Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2014 Nov 13;3:e03895. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03895

Drep-2 is a novel synaptic protein important for learning and memory

Till F M Andlauer 1,2,3, Sabrina Scholz-Kornehl 1, Rui Tian 1,2, Marieluise Kirchner 4, Husam A Babikir 1, Harald Depner 1, Bernhard Loll 5, Christine Quentin 1, Varun K Gupta 1, Matthew G Holt 6, Shubham Dipt 7, Michael Cressy 8, Markus C Wahl 5, André Fiala 7, Matthias Selbach 4, Martin Schwärzel 1, Stephan J Sigrist 1,9,*
Editor: Leslie C Griffith10
PMCID: PMC4229683  PMID: 25392983

Abstract

CIDE-N domains mediate interactions between the DNase Dff40/CAD and its inhibitor Dff45/ICAD. In this study, we report that the CIDE-N protein Drep-2 is a novel synaptic protein important for learning and behavioral adaptation. Drep-2 was found at synapses throughout the Drosophila brain and was strongly enriched at mushroom body input synapses. It was required within Kenyon cells for normal olfactory short- and intermediate-term memory. Drep-2 colocalized with metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs). Chronic pharmacological stimulation of mGluRs compensated for drep-2 learning deficits, and drep-2 and mGluR learning phenotypes behaved non-additively, suggesting that Drep 2 might be involved in effective mGluR signaling. In fact, Drosophila fragile X protein mutants, shown to benefit from attenuation of mGluR signaling, profited from the elimination of drep-2. Thus, Drep-2 is a novel regulatory synaptic factor, probably intersecting with metabotropic signaling and translational regulation.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.001

Research organism: D. melanogaster

eLife digest

Synapses are specialized structures that connect nerve cells to one another and allow information to be transmitted between the cells. Synapses are essential for learning and storing memories. Many proteins that regulate how signals are transmitted at synapses have already been studied. In this manner, much has been learned about their function in learning and memory.

Cells can commit suicide by a process called apoptosis, also known as programmed cell death. Apoptosis is not only triggered in damaged cells but is also necessary for an organism to develop correctly. In fruit flies, the protein Drep-2 is a member of a family of proteins that degrade the DNA of cells that undergo apoptosis.

Andlauer et al. found no evidence that Drep-2 plays a role in apoptosis, but have now found Drep-2 at the synapses of the brain of the fruit fly Drosophila. Drep-2 could be observed in close proximity to another type of protein called metabotropic glutamate receptors. Metabotropic glutamate receptors and their signaling pathways are important for regulating certain changes to the synapses that mediate learning processes. Indeed, Andlauer et al. found that flies that have lost the gene that produces Drep-2 were unable to remember smells when these were paired with a punishment. Stimulating the regulatory glutamate receptors with drugs helped to overcome learning deficits that result from the lack of Drep-2.

Alterations in the production of a protein called FMRP cause fragile X syndrome in humans, the most common form of hereditary mental disability originating from a single gene defect. Flies lacking the FMRP protein show learning deficits that are very similar to the ones seen in flies that cannot produce Drep-2. However, Andlauer et al. observed that flies lacking both Drep-2 and FMRP can learn normally.

Exactly how Drep-2 works in synapses to help with memory formation remains to be discovered, although there are indications that it boosts the effects of signaling from the glutamate receptors and counteracts FMRP. Further research will be needed to establish whether the mammalian proteins related to Drep-2 perform similar roles in the brains of mammals.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.002

Introduction

Caspase family proteases regulate cellular pathways by cleavage of target proteins. They are best known for their roles during programmed cell death. One of their targets is the DNase Dff40/CAD, which degrades DNA during apoptosis (Enari et al., 1998). Dff40 is a member of the DNA fragmentation factor (Dff) family of proteins, characterized by CIDE-N domains that mediate protein–protein interactions (Wu et al., 2008). In Drosophila, four CIDE-N domain proteins were identified and named Dff related protein Drep-1 to Drep-4 (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999). Caspase-regulated Drep-4 is the ortholog of mammalian Dff40/CAD (Yokoyama et al., 2000). Drep-4 is inhibited by Drep-1, the ortholog of Dff45/ICAD, which is also cleaved by caspases (Mukae et al., 2000). The two other Drosophila family members, Drep-2 and -3, were suggested to be additional regulators of apoptosis, solely based on in vitro interactions (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999; Park and Park, 2012).

We show here that Drep-2, contrary to the expectations, is a novel synaptic protein. We have generated drep-2 mutants, which display learning and memory deficits. Further analyses suggest that Drep-2 regulates these processes by intersecting with metabotropic glutamate receptor signaling.

Results

Drosophila encodes two Dff40/CAD-related proteins, Drep-4 and Drep-2 (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999; Yokoyama et al., 2000) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Both proteins are controlled by caspase-mediated cleavage of their inhibitors Drep-1 and Drep-3, respectively (Mukae et al., 2000; Park and Park, 2012, 2013). The roles of Drep-4 and -1 during apoptosis have been firmly established, yet the functions of Drep-2 and -3 have so far remained unknown (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999; Park and Park, 2012; Tan et al., 2012).

Drep-2 is a novel synaptic protein without a discernable role in the regulation of apoptosis

The gene coding for Drep-2 is located on chromosome IIR and contains five exons (Figure 1A). While the last two exons are used in all known isoforms, the first three exons are included alternatively within the four isoforms drep-2-RA to -RD.

Figure 1. Expression and mutants of drep-2.

(A) Genetic scheme of the drep-2 locus on chromosome IIR. The neighboring genes mad-1 and myd88 extend beyond the sequence displayed. The cDNA labeled drep-2-RA was used for rescue experiments. Blue: untranslated regions; green: exons; black lines: deleted regions in the mutants. (B) In situ hybridization of drep-2 reveals a neuronal expression pattern (stage 17). (C) Western blot of adult fly head extracts using the anti-Drep-2C-Term antibody. Drep-2 isoforms are predicted to run at 52 and 58 kDa. The signal is absent in both the drep-2ex13 and the drep-2ex27/Dfw45-30n mutant.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.003

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Drep protein alignment.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Sequence alignment of all four Drosophila Dff proteins, as well as human (HS) and murine (MM) Dff40. Drep-4 has the strongest similarity to Dff40, yet Drep-2 also shows conserved motifs in addition to the CIDE-N domain. The alignment was created using Geneious v5.3.6. (http://www.geneious.com)
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Reduced lifespan of drep-2ex13 mutants.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Comparison to isogenic w1118 control flies: 50% of mutant flies were dead after 21.5 days. Mutant: n = 10 vials (each containing 25 flies), control: n = 11.

High-throughput RT-PCR data indicate a strong enrichment of both drep-2-RA and drep-3 transcripts in the central nervous system (CNS), while drep-4 and -1 are expressed ubiquitously (Graveley et al., 2011). We conducted in situ hybridization using drep-2-RA constructs in order to confirm that the expression of drep-2 is nervous system specific (Figure 1B). Next, we produced polyclonal antibodies against a fusion protein containing the C-terminal half of the protein, which is part of all isoforms (Drep-2C-Term). Western blots from wild-type fly head extracts probed with Drep-2C-Term showed a double band (Figure 1C) of the size expected for Drep-2 isoforms (McQuilton et al., 2012).

We generated drep-2 mutants by FLP-mediated excision between FRT site-bearing transposons to explore the function of Drep-2. The transposons P(XP)d00223 and PBac(RB)e04659 were used for the deletion allele drep-2ex13 (Figure 1A). A second deletion allele, drep-2ex27, was established using transposon lines PBac(RB)e02920 and PBac(RB)e04659. All drep-2 exons are deleted in homozygous drep-2ex13 animals, while no other annotated transcription unit is affected. In the smaller intragenic deletion drep-2ex27, all drep-2 exons apart from the very small (12 bp) first exon are eliminated. Both Drep-2C-Term antibody bands were absent in head extracts of both mutants (Figure 1C), confirming the complete elimination of Drep-2 expression in these deletion alleles. Flies lacking drep-2 were viable and fertile but shorter-lived than the isogenic controls (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

Subsequently, we used the Drep-2C−Term antibody for wholemount immunostainings of Drosophila brains. The synaptic neuropil was strongly labeled throughout the brains of larvae (not shown) and adults (Figure 2). In both drep-2 mutants, Drep-2C−Term staining in the CNS was completely abolished (drep-2ex13: Figure 2A,B).

Figure 2. Synaptic Drep-2 staining in the CNS.

Figure 2.

(AB) Confocal frontal sections of adult Drosophila brains. Anti-Drep-2C-Term and BrpNc82 immunostaining; the latter marks all synaptic active zones. Synaptic Drep-2C-Term signal is visible throughout the brain of wild-type flies (A). Complete loss of the anti-Drep-2C-Term staining can be observed in drep-2ex13 mutants (B). Scale bars: 20 µm. (CE) Frontal sections of wild-type brains, anti-Drep-2C-Term, and BrpNc82 staining. Scale bars: 5 µm. (C) Posterior–dorsal detail showing strong Drep-2 staining in MB calyces (arrow). (D) Anterior frontal section with antennal lobes and MB lobes. (E) Ellipsoid body in the central complex and bulbs (lateral triangles) (E′: magnification of strong Drep-2 staining in bulbs).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.006

As a Dff family member, Drep-2 has been suggested to be involved in the apoptotic regulation of DNA degradation (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999; Park and Park, 2012). However, neuronal cell bodies lacked Drep-2C−Term staining (Figure 2). We produced synaptosome-like preparations by fractionation of the adult Drosophila CNS to biochemically confirm the association of Drep-2 with synapses (Owald et al., 2012; Depner et al., 2014). Drep-2 was strongly enriched in fractions containing synaptic membranes (Figure 3A). By contrast, no enrichment of Drep-2 could be observed in the nuclear fraction.

Figure 3. No evidence for a role of Drep-2 in regulation of apoptosis.

Figure 3.

(A) Synaptosome-like preparation of adult wild-type head extracts (Depner et al., 2014), probed with Drep-2C-Term. Drep-2 is concentrated in fractions containing synaptic membranes. S = supernatant, P = pellet, L = (after) lysis. Please see the protocol by Depner et al. (2014) for a more detailed explanation of the fractionation procedure. (B) Mutants (drep-2ex13) did not show a rough eye phenotype. The facet eyes of flies, highly ordered structures, are often affected in apoptosis mutants. By contrast, the eyes of drep-2 mutants appeared normal. (C) The number of mb247-positive KCs does not differ between drep-2ex13 mutants and controls. GFP was expressed using the MB KC driver mb247-Gal4. GFP-positive cell bodies were counted and compared between genotypes. No significant difference was found between mean cell body counts (Mann–Whitney U test, p = 0.886). Average cell body counts were in the expected range: control = 651, mutant = 669, published = 700 (Schwaerzel et al., 2002). (D) Purified Drep-2 does not degrade linearized plasmid DNA. Left: SDS-PAGE of the final elusion profile of purified Drep-2, loaded onto a HighLoad Superdex S200 16/60 column. Right: Nuclease activity assay of purified Drep-2 analyzed by 1% (wt/vol) agarose gel. Drep-2 was incubated in a time course experiment with linearized plasmid DNA. No nuclease activity could be detected. Instead, Drep-2 seemed to precipitate DNA, as evidenced by high-molecular DNA not entering into the agarose gel when incubated with Drep-2 (arrow).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.007

Mutants lacking drep-2 showed normal exterior morphology, including their facet eyes (Figure 3B). Fly facet eyes are highly ordered structures, already displaying abnormal patterns in cases of moderate misregulation of apoptosis (Wolff and Ready, 1991; Song et al., 2000). The regular facet eyes of drep-2 mutants, therefore, argue against a major function of the protein in apoptosis. Wholemount brain stainings of wildtypes and drep-2ex13 mutants also showed no apparent morphological differences between either genotype (Figure 2A,B; Figure 5—figure supplement 1). If apoptosis was, nevertheless, misregulated in the CNS of drep-2 mutants in vivo, an altered count of cell bodies should be expected in adult flies. Drep-2 staining was especially prominent at KC synapses in the mushroom body (MB) calyx of wildtypes (Figure 2C). We, therefore, quantified the numbers of cell bodies of a subset of MB-intrinsic neurons (KCs). No differences between drep-2 mutants and controls could be observed (Figure 3C).

Drep-2 was reported to degrade DNA in vitro (Park and Park, 2012). This supposed nuclease activity was observed if purified Drep-2 was incubated in vitro with plasmid DNA at a molar ratio of protein:DNA 80:1 (Park and Park, 2012). However, we (Figure 3D) and another previous report (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999) found no evidence of a nuclease activity of Drep-2, even at high concentrations. Instead, Drep-2 appeared to precipitate DNA, as evident by plasmid DNA no longer entering the agarose gel when incubated with Drep-2. This precipitation might have generated the previous impression that DNA is degraded in the presence of Drep-2. Taken together, we could not find evidence for an in vivo role of Drep-2 in regulating apoptosis in the CNS. While a function of the protein related to apoptosis can still not be fully excluded, we decided rather to examine the synaptic functions of Drep-2.

Drep-2 is strongly enriched at the postsynaptic densities of mushroom body input synapses

In order to narrow down a potential site of action of the protein, we examined Drep-2 expression in the adult CNS in more detail. While Drep-2C-Term stained synapses throughout the brain, including optic lobes, antennal lobes, and the central complex (Figure 2), the immunoreactivity was particularly pronounced in the MB calyx (Figures 2C and Figure 4A).

Figure 4. Drep-2 is enriched at KC postsynapses.

(AB) Drep-2C-Term and BrpNc82 staining in animals expressing the construct mb247::Dα7GFP that marks acetylcholine receptors in MB KCs. (A) Detailed image of the MB calyx. Scale bar: 2 µm. (B) Detail of a single microglomerulus in the calyx. Drep-2C-Term overlaps with postsynaptic mb247::Dα7GFP and not with presynaptic Brp. Scale bars: 1 µm. (C) Localization of Drep-2 relative to choline acetyltransferase (ChAT, presynaptic cytosol, C), the postsynaptic ACh receptor subunit Dα7 (antibody staining, C′), and the postsynaptic scaffolding protein Discs large (Dlg, C″). Drep-2 colocalizes with postsynaptic markers. Scale bars: 1 µm. (D) Post-embedding immunoelectron microscopy of Drep-2C-Term in the calyx. Arrows: Clusters of postsynaptic Drep-2C-Term. Scale bars: 100 nm.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.008

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Drep-2 localizes to postsynaptic membranes of KCs in the calyx.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) STED microscopy superresolution recording of Drep-2C-Term; the BrpNc82 channel is in normal confocal mode. The Drep-2 signal does not overlap with presynaptic Brp. Scale bar: 1 µm. (BE) Expression of drep-2 constructs in KCs yields a label resembling the Drep-2 antibody staining. Comparison to BrpNc82; all scale bars: 1 µm. (B) Pan-neural overexpression. Elavc155-Gal4 and UAS-Drep-2mStrawberry; mStrawberry signal is shown. (C) KC-specific overexpression. C305a-Gal4, UAS-Drep-2mStrawberry, and UAS-Dα7GFP; mStrawberry and GFP signals are shown. D-2 = Drep-2mStrawberry, D7 = Dα7GFP. (D) PN-specific overexpression. Gh146-Gal4 and UAS-Drep-2mStrawberry; diffuse mStrawberry is shown. (E) KC-specific expression of UAS-Drep-2 in the drep-2ex13-mutant background. Mb247-Gal4 and untagged UAS-Drep-2; Drep-2C-Term staining is shown.

The principle circuitry processing olfactory information in Drosophila is highly similar to mammals (Davis, 2004). Signals are transferred in the fly's antennal lobe to projection neurons (PNs), which target the mushroom body (MB) calyx and the lateral protocerebrum. PNs synapse in the calyx onto MB-intrinsic Kenyon cells (KCs) by forming large cholinergic presynaptic boutons. These presynaptic specializations are tightly encircled by acetylcholine (ACh) receptor-expressing postsynaptic densities (PSDs) of KC dendritic claws (Yasuyama et al., 2002; Leiss et al., 2009a).

In the MB calyx, the Drep-2C-Term signal overlapped with Dα7 ACh receptor subunits expressed in KC PSDs (Figure 4A,B,C′), which surrounded Bruchpilot (Brp)-positive (but Drep-2-negative) PN presynapses (Figure 4A,B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). Furthermore, Drep-2 colocalized with the postsynaptic protein Discs large, but clearly segregated from presynaptic choline acetyltransferase (Figure 4C). Consistent with endogenous Drep-2 being present at KC-derived PSDs, overexpression of UAS-Drep-2mStrawberry with either pan-neural (elavc155-Gal4) or KC-specific drivers (c305a-Gal4) resulted in an mStrawberry signal equivalent to that of the Drep-2C-Term antibody (Figure 4—figure supplement 1B,C). Overexpression with a PN driver (gh146-Gal4), however, produced only a weak, diffuse expression pattern that bore no similarity to endogenous Drep-2 staining (Figure 4—figure supplement 1D). Moreover, re-expression of UAS-Drep-2 in KCs of drep-2 mutants produced a distinctive label at KC PSDs (Figure 4—figure supplement 1E). Finally, we confirmed the presence of Drep-2 at postsynaptic membranes of PN-KC synapses by immunoelectron microscopy (Figure 4D). Thus, Drep-2 accumulates at postsynaptic specializations of KCs within the MB calyx.

Drep-2 is required in KCs for normal olfactory short- and intermediate-term memory

Kenyon cells have an essential function in olfactory learning (Davis, 2011; Dubnau and Chiang, 2013). Here, conditioned stimuli (odors) get associated with unconditioned stimuli (e.g., electric foot shock). Because Drep-2 was strongly enriched at PSDs of KCs, we wondered whether the protein might contribute to olfactory learning. In aversive olfactory conditioning, flies are trained to learn the difference between a punished and an unpunished odor. We measured short-term memory (STM) directly following training and intermediate-term memory (ITM) after 3 hr. All flies used for behavioral experiments were outcrossed into our isogenic background (w1118) for at least five generations. Performance of controls allowed for the detection of differences in either direction (Figure 5B,C).

Figure 5. Drep-2 is required in KCs for olfactory short- and intermediate-term memory.

(A) Flies mutant for drep-2 sense electric shock and the odors 4-methyl-cyclohexanol (4-MCH) and 3-octanol (3-OCT) normally; there is no difference in mean performance indices between mutants and isogenic w1118 control flies (Mann–Whitney U tests (MWU)). Sample sizes n are indicated with white numbers; grey bars show SEMs. (B) Both mutants drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex27/Dfw45-30n are deficient in aversive olfactory conditioning, 3 min STM in a T-maze. The graph shows mean learning indices and SEMs. Mutants performed significantly worse than isogenic controls (MWU: p = 0.00001 for both comparisons, Bonferroni-corrected significance level α = 0.0167, 3 tests). (C) Re-expression of drep-2 cDNA with elavIII-Gal4 (pan-neural), 30y-Gal4 (MB KCs), or mb247-Gal4 (MB KCs) restores the deficit to normal levels. Heterozygous drep-2ex13 mutants do not display a significant deficit. MWU for individual comparisons showed a significant difference between these groups (α = 0.0042, 12 tests): w1118 and drep-2ex13 (p < 0.00001), drep-2ex13/drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13/+ (p < 0.00001), drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/elavIII-gal4 (p < 0.00001), drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/30y-gal4 (p < 0.00001), drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/mb247-gal4 (p < 0.00001). None of the differences indicated as not significant had a p < 0.12, except for w1118 and drep-2ex13/+ (p = 0.03851; not significant in the case of α = 0.0042). (D) Intermediate-term memory (ITM = ASM + ARM) performance. Mutants (drep-2ex13) are defective in ASM, but not in ARM. The defect can be restored with elavIII-Gal4 or mb247-Gal4 (30y-Gal4 was not used here). Statistical tests were run separately for ITM and ARM. For ITM, MWU for individual comparisons showed a significant difference between these groups (α = 0.00625, 8 tests): w1118 and drep-2ex13 (p < 0.0001), drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/elavIII-gal4 (p < 0.0001), drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/mb247-gal4 (p < 0.0001). For assessing differences in ARM, ITM and ARM performances of each genotype were compared with MWU. The following genotypes showed a significant difference between ITM and ARM (α = 0.0071, 7 tests): w1118 (p < 0.0001), drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/elavIII-gal4 (p = 0.0002), drep-2ex13;uas-drep-2/mb247-gal4 (p = 0.0006). None of the differences indicated as not significant had a p < 0.11.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.010

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. PN-KC synapses appear morphologically normal in drep-2 mutants.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Absence of major neuroanatomical defects in drep-2ex13 mutant brains. MB lobes, Fasciclin II (FasII) staining, maximum intensity projections. Scale bar: 10 μm. (B) Antibody staining of w1118 control and drep-2ex13 mutant brains, using antibodies against the postsynaptic ACh receptor subunit Dα7 and presynaptic BrpN−Term. Focus on microglomeruli of PN-KC synapses in the MB calyx. Microglomeruli of mutants appear structurally normal. Scale bar: 1 µm. (C) Electron microscopy of w1118 control and drep-2ex13 mutant brains. Microglomeruli and postsynaptic KC profiles of mutants appear structurally normal. Scale bars: 100 nm. (D) The number of synapses (active zones) in the MB calyx does not significantly differ between drep-2ex13 mutants and w1118 controls. Syd-1-positive spots were counted and compared between genotypes as described (Kremer et al., 2010). No significant difference was found between the number of spots (MWU, p = 0.62). Average synapse counts were in the range expected (28,000–30,000 [Kremer et al., 2010]).

Mutants lacking drep-2 showed normal naïve sensory acuity, with innate olfactory responses and shock avoidance behavior not significantly different from controls (Figure 5A). However, the STM performance scores of both mutants (drep2ex13 and drep2ex27) were significantly lower than the scores of isogenic w1118 controls (Figure 5B,C). Re-expression of drep-2 cDNA by using either the pan-neural driver elavIII-Gal4 or two different KC-specific drivers (30y-Gal4 and mb247-Gal4) was sufficient to rescue STM scores (Figure 5C). Drep-2 is, therefore, required in KCs for normal olfactory STM.

ITM is composed of two memory components, consolidated anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) and labile anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM) (Quinn and Dudai, 1976; Tully et al., 1994). ASM and ARM rely on different molecular and neuronal mechanisms (Folkers et al., 1993; Tully et al., 1994; Scheunemann et al., 2012). Amnestic cooling abolishes the labile ASM and is, thus, used to separate both components. The drep-2 mutants exhibited regular ARM but were deficient in ASM (Figure 5D), which was evidenced by memory scores not differing between cooled and untreated drep-2 mutants. This loss of ASM was rescued by re-expression of drep-2 using elavIII-Gal4 or KC-specific mb247-Gal4. We conclude that Drep-2 is required in KCs for both STM and ASM, but not for ARM.

Concurrently, morphologies of drep-2-mutant MBs and PN-KC synapses appeared normal (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Furthermore, the number of synapses in the MB calyx did not differ between either genotype (Figure 5—figure supplement 1D). Thus, drep-2-mutant phenotypes should not be caused by gross structural developmental aberrations. Instead, given the synaptic localization of the protein, it appeared likely that Drep-2 might intersect with signaling pathways involved in synaptic plasticity.

Functional overlap between Drep-2 and mGluR in olfactory conditioning

PN-KC synapses, as is the case for many excitatory synapses in the Drosophila CNS, use ACh as the main fast neurotransmitter (Gu and O'Dowd, 2006; Yasuyama et al., 2002). Drep-2 colocalized here with nicotinic ACh receptor subunits (Figure 4). In addition, several types of metabotropic receptor are also expressed in the calyx, including GABAB, dopamine, octopamine, and metabotropic glutamate receptors (Enell et al., 2007; Devaud et al., 2008; Busch et al., 2009; Mao and Davis, 2009; Kanellopoulos et al., 2012). DmGluRA is the single functional metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) in Drosophila (Parmentier et al., 1996), orthologous to mammalian group II/III mGluRs. DmGluRA and the mGluR-associated protein Homer show a characteristic expression throughout the Drosophila brain, with strong expression in the MB calyx (Ramaekers et al., 2001; Diagana et al., 2002; Hamasaka et al., 2007; Urizar et al., 2007; Devaud et al., 2008; Kahsai et al., 2012; Kanellopoulos et al., 2012).

We examined different receptor types and related proteins for colocalization with Drep-2 (not shown). Drep-2 colocalized with both mGluR and Homer throughout the brain (not shown), with very prominent co-labeling in the MB calyx (Figure 6A). This made mGluR signaling a prime candidate for a pathway interacting with Drep-2 function.

Figure 6. Functional overlap between Drep-2 and mGluR in olfactory conditioning.

Figure 6.

(A) Wildtype adult MB calyces stained with Drep-2C-Term and DmGluRA7G11 (first row) or with Drep-2C-Term and anti-Homer (second row). Drep-2 colocalizes tightly with both proteins. The insets show single microglomeruli. Scale bars: 2 µm. (B) Flies carrying the mutation dmGluRA112b are deficient in aversive olfactory conditioning STM when compared to isogenic dmGluRA2b controls that do express DmGluRA; MWU: p = 0.043, α = 0.05. The graph shows mean learning indices and SEMs; sample sizes n are indicated with white numbers. (C) The drep-2ex13 phenotype in olfactory STM can be rescued by raising animals on food containing the DmGluRA agonist 1S,3R-ACPD (ACPD). Food was supplemented throughout development and adulthood with either the DmGluRA receptor antagonist MPEP (9.7 µM) or the agonist ACPD (72.2 µM) diluted in H2O (label: dev+ad). Control animals received only H2O. One group of animals was transferred to food supplemented by ACPD only after eclosion and not during development; the corresponding experiments are indicated by the label +ACPD adult. MPEP lowered the w1118 performance significantly (MWU p = 0.0003). MPEP did not alter drep-2ex13 indices (p = 0.8772) and ACPD did not change w1118 performance (p = 0.1145). ACPD rescued the drep-2 mutant phenotype to control levels if fed during both development and adulthood (comparison of drep-2ex13 +ACPD dev+ad to untreated drep-2ex13: p < 0.00001; comparison to untreated w1118: p = 0.0945). ACPD did not rescue the mutant phenotype if fed only during adulthood (+ACPD adult, no significant difference to untreated drep-2ex13 (p = 0.2281), significant difference to mutants treated with ACPD during both development and adulthood (p < 0.00001)). The difference between untreated w1118 and drep-2ex13 flies was also significant (p < 0.00001). Significance level α = 0.005 (10 tests). (D) Phenotypes of drep-2ex13; dmGluRA112b double mutants were non-additive. Both drep-2ex13 and dmGluRA112b single mutants showed significantly lower olfactory STM than isogenic controls (MWU, p = 0.00008 for both comparisons). Double mutants showed similar learning indices (comparison to w1118: p = 0.00018). The two single mutants and the double mutant did not significantly differ from each other (p > 0.178). α = 0.0083 (6 tests). (E) Loss of drep-2 antagonizes dfmr1 phenotypes in olfactory conditioning STM. Both homozygous drep-2ex13 mutants and heterozygous dfmr1B55/+ mutants are deficient in olfactory learning STM, but double mutants carrying both alleles do learn. The graph shows mean learning indices and SEMs. MWU for individual comparisons (α = 0.01, 5 tests): w1118 and drep-2ex13 p < 0.00001, w1118 and dfmr1B55/+ p = 0.00069, w1118 and drep-2ex13; dfmr1B55/+ p = 0.83751, drep-2ex13 and drep-2ex13; dfmr1B55/+ p < 0.00001, dfmr1B55/+ and drep-2ex13; dfmr1B55/+ p = 0.00071.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.012

Recently, mGluRs have been found to be important for olfactory conditioning in flies. In that work, a decrease of mGluR levels provoked by RNA interference improved performance scores of the olfactory learning mutant dfmr1 (Kanellopoulos et al., 2012). The same effect was observed upon administration of the mGluR antagonist 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)pyridine (MPEP).

We assayed the STM performance of the mGluR mutant dmGluRA112b to independently confirm a function of mGluRs in olfactory learning (Bogdanik et al., 2004). Indeed, a significant reduction in learning scores was observed in these mutants (Figure 6B). In addition to the mGluR antagonist MPEP (McBride et al., 2005; Bolduc et al., 2008; Tauber et al., 2011; Kanellopoulos et al., 2012), the agonist 1S,3R-1-amino-1,3-cyclopentanedicarboxylate (ACPD) has also been shown to be effective in flies (Parmentier et al., 1996; Hamasaka et al., 2007). We, therefore, tested the effects of both MPEP and ACPD on olfactory learning scores of drep-2 mutants (Figure 6C). To this end, we raised flies on food containing either of the two components throughout development and adulthood. In agreement with our result for dmGluRA mutants (Figure 6B), the antagonist (MPEP) significantly decreased learning scores of wild-type flies when fed throughout development (Figure 6C). By contrast, MPEP did not further reduce the learning ability of drep-2 mutants. However, feeding the mGluR agonist ACPD during development effectively rescued the drep-2ex13 phenotype (Figure 6C (+ACPD dev+ad)). At the same time, feeding the agonist to controls did not alter their learning scores. When ACPD was fed only to adult animals after eclosion, it had no discernible effect on the performance of drep-2 mutants (Figure 6C (+ACPD adult)). In summary, artificial activation of mGluR receptors starting during development can compensate for the olfactory learning deficits of drep-2 mutants.

In order to further investigate a potential relationship between Drep-2 and mGluRs, we produced drep-2; dmGluRA double mutants. These double mutants showed learning indices that were very similar to the scores of both single mutants (Figure 6D). As the learning deficits of both mutants did not add up to a stronger impairment in double mutants, it is likely that both proteins converge, at least partially, into a common regulatory pathway.

Loss of drep-2 antagonizes dfmr1 phenotypes

Activation of mGluR signaling is thought to stimulate local synaptic translation through a signaling cascade involving Homer. By contrast, the RNA-binding fragile X mental retardation protein FMRP was shown to repress translation, thereby counteracting mGluR-mediated synaptic translation (Bhakar et al., 2012). Loss of FMRP function causes fragile X syndrome (FXS), the most frequent monogenic intellectual disorder. Pharmacological treatment with allosteric inhibitors of mGluRA was demonstrated to attenuate phenotypic deficits in rodent models of FXS and in FXS patients (Krueger and Bear, 2011; Bhakar et al., 2012; Gross et al., 2012).

In Drosophila, FMRP function and mGluR signaling also behave antagonistically. Importantly, learning phenotypes of dfmr1 mutants lacking FMRP can be rescued by pharmacological inhibition of mGluRs (McBride et al., 2005; Bolduc et al., 2008; Tauber et al., 2011; Kanellopoulos et al., 2012). By contrast, we describe here that drep-2 mutants profited from pharmacological stimulation of mGluRs (Figure 6C). We wondered, therefore, whether drep-2 and dfmr1 mutants would behave antagonistically.

To this end, we generated drep-2; dfmr1 double mutants. Single and double dfmr1 mutants lacking both copies of FMRP (dfmr1B55/dfmr1Δ50M) only hatched in small numbers, insufficient for olfactory conditioning experiments. However, Kanellopoulos et al. (2012) have shown that heterozygous dfmr1 mutants are also deficient in olfactory learning. We, therefore, compared homozygous drep-2 single mutants to heterozygous dfmr1B55/+ single and drep-2ex13/drep-2ex13; dfmr1B55/+ double mutants regarding aversive olfactory conditioning performance. Both single mutants showed decreased olfactory learning (Figure 6E), confirming the published olfactory learning phenotype of dfmr1/+ heterozygotes. Notably, however, performance of drep-2; dfmr1/+ double mutants was indistinguishable from the controls (Figure 6E), despite the deficit of both single mutants. This suggests that the absence of Drep-2 functionally compensates for the loss of FMRP. Our experiments thus provide first evidence that Drep-2 and FMRP display a functional antagonism.

Drep-2 and FMRP found in common complexes

Finally, we began exploring the molecular basis of the behavioral connections between Drep-2 and both mGluR signaling and FMRP. We first examined whether Drep-2 might regulate the protein levels or localization of either mGluR or Homer. However, mGluR and Homer levels appeared unaltered in drep-2 mutants (not shown). Therefore, it appeared more likely for Drep-2 to intersect with signaling processes downstream of the mere metabotropic glutamate receptor complex. We used quantitative affinity purification in combination with mass spectrometry to learn about Drep-2 in vivo interaction partners (Vermeulen et al., 2008; Paul et al., 2011). Unfortunately, the Drep-2C-Term antibody did not precipitate the endogenous Drep-2 protein sufficiently to allow for such an analysis. Thus, a Drep-2GFP fusion protein showing a localization pattern identical to endogenous Drep-2 (Figure 7—figure supplement 1) was expressed pan-neurally using elavc155-Gal4. We isolated protein complexes of this pan-neurally overexpressed Drep-2GFP from fly heads using anti-GFP beads (Figure 7). Parallel pulldowns, using either plain beads or Drep-2GFP-negative lysates, were performed as controls for nonspecific binding (Figure 7A). All three pulldowns were conducted in triplicate and processed and analyzed by high-resolution shotgun proteomics. Proteins were quantified by label-free quantification and specific interaction partners were extracted using t-test statistics (Hubner et al., 2010).

Figure 7. Quantitative mass spectrometry: Drep-2 and FMRP were found in a common protein complex.

(A) Strategy for the identification of Drep-2 interactors by quantitative mass spectrometry. UAS-Drep-2GFP was overexpressed using the pan-neural driver line elavc155-Gal4. (B) Volcano plot showing proteins from Drep-2GFP flies binding to anti-GFP and/or plain control beads. A hyperbolic curve (set at an FDR of 1%) separates GFP-enriched proteins (light pink) from background (grey). Proteins enriched in the control are shown in blue. Proteins that were significantly enriched, both in Drep-2GFP flies and in independent control experiments with wild-type flies, are colored magenta (n = 35). Drep-2 and GFP are shown as green dots. (C) Classification of the 35 core network proteins; multiple counts were allowed. (D) Network of the 35 proteins that were significantly and reproducibly enriched in GFP pulldown experiments (at an FDR of 1%, magenta-colored dots in B). Additional putative interactors of the core network (FDR set at 10%) are shown in white (Supplementary file 2). The circle (node) and font size correspond to the rank within the results (indicated in Supplementary files 1 and 2). The line (edge) width and shade correspond to the number of interactions each of the significantly enriched proteins has with others. The line/edge length is arbitrary. (E) Anti-FMRP probing confirmed the specific presence of FMRP in Drep-2GFP complexes. Head extracts of flies expressing Drep-2GFP or the presynaptic protein Syd-1GFP were processed in parallel. FMRP was only enriched in preparations of Drep-2GFP extracts. Immunoprecipitations were performed using either GFP-Trap-A beads (lanes labeled IP) or blocked agarose beads as binding control (labeled Blank beads).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.013

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Drep-2GFP colocalizes with endogenous Drep-2.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Pan-neural overexpression of UAS-drep-2GFP by elavc155-Gal4. MB calyx stained with anti-GFP, BrpN-Term and Drep-2C-Term. The Drep-2GFP label does not differ from the Drep-2C-Term antibody staining, compare also to Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Scale bars: 10 µm and 1 µm (insets). (B) KC-specific expression of UAS-drep-2GFP by mb247-Gal4 in drep-2ex13 mutants. Staining and scale bars as in (A).

On top of the bait proteins Drep-2 and GFP (the latter because the fusion protein Drep-2GFP was expressed), 35 proteins were robustly enriched over both controls at a false-positive discovery rate (FDR) of 1% (Figure 7B, Supplementary file 1). In order to visualize which of these 35 core proteins are part of a larger grid of putative interactors, an extended protein network was generated (Figure 7D). Proteins that DroID (Murali et al., 2011) or Flybase (McQuilton et al., 2012) lists as (putative) interactors of any of the 35 core proteins (FDR 1%) were added if they fitted the following three conditions: (i) enriched in the pulldowns (elavc155; uas-drep-2GFP flies, GFP beads vs plain beads) at an FDR of 10%, (ii) not enriched in the control experiment (GFP beads, wild-type flies vs elavc155; uas-drep-2GFP flies) at an FDR of 10% (to eliminate false-positives), and (iii) a (predicted) interaction with at least two of the 35 core proteins (Supplementary file 2).

Among the proteins found to be significantly enriched were Drep-2, GFP, and Drep-3, a cognate binding partner of Drep-2 (Inohara and Nuñez, 1999; Park and Park, 2012). Thus, we were successful in precipitating proteins interacting with Drep-2 and not merely peptides binding unspecifically. 14 of the 35 putative core interacting proteins are associated with membranes (Figure 7C, Supplementary file 1), consistent with our observation that Drep-2 localizes to the postsynaptic plasma membrane. However, neither DmGluRA nor Homer could be identified in the preparation.

Both mGluR and FMRP regulate local synaptic translation (Bhakar et al., 2012). Interestingly, 10 of the 35 proteins have been implicated in the control of mRNA translation and stability. Network analysis of putative interacting proteins (Figure 7D) underlined a strong connection of Drep-2 with RNA-associated proteins as well. Among them, several proteins regulating translation were found: for example, the eIF4G-related, cap-independent translation initiation factor NAT1/p97/DAP5 (Levy-Strumpf et al., 1997; Hundsdoerfer et al., 2005) and Caprin, a dendritic translational repressor (Shiina et al., 2005). Notably, Argonaute-2, involved in RNA interference (Ketting, 2011), was also among the most highly enriched proteins (Supplementary files 1 and 2). It is interesting in this context that both Caprin and Argonaute-2 bind to FMRP (Ishizuka et al., 2002; Papoulas et al., 2010). Finally, FMRP was identified within Drep-2 complexes at an FDR cutoff of 5% (Supplementary file 2). In fact, we could directly confirm the presence of FMRP in Drep-2GFP immunoprecipitates by immunoblotting (Figure 7E). FMRP, by contrast, could not be detected in an identically treated control experiment conducted in parallel, in which we precipitated the presynaptic protein Syd-1GFP (Owald et al., 2010) (Figure 7E). Thus, FMRP appears to be present specifically within Drep-2GFP complexes.

Since Drep-2 could be detected in protein complexes containing FMRP, translational control processes constitute a possible place of action of Drep-2. However, further mechanistic analysis will have to work out the details of the regulatory functions executed by the novel synaptic protein Drep-2.

Discussion

We here identify the CIDE-N family protein Drep-2 as a novel synaptic protein expressed in the Drosophila CNS (Figure 2) and important for learning and memory (Figure 5). Loss of Drep-2 did not cause transmission deficits at neuromuscular junctions and photoreceptor synapses (not shown) and we did not observe any structural deficits at synapses (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Thus, Drep-2 is most likely less important for maintaining either base-line transmission or fundamental synaptic architecture. Instead, it might be involved in the regulation of synaptic signaling and plasticity.

Expression of Drep-2 is particularly strong at postsynaptic densities (PSDs) of synapses between projection neurons (PNs) and Kenyon cells (KCs) in the mushroom body (MB) calyx (Figure 4). At these synapses, acetylcholine is released from PNs upon transmission of odor signals (Busto et al., 2010; Gu and O'Dowd, 2006). While elimination of Drep-2 in the whole animal severely impaired olfactory short-term and anesthesia-sensitive intermediate-term memory (Figure 5), re-expression of Drep-2 restricted to KCs was sufficient to fully rescue these learning deficits. As sensation of electric shock, the unconditioned stimulus in aversive olfactory learning, is mediated via dopamine in the MB lobes (Aso et al., 2012), Drep-2 most likely plays its role at PN::KC synapses during reception of the odor, the conditioned stimulus.

Several types of metabotropic receptors are expressed in the calyx: GABAB, dopamine, octopamine, and metabotropic glutamate receptors (Enell et al., 2007; Devaud et al., 2008; Busch et al., 2009; Mao and Davis, 2009; Kanellopoulos et al., 2012). Which exact role this metabotropic signaling plays in synaptic plasticity processes at PN::KC synapses is essentially unknown. Motivated by the close match between Drep-2 and DmGluRA localization on the level of individual PSDs of the PN-KC synapse (Figure 6A), we started to address a potential functional connection between Drep-2 and DmGluRA-dependent signaling and behavior. We observed that the learning deficits of dmGluRA and drep-2 single mutants did not add up to a stronger phenotype in double mutants (Figure 6D). Moreover, the drep-2 memory deficit was effectively rescued by pharmacological stimulation of DmGluRA (Figure 6C). Drep-2 might, therefore, affect plasticity processes downstream of DmGluRA. In this pathway, the protein could be required for extracting relevant aspects of the olfactory information (odor discrimination vs generalization) and thereby influence subsequent learning. Interestingly, preliminary experiments indicate that the learning deficits of drep-2 mutants might involve decreased Ca2+ responses in KCs (not shown).

How might Drep-2 interfere with metabotropic signaling? We were unable to biochemically detect DmGluRA in complexes containing Drep-2, making a direct interaction between both proteins unlikely. Neither could we find any indication of an influence of Drep-2 on mGluR protein levels or localization (not shown). However, we did confirm the presence of the fragile X mental retardation protein FMRP in complexes containing Drep-2 (Figure 7E). In addition to FMRP, several other additional RNA-associated proteins and translational regulators were found by quantitative affinity purification experiments of Drep-2GFP complexes, followed by mass spectrometry-based protein identification and quantification (Figure 7C,D, Supplementary files 1 and 2). It is, therefore, a probable scenario that Drep-2 indirectly regulates local synaptic protein synthesis.

Several studies have demonstrated that FMRP antagonizes mGluR-mediated synaptic translation (Bhakar et al., 2012). Notably, we found evidence of a functional antagonism between Drep-2 and FMRP-mediated plasticity: both drep-2 and heterozygous dfmr1 single mutants were deficient in olfactory conditioning, but drep-2ex13/drep-2ex13; dfmr1B55/+ double mutants showed normal performance (Figure 6E). Thus, Drep-2 might be required downstream of mGluR signaling, probably in the context of synaptic translation, counteracting translational repression executed by FMRP. We observed that only chronic activation of mGluRs starting during development could improve the learning scores of drep-2 mutants (Figure 6C). This result could be explained by chronic misregulation of neuronal translation in the mutants, rendering the synapses insensitive to enhanced mGluR signaling later in life.

At first glance, it might appear surprising that drep-2ex13/drep-2ex13 and dfmr1B55/+ mutants cancel each other out. However, evidence for a tightly balanced control over synaptic translation has been provided: mutations in the gene tsc2 cause tuberous sclerosis, a disease phenotypically similar to fragile X syndrome (FXS). Impaired long-term depression (LTD) in tsc2-mutant mice could be rescued by the application of mGluR agonists (Auerbach et al., 2011). Moreover, murine fmr1 mutants showed exaggerated LTD, while the double mutant exhibited normal LTD. This demonstrated that synaptic proteins, if misregulated by either impaired or excessive mGluR-induced translation, impede appropriate LTD. In this manner, misregulation of opposing effectors can cause similar phenotypes, as is the case for the olfactory learning performance of drep-2, dmGluRA, and dfmr1 mutants.

Fragile X associated tremor/ataxia syndrome (FXTAS) is a late-onset neurodegenerative disorder occurring in carriers of fragile X premutation repeats which is distinct from FXS. The presence of fragile X rCGG premutation repeats in flies activates the microRNA miR-277, which causes neurodegeneration (Tan et al., 2012). One of the targets negatively regulated by miR-277 is Drep-2. Notably, a putative drep-2 hypomorph was shown to enhance the FXTAS neurodegenerative phenotype. While Drep-2 function per se was not investigated in this study and the mechanistic details remain rather elusive, this independent connection between Drep-2 and a scenario related to FXS is also suggestive.

Additional experiments will be required to both examine whether Drep-2 plays a role during translational regulation and to further explore its relationship to metabotropic signaling. Nevertheless, based on our findings, a function of Drep-2 in regulating mGluR-mediated local translation is a possible scenario, which is now open for further investigation. In mammals, Dff-related CIDE proteins have, up to now, mainly been studied in fat tissue (Yonezawa et al., 2011). However, CIDEc is highly expressed in the mammalian brain (Li et al., 2009). It is now an intriguing possibility that Dff family proteins might play non-apoptotic neuronal and synaptic roles in mammals as well.

Materials and methods

Animal rearing and fly strains

All fly strains were reared under standard laboratory conditions (Sigrist et al., 2003) at 25°C and 65–70% humidity, with a constant 12/12 hr light/dark regimen. Flies were fed standard semi-defined cornmeal/molasses medium. Bloomington stock collection strain #5905, w1118, was used as background for both the generation of transgenic animals (Bestgene, Inc., Chino Hills, CA) and for behavioral assays.

The following fly stocks were used: 30y-Gal4 (Yang et al., 1995), c305a-Gal4 (Krashes et al., 2007), elavc155-Gal4 (Lin and Goodman, 1994), elavL3-Gal4 (elavIII-Gal4) (Luo et al., 1994), gh146-Gal4 (Stocker et al., 1997), mb247-Gal4 (Zars et al., 2000), mb247::Dα7GFP (Kremer et al., 2010), UAS-Dα7GFP (Leiss et al., 2009b), Dfw45−30n (Bloomington stock #4966), dmGluRA112b and its control dmGluRA2b (Bogdanik et al., 2004), fmr1B55 (Inoue et al., 2002), and w1118 (Hazelrigg et al., 1984). All flies used for behavioral experiments were outcrossed to w1118 for more than five generations in order to generate an isogenic genetic background.

Generation of transgenic flies

Drep-2ex13 mutants were generated using FLP–FRT recombination between the two stocks drep-2d00223 and drep-2e04659, as previously described (Parks et al., 2004). Drep-2ex27 mutants were created in an analogous manner, using the transposon lines drep-2e02920 and drep-2e04659. In short, one of the elements containing a FRT site was combined with a line expressing the FLP recombinase under a heat shock promoter. These flies were crossed with the strain containing the second element to place both FRT sites in trans. Expression of FLP recombinase was triggered by a heat shock to 37°C. Offspring were collected and mutant candidates were validated by genomic PCR (forward primer: 5′-GCT GCT TGA GTA TGG GTG CA-3′; reverse primer: 5′-GGA GAC ATC CTC TCA AAG C-3′).

We generated transgenic flies expressing either plain drep-2 cDNA or eGFP- or mStrawberry-tagged drep-2 constructs, all under the UAS enhancer. The drep-2 cDNA LD32009 was amplified using the forward primer 5′-CAT GCC ATG GCA ATG GCC AGA GAG GAG TCT CGC-3′ and the reverse primer 5′-CGG GGT ACC AAT TCT GTC CTC CTC ATC CTC TTC C-3′. The amplicon was inserted into the pEnter vector using NcoI and KpnI restriction sites. Invitrogen gateway cloning was used to create the expression constructs from pEnter. The vectors pTWG and pTGW (Carnegie Institution of Washington, Washington, DC) were used for generation of eGFP constructs; eGFP was replaced by mStrawberry by PCR for mStrawberry constructs.

Lack of dmGluRA in drep-2ex13; dmGluRA112b double mutants was validated by single-fly PCR. The sequence of the primers for this PCR was as follows: forward primer: 5′-GGT GCC CCT TGC GGA CCA AA-3′; reverse primer: 5′- TTG TCG TCT GCG GCA CTG GG-3′. Lack of drep-2 was confirmed by stainings.

Adult life span

In order to assay the life span, male flies were placed in groups of 25 animals in small food vials and transferred to fresh vials at least twice a week. Flies were kept at standard conditions. After each transfer, the number of dead and remaining live flies was counted. The number of days for each vial was determined at which 50% of flies were dead.

Aversive olfactory conditioning

All experiments were conducted with three- to 5-day-old animals and carried out in a w1118 genetic background. Flies were raised at 24°C and 60% relative humidity with a 14/10 hr light–dark cycle on cornmeal-based food prepared according to the Würzburg recipe (Guo et al., 1996). Flies were transferred to fresh food vials for up to 48 hr before behavioral experiments. Behavioral experiments were performed in dim red light at 70% relative humidity with 3-octanol (1:150 dilution in mineral oil presented in a 14 mm cup) and 4-methyl-cyclohexanol (1:100 dilution in mineral oil presented in a 14 mm cup) serving as olfactory cues and 120V AC current serving as a behavioral reinforcer.

Associative training was carried out following the single-cycle training procedure previously described (Tully and Quinn, 1985). Electric foot shock was applied after 10 s of odor presentation; afterwards twelve shock/odor pairings were conducted within 50 s. Odors and electric shock were applied in the same manner during conditioning as when testing for sensory acuity. STM was tested immediately after the end of the training session, 3 min after the onset of training. Performance of ITM and ARM was determined 3 hr after training; flies were transferred to neutral containers without food for the resting period. Two groups of flies were separately trained for separation of consolidated ARM and labile ASM, and one group was cooled in an ice-bath (0°C) for 90 s, 2.5 hr after training. Odor memory of this group was tested after a 30-min recovery period, that is, 3 hr after onset of training. Since labile ASM is erased by this procedure, performance of the cooled group is solely due to ARM.

Pharmaceutical components (MPEP (ab120008, Abcam, Cambridge, MA) and 1S,3R-ACPD (#0284, Tocris Bioscience, Bristol, United Kingdom)) were supplemented to liquid fly food, as previously described (McBride et al., 2005; Tauber et al., 2011). 1S,3R-ACPD was used in a concentration of 72.2 µM and MPEP at 9.7 µM (Parmentier et al., 1996; McBride et al., 2005, 2010). Both compounds were diluted in H2O. The same amount of H2O lacking any additional compounds was added to the control food. Flies were either raised throughout their entire development and adulthood on this food or, where indicated, were raised on control food lacking ACPD and only transferred to food containing ACPD after eclosion.

Calculation of behavioral indices was carried out as previously published (Tully and Quinn, 1985). ASM can be calculated by subtracting the performance of the cooled group from an uncooled group. Non-parametric tests (Mann–Whitney U test or Kruskal–Wallis) were used because of the small sample sizes. The significance level α was set to 5%. Asterisks are used to indicate significance in figures (* = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001; ns = p ≥ 0.05). If several genotypes were compared, α and * symbols were adjusted by dividing the significance level by the number of comparisons (Bonferroni correction). Experimental data were analyzed using Microsoft Office 2011 and OriginLab (Northampton, MA) Origin Pro 9.0. Graphs were created using Gnuplot v4.6 (http://www.gnuplot.info) and Adobe Illustrator CS4.

In situ hybridization

In situ hybridizations of whole mount embryos were performed as described by the Berkeley Drosophila Genome Project (http://www.fruitfly.org). The plasmid LD32009 was cut using BamHI and in vitro transcribed using Sp6 RNA polymerase to prepare antisense RNA probes. The plasmid was cut with SmaI and transcribed with T7 RNA polymerase to prepare sense probes.

Antibodies

The drep-2 cDNA LD32009 was amplified using the forward primer 5′-GAC CGT CGA CGT GGG TGT GGG AGC TGT CCA-3′ and the reverse primer 5′-GAC CCT CGA GTG AAT TCT GTC CTC CTC ATC CTC-3′. The amplicon was inserted into the pENTR4 vector (Invitrogen, Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA) using SalI and XhoI restriction sites. Invitrogen gateway cloning was used to create a 6xHis-tagged construct in pDEST17 (Invitrogen). A rabbit serum against this 6xHis-tagged C-terminal Drep-2 fusion protein (amino acids 252-483 of Drep-2-PA) was produced (Seqlab, Göttingen, Germany) and affinity-purified with the same fusion protein.

Antibody concentrations were as follows: mouse anti-BrpNc82 (Wagh et al., 2006) 1:100, guinea pig anti-BrpN-Term 1:800, mouse anti-ChAT4B1 (Yasuyama and Salvaterra, 1999) 1:1000, rat anti-Dα7 (Fayyazuddin et al., 2006) 1:2000, mouse anti-Dlg4F3 (Parnas et al., 2001) 1:500, mouse anti-DmGluRA7G11 (Panneels et al., 2003) 1:100, rabbit anti-Drep-2C-Term 1:500, mouse anti-Fasciclin-II1D4 (Lin and Goodman, 1994) 1:50, mouse anti-FMRP5A11 (Okamura et al., 2004) 1:100, mouse anti-GFP3E6 (Molecular Probes, Life Technologies) 1:500, rabbit anti-GFP (A11122, Life Technologies) 1:1000, guinea pig anti-Homer (Diagana et al., 2002) 1:200, rabbit anti-Syd-1 (Owald et al., 2010) 1:500, mouse anti-α-TubulinDM1A (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) 1:100000, goat anti-mouse Alexa 488 (A11001, Invitrogen) 1:500, goat anti-rabbit Cy3 (111-167-003, Dianova, Hamburg, Germany) 1:500, goat anti-guinea pig Cy3 (106-166-003, Dianova) 1:500, donkey anti-rat Cy3 (712-165-153, Dianova) 1:250, goat anti-rabbit Atto 647N (40839, Sigma-Aldrich) 1:200, and goat anti-rabbit HRP (111-035-144, Dianova) 1:5000.

Immunohistochemistry and imaging

Adult brains were dissected in ice-cold hemolymph-like saline (HL3) solution, fixed for 20 min in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 1x phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), pH 7.2, and then blocked in 5% normal goat serum (NGS) in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100 (PBT) for 30 min. The brains were incubated with primary antibodies together with 5% NGS for 48 hr at room temperature (RT) and then washed in PBT for 3 hr, followed by overnight incubation with secondary antibodies at RT. The brains were then washed for 3 hr with PBT and mounted in VectaShield (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) on slides. 3- to 7-day-old female flies were used for dissections.

Conventional confocal images were acquired at 21°C with a Leica Microsystems (Wetzlar, Germany) TCS SP5 confocal microscope using a 63×, 1.4 NA oil objective for detailed scans and a 20×, 0.7 NA oil objective for overview scans. Lateral pixel size was set to values around 90 nm for detailed scans. Exact values varied, depending on the situation. Typically, 1024 × 1024 images were scanned at 100 Hz using 4× line averaging. All images were acquired using the Leica LCS AF software.

Confocal stacks were processed using ImageJ software (http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij). Deconvolution of images was conducted using MediaCybernetics (Rockville, MD) AutoQuant X2.1.1. Contrast was adapted for visualization, where necessary, using the levels tool in Adobe Photoshop CS4. Images shown in a comparison or quantified were processed with exactly the same parameters. Images were not post-processed before quantification, but exclusively afterwards and only for visualization. Cell body and active zone counts were quantified similarly as described previously (Kremer et al., 2010; Christiansen et al., 2011); the area of interest was segmented in ImageJ and then analyzed in Bitplane (Zürich, Switzerland) Imaris v6.23 using the surface tool. The cell body counts are comparable to the published number of 700 KCs in mb247-Gal4 (Schwaerzel et al., 2002). Active zone numbers were assessed via an anti-Syd-1 staining; counts were similar to the published number of 28,000–30,000 synapses in the calyx (Kremer et al., 2010).

STED microscopy was performed using a Leica Microsystems TCS STED setup equipped with a 100×, 1.4 NA oil immersion STED objective, as previously described (Waites et al., 2011). The depletion laser (Mai Tai Ti:Sapphire; Spectra Physics, Newport, Santa Clara, CA) was set to 760 nm. 1024 × 1024 STED images were scanned at 10 Hz using 2× line averaging. STED images were processed using linear deconvolution software integrated into the Imspector software (Max-Planck-Innovation, München, Germany).

Immunoelectron and electron microscopy

Brains were dissected in HL3 solution and fixed for 20 min at RT with 4% paraformaldehyde and 0.2% glutaraldehyde in a buffer containing 50 mM sodium cacodylate and 50 mM NaCl at pH 7.5. Afterwards, brains were washed twice in the buffer and dehydrated through a series of increasing alcohol concentrations. Samples were embedded in LR-Gold resin by incubation in ethanol/LR-Gold 1:1 solution overnight at 4°C, followed by ethanol/LR-Gold 1:5 solution for 4 hr at RT and, finally, 3× with LR-Gold/0.2% benzil once overnight, then for 4 hr and again overnight. Thereafter, the brains were placed in BEEM (West Chester, PA) capsules covered with LR-Gold/0.2% benzil resin and placed under a UV lamp at 4°C for 5 days to allow for polymerization of the resin.

Following embedding, 70–80 nm sections were cut using a Leica Ultracut E ultramicrotome equipped with a 2-mm diamond knife. Sections were collected on 100 mesh nickel grids (Plano, Wetzlar, Germany) coated with 0.1% Pioloform resin and transferred to a buffer solution (20 mM Tris–HCl, 0.9% NaCl, pH 8.0). Prior to staining, sections were blocked for 10 min with 0.04% BSA in buffer. Sections were incubated with the primary antibody in blocking solution overnight at 4°C. After washing 4× in buffer, sections were incubated in buffer containing the secondary antibody (goat anti-rabbit 10 nm colloidal gold, BBI Solutions, Cardiff, United Kingdom, 1:100) for 2–3 hr at RT. Finally, the sections were washed 4× in buffer and 3x in distilled water. Contrast was enhanced by placing the grids in 2% uranyl acetate for 30 min, followed by 3× washing with water and, afterwards, incubation in lead citrate for 2 min. The grids were then washed 3× with water and dried. Images were acquired on a FEI (Hillsboro, OR) Tecnai Spirit, 120 kV transmission electron microscope equipped with a FEI 2K Eagle CCD camera.

Western blot analysis of adult heads

Fly head protein extraction was performed as follows: flies were decapitated and 20 heads of each genotype were sheared manually in 40 µl of 2% SDS aqueous solution using a micropistil fitting tightly into a 1.5-ml cup. An amount of 4 µl of a 10% Triton-X 100 aqueous solution and 40 µl of 2× sample buffer (Laemmli, 1970) was added, and samples were heated at 95°C for 10 min. After centrifugation for 5 min at 16,000×g, in order to pellet the debris, 8.4 µl of the sample (equivalent to two fly heads) was subjected to denaturing SDS-PAGE using an 8% Tris-Cl gel. Proteins were transferred onto a nitrocellulose membrane, blocked with 5% skim-milk in 1× PBS supplemented with 0.1% Tween-20 and probed with affinity-purified rabbit anti-Drep-2C-Term (#7183; 1:5000) diluted in 5% skim-milk in 1x PBS, supplemented with 0.1% Tween-20, followed by washing steps. Secondary anti-rabbit IgG horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated antibodies (Dianova) and an enhanced chemoluminescence detection system (RPN 2232, GE Healthcare (Little Chalfont, United Kingdom) ECL Prime) with Hyperfilm ECL (GE Healthcare) were used for detection. After NaN3 treatment, membranes were reprobed for α-Tubulin as a loading control, using the monoclonal antibody DM1A (Sigma; 1:100000).

Head fractionation and enrichment blot (synaptosome-like preparation)

The Drosophila head fractionation protocol has recently been published (Depner et al., 2014). It is based on protocols from mammalian subcellular preparations (Huttner et al., 1983; Ahmed et al., 2013). In brief, Drosophila wild-type heads were sheared mechanically in the absence of detergents and differential centrifugation was applied to separate particles according to their size and density. Analytical samples from the fractions obtained were taken and the protein concentration determined. An amount of 10 μg total protein from each fraction was subjected to SDS-PAGE (10% polyacrylamide Tris-Tricine gel (Schägger, 2006)), followed by immunoblotting for Drep-2C-Term. HRP-conjugated goat anti-rabbit antibodies (111-035-144, Dianova, 1:5000) were used for ECL detection. Signals were recorded using an ImageQuant LAS 4000 image reader (GE Healthcare). Films were scanned in transmission mode (Epson (Long Beach, CA) V770 scanner).

Nuclease activity assay

The drep-2 cDNA LD32009 was fused to an N-terminal His-tagged maltose-binding protein for expression and purification of Drep-2. Drep-2 was transformed in Escherichia coli BL21 Rosetta2 (DE3) pLys cells (NEB, Ipswich, MA). Drep-2 was cultured in TB medium at 37°C until an OD of ∼1.0 was reached and subsequently cooled down to 20°C. Protein expression was induced by the addition of 0.5 mM IPTG. Cells grew overnight and were harvested by centrifugation (6 min, 6000 rpm at 4°C). The Drep-2 pellet was resuspended in 20 mM Tris/HCl pH 7.4, 250 mM NaCl, 8 mM imidazole, and 1 mM DTT. Cells were lysed by sonication at 4°C and the supernatant was cleared by 45 min centrifugation (21,500 rpm at 4°C). A Ni2+-NTA (cv ∼1 ml; GE Healthcare) was equilibrated with 20 mM Tris/HCl pH 7.4, 250 mM NaCl, 1 mM MgCl2, 8 mM imidazole, and 1 mM DTT.

Drep-2 was loaded onto the column and washed with 3 cv of equilibration buffer. Drep-2 was eluted in a linear gradient to 20 mM Tris/HCl pH 7.5, 250 mM NaCl, 400 mM imidazole, and 1 mM DTT. The maltose-binding protein was cleaved by TEV proteases, yielding untagged Drep-2 protein, during dialysis in 20 mM Tris/HCl pH 7.4, 100 mM NaCl, and 1 mM DTT and loaded onto a MonoQ 10/100 column (GE Healthcare) equilibrated with 20 mM Tris/HCl pH 7.4 and 1 mM DTT. Drep-2 was eluted in a linear gradient from 100 to 1000 mM NaCl. Size exclusion chromatography was performed with a HighLoad Superdex S200 16/60 column (GE Healthcare) equilibrated with 20 mM Tris/HCl pH 7.5, 250 mM NaCl, and 1 mM DTT.

The actual nuclease activity assay was conducted as follows: an amount of 10 μg of Drep-2 was incubated with 0.3 μg linearized pUC19 plasmid DNA in 15-μl reaction buffer (20 mM Hepes-NaOH pH 7.4, 50 mM NaCl, 5 mM MgCl2) at 37°C in limited digestion experiments. Aliquots were taken at different time intervals and the reaction was stopped by DNA loading dye containing 10 mM EDTA. Samples were electrophoretically separated over a 1% (wt/vol) agarose gel containing ethidium bromide.

Quantitative affinity purification and mass spectrometry

Drep-2 in vivo interaction partners were identified using affinity purification and mass spectrometry (q-AP-MS; Figure 7A). The major challenge in such experiments is to distinguish true interaction partners from non-specific contaminants. Q-AP-MS can solve this problem by comparing the abundance of identified proteins with a control (Vermeulen et al., 2008; Paul et al., 2011).

Drep-2GFP was expressed using elavc155-Gal4. We purified Drep-2GFP from fly heads using a single chain anti-GFP antibody coupled to agarose beads. We performed parallel pulldowns on the same lysates using control agarose beads to control for unspecific binding. As an additional control, GFP-negative lysate from wild-type flies was included into the experiment. We identified a total of 3284 proteins in the pulldown experiments. 202 proteins were significantly enriched in GFP pulldowns compared to plain bead controls. 35 of these proteins could be confirmed in comparative analysis with GFP pulldowns of wild-type lysates and were, therefore, defined as robust interactors/core proteins (permutation-based FDR = 1%; S0 = 1, Figure 7B).

Pulldown experiments for the quantitative affinity purification were conducted in the following manner: an amount of 500 µl of fly heads was immersed in liquid nitrogen and pulverized mechanically with a BioPulverizer (Biospec Products, Bartlesville, OK). Powdered tissue was homogenized in 500 µl cold lysis buffer (50 mM Tris–HCl (pH 7.6), 150 mM NaCl, 1 mM MgCl2, 1 mM EDTA, 10% glycerol, 0.4% DOC, and protease inhibitors (Complete Mini, Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN)). After incubation on ice for 30 min, 500 µl lysis buffer without DOC and Triton were added to a final concentration of 1%. Samples were centrifuged at 14,000×g for 15 min at 4°C to remove insoluble material. The supernatant was transferred to a fresh tube for pull-down experiments.

Immunoprecipitations of GFP-tagged bait proteins were performed in triplicate using GFP-Trap agarose beads (Chromotek, Planegg-Martinsried, Germany) according to the manufacturer's instructions. Soluble protein fractions were incubated with either 25 μl of GFP-Trap or plain control beads for 60 min at 4°C under constant rotation. The beads were washed twice with washing buffer (50 mM Tris–HCl (pH 7.6), 150 mM NaCl, 1 mM MgCl2, 1 mM EDTA, 10% glycerol) and once with PBS. Proteins bound to the beads were eluted by applying 50 μl elution buffer (6 M urea/2 M thiourea) twice and proceeded to in-solution digestion followed by LC-MS/MS analysis.

Liquid chromatography MS/MS analysis was performed as follows: protein eluates were reduced for 30 min at RT in 10 mM dithiothreitol solution, followed by alkylation by 55 mM iodacetamide for 20 min in the dark at RT. The endoproteinase LysC (Wako, Osaka, Japan) was added following a protein:enzyme ratio of 50:1 and incubated for 4 hr at RT. After dilution of the sample with 4x digestion buffer (50 mM ammonium bi-carbonate in water (pH 8.0)), sequence grade modified trypsin (Promega, Madison, WI) was added (same protein:enzyme ratio as for LysC) and digested overnight. Finally, trypsin and Lys-C activity was quenched by acidification of the reaction mixtures with TFA to pH ∼2. Afterwards, peptides were extracted and desalted using StageTips (Rappsilber et al., 2003).

Peptide mixtures were separated by reversed phase chromatography using the EASY-nLC system (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA) on in-house manufactured 20 cm fritless silica microcolumns with an inner diameter of 75 µm. Columns were packed with ReproSil-Pur C18-AQ 3 µm resin (Dr. Maisch GmbH, Ammerbuch-Entringen, Germany). Peptides were separated on an 8–60% acetonitrile gradient (214 min) with 0.5% formic acid at a nanoflow rate of 200 nl/min. Eluting peptides were directly ionized by electrospray ionization and transferred into a Q Exactive mass spectrometer (Thermo Scientific). Mass spectrometry was performed in the data-dependent positive mode with one full scan (m/z range = 300-1700; R = 70,000; target value: 3 × 106; maximum injection time = 120 ms). The ten most intense ions with a charge state greater than one were selected (R = 35,000, target value = 5 x 105; isolation window = 4 m/z; maximum injection time = 120 ms). Dynamic exclusion for selected precursor ions was set to 30 s.

MS/MS data were analyzed by MaxQuant software v1.2.2.5 as described (Cox et al., 2011). The internal Andromeda search engine was used to search MS/MS spectra against a decoy D. melanogaster UniProt database (DROME.2016-06) containing forward and reverse sequences. The search included variable modifications of methionine oxidation and N-terminal acetylation, and fixed modification of carbamidomethyl cysteine. Minimal peptide length was set to six amino acids and a maximum of two missed cleavages was allowed. The FDR was set to 0.01 for peptide and protein identifications. If the identified peptide sequence set of one protein was equal to or contained another protein's peptide set, these two proteins were grouped together and the proteins were not counted as independent hits.

Label-free quantification (LFQ) was performed in MaxQuant as described (Hubner et al., 2010). Unique and razor peptides were considered for quantification with a minimum ratio count of 1. Retention times were recalibrated based on the built-in nonlinear time-rescaling algorithm. MS/MS identifications were transferred between LC-MS/MS runs with the ‘Match between runs’ option, in which the maximal retention time window was set to 2 min. For every peptide, corresponding total signals from multiple runs were compared to determine peptide ratios. Median values of all peptide ratios of one protein then represent a robust estimate of the protein ratio. LFQ intensity values were logarithmized and missing values were imputed with random numbers from a normal distribution whose mean and standard deviation were chosen to best simulate low abundance values below the noise level (width = 0.3; shift = 1.8). GFP pull-down samples and plain-bead control samples were selected as individual groups of three technical replicates each; significantly enriched proteins were determined by a volcano plot-based strategy, combining standard two-sample t-test p-values with ratio information. Significance corresponding to an FDR of 1, 5, or 10% was determined by a permutation-based method (Tusher et al., 2001).

The network of biochemical interactions (Figure 7D) was created using Microsoft Excel 2011, Cytoscape v2.8.3/v3.0.0 (http://www.cytoscape.org) and Adobe Illustrator CS4, following the protocol available at http://protocols.andlauer.net/cytoscape.pdf.

Pulldown experiments

Flies of the genotype elavc155-Gal4; uas-drep-2GFP were used for preparations containing Drep-2GFP and flies of the genotype elavc155-Gal4; uas-syd-1GFP (Owald et al., 2010) were used for preparations containing Syd-1GFP. For each experiment, 500 µl of adult fly heads were mechanically homogenized in 500 µL lysis buffer (50 mM Tris pH 8.0, 150 mM KCl, 1 mM MgCl2, 1 mM EGTA, 10% glycerol-containing protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche Diagnostics)). Sodium deoxycholate (10%) was added to achieve a final concentration of sodium deoxycholate of 0.4% and the lysate was incubated for 30 min on ice. The lysate was diluted 1:1 with sodium deoxycholate-free lysis buffer, then 10% Triton X-100 was added for a final concentration of 1% Triton X-100, and the lysate was rotated at 4°C for 30 min.

After centrifugation for 15 min at 16,000×g, the supernatant was used in immunoprecipitations with GFP-Trap-A beads and blocked agarose beads as binding control (Chromotek). After incubation at 4°C overnight, the beads were washed in buffer without detergent and glycerol. Proteins were eluted from the beads with SDS sample buffer. The samples were separated by one-dimensional SDS-PAGE gradient gel (TGX 4–12% precast, Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA).

Proteins were transferred onto a nitrocellulose membrane and probed with mouse anti-FMRP5A11 (1:100). A secondary anti-mouse IgG horseradish peroxidase (HRP)–conjugated antibody (Dianova) and an enhanced chemoluminescence (ECL) detection system with Hyperfilm ECL (GE Healthcare) were used for detection. After NaN3 treatment, the membranes were re-probed with rabbit anti-Drep2C-term (1:2000) and rabbit anti-GFP (1:1000) (Life technologies, A11122). Films were scanned in transmission mode (Epson V770) and images were imported to Adobe Photoshop.

Acknowledgements

We thank Madeleine Brünner, Anastasia Stawrakakis and Nicole Holton for excellent technical assistance. We thank Reinhard Jahn for the opportunity to perform experiments in his lab at the Max Planck Institute for Biophysical Chemistry in Göttingen, Germany. We are grateful to Irmgard Sinning, Mikiko Siomi, Uli Thomas, and Hugo Bellen for generously contributing antibodies. This work was supported by the following grants: Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) to SJS: SFB958/A6, Exc257, FOR1363; DFG to SSK and MSc: SCHW1410/1-1; DFG to BL and MCW: SFB958/A6; DFG to AF and SD: SFB 889/B9. Freie Universität Berlin DynAge Focus Area to SJS. German Ministry of Research and Education (BMBF) via the Bernstein Center Göttingen (BCCN II/B1) to AF and SD: grant number 01GQ1005A.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659 Exc257, FOR1363 to Stephan J Sigrist.

  • Freie Universität Berlin DynAge Focus Area to Stephan J Sigrist.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659 SCHW1410/1-1 to Sabrina Scholz-Kornehl, Martin Schwärzel.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659 SFB 889/B09 to Shubham Dipt, André Fiala.

  • Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung via the Bernstein Center Göttingen (BCCN II/B1) FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100002347 01GQ1005A to Shubham Dipt, André Fiala.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft FundRef identification ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659 SFB958/A6 to Bernhard Loll, Markus C Wahl, Stephan J Sigrist.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

TFMA, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

SSK, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

MK, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

HD, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

SD, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

RT, Acquisition of data, Drafting or revising the article.

HAB, Acquisition of data, Drafting or revising the article.

CQ, Acquisition of data, Drafting or revising the article.

VKG, Acquisition of data, Drafting or revising the article.

MGH, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data.

MC, Acquisition of data, Drafting or revising the article.

BL, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

MCW, Conception and design, Drafting or revising the article.

AF, Conception and design, Drafting or revising the article.

MSe, Conception and design, Drafting or revising the article.

MSc, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data.

SJS, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1.

Mass spectrometry: core proteins enriched over both controls. The table shows proteins enriched at an FDR of 1%. Ranks are based on the ratios of Drep-2GFP IPs, GFP beads vs. plain beads (compare Figure 7A–B). Putative functions were derived from Flybase. GFP (#6) was removed from this list.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.015

elife03895s001.docx (164.8KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03895.015
Supplementary file 2.

Mass spectrometry: all proteins enriched at an FDR of 10%. The table shows two ratios for all proteins: first, Drep-2GFP animals, GFP beads vs. plain beads and, second, GFP beads, Drep-2GFP vs. wild-type (wt) animals (compare Figure 7A). “+” symbols indicate whether a protein was significantly enriched at a given FDR. Proteins are sorted according to the first ratio. Proteins were labeled in cyan if significantly enriched for both ratios at an FDR of 1%; these proteins constitute the core interactors. Protein names were labeled in red if the protein was positively enriched for the first ratio but negatively enriched for the second one, and thus probably constitutes a false positive. Protein names were labeled in shades of green if the protein was included as an additional interactor in the network of interacting proteins (Figure 7D): dark green = FDR 1%, medium green = FDR 5%, light green = FDR 10%.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.016

elife03895s002.docx (226.9KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03895.016

References

  1. Ahmed S, Holt M, Riedel D, Jahn R. 2013. Small-scale isolation of synaptic vesicles from mammalian brain. Nature Protocols 8:998–1009. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aso Y, Herb A, Ogueta M, Siwanowicz I, Templier T, Friedrich AB, Ito K, Scholz H, Tanimoto H. 2012. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLOS Genetics 8:e1002768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Auerbach BD, Osterweil EK, Bear MF. 2011. Mutations causing syndromic autism define an axis of synaptic pathophysiology. Nature 480:63–68. doi: 10.1038/nature10658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bhakar AL, Dölen G, Bear MF. 2012. The pathophysiology of fragile X (and what it teaches us about synapses). Annual Review of Neuroscience 35:417–443. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bogdanik L, Mohrmann R, Ramaekers A, Bockaert J, Grau Y, Broadie KS, Parmentier ML. 2004. The Drosophila metabotropic glutamate receptor DmGluRA regulates activity-dependent synaptic facilitation and fine synaptic morphology. The Journal of Neuroscience 24:9105–9116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2724-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bolduc FV, Bell K, Cox H, Broadie KS, Tully T. 2008. Excess protein synthesis in Drosophila fragile X mutants impairs long-term memory. Nature Neuroscience 11:1143–1145. doi: 10.1038/nn.2175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Busch S, Selcho M, Ito K, Tanimoto H. 2009. A map of octopaminergic neurons in the Drosophila brain. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 513:643–667. doi: 10.1002/cne.21966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Busto GU, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Davis RL. 2010. Olfactory learning in Drosophila. Physiology 25:338–346. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00026.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Christiansen F, Zube C, Andlauer TFM, Wichmann C, Fouquet W, Owald D, Mertel S, Leiss F, Tavosanis G, Farca Luna AJ, Luna AJ. 2011. Presynapses in Kenyon cell Dendrites in the mushroom body calyx of Drosophila. The Journal of Neuroscience 31:9696–9707. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6542-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cox J, Neuhauser N, Michalski A, Scheltema RA, Olsen JV, Mann M. 2011. Andromeda: a peptide search engine integrated into the MaxQuant environment. Journal of Proteome Research 10:1794–1805. doi: 10.1021/pr101065j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Davis RL. 2004. Olfactory learning. Neuron 44:31–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Davis RL. 2011. Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron 70:8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Depner H, Lützkendorf J, Babikir HA, Sigrist SJ, Holt MG. 2014. Differential centrifugation-based biochemical fractionation of the Drosophila adult CNS. Nature Protocols. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2014.192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Devaud JM, Clouet-Redt C, Bockaert J, Grau Y, Parmentier ML. 2008. Widespread brain distribution of the Drosophila metabotropic glutamate receptor. Neuroreport 19:367–371. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f524c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diagana TT, Thomas U, Prokopenko SN, Xiao B, Worley PF, Thomas JB. 2002. Mutation of Drosophila homer disrupts control of locomotor activity and behavioral plasticity. The Journal of Neuroscience 22:428–436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dubnau J, Chiang AS. 2013. Systems memory consolidation in Drosophila. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23:84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Enari M, Sakahira H, Yokoyama H, Okawa K, Iwamatsu A, Nagata S. 1998. A caspase-activated DNase that degrades DNA during apoptosis, and its inhibitor ICAD. Nature 391:43–50. doi: 10.1038/34112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Enell LE, Hamasaka Y, Kolodziejczyk A, Nässel DR. 2007. gamma-Aminobutyric acid (GABA) signaling components in Drosophila: immunocytochemical localization of GABA(B) receptors in relation to the GABA(A) receptor subunit RDL and a vesicular GABA transporter. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 505:18–31. doi: 10.1002/cne.21472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fayyazuddin A, Zaheer MA, Hiesinger PR, Bellen HJ. 2006. The nicotinic acetylcholine receptor Dalpha7 is required for an escape behavior in Drosophila. PLOS Biology 4:e63. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Folkers E, Drain P, Quinn WG. 1993. Radish, a Drosophila mutant deficient in consolidated memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA 90:8123–8127. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.17.8123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Graveley BR, Brooks AN, Carlson JW, Duff MO, Landolin JM, Yang L, Artieri CG, van Baren MJ, Boley N, Booth BW, Brown JB, Cherbas L, Davis CA, Dobin A, Li R, Lin W, Malone JH, Mattiuzzo NR, Miller D, Sturgill D, Tuch BB, Zaleski C, Zhang D, Blanchette M, Dudoit S, Eads B, Green RE, Hammonds A, Jiang L, Kapranov P, Langton L, Perrimon N, Sandler JE, Wan KH, Willingham A, Zhang Y, Zou Y, Andrews J, Bickel PJ, Brenner SE, Brent MR, Cherbas P, Gingeras TR, Hoskins RA, Kaufman TC, Oliver B, Celniker SE. 2011. The developmental transcriptome of Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 471:473–479. doi: 10.1038/nature09715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gross C, Berry-Kravis EM, Bassell GJ. 2012. Therapeutic strategies in fragile X syndrome: dysregulated mGluR signaling and beyond. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:178–195. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gu H, O'Dowd DK. 2006. Cholinergic synaptic transmission in adult Drosophila Kenyon cells in situ. The Journal of Neuroscience 26:265–272. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4109-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Guo A, Li L, Xia SZ, Feng CH, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. 1996. Conditioned visual flight orientation in Drosophila: dependence on age, practice, and diet. Learning & Memory 3:49–59. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hamasaka Y, Rieger D, Parmentier ML, Grau Y, Helfrich-Förster C, Nässel DR. 2007. Glutamate and its metabotropic receptor in Drosophilaclock neuron circuits. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 505:32–45. doi: 10.1002/cne.21471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hazelrigg T, Levis R, Rubin GM. 1984. Transformation of white locus DNA in drosophila: dosage compensation, zeste interaction, and position effects. Cell 36:469–481. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(84)90240-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hubner NC, Bird AW, Cox J, Splettstoesser B, Bandilla P, Poser I, Hyman A, Mann M. 2010. Quantitative proteomics combined with BAC TransgeneOmics reveals in vivo protein interactions. The Journal of Cell Biology 189:739–754. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200911091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hundsdoerfer P, Thoma C, Hentze MW. 2005. Eukaryotic translation initiation factor 4GI and p97 promote cellular internal ribosome entry sequence-driven translation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA 102:13421–13426. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506536102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Huttner WB, Schiebler W, Greengard P, De Camilli P. 1983. Synapsin I (protein I), a nerve terminal-specific phosphoprotein. III. Its association with synaptic vesicles studied in a highly purified synaptic vesicle preparation. The Journal of Cell Biology 96:1374–1388. doi: 10.1083/jcb.96.5.1374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Inohara N, Nuñez G. 1999. Genes with homology to DFF/CIDEs found in Drosophila melanogaster. Cell Death and Differentiation 6:823–824. doi: 10.1038/sj.cdd.4400570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Inoue S, Shimoda M, Nishinokubi I, Siomi MC, Okamura M, Nakamura A, Kobayashi S, Ishida N, Siomi H. 2002. A role for the Drosophila fragile X-related gene in circadian output. Current Biology 12:1331–1335. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01036-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ishizuka A, Siomi MC, Siomi H. 2002. A Drosophila fragile X protein interacts with components of RNAi and ribosomal proteins. Genes & Development 16:2497–2508. doi: 10.1101/gad.1022002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kahsai L, Carlsson MA, Winther AM, Nässel DR. 2012. Distribution of metabotropic receptors of serotonin, dopamine, GABA, glutamate, and short neuropeptide F in the central complex of Drosophila. Neuroscience 208:11–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kanellopoulos AK, Semelidou O, Kotini AG, Anezaki M, Skoulakis EM. 2012. Learning and memory deficits consequent to reduction of the fragile X mental retardation protein result from metabotropic glutamate receptor-mediated inhibition of cAMP signaling in Drosophila. The Journal of Neuroscience 32:13111–13124. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1347-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ketting RF. 2011. The many faces of RNAi. Developmental Cell 20:148–161. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2011.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Krashes MJ, Keene AC, Leung B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. 2007. Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron 53:103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kremer MC, Christiansen F, Leiss F, Paehler M, Knapek S, Andlauer TFM, Förstner F, Kloppenburg P, Sigrist SJ, Tavosanis G. 2010. Structural long-term changes at mushroom body input synapses. Current Biology 20:1938–1944. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Krueger DD, Bear MF. 2011. Toward fulfilling the promise of molecular medicine in fragile X syndrome. Annual Review of Medicine 62:411–429. doi: 10.1146/annurev-med-061109-134644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Laemmli UK. 1970. Cleavage of structural proteins during the assembly of the head of bacteriophage T4. Nature 227:680–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leiss F, Groh C, Butcher NJ, Meinertzhagen IA, Tavosanis G. 2009a. Synaptic organization in the adult Drosophila mushroom body calyx. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 517:808–824. doi: 10.1002/cne.22184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Leiss F, Koper E, Hein I, Fouquet W, Lindner J, Sigrist S, Tavosanis G. 2009b. Characterization of dendritic spines in the Drosophila central nervous system. Developmental Neurobiology 69:221–234. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Levy-Strumpf N, Deiss LP, Berissi H, Kimchi A. 1997. DAP-5, a novel homolog of eukaryotic translation initiation factor 4G isolated as a putative modulator of gamma interferon-induced programmed cell death. Molecular and Cellular Biology 17:1615–1625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Li YH, Lei T, Chen XD, Xia T, Peng Y, Long QQ, Zhang J, Feng SQ, Zhou L, Yang ZQ. 2009. Molecular cloning, chromosomal location and expression pattern of porcine CIDEa and CIDEc. Molecular Biology Reports 36:575–582. doi: 10.1007/s11033-008-9216-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lin DM, Goodman CS. 1994. Ectopic and increased expression of Fasciclin II alters motoneuron growth cone guidance. Neuron 13:507–523. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90022-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Luo L, Liao YJ, Jan LY, Jan YN. 1994. Distinct morphogenetic functions of similar small GTPases: Drosophila Drac1 is involved in axonal outgrowth and myoblast fusion. Genes & Development 8:1787–1802. doi: 10.1101/gad.8.15.1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mao Z, Davis RL. 2009. Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneity. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 3:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.005.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. McBride SM, Choi CH, Schoenfeld BP, Bell AJ, Liebelt DA, Ferreiro D, Choi RJ, Hinchey P, Kollaros M, Terlizzi AM, Ferrick NJ, Koenigsberg E, Rudominer RL, Sumida A, Chiorean S, Siwicki KK, Nguyen HT, Fortini ME, McDonald TV, Jongens TA. 2010. Pharmacological and genetic reversal of age-dependent cognitive deficits attributable to decreased presenilin function. The Journal of Neuroscience 30:9510–9522. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1017-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McBride SM, Choi CH, Wang Y, Liebelt D, Braunstein E, Ferreiro D, Sehgal A, Siwicki KK, Dockendorff TC, Nguyen HT, McDonald TV, Jongens TA. 2005. Pharmacological Rescue of synaptic plasticity, Courtship behavior, and mushroom body defects in a Drosophila model of fragile x syndrome. Neuron 45:753–764. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McQuilton P St Pierre SE Thurmond J, FlyBase Consortium , 2012. FlyBase 101–the basics of navigating FlyBase. Nucleic Acids Research 40:D706–D714. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkr1030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mukae N, Yokoyama H, Yokokura T, Sakoyama Y, Sakahira H, Nagata S. 2000. Identification and developmental expression of inhibitor of caspase-activated DNase (ICAD) in Drosophila melanogaster. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 275:21402–21408. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M909611199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Murali T, Pacifico S, Yu J, Guest S, Roberts GG, III, Finley RL., Jr 2011. DroID 2011: a comprehensive, integrated resource for protein, transcription factor, RNA and gene interactions for Drosophila. Nucleic Acids Research 39:D736–D743. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq1092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Okamura K, Ishizuka A, Siomi H, Siomi MC. 2004. Distinct roles for Argonaute proteins in small RNA-directed RNA cleavage pathways. Genes & Development 18:1655–1666. doi: 10.1101/gad.1210204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Owald D, Fouquet W, Schmidt M, Wichmann C, Mertel S, Depner H, Christiansen F, Zube C, Quentin C, Körner J, Urlaub H, Mechtler K, Sigrist SJ. 2010. A Syd-1 homologue regulates pre- and postsynaptic maturation in Drosophila. The Journal of Cell Biology 188:565–579. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200908055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Owald D, Khorramshahi O, Gupta VK, Banovic D, Depner H, Fouquet W, Wichmann C, Mertel S, Eimer S, Reynolds E, Holt M, Aberle H, Sigrist SJ. 2012. Cooperation of Syd-1 with Neurexin synchronizes pre- with postsynaptic assembly. Nature Neuroscience 15:1219–1226. doi: 10.1038/nn.3183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Panneels V, Eroglu C, Cronet P, Sinning I. 2003. Pharmacological characterization and immunoaffinity purification of metabotropic glutamate receptor from Drosophila overexpressed in Sf9 cells. Protein Expression and Purification 30:275–282. doi: 10.1016/S1046-5928(03)00100-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Papoulas O, Monzo KF, Cantin GT, Ruse C, Yates JR, Ryu YH, Sisson JC. 2010. dFMRP and Caprin, translational regulators of synaptic plasticity, control the cell cycle at the Drosophila mid-blastula transition. Development 137:4201–4209. doi: 10.1242/dev.055046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Park OK, Park HH. 2012. Dual apoptotic DNA fragmentation system in the fly: Drep2 is a novel nuclease of which activity is inhibited by Drep3. FEBS Letters 586:3085–3089. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2012.07.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Park OK, Park HH. 2013. A putative role of Drep1 in apoptotic DNA fragmentation system in fly is mediated by direct interaction with Drep2 and Drep4. Apoptosis 18:385–392. doi: 10.1007/s10495-013-0815-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Parks AL, Cook KR, Belvin M, Dompe NA, Fawcett R, Huppert K, Tan LR, Winter CG, Bogart KP, Deal JE, Deal-Herr ME, Grant D, Marcinko M, Miyazaki WY, Robertson S, Shaw KJ, Tabios M, Vysotskaia V, Zhao L, Andrade RS, Edgar KA, Howie E, Killpack K, Milash B, Norton A, Thao D, Whittaker K, Winner MA, Friedman L, Margolis J, Singer MA, Kopczynski C, Curtis D, Kaufman TC, Plowman GD, Duyk G, Francis-Lang HL. 2004. Systematic generation of high-resolution deletion coverage of the Drosophila melanogaster genome. Nature Genetics 36:288–292. doi: 10.1038/ng1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Parmentier ML, Pin JP, Bockaert J, Grau Y. 1996. Cloning and functional expression of a Drosophila metabotropic glutamate receptor expressed in the embryonic CNS. The Journal of Neuroscience 16:6687–6694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Parnas D, Haghighi AP, Fetter RD, Kim SW, Goodman CS. 2001. Regulation of postsynaptic structure and protein localization by the Rho-type guanine nucleotide exchange factor dPix. Neuron 32:415–424. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00485-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Paul FE, Hosp F, Selbach M. 2011. Analyzing protein-protein interactions by quantitative mass spectrometry. Methods 54:387–395. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2011.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Quinn WG, Dudai Y. 1976. Memory phases in Drosophila. Nature 262:576–577. doi: 10.1038/262576a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Ramaekers A, Parmentier ML, Lasnier C, Bockaert J, Grau Y. 2001. Distribution of metabotropic glutamate receptor DmGlu-A in Drosophila melanogaster central nervous system. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 438:213–225. doi: 10.1002/cne.1310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Rappsilber J, Ishihama Y, Mann M. 2003. Stop and go extraction tips for matrix-assisted laser desorption/ionization, nanoelectrospray, and LC/MS sample pretreatment in proteomics. Analytical Chemistry 75:663–670. doi: 10.1021/ac026117i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Schägger H. 2006. Tricine-SDS-PAGE. Nature Protocols 1:16–22. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Scheunemann L, Jost E, Richlitzki A, Day JP, Sebastian S, Thum AS, Efetova M, Davies S-A, Schwärzel M. 2012. Consolidated and labile odor memory are separately Encoded within the Drosophila brain. The Journal of Neuroscience 32:17163–17171. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3286-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Schwaerzel M, Heisenberg M, Zars T. 2002. Extinction antagonizes olfactory memory at the subcellular level. Neuron 35:951–960. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00832-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Shiina N, Shinkura K, Tokunaga M. 2005. A novel RNA-binding protein in neuronal RNA granules: regulatory machinery for local translation. The Journal of Neuroscience 25:4420–4434. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0382-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sigrist SJ, Reiff DF, Thiel PR, Steinert JR, Schuster CM. 2003. Experience-dependent strengthening of Drosophila neuromuscular junctions. The Journal of Neuroscience 23:6546–6556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Song Z, Guan B, Bergman A, Nicholson DW, Thornberry NA, Peterson EP, Steller H. 2000. Biochemical and genetic interactions between Drosophila caspases and the proapoptotic genes rpr, hid, and grim. Molecular and Cellular Biology 20:2907–2914. doi: 10.1128/MCB.20.8.2907-2914.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stocker RF, Heimbeck G, Gendre N, de Belle JS. 1997. Neuroblast ablation in Drosophila P[GAL4] lines reveals origins of olfactory interneurons. Journal of Neurobiology 32:443–456. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199705)32:53.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tan H, Poidevin M, Li H, Chen D, Jin P. 2012. MicroRNA-277 modulates the neurodegeneration caused by Fragile X premutation rCGG repeats. PLOS Genetics 8:e1002681. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tauber JM, Vanlandingham PA, Zhang B. 2011. Elevated levels of the vesicular monoamine transporter and a novel repetitive behavior in the Drosophila model of fragile X syndrome. PLOS ONE 6:e27100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tully T, Quinn WG. 1985. Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 157:263–277. doi: 10.1007/BF01350033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tully T, Preat T, Boynton S, Del Vecchio M. 1994. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell 79:35–47. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tusher VG, Tibshirani R, Chu G. 2001. Significance analysis of microarrays applied to the ionizing radiation response. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA 98:5116–5121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.091062498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Urizar NL, Yang Z, Edenberg HJ, Davis RL. 2007. Drosophila homer is required in a small set of neurons including the ellipsoid body for normal ethanol sensitivity and tolerance. The Journal of Neuroscience 27:4541–4551. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0305-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vermeulen M, Hubner NC, Mann M. 2008. High confidence determination of specific protein-protein interactions using quantitative mass spectrometry. Current Opinion in Biotechnology 19:331–337. doi: 10.1016/j.copbio.2008.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wagh DA, Rasse TM, Asan E, Hofbauer A, Schwenkert I, Dürrbeck H, Buchner S, Dabauvalle MC, Schmidt M, Qin G, Wichmann C, Kittel R, Sigrist SJ, Buchner E. 2006. Bruchpilot, a protein with homology to ELKS/CAST, is required for structural integrity and function of synaptic active zones in Drosophila. Neuron 49:833–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Waites CL, Leal-Ortiz SA, Andlauer TF, Sigrist SJ, Garner CC. 2011. Piccolo regulates the dynamic assembly of presynaptic f-actin. The Journal of Neuroscience 31:14250–14263. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wolff T, Ready DF. 1991. Cell death in normal and rough eye mutants of Drosophila. Development 113:825–839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wu C, Zhang Y, Sun Z, Li P. 2008. Molecular evolution of Cide family proteins: novel domain formation in early vertebrates and the subsequent divergence. BMC Evolutionary Biology 8:159. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-8-159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Yang M, Armstrong JD, Vilinsky I, Strausfeld N, Kaiser K. 1995. Subdivision of the Drosophila mushroom bodies by enhancer-trap expression patterns. Neuron 15:45–54. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90063-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Yasuyama K, Salvaterra PM. 1999. Localization of choline acetyltransferase-expressing neurons in Drosophila nervous system. Microscopy Research and Technique 45:65–79. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0029(19990415)45:23.0.CO;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yasuyama K, Meinertzhagen IA, Schürmann F-W. 2002. Synaptic organization of the mushroom body calyx in Drosophila melanogaster. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 445:211–226. doi: 10.1002/cne.10155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yokoyama H, Mukae N, Sakahira H, Okawa K, Iwamatsu A, Nagata S. 2000. A novel activation mechanism of caspase-activated DNase from Drosophila melanogaster. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 275:12978–12986. doi: 10.1074/jbc.275.17.12978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Yonezawa T, Kurata R, Kimura M, Inoko H. 2011. Which CIDE are you on? Apoptosis and energy metabolism. Molecular Biosystems 7:91–100. doi: 10.1039/c0mb00099j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zars TD, Fischer M, Schulz R, Heisenberg M. 2000. Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science 288:672–675. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5466.672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2014 Nov 13;3:e03895. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03895.017

Decision letter

Editor: Leslie C Griffith1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for sending your work entitled “Drep-2 is a novel synaptic protein important for learning and memory” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by K VijayRaghavan (Senior editor) and 4 reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors.

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewers discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission:

This paper presents a beautiful characterization of Drep2, a surprising new learning and memory mutant. The STED and co-localization experiments played well with the genetic interactions with dFMR1 and mGluRA in olfactory learning and take this out of the realm of simply being a “new gene” identification paper. However, after a long discussion amongst the reviewing editor and the three reviewers there was agreement that this version of the manuscript is not suitable for publication. The overall consensus was that the manuscript needs to be streamlined both in terms of the data that are presented and in terms of the interpretation of the experiments, especially the relationship of Drep2 to dFMR1 and mGluRA. While the central findings in the paper (localization, role in olfactory learning) are solid, the way the paper is structured, the numerous ancillary data sets and the unfocused discussion tend to distract from what could be a strong message. To some degree these might be considered cosmetic problems, but they make the study seem much weaker than it probably is and more importantly, the lack of clarity in the Discussion results in a somewhat misleading bottom line about Drep2 function.

I am appending the complete reviews for your consideration, but the main points that need to be addressed if you choose to submit a revision are enumerated below. The reviewers all feel that addressing these issues in the manuscript would make this a much stronger vehicle for highlighting the interesting central findings on Drep2 localization and its role in olfactory associative learning.

1) Ethanol data. This data set did not seem particularly “on point” for the main conclusions of the paper and only served to distract the readers from the take home message. More concerning was that there were a number of controls that were either not done or not reported, which makes the conclusions of this part of the study tenuous. Given that these data are not central to the manuscript's main point, recommend they be removed.

2) Courtship data. In the view of several of the reviewers, this data set is not interpretable as demonstrating a learning/memory defect. The fact that the mutants do not show any appreciable level of initial courtship means that there was no opportunity for these males to actually learn since performance of courtship is required for training (the behavior has operant features). While the reintroduction of Drep2 rescues learning, it also rescues initial courtship meaning this experiment does not necessarily imply that Drep2 has a role in courtship learning, it could have simply allowed the animals to be trained. We recommend these data are removed since they are not central to the paper.

3) Imaging data. The alpha lobe imaging data were felt to be peripheral to the point of the paper, not because this would not be a valid way of looking at the mutant, but rather that the particular experiment the authors chose to do was not particularly relevant. Why look at the alpha lobes? Changes in Ca+2 and its function in learning is better understood elsewhere like the gamma lobes, and given the clear role of Drep2 in calyx why not look there? The alpha lobes are far downstream from the calyx and the presumptive site of Drep2 action, and probably not important for STM. It is not at all clear what the difference shown represents. The data as they stand could be removed, though the paper could conceivably be strengthened by addition of imaging from either gamma lobe or calyx.

4) Statistics. The authors should explain and justify the particular non-parametric statistical tests they used- are the data not normally distributed?

5) Writing. Language was in many places rather stilted and could used editing to make sure that it is clear. More problematic was the repetitious nature of much of the Introduction and the over-written Discussion that never really gave the reader a clear take-home message (see point 6 below).

6) Mechanism/model for Drep2 function. While this paper suggests that Drep2 is acting in some way with mGluRA and dFMR1, the exact mechanism is not clear. The data strongly suggest that this interaction may happen early in development (or at least have an important developmental component) since rescue only works if the protein is present developmentally. The authors are encouraged to show the negative result with the adult-only rescue since this constrains models. While the current data make it impossible to posit a very specific model, the authors should at least give a cogent set of possibilities. As is, there is an implication in the paper that Drep2 may function like mGluR acutely and this is misleading.

Reviewer #1:

In this manuscript the authors present a very extensive characterization of mutants in Drep-2, a protein initially thought to be involved in apoptosis, which now they convincingly show to be associated with synapses and at least in the adult mushroom bodies resides on the postsynaptic side. The authors convincingly show that the protein has a role in glutamatergic signaling-dependent olfactory learning and memory.

Nevertheless there are multiple issues to be resolved to improve presentation and above all readability of the manuscript!

1) The manuscript is way too long, unnecessarily in my opinion.

2) The writing style and language need to be improved significantly.

3) There is a plethora of data in the supplement that do not add much to the conclusions of the already very large data set.

4) The Discussion is unnecessarily too long in my opinion.

More specifically:

1) The language and style must be improved because as it is the manuscript does not read well, is full of errors in syntax, colloquialisms and sections appear disconnected as if written in different times by different people. There appears to be no rhyme or reason in the way the Materials and Methods section was assembled. Genetics and husbandry, transgenics, longevity and behavioral methods customarily are placed in proximity, whereas the molecular techniques are grouped together.

2) Figure 1 and 2 do not follow the text presentation and in my opinion would benefit the manuscript and help the reader if Figure 1 was made in sequence of panels: Figure1D, 1A, 1B, Figure 2, 1E Figure 2 should then be the fractionation (current Figure 1 C) and Figure 1–figure supplement 2. The text in the results sections should be changed to reflect this.

3) What are the bands around 40kD in the fractionation blot? What are the different fractions? What do the names P1 slof and solid mean? Please describe.

4) Why is counting the number of Kenyon cells in control and mutant animals a measure of apoptosis?

5) Why use mushroom body driver c305a with its limited coverage of MB neurons rather than any other one including mb247 which the authors use for many other applications?

6) Although it is very nice data, how does inclusion of all the ethanol sensitivity data add to a paper dealing with learning and memory, at least according to its present title?

7) Shouldn't the calcium imaging data be part of the main figures of the manuscript. They present a significant point, but if part of the main manuscript the effect of the mutation must be rescued.

Reviewer #2:

This new manuscript from Andlauer et al. provides a functional description of a new postsynaptic protein, Drep-2, which genetically interacts with FMRP and mGluRA in associative learning. The findings are significant and interesting. The work is technically impressive. The authors move from a protein with a completely unknown function, through a series of impressive localization studies that demonstrate enrichment around post-synaptic densities. They generate null alleles, and then use these alleles demonstrate a role for Drep-2 in alleles in associative short and intermediate term memory. Expression of Drep-2 in the mushroom body neurons is sufficient to rescue the memory phenotypes of the Drep-2 null alleles. Importantly, the authors show that the Drep-2 short term memory phenotype is also rescued by developmental activation of mGluRA receptors and by mutation of the dFMR1, demonstrating a genetic interaction with the proteins. The authors also perform proteomics to isolate the Drep-2 core network of interacting proteins, which include FRMP and several proteins involved in translational control. This work takes Drep-2 from an unknown function to a new component in the FMRP pathway regulating postsynaptic plasticity.

Interestingly, the authors also present some important negative results: that contrary to naïve expectations, Drep-2 does not appear to affect apoptosis and that the loss of this protein does not impact synapse structure. Overall, I believe these experiments were carefully and thoughtfully performed.

The author's movement through the characterization of Drep-2 is impressive, but also somewhat uneven. The authors further examined the Drep-2 null alleles for phenotypes and genetics interactions in courtship conditioning, locomotor behavior and ethanol sedation behavioral paradigms to show demonstrate Drep-2 antagonizes dFRM1. The null allele has a phenotype in each of these behaviors. In courtship conditioning and locomotor behavior, the authors do not show that the phenotype is due to the loss of Drep-2 through genetic rescue. Also in courtship conditioning, an unpublished automated system is used to score the many assays they have run. The median learning scores using this system are quite low and may miss many courtship behaviors. Alternatively, the assay set up may be suppressing courtship. The authors also do not provide any indications if hyperactivity can provide false positives for courtship with this system. Is it possible that hyperactive drep-2EX13 flies are not courting, and therefore not learning, but their high level of activity is somehow scored as courting behavior in this system?

The ethanol sedation phenotype was rescued, but I had some concerns. In the description of this experiment, the authors focused on Drep-2 expression in the ring neurons of the ellipsoid body—the site of the homer requirement in ethanol sedation and tolerance. However, they never show that the Drep-2 is required in these neurons. When discussing the absence of an interaction with mGluRA, the authors should also cite the possibility that Drep-2 may be required outside of the ring neurons for normal levels of sedation. It is common to perform ethanol sedation experiments separately in males and female as the differences in body size lead to large differences in the rate of sedation. As far as I can tell, this was not done and there is no indication of whether the number of females/males was controlled between genotypes. The rate of sedation was so quick, that there may be a floor effect for the drep-2 mutants, which could confound the ability of ACPD to rescue this phenotype. Also, I am unclear of the status of the white gene in the mutant genotypes. The level of white activity can dramatically impact the rate of sedation (Chan, et al. 2014. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 38:1582-1593). It is highly unlikely that the rescue is due to increasing the copies of mini-white, but the authors need to indicate whether the mutants are in a w1118 or w+ background. Also the authors need to justify using frozen fly weight and not protein content as was done in Moore, et al., to normalize ethanol content in the absorption studies. Although, these experiments on ethanol sedation are potentially interesting and could add value to this manuscript, they are not necessary.

Reviewer #3:

This manuscript describes a thorough set of experiments to characterize the role of the CIDE-N domain protein drep-2 in the Drosophila CNS. The role of this protein in regulating learning and memory, presumably via modulation of the postsynaptic terminal in the MB calyx, is novel, and the functional antagonism with dfmr intriguing. The experiments are a tour de force, and the major conclusions could open some promising avenues of future investigation (e.g., of fragile X phenotypes). I have no major concerns.

Minor comments:

This is a dense manuscript, and a schematic outlining the expression patterns of the various proteins (drep-2, mGluR, dfmr), as well as showing the hypothesized interactions between them would significantly help the reader to digest the conclusions. While the nature interaction between drep-2 and mGluR signaling is not completely worked out, showing the directionality of the effects of each manipulation on learning would help.

Figure S1 shows some nice imaging data suggesting that the MB responses to odorants are attenuated. In panel C, error bars are missing for the drep-2 trace during the odor presentation window (in my copy, at least). Also, the caption should note the location of the recordings (tip of the alpha lobe).

In the last paragraph of the Introduction, the authors abruptly break from discussing previous findings into a discussion of the conclusions in the current manuscript. The last 3-4 sentences are better suited to the Results and Discussion. The authors could replace them with something to the effect of “Here we have examined a potential synaptic role for Drep-2, a CIDE-N domain protein, and its role in regulating learning and memory through interaction with mGluR signaling”.

The authors state: “Thus, we obviously produced two null situations for drep-2 and proved the specificity...”. This sounds odd (the “proved” part); I suggest changing to something along the lines of “Thus, the drep-2C-Term antibody demonstrated specificity in both western blots and immune staining. Based on the lack of detectable expression, we conclude that both mutants are null alleles.”

Also: “The adenylyl cyclase Rutabaga mediates coincidence detection between the conditioned and the unconditioned stimulus in KC γ-lobes (Qin et al., 2012)...” Rut-dependent coincidence detection has been directly observed in the alpha lobe by two groups (Tomchik and Davis, 2009; Neuron 64: 510-521; Gervasi et al., 2010; 65: 516-529); these papers should be cited here, particularly since the alpha lobe was imaged.

Reviewer #4:

This paper provides a very thorough characterization of Drep2, a novel synaptic protein with a function in learning and other behaviors. The authors show convincing mutant phenotypes and provide a truly detailed and beautiful accounting of where Drep2 is expressed and its subsynaptic localization in Kenyon cells. This careful work and the novel nature of the Drep2 protein make the work interesting.

The main problem I have with this manuscript is the lack of actual molecular insight into Drep2 function. The data on dfmr and mGluRA are suggestive, but the story is clearly not straightforward and trying to make it fit the dfmr/mGluR template just does not work; it only serves to highlight the fact that something else is going on and the lack of concrete mechanism. There are several pieces of data that lead to significant questions that are not answered in this paper.

A) mGluRA is not present in the mass spec data set. This does not support a direct adult interaction.

B) The data showing normal mGluR levels in Drep2 mutants is also not consistent with direct interaction.

C) The fact that mGluR agonists can only influence the Drep2 phenotype if present during development is a huge red flag. If there was a direct interaction, one should be able to get acute interaction. This smacks of some either circuit-level or pathway-level compensation that could be very indirect.

D) There is no evidence that the interaction between mGluR and Drep2 occurs in KCs. The authors use drugs and this makes things very hard to interpret. They cite the Kanellopoulos paper which uses ACUTE mGluR RNAi to demonstrate interaction in KCs with dfmr. This cannot be extrapolated to Drep2 because of the developmental nature of that interaction. Again, the issue of circuit level compensation becomes important.

E) The courtship data do not necessarily suggest a “learning” interaction of dfmr and Drep2. Dfmr males do not court and if males do not court in this paradigm they cannot learn since there is an operant component to this behavior. Drep2 mutants do court and loss of Drep2 in the double rescues courtship. The fact that the dfmr;Drep2 males now court could be the basis of the rescue, not an enhancement of learning.

In the final analysis, while the involvement of a protein in the Diff family in synapses is interesting, without a novel and clear mechanism for its function this paper is not going to be of general interest.

eLife. 2014 Nov 13;3:e03895. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03895.018

Author response


1) Ethanol data. This data set did not seem particularly “on point” for the main conclusions of the paper and only served to distract the readers from the take home message. More concerning was that there were a number of controls that were either not done or not reported, which makes the conclusions of this part of the study tenuous. Given that these data are not central to the manuscript's main point, recommend they be removed.

We agree that the ethanol data did not really fit to the story of the manuscript. Therefore, we had originally placed it in supplemental figures only addressed in the Discussion. The data were included because we originally wanted to show all mutant phenotypes we had observed. However, we fully agree that there were too many ancillary datasets included in this manuscript. Thus, to improve the flow of the manuscript, we have now concentrated on olfactory conditioning and removed ethanol sensitivity, locomotor hyperactivity, courtship conditioning, and functional imaging experiments.

2) Courtship data. In the view of several of the reviewers, this data set is not interpretable as demonstrating a learning/memory defect. The fact that the mutants do not show any appreciable level of initial courtship means that there was no opportunity for these males to actually learn since performance of courtship is required for training (the behavior has operant features). While the reintroduction of Drep2 rescues learning, it also rescues initial courtship meaning this experiment does not necessarily imply that Drep2 has a role in courtship learning, it could have simply allowed the animals to be trained. We recommend these data are removed since they are not central to the paper.

We agree and the data have been removed.

3) Imaging data. The alpha lobe imaging data were felt to be peripheral to the point of the paper, not because this would not be a valid way of looking at the mutant, but rather that the particular experiment the authors chose to do was not particularly relevant. Why look at the alpha lobes? Changes in Ca+2 and its function in learning is better understood elsewhere like the gamma lobes, and given the clear role of Drep2 in calyx why not look there? The alpha lobes are far downstream from the calyx and the presumptive site of Drep2 action, and probably not important for STM. It is not at all clear what the difference shown represents. The data as they stand could be removed, though the paper could conceivably be strengthened by addition of imaging from either gamma lobe or calyx.

We have imaged the alpha lobes because of methodological reasons. However, we agree that these data are not well suited to draw conclusions regarding the function of Drep-2 in the calyx. We have therefore removed the data and now only refer to them briefly in the Discussion as preliminary data. We added the sentence: “Interestingly, preliminary experiments indicate that the learning deficits of drep-2 mutants might involve decreased Ca2+ responses in KCs (not shown).”

4) Statistics. The authors should explain and justify the particular non-parametric statistical tests they used- are the data not normally distributed?

In the current Figures 3C, 5, Figure 5–figure supplements 1, and 6, several samples had sizes below 20, making it difficult to reliably assess whether the data are normally distributed or not. We thus preferred to use non-parametric tests (Kruskal-Wallis and Mann-Whitney U). Although some of our experiments had higher sample sizes, we did not want to mix different tests for statistical significance and decided to use non-parametric tests for all experiments.

We used additional median-based methods for the courtship conditioning and ethanol sensitivity data, both of which have now been removed. This was due to the incidence of a few extreme outliers that skewed the results when parametric/mean-based methods were used. Median-based assessment of the data had thus delivered results that better corresponded to the true distribution of data points.

5) Writing. Language was in many places rather stilted and could used editing to make sure that it is clear. More problematic was the repetitious nature of much of the Introduction and the over-written Discussion that never really gave the reader a clear take-home message (see point 6 below).

We apologize for the poor language of our manuscript. We worked on the manuscript for a long time and the repeated addition and removal of sentences and sections obviously led to poor readability. We have now worked on both language and readability and have also involved native English speakers for language corrections. We really hope that the flow and language level have been substantially improved.

6) Mechanism/model for Drep2 function. While this paper suggests that Drep2 is acting in some way with mGluRA and dFMR1, the exact mechanism is not clear. The data strongly suggest that this interaction may happen early in development (or at least have an important developmental component) since rescue only works if the protein is present developmentally. The authors are encouraged to show the negative result with the adult-only rescue since this constrains models. While the current data make it impossible to posit a very specific model, the authors should at least give a cogent set of possibilities. As is, there is an implication in the paper that Drep2 may function like mGluR acutely and this is misleading.

We have worked heavily on the discussion and hope that our hypothesis for the function of Drep-2 has now become clearer. We did not intend to suggest that Drep-2 interacts physically with mGluR (but, apparently, did unwillingly so) and did not stress enough that we could not rescue mutant deficits acutely in adults. We are confident that we have now made these points clear in the revised version of the manuscript.

Reviewer #1:

In this manuscript the authors present a very extensive characterization of mutants in Drep-2, a protein initially thought to be involved in apoptosis, which now they convincingly show to be associated with synapses and at least in the adult mushroom bodies resides on the postsynaptic side. The authors convincingly show that the protein has a role in glutamatergic signaling-dependent olfactory learning and memory.

Nevertheless there are multiple issues to be resolved to improve presentation and above all readability of the manuscript!

We thank the reviewer for their positive opinion and fully agree that we had to work on the readability of the manuscript.

1) The manuscript is way too long, unnecessarily in my opinion.

We have now shortened all parts of the manuscript significantly, particularly the Introduction and Discussion.

2) The writing style and language need to be improved significantly.

As outlined above, we went through a sincere effort of improving the style, flow and, importantly, the language of the manuscript.

3) There is a plethora of data in the supplement that do not add much to the conclusions of the already very large data set.

We have now condensed the supplemental data substantially. The previous supplemental Figure 1–figure supplement 2 has been included as a main figure (Figure 3); Figure 3–figure supplement 1-3 have been removed.

4) The Discussion is unnecessarily too long in my opinion.

We fully agree. We have shortened the Discussion to approximately half of its previous length.

More specifically:

1) The language and style must be improved because as it is the manuscript does not read well, is full of errors in syntax, colloquialisms and sections appear disconnected as if written in different times by different people. There appears to be no rhyme or reason in the way the Materials and Methods section was assembled. Genetics and husbandry, transgenics, longevity and behavioral methods customarily are placed in proximity, whereas the molecular techniques are grouped together.

We do apologize for the poor language and writing style. During the work on the manuscript, we changed parts of it so many times that it eventually lost the character of a coherent text. We hope that this issue has been resolved in the revised version. We have also rearranged the Methods, as suggested by the reviewer.

2) Figure 1 and 2 do not follow the text presentation and in my opinion would benefit the manuscript and help the reader if Figure 1 was made in sequence of panels: Figure1D, 1A, 1B, Figure 2, 1E Figure 2 should then be the fractionation (current Figure 1 C) and Figure 1–figure supplement 2. The text in the results sections should be changed to reflect this.

We are grateful for this suggestion by the reviewer and have changed figures and text accordingly.

3) What are the bands around 40kD in the fractionation blot? What are the different fractions? What do the names P1 slof and solid mean? Please describe.

The bands constitute an unspecific signal (they are still present in the null mutant, see the western blot in Figure 1C) that is visible especially clearly under the conditions of this blot. We decided not to cut off the blot above these bands but instead included them as a control, independently validating that we had loaded comparable amounts of protein in each lane. A detailed protocol outlining the procedure of fractionation has now been accepted for publication and is currently in print in Nature Protocols: Depner, H., Lützkendorf, J., Babikir, H.A., Sigrist, S.J., and Holt, M.G. (2014): Differential centrifugation-based biochemical fractionation of the Drosophila adult CNS. Nature Protocols, 10.1038/nprot.2014.192.

We now reference this protocol in the main text, the figure legends and the Methods section. We feel that explaining the method in more detail than outlined in the Methods section is beyond the scope of this manuscript. We thus refer now to the aforementioned publication.

4) Why is counting the number of Kenyon cells in control and mutant animals a measure of apoptosis???

Given the strong expression of Drep-2 in mushroom body Kenyon cells (KCs) already during larval stages, a potential role of Drep-2 in the regulation of apoptosis might have resulted in altered numbers of KC cell bodies. We agree with the reviewer that this is not a direct measure of apoptosis. However, we do think that an altered count of KCs could be expected if Drep-2 was mainly a regulator of apoptosis. Therefore this measurement is, in combination with the additional data provided in Figure 3, an indication that it is unlikely that apoptosis is misregulated in the CNS of drep-2 mutants. We have now used more caution in our interpretation of this experiment): “If apoptosis was, nevertheless, misregulated in the CNS of drep-2 mutants in vivo, an altered count of cell bodies should be expected in adult flies. Drep-2 staining was especially prominent at KC synapses in the mushroom body (MB) calyx of wildtypes (Figure 2C). We, therefore, quantified the numbers of cell bodies of a subset of MB-intrinsic neurons (KCs). No differences between drep-2 mutants and controls could be observed (Figure 3C).”

5) Why use mushroom body driver c305a with its limited coverage of MB neurons rather than any other one including mb247 which the authors use for many other applications?

We did the experiment to demonstrate that KC-expressed Drep-2 accumulates at PSDs of KCs within the MB calyx, in the same manner as the endogenous protein does. C305a-Gal4 drives expression in the subset of α’β’-KCs. We have demonstrated in a previous analysis, that these KCs are exclusively postsynaptic in the MB calyx and do not form presynaptic specializations here (Christiansen, F., Zube, C., Andlauer, T.F.M., Wichmann, C., Fouquet, W., Owald, D., Mertel, S., Leiss, F., Tavosanis, G., Farca Luna, A.J., Fiala, A., and Sigrist, S.J. (2011): Presynapses in Kenyon cell dendrites in the mushroom body calyx of Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 9696–9707.). We have also shown that mb247-Gal4, by contrast, drives expression within different subtypes of KCs, which, in addition to postsynapses, also form presynaptic specializations (active zones) in the calyx. We, therefore, preferred using c305a-Gal4 here to simply avoid any possible confusion between pre- and postsynaptic specializations.

6) Although it is very nice data, how does inclusion of all the ethanol sensitivity data add to a paper dealing with learning and memory, at least according to its present title?

We agree with the reviewer and have removed these data, as they are not needed for the main conclusions and the flow of the manuscript.

7) Shouldn't the calcium imaging data be part of the main figures of the manuscript. They present a significant point, but if part of the main manuscript the effect of the mutation must be rescued.

We also agree with the reviewer here. Unfortunately, generation of rescue constructs for this experiment (in an isogenized background) would require a significant amount of time. In order not to delay resubmission of the manuscript by a long time, we have decided to remove this dataset from the manuscript. We will consider conducting optimally suited functional imaging experiments on drep-2 mutants in the future. We hope that the reviewer will understand this decision.

Reviewer #2:

The ethanol sedation phenotype was rescued, but I had some concerns. In the description of this experiment, the authors focused on Drep-2 expression in the ring neurons of the ellipsoid body—the site of the homer requirement in ethanol sedation and tolerance. However, they never show that the Drep-2 is required in these neurons. When discussing the absence of an interaction with mGluRA, the authors should also cite the possibility that Drep-2 may be required outside of the ring neurons for normal levels of sedation. It is common to perform ethanol sedation experiments separately in males and female as the differences in body size lead to large differences in the rate of sedation. As far as I can tell, this was not done and there is no indication of whether the number of females/males was controlled between genotypes. The rate of sedation was so quick, that there may be a floor effect for the drep-2 mutants, which could confound the ability of ACPD to rescue this phenotype.

We have also removed ethanol sedation measurements from the manuscript, as suggested by several reviewers and the editor. Of note, only female flies were analyzed in the ethanol sensitivity experiments.

Also, I am unclear of the status of the white gene in the mutant genotypes. The level of white activity can dramatically impact the rate of sedation (Chan, et al. 2014. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 38:1582-1593). It is highly unlikely that the rescue is due to increasing the copies of mini-white, but the authors need to indicate whether the mutants are in a w1118 or w+ background. Also the authors need to justify using frozen fly weight and not protein content as was done in Moore, et al., to normalize ethanol content in the absorption studies. Although, these experiments on ethanol sedation are potentially interesting and could add value to this manuscript, they are not necessary.

Although these data have now been removed, we still want to point out that we did account for the genetic background of our mutants. We agree that the genetic situation is somewhat confusing. Flies mutant for drep-2 are, in general, in a w1118 background. However, they carry a mini-white insertion on the second chromosome as a remnant of the p-elements used for generating the mutant. Therefore, drep-2 mutants have red eyes. To account for this situation, we have generated a wildtype control strain that carries a white+ gene but is otherwise in a w1118 background. We included two sets of controls for ethanol sedation experiments: one with white eyes and one with red eyes. Red-eyed controls showed lower ethanol sensitivity than white-eyed controls, but red-eyed mutants showed a higher sensitivity than both controls. Therefore, we were confident that the increased sensitivity of the mutants was not due to the white gene.

Reviewer #3:

This is a dense manuscript, and a schematic outlining the expression patterns of the various proteins (drep-2, mGluR, dfmr), as well as showing the hypothesized interactions between them would significantly help the reader to digest the conclusions. While the nature interaction between drep-2 and mGluR signaling is not completely worked out, showing the directionality of the effects of each manipulation on learning would help.

We have seriously considered including a figure outlining our model. However, while we believe that our hypothesis of a functional interaction between Drep-2, DmGluRA and FMRP is sound, the details of this interaction are still rather vague. Therefore, as much as we would like to, we do not think that we should provide a model right now, which would, to the casual reader, suggest a level of certainty on the molecular interactions that we do not have yet. However, if the reviewers continue considering inclusion of a model or scheme as helpful to the reader, we would be happy to provide the following model (Author response image 1), which summarizes our current hypothesis:

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

Figure S1 shows some nice imaging data suggesting that the MB responses to odorants are attenuated. In panel C, error bars are missing for the drep-2 trace during the odor presentation window (in my copy, at least). Also, the caption should note the location of the recordings (tip of the alpha lobe).

We thank the reviewer for spotting this formatting mistake.

In the last paragraph of the Introduction, the authors abruptly break from discussing previous findings into a discussion of the conclusions in the current manuscript. The last 3-4 sentences are better suited to the Results and Discussion. The authors could replace them with something to the effect of “Here we have examined a potential synaptic role for Drep-2, a CIDE-N domain protein, and its role in regulating learning and memory through interaction with mGluR signaling”.

We agree with the reviewer and have changed the Introduction accordingly.

The authors state: “Thus, we obviously produced two null situations for drep-2 and proved the specificity...”. This sounds odd (the “proved” part); I suggest changing to something along the lines of “Thus, the drep-2C-Term antibody demonstrated specificity in both western blots and immune staining. Based on the lack of detectable expression, we conclude that both mutants are null alleles.”

We have adapted the text accordingly.

Also: “The adenylyl cyclase Rutabaga mediates coincidence detection between the conditioned and the unconditioned stimulus in KC γ-lobes (Qin et al., 2012)...” Rut-dependent coincidence detection has been directly observed in the alpha lobe by two groups (Tomchik and Davis, 2009; Neuron 64: 510-521; Gervasi et al., 2010; 65: 516-529); these papers should be cited here, particularly since the alpha lobe was imaged.

During the process of condensing our manuscript and the removal of imaging data, we have removed this sentence.

Reviewer #4:

The main problem I have with this manuscript is the lack of actual molecular insight into Drep2 function. The data on dfmr and mGluRA are suggestive, but the story is clearly not straightforward and trying to make it fit the dfmr/mGluR template just does not work; it only serves to highlight the fact that something else is going on and the lack of concrete mechanism. There are several pieces of data that lead to sinificant questions that are not answered in this paper.

A) mGluRA is not present in the mass spec data set. This does not support a direct adult interaction.

B) The data showing normal mGluR levels in Drep2 mutants is also not consistent with direct interaction.

C) The fact that mGluR agonists can only influence the Drep2 phenotype if present during development is a huge red flag. If there was a direct interaction, one should be able to get acute interaction. This smacks of some either circuit-level or pathway-level compensation that could be very indirect.

We agree with the reviewer that we did not find any evidence for a direct interaction of Drep-2 and DmGluRA. On the contrary, based on our data, such a physical interaction of both proteins is rather unlikely. We apologize that, apparently, the presentation of data and formulations within the text nevertheless gave the impression that we suggested such an interaction. We hope that we could clearly make the point in the revised version of the manuscript that we do not believe that Drep-2 interacts directly with DmGluRA. However, we think that our data suggests that Drep-2 stimulates a component of a pathway downstream of DmGluRA.

D) There is no evidence that the interaction between mGluR and Drep2 occurs in KCs. The authors use drugs and this makes things very hard to interpret. They cite the Kanellopoulos paper which uses ACUTE mGluR RNAi to demonstrate interaction in KCs with dfmr. This cannot be extrapolated to Drep2 because of the developmental nature of that interaction. Again, the issue of circuit level compensation becomes important.

We have now tried to make it clear throughout the manuscript now that we could only rescue the deficits of drep-2 mutants when we stimulated mGluRs starting already in development and that this is not the same situation as described by Kanellopoulos et al.

E) The courtship data do not necessarily suggest a “learning” interaction of dfmr and Drep2. Dfmr males do not court and if males do not court in this paradigm they cannot learn since there is an operant component to this behavior. Drep2 mutants do court and loss of Drep2 in the double rescues courtship. The fact that the dfmr;Drep2 males now court could be the basis of the rescue, not an enhancement of learning.

The reviewer is absolutely right. We nevertheless thought that reporting the functional antagonism between FMRP and Drep-2 in courtship conditioning is interesting. In response to the concerns of the reviewers we have decided to remove the courtship data entirely.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1.

    Mass spectrometry: core proteins enriched over both controls. The table shows proteins enriched at an FDR of 1%. Ranks are based on the ratios of Drep-2GFP IPs, GFP beads vs. plain beads (compare Figure 7A–B). Putative functions were derived from Flybase. GFP (#6) was removed from this list.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.015

    elife03895s001.docx (164.8KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03895.015
    Supplementary file 2.

    Mass spectrometry: all proteins enriched at an FDR of 10%. The table shows two ratios for all proteins: first, Drep-2GFP animals, GFP beads vs. plain beads and, second, GFP beads, Drep-2GFP vs. wild-type (wt) animals (compare Figure 7A). “+” symbols indicate whether a protein was significantly enriched at a given FDR. Proteins are sorted according to the first ratio. Proteins were labeled in cyan if significantly enriched for both ratios at an FDR of 1%; these proteins constitute the core interactors. Protein names were labeled in red if the protein was positively enriched for the first ratio but negatively enriched for the second one, and thus probably constitutes a false positive. Protein names were labeled in shades of green if the protein was included as an additional interactor in the network of interacting proteins (Figure 7D): dark green = FDR 1%, medium green = FDR 5%, light green = FDR 10%.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03895.016

    elife03895s002.docx (226.9KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03895.016

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES