrotemporal structure similar to that of consonant–vowel–consonant speech syllables. All stimuli were designed to contain either very rapid frequency changes (within 40 ms) or slowed frequency transitions (extended transition of 200 ms). Stimuli incorporating both rapid and slowed transitions included high (250 Hz F0) and low (125 HZ F0) pitched stimuli. A behavioral interleaved gradient imaging design (Hall et al. 1999; Gaab et al. 2007a, 2007b, 2008) was employed allowing stimuli to be presented without interference from the MR scanner background noise. One single high- or low-pitched sound lasting 600 ms was presented every 2850 ms, while image acquisition accounted for 1995 ms. The 600-ms tones were randomly presented (jittered) within the 855-ms time window. Stimuli were presented in 8 blocks of each type (rapid frequency transition, slowed frequency transition, or rest), with 8 items per block (total of 8 blocks with tones incorporating rapid frequency transition, 8 blocks with slowed frequency transitions and 8 rest blocks). In each block, 50% of the stimuli were high pitched and 50% were low pitched; presented in a randomized order. A 2850-ms cue was used before the start of every experimental or rest block. Participants were asked to indicate the pitch (high/low) of each stimulus by button press. An alien-themed cover story was used to motivate participants and to conduct the experiment in a child-friendly and age-appropriate way (Raschle et al. 2009). During the rest condition, a fixation cross was presented and participants were instructed to stay very still without pressing any buttons.
PP Task
The stimuli and task have been described previously (for details, see Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a). All children listened to 2 consecutively presented common object-words, spoken in a male or female voice, accompanied by corresponding pictures. During the experimental condition (first sound matching; FSM) children indicated via button press whether the first sound of the 2 presented object-words matched. During the control condition (voice matching; VM) participants were to decide whether it was the same voice (same gender) presenting the 2 object-words or not. Experimental and control task were matched with a rest condition (fixation cross). Each trial lasted for 6 s: the 2 object-words were presented for 2 s each, following by a question mark presented for 2 s. This setup allowed for presentation of the 2 words without interference from the MR scanner in a behavioral interleaved gradient design (Hall et al. 1999; Gaab et al. 2007a, 2007b, 2008). A block design was employed to incorporate a total of 7 blocks (4 trials in each block) of experimental and control trials. The whole experiment consisted of 2 separate runs, to accommodate the younger participants, lasting around 5–6 min each.
In-Scanner Performance
Button presses and reaction times (RTs) were recorded during in-scanner performance for all participants. During the RAP task, children were instructed to indicate the pitch (high/low) of the presented stimuli as quickly and accurately as possible after stimulus presentation. Children were allowed to correct their responses until the beginning of the next stimulus presentation (maximum correction time = 2 s; ending at the time of the start of a consecutive trial). To ensure that all participants were engaged in the task, children with more than 25% missed trials were excluded from the imaging analyses. Pitch-identification and RT were compared between children with and without a familial risk for DD using independent sample t-tests using SPSS software. Due to a technical problem, 1 FHD+ and 2 FHD− children had no in-scanner data recorded for RAP. All 3 children were still included in the imaging analyses as their performance during the training session indicated that the tasks were well understood. For the PP task, FSM scores, VM scores, and RT were compared between children with and without a familial risk for DD. Due to a technical problem, 1 FHD+ had no in-scanner data recorded for the PP task and 1 FHD+ child had only data for 1 run (FSM). Both children were included in the imaging analyses as their performance during the training session indicated that the tasks were well understood.
fMRI—Acquisition and Analyses
Each experimental run included the acquisition of 112 functional whole-brain images for the RAP task and 60 for the PP task. Images were acquired with a 32-slice echo planar imaging-interleaved sequence on a SIEMENS 3T Trio MR scanner, including the following specifications: TR 2850 ms (RAP task)/6000 ms (PP task); TA 1995 ms; TE 30 ms; flip angle 90°; field of view 194 mm; voxel size 3 × 3 × 4 mm; slice thickness 4 mm. Before the start of the first block, additional functional images were obtained and later discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects.
Image processing and analyses were carried out using SPM5 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) executed in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). To adjust for movement artifacts within the acquired fMRI time series, we first realigned all images using a least squares approach with reference to the first image (after discarding the first images to allow for T1 equilibration effects). Next, all images were spatially normalized into standard space, as defined by the ICBM, NIH-20 project (Talairach and Tournoux 1998; Ashburner and Friston 2005) and finally smoothed with an 8-mm full-width at half-maximum isotropic kernel to remove noise and effects due to residual differences in functional and structural anatomy during inter-subject averaging (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/doc/spm5_manual.pdf).
Due to the age of the participants, a rigorous procedure for artifact detection was chosen. Particularly, to visualize motion, plot potential movement artifacts and review analysis masks of each subject, we used the art-imaging toolbox (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect). Upon visual inspection of all raw images, the art-imaging toolbox was used to plot differences in motion between consecutive images and to review artifactual time points: First, we identified all images that exceeded a movement threshold of 3 mm and a rotation threshold of 0.05 mm. Then, we visually inspected every image exceeding the said threshold and those images containing artifacts (e.g., missing voxels, stripes, ghosting, or intensity differences) were discarded from further analyses. There were no significant differences in the number of omitted scans per group (P > 0.5). Additionally, the art-imaging toolbox was used to create an explicit mask, excluding the identified artifactual time points, and to save movement regressors. Movement regressors were modeled as cofounds within the general linear model and explicit masking was performed during each subject's first-level analysis to assure inclusion of each voxel of the analysis mask.
The general linear approach in SPM5 was used to analyze the data in a block design for each subject. Contrast images for experimental > control condition (RAP task: “Fast Transition (FT) > Slow Transition (ST)”/PP task: “FSM > VM”) were obtained. Finally, second-level analyses using 1 and 2 sample t-tests were performed in order to examine functional differences during RAP and PP within each group and between children with and without a familial risk for DD. Results are reported at a significance level of P < 0.005, uncorrected; extent threshold of 50 voxels for each group separately and for those regions that showed significantly more activation in FHD− compared with FHD+ children.
Region of Interest Analyses
Two main regions of interest (ROIs) analyses were performed, to (I) assess the relationship between the neural activation during RAP and standardized assessments of PP; and (II) further investigate the relationship between brain activation during both rapid auditory and PP.
ROI Analyses—Part (I)
The goal of this ROI analysis was to examine the relationship between weighted parameter estimates in brain regions observed in the current group of participants and their behavioral prereading scores for PP (CTOPP blending). Functional ROIs were based on the second-level group comparison (FHD+<FHD−) during RAP (FT>ST). The mean parameter estimates during RAP in left-hemispheric prefrontal ROI were extracted from each participant's first-level analysis and correlated with their prereading measures (PP based on CTOPP blending).
ROI Analyses—Part (II)
To further examine the relationship between neuronal activation during RAP and neuronal activation PP, we performed a separate ROI analysis using independent anatomical ROIs, 1 set for each task (named RAP ROIs and PP ROIs). Two studies comparing children and adults with and without a diagnosis of DD, using the same RAP task employed here, have demonstrated the involvement of left inferior frontal brain regions during RAP (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). Therefore, we defined 2 left-hemispheric frontal brain ROIs (RAP ROIs BA9 and 46: left middle/superior frontal gyri) using the Wake Forest University (WFU) PickAtlas toolbox (Maldjian et al. 2003; Maldjian et al. 2004) in SPM5. The mean parameter estimates during RAP (FT>ST) were extracted from the first-level T-contrast of each participant. In a second step, PP ROIs were defined based on ample evidence of neuronal dysfunction (hypoactivation) in left temporoparietal and occipitotemporal brain regions during reading and reading-related tasks in individuals with DD (for reviews, see McCandliss and Noble 2003; Schlaggar and McCandliss 2007; Gabrieli 2009). Therefore, 3 left-hemispheric posterior ROIs were defined (PP ROIs BA37, BA40, and a BA41/42/22: occipitotemporal and parietotemporal) using the WFU PickAtlas toolbox (Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft et al. 2003; Maldjian, Laurienti and Burdette 2004) in SPM5. The mean parameter estimates were extracted from the first-level T-contrast of our PP experiment (FSM>VM). Correlational analyses were then used to relate mean parameter estimates within the 2 RAP and the 3 PP ROIs using SPSS software package, version 19.0 (SPSS, Inc. (1999) SPSS Base 10.0 for Windows User's Guide. SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
Results
Demographics and Behavioral Group Characteristics
Demographics and behavioral group characteristics for all 28 participants are provided in Table 1. Children with a familial risk for DD (FHD+) scored significantly lower than children without a familial risk for DD (FHD−) on standardized assessments of expressive language skills (CELF Expressive Language (t(26) = −2.119; P = 0.044)) and RAN (t(25) = −3.313; P = 0.003). No differences were observed in age (age at imaging session, t(26) = 0.700; P = 0.490/age at psychometric session, t(26) = 0.580; P = 0.567), verbal (t(26) = 0.266; P = 0.792) or nonverbal IQ (t(26) = −0.279; P = 0.783), or socioeconomic status (e.g., parental education or income, P > 0.05; Table 1).
Table 1.
FHD+ | FHD− |
P-values Sig. 2-tailed |
|
---|---|---|---|
Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | FHD+ vs. FHD− | |
n | 14 | 14 | |
Age (in months/psychometrics session) | 69.05 ± 4.98 | 67.71 ± 7.04 | 0.567 |
Age (in months/imaging session) | 70.69 ± 4.76 | 69.16 ± 6.64 | 0.490 |
Core language | 104.71 ± 10.43 | 111.00 ± 9.78 | 0.112 |
Receptive language | 106.14 ± 13.83 | 109.86 ± 11.66 | 0.449 |
Expressive language | 101.86 ± 11.11 | 110.71 ± 11.01 | 0.044* |
Language contenta | 101.31 ± 10.79 | 108.67 ± 11.22 | 0.108 |
Language structure | 105.14 ± 11.99 | 111.14 ± 9.81 | 0.159 |
CTOPP | |||
Elision | 9.14 ± 1.88 | 10.64 ± 2.68 | 0.098 |
Blendingb | 10.46 ± 1.90 | 11.36 ± 1.39 | 0.172 |
Nonword repetitionb | 9.54 ± 2.47 | 9.86 ± 2.25 | 0.729 |
RAN | |||
Objectsc | 89.93 ± 11.27 | 104.46 ± 11.52 | 0.003** |
VATT | |||
Inflectiond | 27.00 ± 5.05 | 24.00 ± 9.87 | 0.368 |
Repetitiond | 36.00 ± 4.02 | 38.60 ± 1.26 | 0.064 |
KBIT | |||
Verbal abilityc | 111.5 ± 9.64 | 110.43 ± 11.56 | 0.792 |
Nonverbal abilityc | 100.71 ± 11.43 | 101.93 ± 11.64 | 0.783 |
Mean ± SD | Mean ± SD | Sig. 2-tailed (independent samples t-test) | |
In-scanner performance (raw scores; maxima = 128) | |||
RAPa(pitch-discrimination) | |||
Correct | 86.46 ± 21.93 | 99.17 ± 25.86 | 0.197 |
Incorrect | 28.61 ± 14.96 | 21.00 ± 23.61 | 0.341 |
RT (ms) | 1125 ± 212.34 | 1028.97 ± 184.70 | 0.238 |
Mean rank | Mean rank | Sig. 2-tailed (Mann–Whitney) | |
Socioeconomic status parental educatione | 11.59 | 14.11 | 0.387 |
Income (total family income for last 12 months)f | 12.79 | 13.19 | 0.882 |
Note:
Measures (standard scores are reported).
a13 FHD+/12 FHD− (3 children did not finish all testing).
b13 FHD+/14 FHD− (1 child did not finish all testing).
c14 FHD+/13 FHD− (1 child did not finish all testing).
d12 FHD+/10 FHD− (6 children did not finish all testing).
eParental Education scores are calculated according to the 7-point Hollingshead Index Educational Factor Scale, summed for husband and wife and divided by 2.
fScale where 1 = 0–5000$, 2 = 5000–11 999$, 3 = 12 000–15 999$, 4 = 16 000–24 999$, 5 = 25 000–34 999$, 6 = 35 000–49 900$, 7 = 50 000–74 999$, 8 = 75 000–99 999$, 9 = 100E000+ $, 10 = do not know, 11 = no response.
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; 2-tailed t-test; all other t-tests nonsignificant at threshold of P = 0.05.
In-Scanner Performance—Results
RAP Task
There were no differences in pitch-discrimination (P = 0.197) or RT (P = 0.238) between children with or without a familial risk for DD (FHD+ mean raw score for pitch-discrimination: 86.46 ± 21.93, RT = 1126 ms/FHD− mean raw score for pitch-discrimination: 99.16 ± 25.90, RT = 1029 ms; Table 1).
Phonological Processing Task
Children with a familial risk for DD (FHD+; mean = 16.60 [Nmax = 28]) were significantly less accurate on FSM than children without a familial risk (FHD−; mean = 21.83; P = 0.015). There was no performance difference on VM and RT did not differ between groups on either experimental or control task (P > 0.05).
fMRI Results
RAP Task
Whole-brain analysis revealed 2 brain regions in children without a familial risk for DD that were more active during rapid compared with slowed auditory processing (FT > ST; Table 2, Fig. 1b). These regions included the inferior/middle frontal and precentral/middle frontal gyrus. Children with a familial risk for DD showed no difference in brain activation during the processing of rapid compared with slowed stimuli (Table 2, Fig. 1a). A direct comparison between children with and without a familial risk for DD (FHD+<FHD−) during blocks of fast compared with slow stimuli (FT>ST) revealed differences in left-hemispheric frontal brain areas (superior/medial, inferior/middle and precentral/middle gyrus) as well as in the left cerebellum/fusiform gyrus and right precentral/middle frontal gyrus (Table 2, Fig. 1c). The opposite contrast (FHD+>FHD−) did not yield any significant voxels. Furthermore, to rule out gender effects, we performed a ROI analysis for the neuronal activation in the left prefrontal ROI (inferior/middle, superior/medial, and precentral/middle frontal gyrus) for males only. Results (FHD−>FHD+) reveal significant differences in neuronal activation during RAP in the inferior/middle frontal and precentral/middle frontal gyrus as previously reported in the mixed group.
Table 2.
Region | Brodmann area | x | y | z | Z | Size, voxels |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Prereading children without a familial risk for dyslexia (FHD−/n = 14) | ||||||
Frontal lobe | ||||||
Inferior/middle frontal gyrus (L) | 9 | −52 | 12 | 32 | 3.95 | 52 |
Precentral/middle frontal gyrus (L) | 3/4/6 | −50 | −12 | 48 | 3.90 | 145 |
Prereading children with a familial risk for dyslexia (FHD+/n = 14) | ||||||
No brain activation at P = 0.005, uc (k = 50) | ||||||
Group difference (children with a familial risk for dyslexia < children without a familial risk) | ||||||
Frontal lobe | ||||||
Superior/medial frontal gyrus (L) | 9/10 | −2 | 58 | 20 | 3.82 | 147 |
Inferior/middle frontal gyrus (L) | 9/45/46 | −54 | 12 | 30 | 3.97 | 136 |
Precentral/middle frontal gyrus (L) | 9/45/46 | −44 | −8 | 50 | 3.48 | 93 |
Precentral/middle frontal gyrus (R) | 6 | 22 | −18 | 64 | 3.25 | 51 |
Other | ||||||
Cerebellum/fusiform gyrus (L) | 19 | −24 | −82 | −26 | 3.64 | 65 |
PP Task
In a smaller group of 23 children (10 FHD+/13 FHD−), previous findings of hypoactivations in children with, compared with without, a familial risk for DD in left-hemispheric posterior reading networks were confirmed (Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a). Group differences indicate a disrupted neural response during PP in FHD+ children within left middle occipital gyrus/cuneus (x = −18, y = −92, z = 8) and left superior temporal gyrus (x = −26, y = −58, z = 16). Figure 1d incorporates the results of group differences (FHD−>FHD+) in neuronal activation during both, rapid auditory (in red) and PP (in green). These tasks have been conducted in randomized sequential order, but are both rendered on the same brain for displaying purposes.
Region of Interest Analyses—Results
ROI Analyses—Part (I)
To assess the relationship of neuronal activity during RAP and standardized behavioral assessments of PP, mean parameter estimates were extracted for RAP (FT>ST) based on ROIs defined by our second-level group differences (FHD+<FHD−). Neuronal activation within left-hemispheric prefrontal ROIs was correlated with standardized assessments of PP (CTOPP blending). Table 3 gives an overview of the results and demonstrates that neuronal activation during RAP in left precentral/middle frontal gyrus positively correlates with phonological skills (P = 0.007).
Table 3.
Pearson correlations between RAP and phonological processing (CTOPP blending) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|
RAP ROIs (left hemisphere) |
||||
Superior/medial frontal gyrus (P-values) | Inferior/middle frontal gyrus (P-values) | Precentral/middle frontal gyrus (P-values) | ||
Phonological processing | CTOPP blending | −0.049 (0.807) | 0.045 (0.825) | 0.508* (0.007) |
Note: *Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
ROI Analyses—Part (II)
To further assess the neuronal relationship between RAP and PP, additional ROI analyses were conducted. Two independent left-hemispheric RAP ROIs and 3 independent occipitotemporal and parietotemporal PP ROIs were used to extract mean parameter estimates for RAP (FT>ST) and PP (FSM>VM), respectively. The neuronal activation during RAP within specified RAP ROIs was then compared with neuronal activation during PP within specified PP ROIs through correlational analysis. Within the whole group of participants, neuronal activation during RAP in left middle frontal gyrus (BA9) positively correlated with neuronal activation during PP in parietotemporal (BA22/41/42; P = 0.038) and occipitotemporal areas of the brain (BA37; P = 0.005; see Table 4).
Table 4.
Pearson correlations between RAP and PP ROI |
|||
---|---|---|---|
RAP ROIs (left hemisphere) | |||
Brodmann area 9 (P-values) | Brodmann area 46 (P-values) | ||
PP ROIs (left hemisphere) | Brodmann area 22/41/42 | 0.435* (0.038) | 0.174 (0.426) |
Brodmann area 40 | 0.243 (0.264) | 0.225 (0.303) | |
Brodmann area 37 | 0.561** (0.005) | 0.207 (0.343) |
Note: *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
Discussion
The presented results demonstrate that prereading children with a familial risk for DD already show a neuronal disruption of left prefrontal brain regions during rapid spectrotemporal processing similar to that seen in older children and adults with a diagnosis of DD (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). This atypical activation pattern was observed despite the covert nature of the task employed; participants were asked to indicate the pitch of the tones, but neuronal response to blocks of tones with altered initial transitions (rapid or slowed) was measured. Furthermore, neuronal activation during RAP within left prefrontal brain regions positively correlates with behavioral standardized PP skills. We here also confirm previous findings of a disrupted neural response to PP in left-hemispheric posterior reading networks in prereading children with a familial risk for DD in a subsample of children published in Raschle, Zuk, Gaab (2012a). Correlational analyses indicate a link between neuronal activation patterns during rapid auditory and neuronal activation patterns during PP. Finally, children at familial risk for DD score significantly lower on standardized tests of expressive language skills and RAN. No differences in IQ, home literacy environment, or socioeconomic status were observed. In line with our previous publications (Raschle et al. 2011; Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a), we suggest that behavioral and neuronal differences characteristic of individuals with DD may already be present at birth or develop within the first few years of life. Furthermore, neuronal activation in prefrontal brain regions during RAP seems to be associated with neuronal activation during PP in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions. Since all children in the present study were prereaders at the time of testing, the observed differences cannot be due to any effects related to reading instruction or reading failure per se.
Our results demonstrate an early neuronal disruption in prefrontal brain regions during RAP in prereading children at risk for DD. Various neuroimaging studies have implicated left prefrontal brain regions during language and auditory processing tasks in typical reading children and adults (e.g., Gabrieli et al. 1998; Price 1998; Pugh et al. 2001, 2012), when comparing those with reading disabilities to typical readers (Cao et al. 2006; Hoeft et al. 2006; Booth et al. 2007; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch, et al. 2007; Kovelman et al. 2011) or good with poor beginning readers (Bach et al. 2010). Along with the basal ganglia, the left dorsal inferior frontal gyrus has been described as part of the anterior reading circuit associated with higher-level phonological recoding in mature readers (Pugh et al. 2001; Booth et al. 2007). Neuronal activation within the left prefrontal brain region has been described during PP or awareness (Devlin et al. 2003; Kovelman et al. 2011), letter substitution tasks (Bach et al. 2010), RAP (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007), phoneme memory tasks (Beneventi et al. 2010), and semantic analysis (Gabrieli et al. 1998). Studies with and without participants with reading disabilities have specifically implicated left dorsolateral prefrontal brain regions during the processing of transient acoustic features, such as the manipulation of rapidly changing speech and nonspeech sounds (Belin et al. 1998; Temple et al. 2000; Poldrack et al. 2001; Temple 2002; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). For example, by using the exact same stimuli as in the current publication, a disrupted response to rapid acoustic stimuli has been demonstrated in children and adults with DD, when compared with typical reading controls (Temple et al. 2000; Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). Interestingly, the neuronal alterations seen in children with DD was shown to be partly ameliorated through remediation and led to improved language and reading abilities (Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007).
Furthermore, we here demonstrate a link between the neuronal activation during RAP in preliterate children at risk for DD and behavioral assessments of prereading skills. The mean parameter estimates during RAP positively correlates with PP skills (CTOPP blending). Behavioral studies investigating RAP and PP abilities have both shown to predict later language skills and development (Juel 1988; Scarborough et al. 1991; Benaisch and Tallal 2002; Choudhury et al. 2007). Our findings could indicate that RAP skills may be involved in the development of prereading skills, such as PP abilities but a causal conclusion cannot be drawn without longitudinal analyses. However, this would be in line with ample behavioral evidence implicating auditory processing difficulties in developmental language disorders (e.g., Tallal and Piercy 1973; Elliott et al. 1989; Wright et al. 1997). However, it is notable that there have also been ample findings that failed to replicate an association between rapid auditory and PP deficits or have not replicated auditory deficits in DD (France et al. 2002; Breier et al. 2003; Georgiou et al. 2010; Willburger and Landerl 2010). Again, other studies have found a link between rapid auditory and PP in DD, but only in a subset of children or adults with reading disabilities (Ramus 2003; Gibson et al. 2006). However, it is important to note that there were no significant differences in PP between the 2 groups and significant group differences were only observed for RAN and expressive language skills. Interestingly, mean standardized scores for RAN in FHD+ as a group were below the mean of the norming sample (mean: 89.93 ± 11.27 for FHD+) whereas standardized scores for expressive language were right at the mean of the norming sample (mean: 101.86 ± 11.11 for FHD+). This raises the question whether the FHD+ children can be considered at risk for DD based on their behavioral scores which is important for the interpretation of our results. It has been suggested that phonological awareness and naming speed variables contribute uniquely to different aspects of reading (Wolf and Bowers 1999) suggesting the presence of 2 single-deficit and 1 double-deficit subtype with more pervasive and severe impairments in both PP and naming speed. Furthermore, several studies have shown that RAN is one of the key predictors of reading disability in preschool (Badian 1994; Puolakanaho et al. 2007; 2008) and in one of our previous studies, we could show that it positively correlates with gray matter indices in left temporoparietal and occipital-temporal regions prior to reading onset. It remains unclear which of the children here studied will receive a diagnosis of DD in elementary school and whether these children (and how many) will show a single-deficit in RAN, as decribed by Wolf and Bowers (1999), or not but our current results in a relative small sample suggets that FHD+ children with an isolated RAN deficit in preschool also show the characteristic brain deficits in posterior temporoparietal and occipitotemporal regions during PP and left prefrontal regions during RAP. Furthermore, the groups differ in expressive language scores and deficits in expressive language skills have been show to be a predictor of later reading disability (e.g., Scarborough 1990, 1998; Stanovich and Siegel 1994). However, we do not think that our FHD+ group can be considered impaired in expressive language or even qualify for a diagnosis of specific language imapirment at this point, but our longitudinal study design will allow us to observe the developmental trajectories of expressive language oever time and how it relates to brain activation in the observed key regions.
During PP, similar activation patterns as previously described in Raschle, Zuk, Gaab (2012a) were observed in our smaller sample which consists of 19 children from Raschle, Zuk, Gaab (2012a) and 4 new children (3 FHD+/1 FHD−). Neuroimaging data implicates a left-hemispheric specialized reading network in older children and adults (Pugh et al. 2001), which is disrupted in individuals with DD (Temple et al. 2001; Shaywitz et al. 2002; Maurer et al. 2007; Blau et al. 2010). Our findings are in line with research suggesting an early specialization of the reading network in young children (Gaillard et al. 2003; Vaessen and Blomert 2010) and a disruption of its main components in children at risk for DD (even preliterate; e.g., Simos et al. 2000; Maurer et al. 2007, 2009; Specht et al. 2009; Brem et al. 2010; Raschle et al. 2011; Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a), similar to adult studies in DD.
It has been shown previously that regions within the left inferior frontal cortex of the brain maybe similarly sensitive to transient measures of acoustic features of speech and those requiring PP abilities (Poldrack et al. 2001). However, it is to note that a direct involvement of left prefrontal brain regions during PP (neuronal activation in left prefrontal cortex during PP) was not seen in the current sample or within a previously published group of children with or without a familial risk for dyslexia (Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a). This may be explained by various findings of a developmental component on neuronal activation within inferior frontal brain regions during reading-related task, observable by activation increases in this region with age (Turkeltaub et al. 2003; Brown et al. 2005; Bitan, Cheon, Lu, Burman and Booth 2007; Bitan, Cheon, Lu, Burman, Gitelman et al. 2009). The young age of the participants studied here, may thus explain the missing involvement of the left prefrontal cortex during PP. Enhanced left inferior frontal gyrus activation during PP in adults with DD compared with controls is oftentimes interpreted representing as compensatory mechanisms in individuals who struggle to read. Compensation is hereby reflected by greater reliance on articulatory processes when PP is disrupted (e.g., Shaywitz et al. 1998; Brunswick et al. 1999; MacSweeney et al. 2009; Richlan et al. 2009), leading to overactivation in DD compared with typical reading subjects in left inferior frontal gyrus. However, all the children in the current sample were still preliterate at the time of testing and compensatory mechanisms are unlikely in place yet. Our findings may reflect an early engagement of left inferior frontal brain regions during RAP, while the importance of this region for PP tasks is not yet developed in preliterate children, independent of familial risk for reading disabilities. We thus suggest that within the left prefrontal cortex the basic auditory mechanisms for processing rapid spectrotemporal features of sounds are already developed in preliterate typically developing children, but dysfunctional in preliterate children at risk for dyslexia. For both, children with and without a familial risk for reading failure, this brain region is not yet employed in prereading tasks, such as PP. Longitudinal studies integrating neuroimaging and behavioral findings, ideally from a very young age on, will be needed to investigate how this brain region develops and to see whether phonological and RAP are independent components or not.
A Link Between Neuronal Activation During Rapid Auditory and Phonological Processing?
The current findings may indicate a potential link between the neural systems during phonological and RAP in the prereading brain. The correlational analysis demonstrated a link between neuronal activation during RAP in left prefrontal brain regions and neuronal activation during PP in posterior parietotemporal and occipitotemporal areas of the brain. While the prefrontal cortex has been implicated during higher level phonological recoding, the posterior dorsal and ventral brain regions are especially linked to graphem-phoneme mapping, letter identification and fluent reading (Pugh et al. 2000). In particular, the left occipitotemporal brain area has been suggested to be seat of the visual word form area, a brain region critical for visual word processing (McCandliss et al. 2003). The importance of this brain region in the prereading brain has been implicated by various neuroimaging studies (e.g., van Atteveldt et al. 2004; Maurer et al. 2007; Specht et al. 2009; Brem et al. 2010). For example, Brem et al. (2010) observed that occipitotemporal print sensitivity develops during the earliest phase of reading acquisition in childhood, suggesting that a crucial part of the later reading network first adopts a role in mapping print and sound (Brem et al. 2010). Parietotemporal brain regions have been found to be particularly crucial for the integration of letter and speech sounds (van Atteveldt et al. 2004) and are activated during neuroimaging tasks of reading (for reviews, see Pugh et al. 2001; Schlaggar and McCandliss 2007). We here demonstrated a correlation between the neuronal activation during RAP in left prefrontal brain regions and PP in posterior areas of the brain. These findings may be interpreted as initial evidence that RAP and phonological abilities required to learn to read is influential to each other during reading acquisition. Because reading acquisition is highly dependent on fine-grained auditory processing skills, it has been reiterated in the literature that improving the neural response to sound processing is likely linked to enhanced reading skills (for reviews, see, e.g., Hornickel et al. 2012; Tallal 2012).
However, some precautions need to be noted. Our findings are in line with results by Pugh et al. (2012) who observed shared brain pathways and thus a link between temporal auditory and PP and underline the importance between sound processing and reading acquisition (Hornickel et al. 2012; Tallal 2012). However, in agreement with Pugh et al. (2012) and others before (Ramus et al. 2006), the current results cannot be interpreted as a causal relationship between rapid auditory or PP. The differences seen in this group of preliterate children may be simply explained by common cortical and subcortical networks that are less optimally organized in children and adults with reading disabilities or young children at risk for such (Pugh et al. 2012; Ramus et al. 2006). Longitudinal studies integrating neuroimaging and behavioral findings, ideally from a very young age on, will be needed.
The Many Faces of DD
Our current and previous findings speak for a range of functional (Raschle, Zuk, Gaab 2012a) and structural (Raschle et al. 2011) alterations, which are already observed in prereading children with a familial risk for DD. DD is a language-based learning disability with a core deficit in PP. However, DD is often accompanied by various perceptual deficits, including those involving visual (Eden, VanMeter, Rumsey, Maisog et al. 1996; Eden, VanMeter, Rumsey, Zeffiro 1996; Grinter et al. 2010; Lipowska et al. 2011), auditory (Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007; Stefanics et al. 2011), and motor abilities (Stoodley et al. 2006; Brookes et al. 2010). Auditory processing deficits are among the most commonly observed deficits in DD next to PP issues. For example, by reviewing previous studies including individual subject data, Ramus et al. (2003) concluded that 39% of individuals with dyslexia also displayed an auditory deficit. But, even though auditory impairments are often observed in individuals with DD, they are not present in every individual with a clinical diagnosis of DD. Due to the lack of auditory processing impairment in some individuals with DD it has been argued that the auditory processing deficits cannot be causal to the disability itself (White et al. 2006). These and similar findings have driven the idea of different subtypes of DD (e.g., Heim et al. 2008), covering the wide range of individuals with and without auditory processing difficulties or similar sensorimotor challenges. Our results may be interpreted as evidence for the presence of neuronal deficits of rapid auditory and PP in prereading children at risk for DD and may suggest a connection of these. However, without a continuing investigation using longitudinal designs, it cannot yet answer the question about the causality of either one of these deficits in shaping the development in reading failure.
By investigating young children with a familial risk for DD, our study results offer a chance to better understand neural premarkers of DD. To date, there is a line of research investigating early neuroimaging markers of later reading ability. Most of this research derives from electrophysiological studies, using event-related potential measures to improve our understanding of reading development (e.g., Molfese, Molfese and Modgline 2001; Molfese, Modglin and Molfese 2003; Maurer, Bucher, Brem and Brandeis 2003; Maurer, Bucher, Brem, Benz et al. 2009; Guttorm, Leppanen, Poikkeus et al. 2005; Guttorm, Leppanen, Hamalainen et al. 2010). However, some studies have successfully begun to incorporate the use of (f)MRI for the means of predicting reading outcome in developmental samples with and without familial risk for DD (e.g., Specht et al. 2009; Hoeft et al. 2011). For example, Hoeft et al. (2011) conducted a prospective longitudinal study in older children aged 11–14 years over the course of 2.5 years to examine the potential of fMRI or diffusion tensor imaging to predict reading improvement in DD (Hoeft et al. 2011). Initial evidence suggests that a combination of neurophysiological and behavioral measures may increase the accuracy of prediction over a single measure alone (Maurer et al. 2009; Hoeft et al. 2011). It remains to be investigated, whether the present findings of left prefrontal hypoactivations in prereading children at risk for DD may be used for the early identification of children at risk for DD and whether prereading children may already benefit from remediation as shown in 10-year-old children (Gaab, Gabrieli, Deutsch et al. 2007). An early identification of children at risk for developmental disabilities, such as DD, is crucial for the development, evaluation, and implementation of early remediation programs. Overall, an early identification and remediation of reading disabilities may reduce social, psychological, and clinical challenges associated with the progress of developmental disabilities (McNorgan et al. 2011).
Even though our data are evidence for the presence of neuronal deficits of rapid auditory and PP in prereading children at risk for DD and suggest a connection of these, there are some important limitations to note. It has been reported that 30–64% of children with a parent or first-degree relative with reading difficulties will develop difficulties themselves (Gilger et al. 1992; Schulte-Korne et al. 1996; Pennington and Lefly 2001). We cannot be certain about whom exactly or how many participants, will develop a reading disability ultimately and/or receive a clinical diagnosis of DD. Follow-up and large-scale longitudinal studies will be required to assess these questions further. However, findings of various neuronal and behavioral alterations in children with a familial risk for DD compared with typically developing controls fit the idea of a more comprehensive model of DD (e.g., Goswami 2011). Another potential caveat is the fact that previous studies indicate that not all individuals with DD do present difficulties in RAP (Ramus 2003; Gibson et al. 2006; Georgiou et al. 2010; Willburger and Landerl 2010). Longitudinal designs and follow-up assessments on the children tested here may shed more light on these questions.
Furthermore, it is important to note that our results may have been influenced by environmental variables such as home literacy or socioeconomic status. Although there are no significant differences observed in these variables between the groups in our current sample, there are some marginal trends suggesting for instance that the quality of the home literacy environment in the FHD− group may be slightly better than in the FHD+ group. It remains unclear whether this has any influence on brain activation in the key regions observed in the current study but future studies need to investigate the relationship between environmental variables important for reading development and neural deficits characteristic for DD.
Conclusion
In this article, we demonstrate differences in rapid auditory and PP in prereading children with, compared with without, a familial risk of DD and offer initial evidence for a potential link between rapid auditory and PP skills prior to reading acquisition. The current study is a first step toward broadening our knowledge about the neural phenotype, and thus core characteristics, of preliterate children at risk for DD. Future studies employing longitudinal designs should be used to investigate the developmental trajectories of the neural disruption in DD and to determine whether these markers may be used for early identification of children at risk for DD. The identification of very young children and/or infants at risk for reading disability coupled with the onset of early remediation may induce more beneficial maturational trajectories (Dekker and Karmiloff-Smith 2011).
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material can be found at: http://www.cercor.oxfordjournals.org/.
Funding
This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (1R01HD065762-01/02 to N.G.); Charles H. Hood Foundation (to N.G.); Boston Children's Hospital Pilot Grant (to N.G.); the Swiss National Foundation (to N.M.R); and the Janggen-Pöhn Stiftung (to N.M.R.).
Supplementary Material
Notes
We thank all participating families. Conflict of Interest: None declared.
References
- Abrams DA, Nicol T, Zecker SG, Kraus N. Auditory brainstem timing predicts cerebral asymmetry for speech. J Neurosci. 2006;26:11131–11137. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2744-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. NeuroImage. 2005;26:839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bach S, Brandeis D, Hofstetter C, Martin E, Richardson U, Brem S. Early emergence of deviant frontal fMRI activity for phonological processes in poor beginning readers. NeuroImage. 2010;53:682–693. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badian NA. Preschool prediction: orthographic and phonological skills, and reading. Ann Dyslexia. 1994;44:3–25. doi: 10.1007/BF02648153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banai K, Hornickel J, Skoe E, Nicol T, Zecker S, Kraus N. Reading and subcortical auditory function. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2699–2707. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beitchman JH, Nair R, Clegg M, Ferguson B, Patel PG. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in children with speech and language disorders. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1986;25:528–535. doi: 10.1016/s0002-7138(10)60013-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belin P, Zilbovicius M, Crozier S, Thivard L, Fontaine A, Masure MC, Samson Y. Lateralization of speech and auditory temporal processing. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:536–540. doi: 10.1162/089892998562834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benasich AA, Choudhury N, Friedman JT, Realpe-Bonilla T, Chojnowska C, Gou Z. The infant as a prelinguistic model for language learning impairments: predicting from event-related potentials to behavior. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:396–411. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benaisch AA, Tallal P. Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts later language impairment. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:31–49. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00098-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beneventi H, Tonnessen FE, Ersland L, Hugdahl K. Executive working memory processes in dyslexia: behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scand J Psychol. 2010;51:192–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00808.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bitan T, Cheon J, Lu D, Burman DD, Booth JR. Developmental increase in top-down and bottom-up processing in a phonological task: an effective connectivity, fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:1135–1145. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bitan T, Cheon J, Lu D, Burman DD, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Booth JR. Developmental changes in activation and effective connectivity in phonological processing. NeuroImage. 2007;38:564–575. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blau V, Reithler J, van Atteveldt N, Seitz J, Gerretsen P, Goebel R, Blomert L. Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain. 2010;133:868–879. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Booth JR, Bebko G, Burman DD, Bitan T. Children with reading disorder show modality independent brain abnormalities during semantic tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:775–783. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breier JI, Fletcher JM, Foorman BR, Klaas P, Gray LC. Auditory temporal processing in children with specific reading disability with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46:31–42. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brem S, Bach S, Kucian K, Guttorm TK, Martin E, Lyytinen H, Brandeis D, Richardson U. Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter-speech sound correspondences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:7939–7944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904402107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- British Dyslexia Association. What is dyslexia? 2012. In: Stein J, Saunders K, editors. Oxford: The Dyslexia Handbook. p. 1–296. Available from: URL http://www.bdadyslexia.org.uk/about-dyslexia/further-information/dyslexia-research-information-.html .
- Brookes RL, Tinkler S, Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Striking the right balance: motor difficulties in children and adults with dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2010;16:358–373. doi: 10.1002/dys.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TT, Lugar HM, Coalson RS, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:275–290. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunswick N, McCrory E, Price CJ, Frith CD, Frith U. Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: a search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? Brain. 1999;122(10):1901–1917. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.10.1901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao F, Bitan T, Chou TL, Burman DD, Booth JR. Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:1041–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01684.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childs B, Finucci JM. Genetics, epidemiology, and specific reading disability. New York: Guilford; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Choudhury N, Leppanen PH, Leevers HJ, Benasich AA. Infant information processing and family history of specific language impairment: converging evidence for RAP deficits from two paradigms. Dev Sci. 2007;10:213–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00546.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dekker TM, Karmiloff-Smith A. The dynamics of ontogeny: a neuroconstructivist perspective on genes, brains, cognition and behavior. Prog Brain Res. 2011;189:23–33. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53884-0.00016-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demonet JF, Taylor MJ, Chaix Y. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet. 2004;363:1451–1460. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16106-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denney MK, English JP, Gerber M, Leafstedt J, Rutz M. Family and home literacy practices: mediating factors for preliterate English learners at risk. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Associations; Seattle WA. 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Denton K, Germino-Hausken E, West J. U.S. Department of Education. National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, D.C: America's Kindergarteners, NCES 2000–2007; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Devlin JT, Matthews PM, Rushworth MF. Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15:71–84. doi: 10.1162/089892903321107837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diaz B, Hintz F, Kiebel SJ, von Kriegstein K. Dysfunction of the auditory thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:13841–13846. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119828109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckert MA, Leonard CM, Wilke M, Eckert M, Richards T, Richards A, Berninger V. Anatomical signatures of dyslexia in children: unique information from manual and voxel based morphometry brain measures. Cortex. 2005;41:304–315. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70268-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eden GF, VanMeter JW, Rumsey JM, Maisog JM, Woods RP, Zeffiro TA. Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature. 1996;382:66–69. doi: 10.1038/382066a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eden GF, VanMeter JW, Rumsey JM, Zeffiro TA. The visual deficit theory of developmental dyslexia. Neuroimage. 1996;4:S108–S117. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eklund KM, Torppa M, Lyytinen H. Predicting reading disability: early cognitive risk and protective factors. Dyslexia. 2013;19(1):1–10. doi: 10.1002/dys.1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elbro C, Borstrøm I, Petersen DK. Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: the importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Res Q. 1998;33:36–60. [Google Scholar]
- Elliott LL, Hammer MA, Scholl ME, Carrell TD, Wasowicz JM. Discrimination of rising and falling simulated single-formant frequency transitions: practice and transition duration effects. J Acoust Soc Am. 1989;86:945–953. doi: 10.1121/1.398729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felton R, Naylor C, Wood F. Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain Lang. 1990;39:485–487. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(90)90157-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrer E, Shaywitz BA, Holahan JM, Marchione K, Shaywitz SE. Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(1):93–101. doi: 10.1177/0956797609354084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitch RH, Tallal P, Brown CP, Galaburda AM, Rosen GD. Induced microgyria and auditory temporal processing in rats: a model for language impairment? Cereb Cortex. 1994;4:260–270. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.3.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flowers DL. Neuropsychological profiles of persistent reading disability and reading improvement. In: Joshi RM, Leong CK, editors. Developmental and aquired dyslexia: neuropsychological and neurolinguistic perspectives. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- France SJ, Rosner BS, Hansen PC, Calvin C, Talcott JB, Richardson AJ, Stein JF. Auditory frequency discrimination in adult developmental dyslexics. Percept Psychophys. 2002;64:169–179. doi: 10.3758/bf03195783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaab N, Gabrieli JD, Deutsch GK, Tallal P, Temple E. Neural correlates of rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dyslexia and ameliorated with training: an fMRI study. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2007;25:295–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaab N, Gabrieli JD, Glover GH. Assessing the influence of scanner background noise on auditory processing. I. An fMRI study comparing three experimental designs with varying degrees of scanner noise. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007a;28:703–720. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaab N, Gabrieli JD, Glover GH. Assessing the influence of scanner background noise on auditory processing. II. An fMRI study comparing auditory processing in the absence and presence of recorded scanner noise using a sparse design. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007b;28:721–732. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaab N, Gabrieli JD, Glover GH. Resting in peace or noise: scanner background noise suppresses default-mode network. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:858–867. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabrieli JD. Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science. 2009;325:280–283. doi: 10.1126/science.1171999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabrieli JD, Poldrack RA, Desmond JE. The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:906–913. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaillard WD, Balsamo LM, Ibrahim Z, Sachs BC, Xu B. fMRI identifies regional specialization of neural networks for reading in young children. Neurology. 2003;60:94–100. doi: 10.1212/wnl.60.1.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galaburda A, LoTurco J, Ramus F, Fitch RH, Rosen GD. From genes to behavior in developmental dyslexia. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1213–1217. doi: 10.1038/nn1772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher A, Frith U, Snowling MJ. Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:203–213. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georgiou GK, Protopapas A, Papadopoulos TC, Skaloumbakas C, Parrila R. Auditory temporal processing and dyslexia in an orthographically consistent language. Cortex. 2010;46:1330–1344. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson CJ, Gruen JR. The human lexinome: genes of language and reading. J Commun Disord. 2008;41:409–420. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson LY, Hogben JH, Fletcher J. Visual and auditory processing and component reading skills in developmental dyslexia. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2006;23:621–642. doi: 10.1080/02643290500412545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilger JW, Pennington BF, Green P, Smith SM, Smith SD. Reading disability, immune disorders and non-right-handedness: twin and family studies of their relations. Neuropsychologia. 1992;30:209–227. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90001-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U. Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. Dyslexia. 2000;6:133–151. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909(200004/06)6:2<133::AID-DYS160>3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U. A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U, Fosker T, Huss M, Mead N, Szucs D. Rise time and formant transition duration in the discrimination of speech sounds: the Ba-Wa distinction in developmental dyslexia. Dev Sci. 2011;14:34–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00955.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U, Thomson J, Richardson U, Stainthorp R, Hughes D, Rosen S, Scott SK. Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: a new hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:10911–10916. doi: 10.1073/pnas.122368599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U, Wang HL, Cruz A, Fosker T, Mead N, Huss M. Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:325–337. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grinter EJ, Maybery MT, Badcock DR. Vision in developmental disorders: is there a dorsal stream deficit? Brain Res Bull. 2010;82:147–160. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guttorm TK, Leppanen PH, Hamalainen JA, Eklund KM, Lyytinen HJ. Newborn event-related potentials predict poorer pre-reading skills in children at risk for dyslexia. J Learn Disabil. 2010;43:391–401. doi: 10.1177/0022219409345005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guttorm TK, Leppanen PH, Poikkeus AM, Eklund KM, Lyytinen P, Lyytinen H. Brain event-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later language development in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex. 2005;41:291–303. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70267-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall DA, Haggard MP, Akeroyd MA, Palmer AR, Summerfield AQ, Elliott MR, Gurney EM, Bowtell RW. “Sparse” temporal sampling in auditory fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;7:213–223. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:3<213::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-N. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampson M, Olson IR, Leung HC, Skudlarski P, Gore JC. Changes in functional connectivity of human MT/V5 with visual motion input. Neuroreport. 2004;15:1315–1319. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000129997.95055.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Renvall H. Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:525–532. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01801-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim S, Eulitz C, Elbert T. Altered hemispheric asymmetry of auditory P100m in dyslexia. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003a;17(8):1715–1722. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02596.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim S, Eulitz C, Elbert T. Altered hemispheric asymmetry of auditory N100m in adults with developmental dyslexia. Neuroreport. 2003b;14(3):501–504. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200303030-00041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim S, Tschierse J, Amunts K, Wilms M, Vossel S, Willmes K, Grabowska A, Huber W. Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 2008;68:73–82. doi: 10.55782/ane-2008-1674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeft F, Hernandez A, McMillon G, Taylor-Hill H, Martindale JL, Meyler A, Keller TA, Siok WT, Deutsch GK, Just MA, et al. Neural basis of dyslexia: a comparison between dyslexic and nondyslexic children equated for reading ability. J Neurosci. 2006;26:10700–10708. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4931-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeft F, McCandliss BD, Black JM, Gantman A, Zakerani N, Hulme C, Lyytinen H, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Glover GH, Reiss AL, et al. Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:361–366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornickel J, Skoe E, Nicol T, Zecker S, Kraus N. Subcortical differentiation of stop consonants relates to reading and speech-in-noise perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:13022–13027. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901123106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornickel J, Zecker SG, Bradlow AR, Kraus N. Assistive listening devices drive neuroplasticity in children with dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:16731–16736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206628109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Donohue BC. Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:8939–8944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.15.8939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huss M, Verney JP, Fosker T, Mead N, Goswami U. Music, rhythm, rise time perception and developmental dyslexia: perception of musical meter predicts reading and phonology. Cortex. 2010 doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juel C. Learning to read and write: a longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. J Educ Psychol. 1988;80:437–447. [Google Scholar]
- Katzir T, Lesaux NK, Kim Y-S. The role of reading self-concept and home literacy practices in fourth grade reading comprehension. Read Writing Interdiscip J. 2009 [Google Scholar]
- Kovelman I, Norton ES, Christodoulou JA, Gaab N, Lieberman DA, Triantafyllou C, Wolf M, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gabrieli JD. Brain basis of phonological awareness for spoken language in children and its disruption in dyslexia. Cereb Cortex. 2011 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kronbichler M, Wimmer H, Staffen W, Hutzler F, Mair A, Ladurner G. Developmental dyslexia: gray matter abnormalities in the occipitotemporal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:613–625. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linkersdorfer J, Lonnemann J, Lindberg S, Hasselhorn M, Fiebach CJ. Grey matter alterations co-localize with functional abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: an ALE meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2012;7:e43122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipowska M, Czaplewska E, Wysocka A. Visuospatial deficits of dyslexic children. Med Sci Monit. 2011;17:CR216–CR221. doi: 10.12659/MSM.881718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyon GR. Toward a definition of dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. 1995;45:3–27. doi: 10.1007/BF02648210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyon GR, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. A definition of dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. 2003;53:1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Lyytinen H, Aro M, Eklund K, Erskine J, Guttorm T, Laakso ML, Leppanen PH, Lyytinen P, Poikkeus AM, Torppa M. The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: birth to early school age. Ann Dyslexia. 2004;54:184–220. doi: 10.1007/s11881-004-0010-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacSweeney M, Brammer MJ, Waters D, Goswami U. Enhanced activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in deaf and dyslexic adults during rhyming. Brain. 2009;132:1928–1940. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Burdette JH. Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas. Neuroimage. 2004;21:450–455. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurer U, Brem S, Bucher K, Kranz F, Benz R, Steinhausen HC, Brandeis D. Impaired tuning of a fast occipito-temporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. Brain. 2007;130:3200–3210. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurer U, Bucher K, Brem S, Benz R, Kranz F, Schulz E, van der Mark S, Steinhausen H-C, Brandeis D. Neurophysiology in preschool improves behavioral prediction of reading ability throughout primary school. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:341–348. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurer U, Bucher K, Brem S, Brandeis D. Altered responses to tone and phoneme mismatch in kindergartners at familial dyslexia risk. Neuroreport. 2003;14:2245–2250. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200312020-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McArthur GM, Bishop DV. Auditory perceptual processing in people with reading and oral language impairments: current issues and recommendations. Dyslexia. 2001;7:150–170. doi: 10.1002/dys.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7:293–299. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00134-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCandliss BD, Noble KG. The development of reading impairment: a cognitive neuroscience model. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2003;9:196–204. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNorgan C, Alvarez A, Bhullar A, Gayda J, Booth JR. Prediction of reading skill several years later depends on age and brain region: implications for developmental models of reading. J Neurosci. 2011;31:9641–9648. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0334-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molfese VJ, Modglin A, Molfese DL. The role of environment in the development of reading skills: a longitudinal study of preschool and school-age measures. J Learn Disabil. 2003;36:59–67. doi: 10.1177/00222194030360010701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molfese VJ, Molfese DL, Modgline AA. Newborn and preschool predictors of second-grade reading scores: an evaluation of categorical and continuous scores. J Learn Disabil. 2001;34:545–554. doi: 10.1177/002221940103400607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris D, Bloodgood JW. Developmental steps in learning to read: a longitudinal study in kindergarten and first grade. Read Res Q. 2003;38(3):302–328. [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien BA, Wolf M, Lovett MW. A taxometric investigation of developmental dyslexia subtypes. Dyslexia. 2012;18:16–39. doi: 10.1002/dys.1431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington BF. Annotation: the genetics of dyslexia. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;31:193–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb01561.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington BF, Lefly DL. Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Dev. 2001;72:816–833. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pernet C, Andersson J, Paulesu E, Demonet JF. When all hypotheses are right: a multifocal account of dyslexia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2278–2292. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA, Temple E, Protopapas A, Nagarajan S, Tallal P, Merzenich M, Gabrieli JD. Relations between the neural bases of dynamic auditory processing and phonological processing: evidence from fMRI. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13:687–697. doi: 10.1162/089892901750363235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price CJ. The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production. Trends Cogn Sci. 1998;2:281–288. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01201-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugh KR, Landi N, Preston JL, Mencl WE, Austin AC, Sibley D, Fulbright RK, Seidenberg MS, Grigorenko EL, Constable RT, et al. The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain Lang. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.004. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Jenner AR, Katz L, Frost SJ, Lee JR, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia) Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2000;6:207–213. doi: 10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:3<207::AID-MRDD8>3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Jenner AR, Katz L, Frost SJ, Lee JR, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. J Commun Disord. 2001;34:479–492. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(01)00060-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puolakanaho A, Ahonen T, Aro M, Eklund K, Lepaennen PHT, Poikkeus AM, Tolvanen A, Torppa M, Lyytinen H. Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to second grade reading outcomes: strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(4):353–370. doi: 10.1177/0022219407311747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puolakanaho A, Ahonen T, Aro M, Eklund K, Lepaennen PHT, Poikkeus AM, Tolvanen A, Torppa M, Lyytinen H. Very early phonological and language skills: estimating individual risk of reading disability. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(9):923–931. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01763.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F. Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:212–218. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00035-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F, Rosen S, Dakin SC, Day BL, Castellote JM, White S, Frith U. Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain. 2003;126:841–865. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus, F, White S, Frith U. Weighing the evidence between competing theories of dyslexia. This is a response to commentaries on White et al. (2006) by Bishop (2006), Goswami (2006), Nicolson and Fawcett (2006) and Tallal (2006) Dev. Sci. 2006;9:265–269. [Google Scholar]
- Raschle NM, Chang M, Gaab N. Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. Neuroimage. 2011;57:742–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raschle NM, Lee M, Buechler R, Christodoulou JA, Chang M, Vakil M. Making MR imaging child's play—pediatric neuroimaging protocol, guidelines and procedure. JoVE. 2009;(29) doi: 10.3791/1309. e1309. doi:10.3791/1309 (2009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raschle NM, Zuk J, Gaab N. Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012a;109:2156–2161. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107721109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raschle NM, Zuk J, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Sliva DD, Franceschi A, Grant PE, Benasich AA, Gaab N. Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: challenges and practical guidelines. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012b;1252:43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed MA. Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in reading disabled children. J Exp Child Psychol. 1989;48:270–292. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(89)90006-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson U, Leppanen PH, Leiwo M, Lyytinen H. Speech perception of infants with high familial risk for dyslexia differ at the age of 6 months. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;23:385–397. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2303_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3299–3308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruff S, Cardebat D, Marie N, Demonet JF. Enhanced response of the left frontal cortex to slowed down speech in dyslexia: an fMRI study. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1285–1289. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200207190-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rvachew S, Grawburg M. Correlates of phonological awareness in preschoolers with speech sound disorders. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2006;49:74–87. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS. Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ. Ann Dyslexia. 1998;48:115–136. [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS. Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Dev. 1990;61:1728–1743. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS, Dobrich W, Hager M. Preschool literacy experience and later reading achievement. J Learn Disabil. 1991;24:508–511. doi: 10.1177/002221949102400811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlaggar BL, McCandliss BD. Development of neural systems for reading. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:475–503. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulte-Korne G, Deimel W, Muller K, Gutenbrunner C, Remschmidt H. Familial aggregation of spelling disability. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37:817–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01477.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seigel LS. IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil. 1989;22(8):469–478. doi: 10.1177/002221948902200803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord W. The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, Constable RT, Marchione KE, Fletcher JM, Lyon GR, et al. Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:101–110. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01365-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz S. Dyslexia. N Engl J Med. 1998;338:307–312. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801293380507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Dyslexia (specific reading disability) Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1301–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Constable RT, Mencl WE, Shankweiler DP, Liberman AM, Skudlarski P, Fletcher JM, et al. Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:2636–2641. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.5.2636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel LS. Perspectives on dyslexia. Paediatr Child Health. 2006;11(9):581–587. doi: 10.1093/pch/11.9.581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simos PG, Breier JI, Fletcher JM, Foorman BR, Bergman E, Fishbeck K, Papanicolaou AC. Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during non-word reading: a magnetic source imaging study. Neurosci Lett. 2000;290:61–65. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)01322-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling MJ, Muter V, Carroll J. Children at family risk of dyslexia: a follow-up in early adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:609–618. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Specht K, Hugdahl K, Ofte S, Nygard M, Bjornerud A, Plante E, Helland T. Brain activation on pre-reading tasks reveals at-risk status for dyslexia in 6-year-old children. Scand J Psychol. 2009;50:79–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00688.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich KE, Siegel LS. Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: a regression-based test of phonological-core variable-difference model. J Educ Psychol. 1994;86:24–53. [Google Scholar]
- Stefanics G, Fosker T, Huss M, Mead N, Szucs D, Goswami U. Auditory sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: a longitudinal ERP study. Neuroimage. 2011;57:723–732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley CJ, Harrison EP, Stein JF. Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:795–798. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Toumoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: an approach to cerebral imaging. Stuttgart: Thieme Verlag; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Talcott JB, Witton C, McLean MF, Hansen PC, Rees A, Green GG, Stein JF. Dynamic sensory sensitivity and children's word decoding skills. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:2952–2957. doi: 10.1073/pnas.040546597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P. Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain Lang. 1980;9:182–198. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(80)90139-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P. Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:721–728. doi: 10.1038/nrn1499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P. Improving neural response to sound improves reading. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:16406–16407. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214122109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P, Gaab N. Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience and language development. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29:382–390. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P, Piercy M. Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature. 1973a;241:468–469. doi: 10.1038/241468a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P, Piercy M. Developmental aphasia: impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia. 1973b;11:389–398. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(73)90025-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P, Piercy M. Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia. 1974;12:83–93. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(74)90030-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Temple E. Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:178–183. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00303-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Temple E, Poldrack RA, Protopapas A, Nagarajan S, Salz T, Tallal P, Merzenich MM, Gabrieli JD. Disruption of the neural response to rapid acoustic stimuli in dyslexia: evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:13907–13912. doi: 10.1073/pnas.240461697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Temple E, Poldrack RA, Salidis J, Deutsch GK, Tallal P, Merzenich MM, Gabrieli JD. Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study. Neuroreport. 2001;12:299–307. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao FM, Liu HM, Kuhl PK. Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: a longitudinal study. Child Dev. 2004;75:1067–1084. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00726.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkeltaub PE, Gareau L, Flowers DL, Zeffiro TA, Eden GF. Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:767–773. doi: 10.1038/nn1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaessen A, Blomert L. Long-term cognitive dynamics of fluent reading development. J Exp Child Psychol. 2010;105:213–231. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Atteveldt N, Formisano E, Goebel R, Blomert L. Integration of letters and speech sounds in the human brain. Neuron. 2004;43:271–282. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Lely HK. Verb Agreement and Tense Test (VATT) London: Centre for Developmental Language Disorders and Cognitive Neuroscience (DLDCN.COM); 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Vellutino FR. Dyslexia: theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel A, Adelman P. The success of college students with learning disabilities: factors related to ecuational attainment. J Learn Disabil. 1992;25:430–431. doi: 10.1177/002221949202500703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. The comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin: PRO-ED, Inc; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, Paramasivam M, Thomas A, Bai J, Kaminen-Ahola N, Kere J, Voskuil J, Rosen GD, Galaburda AM, Loturco JJ. DYX1C1 functions in neuronal migration in developing neocortex. Neuroscience. 2006;143:515–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White S, Milne E, Rosen S, Hansen PC, Swettenham J, Frith U, Ramus F. The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: a multiple case study of dyslexic children. Dev. Sci. 2006;9(3):237–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00483.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willburger E, Landerl K. Anchoring the deficit of the anchor deficit: dyslexia or attention? Dyslexia. 2010;16:175–182. doi: 10.1002/dys.404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf BM, Bowers PG. The double deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. J Learn Disabil. 1999;91:1–24. [Google Scholar]
- Wolf M, Denckla MB. RAN/RAS: rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating. Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Woodcock RW. Woodcock reading mastery test: revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Wright BA, Lombardino LJ, King WM, Puranik CS, Leonard CM, Merzenich MM. Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children. Nature. 1997;387:176–178. doi: 10.1038/387176a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright CM, Conlon EG. Auditory and visual processing in children with dyslexia. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34:330–355. doi: 10.1080/87565640902801882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y, Hong-Yan B. Unilateral implicit motor learning deficit in developmental dyslexia. Int J Psychol. 2011;46:1–8. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2010.509800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.