Skip to main content
Molecular Biology and Evolution logoLink to Molecular Biology and Evolution
. 2014 Aug 28;31(12):3240–3249. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msu257

Genetic Differentiation of Hypothalamus Parentally Biased Transcripts in Populations of the House Mouse Implicate the Prader–Willi Syndrome Imprinted Region as a Possible Source of Behavioral Divergence

Anna Lorenc 1,, Miriam Linnenbrink 1, Inka Montero 1,, Markus B Schilhabel 2, Diethard Tautz 1,*
PMCID: PMC4245819  PMID: 25172960

Abstract

Parentally biased expression of transcripts (genomic imprinting) in adult tissues, including the brain, can influence and possibly drive the evolution of behavioral traits. We have previously found that paternally determined cues are involved in population-specific mate choice decisions between two populations of the Western house mouse (Mus musculus domesticus). Here, we ask whether this could be mediated by genomically imprinted transcripts that are subject to fast differentiation between these populations. We focus on three organs that are of special relevance for mate choice and behavior: The vomeronasal organ (VNO), the hypothalamus, and the liver. To first identify candidate transcripts at a genome-wide scale, we used reciprocal crosses between M. m. domesticus and M. m. musculus inbred strains and RNA sequencing of the respective tissues. Using a false discovery cutoff derived from mock reciprocal cross comparisons, we find a total of 66 imprinted transcripts, 13 of which have previously not been described as imprinted. The largest number of imprinted transcripts were found in the hypothalamus; fewer were found in the VNO, and the least were found in the liver. To assess molecular differentiation and imprinting in the wild-derived M. m. domesticus populations, we sequenced the RNA of the hypothalamus from individuals of these populations. This confirmed the presence of the above identified transcripts also in wild populations and allowed us to search for those that show a high genetic differentiation between these populations. Our results identify the Ube3a–Snrpn imprinted region on chromosome 7 as a region that encompasses the largest number of previously not described transcripts with paternal expression bias, several of which are at the same time highly differentiated. For four of these, we confirmed their imprinting status via single nucleotide polymorphism-specific pyrosequencing assays with RNA from reciprocal crosses. In addition, we find the paternally expressed Peg13 transcript within the Trappc9 gene region on chromosome 15 to be highly differentiated. Interestingly, both regions have been implicated in Prader–Willi nervous system disorder phenotypes in humans. We suggest that these genomically imprinted regions are candidates for influencing the population-specific mate-choice in mice.

Keywords: imprinting, genetic differentiation, RNASeq, Mus musculus, Mus domesticus, hypothalamus, vomeronasal organ, liver

Introduction

Parent-of-origin effects on gene function and expression (genomic imprinting) have been implicated in many developmental processes and genetic diseases (Wood and Oakey 2006). Imprinted genes are autosomal loci expressed from only one parental allele due to epigenetically inherited differential methylation marks. However, exclusion of the alternative parental allele does not need to be complete, that is, the expression of the two parental alleles to different degrees, depending on their parent-of-origin, is also known as genomic imprinting (Wolf et al. 2008; Gregg, Zhang, Weissbourd, et al. 2010; DeVeale et al. 2012). Most studies on genomic imprinting have focused on effects in early development, but independent of the early embryonic effects genomic imprinting can also influence behavioral patterns controlled by the adult brain, such as maternal care or social dominance (Wilkins and Haig 2003; Wolf and Hager 2009; Curley 2011; Garfield et al. 2011).

We have recently found that mate choice decisions in mice appear to be influenced by paternally inherited cues; these cues appear to be subject to fast evolution and thus contribute to within-species population divergence (Montero et al. 2013). In the respective study, we conducted an experiment that allowed the free choice of mates in a seminatural environment. We used two wild-caught populations of Mus musculus domesticus from France and Germany, which diverged approximately 3,000 years ago. Mating success was assessed through molecular paternity analysis. F1 offspring showed a general assortative pattern according to their population of origin. Matings between a hybrid and an animal of pure origin showed a strong preference for matching the paternal side of the hybrid. The study therefore suggests that a combination of learned and inherited cues must exist to allow such differential decisions. Moreover, the inherited cues must have diverged between the two populations to allow the population-specific decisions, that is, they are expected to belong to the molecularly highly differentiated fraction of their genomes. Given that the F1 hybrids in the study shared the same autosomal genome combinations yet displayed differential choice according to their paternal population origin, it seems possible that genomically imprinted loci mediated the respective genetic component. Here, we search for such candidate loci by identifying imprinted transcripts that are highly diverged and thus could be involved in the observed behavioral pattern.

Imprinted loci can be detected in reciprocal crosses between animals that differ in diagnostic single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) using RNA sequencing (RNAseq) (Wang and Clark 2014). However, this approach has yielded conflicting results, because the statistical evaluation of the data can be problematic. Based on RNAseq data and binomial sampling statistics, Gregg, Zhang, Weissbourd, et al. (2010) suggested that a large number of brain-expressed genes is affected by imprinting (Gregg, Zhang, Butler, et al. 2010). However, DeVeale et al. (2012) showed that this approach leads to an overestimate of imprinted genes and they suggest a statistical procedure that reduces biases in RNASeq data by estimating a false discovery rate (FDR) from the data through mock comparisons between samples of the same cross direction. This leads to much fewer identified loci, but independent confirmation assays showed that this is the more reliable way to find imprinted transcripts.

In our study, we focus on genes expressed in the hypothalamus (HYP), the vomeronasal organ (VNO), and the liver (LIV), because these are of particular interest for behavior and mate recognition. The HYP controls sexual behavior and maternal care and is responsive to olfactory stimuli, such as pheromones. The VNO is the olfactory sense organ in mice that detects pheromones. It has been implicated in detecting major histocompatibility complex (MHC) peptides that are thought to be relevant for optimal mate choice (Leinders-Zufall et al. 2009). The LIV produces the major urinary proteins (MUPs) that are excreted with the urine and play a general role in chemical communication among mice, including signaling of reproductive status (Roberts et al. 2010; Janotova and Stopka 2011; Nelson et al. 2013; Stockley et al. 2013).

We first used RNAseq data from reciprocal crosses of inbred strains from two Mus subspecies: M. m. domesticus and M. m. musculus. We apply the recommendations for deriving a false discovery rate (FDR) from the data proposed by DeVeale et al. (2012) and confirm their findings that a stringent statistical treatment is advantageous to obtain reliable data. We find many previously studied imprinted transcripts but also a number of new ones, most of which may be noncoding long RNAs and are of yet unknown function. A large number of novel imprinted transcripts is found in a region of chromosome 7 that is orthologous to the region involved in the Prader–Willi/Angelman syndromes in humans. These are complex disorders with parent-of-origin effects, including behavioral and cognitive deficiencies, that are driven by deletions or disruptions of imprinted gene clusters on human chromosome 15 (Horsthemke and Wagstaff 2008; Cassidy et al. 2012).

After having identified the imprinted transcripts in the cross of the inbred Mus subspecies, we also sequenced HYP RNA from eight individuals from each of the two M. m. domesticus populations of our original mate choice study (see above). Interestingly, we find that the transcripts in the Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS) region show highly differentiated alleles between the populations. Further, we find a second highly differentiated transcript, Peg13 on chromosome 15, that has also been implicated in Prader–Willi-like phenotypes in humans. We propose that these candidate genes could contribute to behavioral divergence patterns of natural mouse populations.

Results

Tissues for RNAseq analysis were derived from reciprocal crosses of animals from two inbred strains, WSB and PWD, that represent the two subspecies M. m. domesticus and M. m. musculus, respectively. RNA was extracted from the LIV, HYP, and the VNO. Between 16 and 53 million uniquely mapping paired reads were obtained for each cross direction in each tissue from several individuals (supplementary table S1, Supplementary Material online).

SNPs were initially considered to be imprinted when they showed the same direction of change in the reciprocal crosses (i.e., same parent of origin effect) with an initial P value cutoff of 0.05 (χ2 test). For example, overrepresentation of the WSB allele in the PWDxWSB cross and overrepresentation of the PWD allele in the WSBxPWD cross would be called paternally biased expression. However, for most of these “significant” SNPs the actual differences between maternal and paternal read counts were rather small. Because DeVeale et al. (2012) show that the application of such a simple cutoff leads to many false positives, we have followed their approach of a mock comparison to estimate a P value that would reduce the empirical FDR to less than 0.05 (fig. 1). The proposed mock comparison is a simple negative control that accounts for systematic error, technical variation, and biological variation. Basically, it asks how many SNPs exceed significance in a mock reciprocal cross (i.e., comparing samples with the same parental background as though they were from reciprocal crosses). In such a comparison, any reciprocally biased expression cannot be caused by genomic imprinting and is a measure of the technical and biological variation of the experimental approach (DeVeale et al. 2012).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Determination of FDR cutoff for the three tissues. To find a chi-square test cutoff corresponding to an FDR of 5%, reads from the same cross direction were split randomly into two groups and analyzed as if they originated from reciprocal crosses. Then the ratio of SNPs from such mock comparisons to the number of SNPs identified in reciprocal crosses was calculated. Values are plotted as a function of different P value cutoffs. The acceptable level of false positives was set to 5% (dashed lines), and corresponding cutoff values were applied for the data analysis. The relative variability in the LIV data is due to the small number of actually imprinted transcripts.

Based on this analysis, we use cutoff P values of P < 102 for HYP, 106 for VNO, and 105 for LIV. In addition, for those transcripts that were not already known from other studies we required that at least two SNPs within the annotated transcript show the same bias. These criteria were used to obtain the list of significantly imprinted transcripts for the three tissues (table 1). The largest number was found in HYP, and the smallest was found in LIV; these results support the notion that the brain is subject to more imprinting than other tissues.

Table 1.

List of Imprinted Transcripts Identified in the Present Study.

Region in mm10 Gene Name LIV HYP VNO Status SNPs Fst > 0.8
chr1:63,273,269-63,314,575 Zdbf2 pat > 0.95 Known
chr1:63,445,904-63,596,515 Adam23 + pat > 0.55 + Knowna,b

chr2:152,669,461-152,708,668 H13 (short) pat > 0.8 pat > 0.95 Known
chr2:152,669,461-152,708,668 H13 (long) mat > 0.7 mat > 0.8 mat > 0.6 Known
chr2:152,780,668-152,831,682 Bcl2l1 + pat > 0.55 + Knowna

chr2:157,556,362-157,566,361 Blcap + mat > 0.62 + Known
chr2:157,560,110-157,562,519 Nnat pat > 0.99 pat > 0.75 Known

chr6:4,674,350-4,747,204 Sgce pat > 0.95 pat > 0.95 Known
chr6:4,747,306-4,760,516 Peg10 pat > 0.95 Known
chr6:4,903,320-5,165,661 Ppp1r9a mat > 0.55 + Known
chr6:5,383,386-5,433,021 Asb4 mat > 0.8 Known

chr6:30,401,909-30,455,174 Klhdc10 + mat > 0.55 + Knowna
chr6:30,738,050-30,748,466 Mest pat > 0.95 pat > 0.95 Known
chr6:30,804,784-30,807,552 Copg2os2 pat > 0.95 Known
chr6:30,809,559-30,896,760 Copg2 + mat > 0.7 + Known

chr6:58,833,700-58,920,396 Herc3 mat > 0.65 + Known
chr6:58,905,233-58,907,126 Nap1l5 pat > 0.99 pat > 0.95 Known

chr7:6,671,269-6,672,888 AK003710 mat > 0.9 New
chr7:6,675,443-6,696,432 Zim1 mat > 0.9 Known
chr7:6,703,901-6,730,554 Peg3 pat > 0.95 pat > 0.99 pat > 0.99 Known
chr7:6,730,741-6,967,220 Usp29 pat > 0.99 Known

chr7:59,228,750-59,306,727 Ube3a + mat > 0.8 + Known
chr7:59,262,923-59,263,934 AK038761 pat > 0.99 Newc
chr7:59,281,852-59,290,247 A230073K19Rik pat > 0.99 Newc 1
chr7:59,307,924-59,324,149 C230091D08Rik + mat > 0.8 + Knowna
chr7:59,327,318-59,328,016 AK020709 pat > 0.95 New
chr7:59,937,467-59,975,759 D7Ertd715e pat > 0.99 Newc 4
chr7:59,976,740-59,980,676 AK139082 pat > 0.99 Newc 1
chr7:59,982,501-60,140,219 Snrpn / Snurf pat > 0.99 pat > 0.99 pat > 0.99 Known 1
chr7:61,010,256-61,012,230 AK046019 pat > 0.99 Known
chr7:61,072,752-61,089,737 AK038418 pat > 0.95 New
chr7:61,089,568-61,221,965 DOKist4 pat > 0.99 Knowna
chr7:61,529,410-61,615,327 B230209E15Rik pat > 0.95 New
chr7:61,705,850-61,927,574 A230057D06Rik pat > 0.95 New
chr7:61,751,446-61,753,692 AK031915/AK046509 pat > 0.99 + New
chr7:61,930,789-61,982,715 ENSMUST00000181804 pat > 0.99 New
chr7:61,930,944-61,934,821 AK048029 pat > 0.99 New
chr7:62,348,277-62,349,927 Ndn pat > 0.99 pat > 0.95 Known
chr7:62,376,979-62,381,640 Magel2 pat > 0.99 Known

chr7:128,439,777-128,461,513 Tial1 + mat > 0.65 + New
chr7:128,611,365-128,696,436 Inpp5f + pat > 0.8 + Known

chr7:142,575,532-142,578,146 H19 mat > 0.99 Known
chr7:142,650,768-142,658,804 Igf2 mat > 0.55 pat > 0.8 Known
chr7:143,107,254-143,427,042 Kcnq1 pat > 0.99 Known
chr7:143,458,339-143,461,050 Cdkn1c mat > 0.95 Known

chr9:89,909,775-90,026,979 Rasgrf1 pat > 0.95 Known

chr10:13,090,788-13,131,695 Plagl1 pat > 0.99 pat > 0.99 Known


chr11:11,814,101-11,890,408 Ddc + mat > 0.55 Known

chr11:11,930,499-12,037,420 Grb10 pat > 0.95 mat > 0.95 Known

chr11:22,972,005-22,976,496 Zrsr1 pat > 0.95 pat > 0.95 pat > 0.95 Known
chr11:22,899,728-22,982,284 Commd1/Murr + mat > 0.85 + Known

chr12:108,860,030-108,893,211 Wars + pat > 0.55 + Knowna,b
chr12:109,032,182-109,068,217 Begain pat > 0.75 Known
chr12:109,453,455-109,463,336 Dlk1 pat > 0.95 Known
chr12:109,542,023-109,568,594 Meg3 mat > 0.95 mat > 0.95 Known
chr12:109,589,193-109,600,330 Rtl1 mat > 0.8 Known
chr12:109,603,945-109,661,711 Rian mat > 0.99 mat > 0.99 Known
chr12:109,734,825-109,749,457 Mirg mat > 0.99 Known

chr13:107,413,865-107,414,767 ENSMUST00000061241 mat > 0.65 mat > 0.65 + New

chr15:72,506,991-72,508,007 AK039650 mat > 0.95 Knowna
chr15:72,589,620-73,061,204 Trappc9 + mat > 0.7 + Known
chr15:72,805,600-72,810,324 Peg13 pat > 0.99 pat > 0.99 pat > 0.99 Known 5
chr15:73,098,490-73,099,318 DQ715667 mat > 0.7 + Knowna
chr15:73,101,625-73,184,947 Eif2c2 + mat > 0.7 + Knowna

chr17:77,674,376-77,674,702 ENSMUST00000168236 mat > 0.55 mat > 0.55 mat > 0.6 New

chr18:12,972,252-12,992,948 Impact pat > 0.8 pat > 0.95 pat > 0.75 Known

Note.—Expression status is designed as “+” when expressed but not imprinted in a given tissue, as “−” when not expressed, and as the degree of maternal (mat) or paternal (pat) bias when imprinted. Chromosomal regions with clustered transcripts are separated by horizontal lines.

aFirst described by DeVeale et al. (2012).

bConfirmed by pyrosequencing in DeVeale et al. (2012).

cConfirmed by pyrosequencing in the present study.

We also found a large number of apparently imprinted transcripts on the X-chromosome, but these are not included in table 1 because the expression bias on the X-chromosome could also be due to unequal ratios of X-chromosome inactivation (Wang et al. 2010; Wang and Clark 2014). The respective SNPs are included in the overall results table (supplementary table S2, Supplementary Material online), but we do not discuss them further.

We find 64 (26 maternal/38 paternal) transcripts preferentially expressed from one parental allele in the HYP, 20 (6/14) in the VNO, and 8 (4/4) in the LIV. They fall into 13 chromosomal regions with two or more imprinted transcripts and seven regions containing only a single imprinted transcript (table 1). Many transcripts are expressed in two or all three tissues but may be imprinted in only one of them (table 1). No transcripts were found that are exclusively imprinted in the LIV, and only two were found that are exclusively imprinted in the VNO. A total of 45 transcripts with parentally biased expression (20 maternal/25 paternal) were found only in the HYP. Approximately two-thirds of the transcripts detected as significantly imprinted in our study are well-known imprinted genes (table 1). In addition, 9 of the 24 novel transcripts were also found in the DeVeale et al. (2012) study, and two of them were confirmed by pyrosequencing by these authors (table 1).

Three genes showed opposite imprinting in different tissues. Grb10 (chr11) is paternally expressed in HYP and maternally in VNO. This asymmetry was noted before where Grb10 is expressed from the paternal allele in the whole brain and from the maternal one in other tissues (Arnaud et al. 2003). It is interesting to note that the sensory epithelia of the VNO behave like the other tissues in this respect. The second locus that shows opposite imprinting is Igf2 (chr7); it shows a slight but significant maternal bias in HYP and a stronger paternal bias in VNO. Igf2 has so far mostly been known to be only paternally expressed during development and in various tissues (Chao and D'Amore 2008), but Gregg, Zhang, Weissbourd, et al. (2010) found this paternal and maternal expression in their data as well. Finally, the minor histocompatibility gene H13 (chr2) has two transcript variants with opposite patterns of imprinting in our data. This confirms the results by Wood et al. (2008) that differential utilization of polyadenylation sites can be epigenetically regulated.

Most of the detected genes occurred in clusters (table 1) in agreement with what is generally known for imprinted genes (Wood and Oakey 2006). In fact, most of the imprinted transcripts found in our survey that were not previously described are part of known clusters. The two exceptions are ENSMUST00000061241 (chr13) and ENSMUST00000168236 (chr17) that showed slight maternal biases (table 1). The largest number of new imprinted transcripts was found in the region between Ube3a and Magel2 on chromosome 7, which represents the region that is involved in the Prader–Willi/Angelman syndromes in humans. Several of these are nested within other transcription units, such as AK038761 and A230073K19Rik within Ube3a. In fact, there appear to be even further imprinted transcripts in the region, because we found SNPs that do not correspond to the annotated named transcripts on which we have focused the analysis. However, we refrain from discussing these, because it is currently difficult to assess whether these might simply correspond to splicing variants of the annotated genes.

Differentiation between Populations

The populations used in our previous study of mating preferences (Montero et al. 2013) are both from the subspecies M. m. domesticus that have been separated since no more than 3,000 years. Although these populations are genetically well differentiated (Ihle et al. 2006; Teschke et al. 2008; Staubach et al. 2012), they harbor only few differentially fixed SNPs (Staubach et al. 2012) that could be used for a systematic assessment of the imprinting status. However, as we are specifically interested in loci that show rapid divergence between the two populations, we have sequenced total HYP RNA from eight animals from each of the two populations to confirm the presence of the newly discovered transcripts in these wild populations. In addition, we assessed which of the transcripts show a particularly high genetic differentiation, that is, which may contribute to the observed differential mate recognition between the populations.

We aligned the reads to the mouse transcriptome and confirmed that all transcripts recovered from the inbred strains were also present in the wild-derived mice. We also assessed whether any of them is differentially expressed between the populations, based on read count coverage, but we did not find significant differences (not shown). We then calculated Fst for all transcripts and checked whether any of the imprinted transcripts had a high Fst, that is, shows strong genetic differentiation. The average Fst across all SNPs in the transcripts was 0.28, and 2.5% of the SNPs had an Fst > 0.8. An Fst > 0.8 implies a particularly high differentiation between the populations, that is, this is the pattern we were looking for. Using this value as a cutoff for highly differentiated SNPs, we checked all imprinted transcripts for whether they harbor SNPs with an Fst above this cutoff. We find five such transcripts, which are all paternally expressed (table 1).

Two of the transcripts, D7Ertd715e on chromosome 7 and Peg13 on chromosome 15, show several of such highly differentiated SNPs with Fst > 0.8 (table 1). In addition, three transcripts in the vicinity of D7Ertd715e, namely A230073K19Rik, AK139082, and Snrpn/Snurf, show single SNPs with Fst > 0.8. In fact, two other transcripts in the region, Ube3a and AK03876, harbor one SNP each with Fst > 0.7; this suggests that the whole region between Ube3a and Snrpn/Snurf is highly differentiated between the two mouse populations.

Given the high differentiation of these genes between the two populations, it was possible to design pyrosequencing assays for given SNPs to allow the verification of their imprinting status in individuals of both populations and their reciprocal crosses. For this we extracted HYP RNA of three biological replicates per cross. First, we confirmed the pyrosequencing approach for testing WSB/PWD reciprocal crosses for all four transcripts (fig. 2) and assessed the differential expression status in pure individuals of both wild-caught populations. Finally, we confirmed for each of the four tested transcripts in reciprocal crosses of both natural populations that they show the same expression bias as found in the WSBxPWD crosses (fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Pyrograms confirming the imprinting status at four loci in six groups of mice. The loci tested were (A) A230073K19Rik, (B/) AK038761, (C) AK139082, and (D) D7Ertd715e. Reciprocal crosses of WSB and PWD inbred strains as well as pure outbred French (MC), pure outbred German (CB) individuals, and reciprocal crosses of these strains were used with three samples per group (female is always listed first in the cross designations). The percent contribution of the one or other allele is listed in each field. Note that not all SNPs are completely diagnostic, but the data are consistent for the respective crosses. In cases where two peaks show the same height but the percent contribution confirms the skew of allelic expression, this is due to the amplicon sequence given for pyro-sequencing. If a SNP is preceded by a nucleotide being the same as one of the two SNP states, three scenarios can occur: 1) equal expression results in the one peak being twice as high as the other with normal height (not present in our data set), 2) unique expression of the allele preceded by the same nucleotide results in a peak twice as high as a normal peak and no peak for the second allele (e.g., individual 7), and 3) if just the second allele is expressed, this results in the first peak from the detected SNP-preceding nucleotide and the second normal height peak resulting from the expressed allele. Amplicon sequences are listed in supplementary table S3, Supplementary Material online.

Discussion

There is a growing realization that parent-of-origin effects play a significant role in the development and behavior of mammals (Wolf and Hager 2009; Curley 2011; Garfield et al. 2011). This corresponds to an increasing number of identified genes that show a parent-of-origin expression bias, that is, imprinting. Using next-generation sequencing approaches, it has become easier to generate data to identify imprinted genes, but these techniques have their own biases that can lead to artifacts when not properly controlled for. Our experimental design and data analysis procedure avoids technical and other biases by following the general recommendations proposed by Wang and Clark (2014). In addition, we have applied the statistical approach proposed by DeVeale et al. (2012) and used mock comparisons within a given data set to control for false positives. Our results confirm the observations of DeVeale et al. (2012) that this leads to a fairly reliable exclusion of false positives while retaining good sensitivity, and therefore we recommend this as a standard procedure for future analyses.

The problem with overcalling candidate loci exists mostly for cases with weakly biased expression, for example, 45% of the transcript from one parental allele versus 55% from the other. Wang and Clark (2014) suggest to raise the cutoff to 35–65% to avoid this uncertainty zone all together. On the other hand, some genes with only a small bias (Adam 23, Bcl2l1, Klhdc10, and Wars) were independently discovered in our data set and the DeVeale et al. (2012) data set. Two of them (Adam23 and Wars) were actually confirmed by quantitative pyrosequencing in the DeVeale et al. (2012) study. This gives confidence that the derivation of an FDR from the data is a suitable alternative to setting a more stringent cutoff. But independent of this, most of the newly discovered imprinted transcripts in our study would actually be called by both cutoff criteria (table 1); hence, we can consider them as real candidates for new imprinted genes in the mouse.

Many of the newly identified imprinted transcripts are located within a highly studied prototypic imprinted region that consists of a 3 Mb large cluster of genes located on chromosome 7. A partial deletion of the region (mostly encompassing Ube3a) in mice results in impaired learning and altered ultrasonic communication (Jiang et al. 2010). These are phenotypes that we had also implicated in the explanation in our mating preference study (Montero et al. 2013). We found that recognizing the mate from the appropriate populations involved a learning phase, that is, mice coming directly from cages into the seminatural environment were not yet primed for showing population-specific mate choice. Further, akin to what is known about behavioral imprinting of songs in birds, we suggested that ultrasonic vocalization (USV) could be involved in the mate choice decisions as well (Montero et al. 2013). In fact, in a parallel study on USV we found differences in song patterns between the two populations (von Merten et al. 2014).

Intriguingly, the same general region including Ube3a and Snrpn/Snurf harbors a cluster of four of the five detected transcripts with particularly high genetic differentiation between the populations. It represents a size of 1.1 Mb and thus appears to belong to the larger regions of high differentiation between these populations. In a previous genome-wide study based on high-density SNP chips to trace selective sweeps, we found about 300 similarly high differentiated regions with an average size of about 80 kb and a maximum size of up to 1.4 Mb between the German and French populations (Staubach et al. 2012). However, the Ube3a/Snrpn/Snurf region was not found as a candidate in this previous study, because it is not well represented by SNPs on the microarray, that is, it escaped the strict statistical criteria that we had applied in the study. A more detailed analysis of the region will become possible through analyzing full-genome resequencing data for these populations.

High genetic differentiation can be caused by positive selection in one or both populations or by random fixation along the lineages. However, the latter is less likely since regions of this size would be broken up by recombination under conditions of random coalescence processes. Still, although we cannot so far distinguish between selective and random events in this case with confidence, the very fact that it is a highly differentiated region implies that it could contribute to behavioral differences between the populations.

The whole PWS region that encompasses the genes Ube3a and Snrpn/Snurf has been first identified in humans as a sporadic disorder in which affected children have developmental delay, a characteristic behavioral profile, and a behaviorally caused obesity problem (Cassidy et al. 2012). They also suffer cognitive and speech and language impairments. The molecular pathogenesis and the roles of specific PWS genes in the complex phenotype are still poorly understood. At least 11 genes were found to be inactivated in PWS, most often because of a sporadic deletion of the paternal allele combined with the normal process of genomic imprinting that silences the maternal allele of these genes. The mouse PWS homologous region on chromosome 7 has been targeted in more than 30 mouse models of PWS that partly mimic the human phenotypes (Resnick et al. 2013). Most genes in the PWS region are paternally expressed; only Ube3a is maternally expressed. Hence, our finding that most of the newly discovered HYP transcripts in the region are also paternally expressed is in line with these patterns. The paternal expression is controlled by an imprinting center, and its deletion abolishes paternal expression of the genes and results in PWS in humans and a comparable phenotype in mice (Yang et al. 1998).

The evolution of the PWS region has been mostly studied at a larger evolutionary scale so far. Imprinting in the region appears to have evolved more than a 100 Ma after the fusion of two originally nonimprinted regions that contained Ube3a and Snrpn (Rapkins et al. 2006). The region has then acquired additional transcripts at least partly by retrotransposition (Chai et al. 2001; Rapkins et al. 2006) with rodent-specific (Chai et al. 2001) and human-specific variants (Neumann et al. 2014). Hence, the region appears to be generally actively evolving.

The second highly differentiated transcript, Peg13, has not been functionally studied so far but is located in the intron of a maternally expressed gene (Trappc9) that has been implicated in humans with intellectual disability disorders (Kakar et al. 2012; Marangi et al. 2013) and may be involved in its regulation (Court et al. 2014). Interestingly, patients with loss-of-function alleles show symptoms very similar to Prader–Willi-like phenotypes (Marangi et al. 2013). Although it seems too early to speculate about interactions of genetic pathways between these regions, it seems interesting that the two most highly differentiated paternally expressed transcripts that we found in our screen are related to regions involved in cognitive ability syndromes.

Three other previously studied genes with a paternal expression bias and a behavioral function could have been relevant for the effects that we have observed in the German versus French populations studied in Montero et al. (2013). These are Grb10, Peg3, and Rasgrf1. Grb10 was implicated in adult social dominance behavior (Garfield et al. 2011), and Peg3 was suggested to have evolved to regulate males experience-dependent preference for receptive females (Swaney et al. 2008). Rasgrf1 has been implicated in learning and memory, but the paternal expression appears to affect mostly olfactory learning and memory in neonatal mice (Drake et al. 2011). All three transcripts are neither differentiated between the populations, nor do they show signs of differential expression (as assessed by read counts in the HYP RNAseq data). Accordingly, we consider it as less likely that they contribute to the differential recognition effect that we had seen between members of the population. The same is true for the MUPs that are produced in the LIV and for which a transgenerational regulatory effect has been suggested (Nelson et al. 2013). Although we do see them expressed in our data, they show no signs of imprinting or enhanced differentiation. However, as MUPs represent a complex multigene family with variable copy numbers (Logan et al. 2008; Karn and Laukaitis 2012), there could also be an annotation and mapping problem that will need further investigation.

It has been shown that imprinted genes can influence adult behavior, including mating (Curley 2011). Our results point to the PWS region genes as candidates to be investigated for their role in mating behavior. However, because this behavior is complex, it remains to be further investigated whether imprinted genes are directly or only indirectly involved in explaining the paternally determined preference behavior observed by Montero et al. (2013). Investigating the architecture of parent-of-origin effects in mice, Mott et al. (2014) suggest that these are not necessarily controlled directly by imprinted genes but may depend on interactions with nonimprinted genes. Hence, any other of the hundreds of highly differentiated regions between the French and German population (Staubach et al. 2012) could also be involved in the population-specific mate choice. For example, we also found for two genes involved in USV population-specific selective sweeps (von Merten et al. 2014). Still, based on the cumulative evidence discussed above it would seem that the genes within the Ube3aSnrpn imprinted region on chromosome 7, possibly together with the Trappc9 region on chromosome 13, are good candidates for being involved in population-specific mate choice decisions.

Materials and Methods

Crosses and Samples

For an initial screen for imprinted transcripts in our target tissues in house mice, we generated crosses between inbred stains of M. m. domesticus (strain WSB/EiJ) and M. m. musculus (PWD/PhJ). Reciprocal crosses were set up and female offspring (3–6 individuals per genotype and cross direction, supplementary table S1 Supplementary Material online) were sacrificed at the age of 11–13 weeks to prepare the HYP, the VNO, and the LIV from the same individuals. All dissections were done by the same person (I.M.) following personal instructions and standardized protocols designed by Valery Grinevich (MPI for Medical Research Heidelberg for HYP) and Masayo Omura (MPI for Biophysics, Frankfurt for VNO). For the HYP, brains were removed and further dissection was performed on wet ice using gross anatomical borders (i.e., optic chiasm arms, optic tracts, and mammillary body) as markers to remove the HYP. For the VNO, we followed the first steps of the procedure described in Meeks and Holy (2009). Prepared tissues were immediately frozen on dry ice and kept at −70 ° until RNA preparation.

Sequencing libraries were constructed from the polyadenylated fraction of the RNA and sequenced as 100 bp paired-ends on an Illumina HiSeq sequencer. Libraries were prepared with the Illumina TruSeq RNA Sample Preparation Kit v2. To confirm the occurrence of the newly identified candidate transcripts in the wild type populations and to quantify allele frequencies of differentiating SNPs, we sequenced HYP RNA from 16 individuals from two wild-caught outbred M. m. domesticus populations that consisted of eight unrelated individuals each from France and Germany. Supplementary table S1, Supplementary Material online, provides the read statistics for both experiments. More details on the derivation and molecular analysis of these populations can be found in Ihle et al. (2006), Teschke et al. (2008), and Staubach et al. (2012). To confirm the imprinting status for a subset of the transcripts, we generated reciprocal crosses from these populations and prepared HYP RNA for pyrosequencing assays. Part of the preparations for this experiment were performed by a second person trained by I.M.

SNP Identification

Reads were aligned by TopHat 2.0.4 to the mm10 mouse reference genome (GRCm38) using the transcriptome defined by Mus_musculus.GRCm38.68. Up to four mismatches (up to three for German and French population samples) were allowed. Only uniquely mapping reads were kept and duplicated reads were discarded as PCR/optical artifacts with Picard tools. Reads were realigned around indels and recalibrated according to known SNPs with GATK by using SNPs derived from sequenced PWD RNA samples (supplementary table S1, Supplementary Material online) as well as available SNPs for the strains PWD, WSB, and B6. SNP calling was performed with Unified Genotyper from GenomeAnalysisTK-2.1-11. The tissues were analyzed separately. PWD SNPs were called from PWD samples and WSB SNPs were called from PWDxWSB and WSBxPWD samples together. At least eight reads per sample were required for SNP calling, and only genotypes without strand bias (GATK FS parameter >60) and that were consistent in all individuals from one cross direction were kept.

To call SNPs between French and German populations, deduplicated uniquely mapping reads with a depth of at least ten per sample and no strand bias were used. A subset of SNPs where only PWD or WSB differ from the reference genomes, or both differ but in different ways, was identified as SNPs between WSB and PWD. To minimize strain bias in mapping, two strain-specific genomes were then constructed in silico by introducing all identified PWD or WSB SNPs into the reference genome. All raw reads from F1s were remapped to both reference genomes (as described above), and a maximum of one mismatch was allowed. If a read was mapping to different coordinates in the PWD and WSB genomes, the better quality mapping was kept. Uniquely mapping deduplicated reads were used for quantification of allelic expression.

Detection of Imprinting

For all SNPs between WSB and PWD, we counted remapped reads in each of the parental versions. Reads from all individuals from a given cross direction were summed. Significance of allele-specific expression (ASE) was tested by a Chi-square test against equal proportions. Loci with the same parental ASE bias in reciprocal crosses were considered to be imprinted. To set an appropriate significance cutoff, we followed the procedure described by DeVeale et al. (2012). Briefly, we combined reads from three samples from a given cross direction and compared allelic expression for every SNP with pooled reads depth ≥24. We also performed the same analysis with reads from the same cross direction (WSBxPWD) split into two groups. Such mock comparison should not detect any imprinting, and therefore estimates a cutoff that limits the FDR to the desired level (see fig. 1 for further explanation). Three sets of 60 million reads each were tested in this way. The ratios of significant SNPs for the mock comparison and the real comparison were then plotted for a range of cutoff values, and the value representing an FDR of ≤0.05 was used for the identification of imprinted transcripts (fig. 1). All identified SNPs and their allelic imbalance values are provided in supplementary table S2, Supplementary Material online. The transcripts listed as imprinted in table 1 are annotated and named UCSC genes from the mm10 reference annotation. Additional SNPs were seen outside of annotated gene regions and usually correspond to expressed sequence tags that so far lack annotations. We have refrained from adding these to the list to remain conservative.

Assessment of Genetic Differentiation

SNPs were called for HYP transcripts as described above in individuals from French and German populations. Variance components and fixation indices (Fst) between the two populations were estimated according to Weir and Cockerham (1984) as implemented in R by Eva Chan (http://www.evachan.org/rscripts.html, last accessed September 5, 2014). As we focus on imprinted transcripts, we used a “single-parent” correction: Heterozygous positions were replaced with a randomly chosen allele and Fst was computed. The median value of Fst computed from 100 draws was used as the final value for any given position.

Confirmation by Pyrosequencing

Diagnostic SNPs were chosen from four candidate transcripts to design primers for their respective pyrosequencing assay (supplementary table S3, Supplementary Material online) using the Pyromark Assay Design software 2.0, assay scores >97. Hypothalamus RNA from pure breeds and reciprocal crosses, from three individuals each, was used to generate biotinylated amplicons using the Pyromark OneStep RT-PCR kit (Qiagen). Pyrosequencing was done on a Pyromark ID PSQ 96 MA Sequencer (Qiagen) following the manufacturer’s instructions, and allele frequencies were determined using the instruments' software.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary tables S1–S3 are available at Molecular Biology and Evolution online (http://www.mbe.oxfordjournals.org/).

Supplementary Data

Acknowledgments

The authors are indebted to our mouse team for supporting the experiment. The authors thank Valery Grinevich (MPI for Medical Research Heidelberg) and Masayo Omura (MPI of Biophysics, Frankfurt) for tips and tricks for the preparation of the HYP and the VNO and Natascha Hasenkamp for preparing HYP samples from wild mice. This study was financed by institutional funds of the Max-Planck Society.

References

  1. Arnaud P, Monk D, Hitchins M, Gordon E, Dean W, Beechey CV, Peters J, Craigen W, Preece M, Stanier P, et al. Conserved methylation imprints in the human and mouse GRB10 genes with divergent allelic expression suggests differential reading of the same mark. Hum Mol Genet. 2003;12:1005–1019. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddg110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Cassidy SB, Schwartz S, Miller JL, Driscoll DJ. Prader–Willi syndrome. Genet Med. 2012;14:10–26. doi: 10.1038/gim.0b013e31822bead0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chai JH, Locke DP, Ohta T, Greally JM, Nicholls RD. Retrotransposed genes such as Frat3 in the mouse Chromosome 7C Prader-Willi syndrome region acquire the imprinted status of their insertion site. Mamm Genome. 2001;12:813–821. doi: 10.1007/s00335-001-2083-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chao W, D'Amore PA. IGF2: epigenetic regulation and role in development and disease. Cytokine Growth Factor Rev. 2008;19:111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.cytogfr.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Court F, Camprubi C, Garcia CV, Guillaumet-Adkins A, Sparago A, Seruggia D, Sandoval J, Esteller M, Martin-Trujillo A, Riccio A, et al. The PEG13-DMR and brain-specific enhancers dictate imprinted expression within the 8q24 intellectual disability risk locus. Epigenetics Chromatin. 2014;7:5. doi: 10.1186/1756-8935-7-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Curley JP. Is there a genomically imprinted social brain? Bioessays. 2011;33:662–668. doi: 10.1002/bies.201100060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. DeVeale B, van der Kooy D, Babak T. Critical evaluation of imprinted gene expression by RNA-Seq: a new perspective. PLoS Genet. 2012;8:e1002600. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Drake NM, DeVito LM, Cleland TA, Soloway PD. Imprinted Rasgrf1 expression in neonatal mice affects olfactory learning and memory. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:392–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00678.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Garfield AS, Cowley M, Smith FM, Moorwood K, Stewart-Cox JE, Gilroy K, Baker S, Xia J, Dalley JW, Hurst LD, et al. Distinct physiological and behavioural functions for parental alleles of imprinted Grb10. Nature. 2011;469:534–538. doi: 10.1038/nature09651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gregg C, Zhang JW, Butler JE, Haig D, Dulac C. Sex-specific parent-of-origin allelic expression in the mouse brain. Science. 2010;329:682–685. doi: 10.1126/science.1190831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gregg C, Zhang JW, Weissbourd B, Luo SJ, Schroth GP, Haig D, Dulac C. High-resolution analysis of parent-of-origin allelic expression in the mouse brain. Science. 2010;329:643–648. doi: 10.1126/science.1190830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Horsthemke B, Wagstaff J. Mechanisms of imprinting of the Prader-Willi/Angelman region. Am J Med Genet A. 2008;146A:2041–2052. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.32364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ihle S, Ravaoarimanana I, Thomas M, Tautz D. An analysis of signatures of selective sweeps in natural populations of the house mouse. Mol Biol Evol. 2006;23:790–797. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msj096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Janotova K, Stopka P. The level of major urinary proteins is socially regulated in wild Mus musculus musculus. J Chem Ecol. 2011;37:647–656. doi: 10.1007/s10886-011-9966-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Jiang YH, Pan YZ, Zhu L, Landa L, Yoo J, Spencer C, Lorenzo I, Brilliant M, Noebels J, Beaudet AL. Altered ultrasonic vocalization and impaired learning and memory in Angelman syndrome mouse model with a large maternal deletion from Ube3a to Gabrb3. PLoS One. 2010;5:e12278. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kakar N, Goebel I, Daud S, Nurnberg G, Agha N, Ahmad A, Nurnberg P, Kubisch C, Ahmad J, Borck G. A homozygous splice site mutation in TRAPPC9 causes intellectual disability and microcephaly. Eur J Med Genet. 2012;55:727–731. doi: 10.1016/j.ejmg.2012.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Karn RC, Laukaitis CM. The roles of gene duplication, gene conversion and positive selection in rodent Esp and Mup pheromone gene families with comparison to the Abp family. PLoS One. 2012;7:e47697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Leinders-Zufall T, Ishii T, Mombaerts P, Zufall F, Boehm T. Structural requirements for the activation of vomeronasal sensory neurons by MHC peptides. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:1551–U1598. doi: 10.1038/nn.2452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Logan DW, Marton TF, Stowers L. Species specificity in major urinary proteins by parallel evolution. PLoS One. 2008;3:e3280. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Marangi G, Leuzzi V, Manti F, Lattante S, Orteschi D, Pecile V, Neri G, Zollino M. TRAPPC9-related autosomal recessive intellectual disability: report of a new mutation and clinical phenotype. Eur J Hum Genet. 2013;21:229–232. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2012.79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Meeks JP, Holy TE. An ex vivo preparation of the intact mouse vomeronasal organ and accessory olfactory bulb. J. Neurosci Methods. 2009;177:440–447. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Montero I, Teschke M, Tautz D. Paternal imprinting of mating preferences between natural populations of house mice (Mus musculus domesticus) Mol Ecol. 2013;22:2549–2562. doi: 10.1111/mec.12271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mott R, Yuan W, Kaisaki P, Gan XC, Cleak J, Edwards A, Baud A, Flint J. The architecture of parent-of-origin effects in mice. Cell. 2014;156:332–342. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Nelson AC, Cauceglia JW, Merkley SD, Youngson NA, Oler AJ, Nelson RJ, Cairns BR, Whitelaw E, Potts WK. Reintroducing domesticated wild mice to sociality induces adaptive transgenerational effects on MUP expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:19848–19853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310427110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Neumann LC, Feiner N, Meyer A, Buiting K, Horsthemke B. The imprinted NPAP1 gene in the Prader-Willi syndrome region belongs to a POM121-related family of retrogenes. Genome Biol Evol. 2014;6:344–351. doi: 10.1093/gbe/evu019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Rapkins RW, Hore T, Smithwick M, Ager E, Pask AJ, Renfree MB, Kohn M, Hameister H, Nicholls RD, Deakin JE, et al. Recent assembly of an imprinted domain from non-imprinted components. PLoS Genet. 2006;2:1666–1675. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Resnick JL, Nicholls RD, Wevrick R. Recommendations for the investigation of animal models of Prader-Willi syndrome. Mamm Genome. 2013;24:165–178. doi: 10.1007/s00335-013-9454-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Roberts SA, Simpson DM, Armstrong SD, Davidson AJ, Robertson DH, McLean L, Beynon RJ, Hurst JL. Darcin: a male pheromone that stimulates female memory and sexual attraction to an individual male's odour. BMC Biol. 2010;8:75. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-8-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Staubach F, Lorenc A, Messer PW, Tang K, Petrov DA, Tautz D. Genome patterns of selection and introgression of haplotypes in natural populations of the house mouse (Mus musculus) PLoS Genet. 2012;8:e1002891. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Stockley P, Bottell L, Hurst JL. Wake up and smell the conflict: odour signals in female competition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013;368:20130082. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Swaney WT, Curley JP, Champagne FA, Keverne EB. The paternally expressed gene Peg3 regulates sexual experience-dependent preferences for estrous odors. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122:963–973. doi: 10.1037/a0012706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Teschke M, Mukabayire O, Wiehe T, Tautz D. Identification of selective sweeps in closely related populations of the house mouse based on microsatellite scans. Genetics. 2008;180:1537–1545. doi: 10.1534/genetics.108.090811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. von Merten S, Hojer S, Pfeifle C, Tautz D. A role for ultrasonic vocalisation in social communication and divergence of natural populations of the house mouse (Mus musculus domesticus) PLoS One. 2014;9(5):e97244. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Wang X, Clark AG. Using next-generation RNA sequencing to identify imprinted genes. Heredity. 2014;113(2):156–166. doi: 10.1038/hdy.2014.18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Wang X, Soloway PD, Clark AG. Paternally biased X inactivation in mouse neonatal brain. Genome Biol. 2010;11:R79. doi: 10.1186/gb-2010-11-7-r79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Weir BS, Cockerham CC. Estimating F-statistics for the analysis of population-structure. Evolution. 1984;38:1358–1370. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1984.tb05657.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Wilkins JF, Haig D. What good is genomic imprinting: the function of parent-specific gene expression. Nat Rev Genet. 2003;4:359–368. doi: 10.1038/nrg1062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wolf JB, Cheverud JM, Roseman C, Hager R. Genome-wide analysis reveals a complex pattern of genomic imprinting in mice. PLoS Genet. 2008;4:e1000091. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wolf JB, Hager R. Selective abortion and the evolution of genomic imprinting. J Evol Biol. 2009;22:2519–2523. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01874.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Wood AJ, Oakey RJ. Genomic imprinting in mammals: emerging themes and established theories. PLoS Genet. 2006;2:1677–1685. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wood AJ, Schulz R, Woodfine K, Koltowska K, Beechey CV, Peters J, Bourc'his D, Oakey RJ. Regulation of alternative polyadenylation by genomic imprinting. Genes Dev. 2008;22:1141–1146. doi: 10.1101/gad.473408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Yang T, Adamson TE, Resnick JL, Leff S, Wevrick R, Francke U, Jenkins NA, Copeland NG, Brannan CI. A mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome imprinting-centre mutations. Nat Genet. 1998;19:25–31. doi: 10.1038/ng0598-25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

Articles from Molecular Biology and Evolution are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES