Abstract
Explaining the evolution and maintenance of polyandry remains a key challenge in evolutionary ecology. One appealing explanation is the sexually selected sperm (SSS) hypothesis, which proposes that polyandry evolves due to indirect selection stemming from positive genetic covariance with male fertilization efficiency, and hence with a male's success in postcopulatory competition for paternity. However, the SSS hypothesis relies on verbal analogy with “sexy-son” models explaining coevolution of female preferences for male displays, and explicit models that validate the basic SSS principle are surprisingly lacking. We developed analogous genetically explicit individual-based models describing the SSS and “sexy-son” processes. We show that the analogy between the two is only partly valid, such that the genetic correlation arising between polyandry and fertilization efficiency is generally smaller than that arising between preference and display, resulting in less reliable coevolution. Importantly, indirect selection was too weak to cause polyandry to evolve in the presence of negative direct selection. Negatively biased mutations on fertilization efficiency did not generally rescue runaway evolution of polyandry unless realized fertilization was highly skewed toward a single male, and coevolution was even weaker given random mating order effects on fertilization. Our models suggest that the SSS process is, on its own, unlikely to generally explain the evolution of polyandry.
Keywords: Fertilization efficiency, genetic covariance, indirect selection, polyandry, runaway coevolution, sperm competition, sexy-son
Female mating with multiple males within a single reproductive event is a widespread form of polyandry that has profound evolutionary consequences (Pizzari and Wedell 2013; Taylor et al. 2014). Such polyandry creates the opportunity for postcopulatory sexual selection and thereby drives the evolution of traits mediating sperm competition and cryptic female choice (Parker 1970; Kvarnemo and Simmons 2013; Parker and Birkhead 2013). Such polyandry might also alter the magnitudes of sexual conflict and mutation load and thereby affect population persistence (Holman and Kokko 2013). However, the fundamental questions of why such polyandry evolves and how it is maintained remain much debated (Simmons 2005; Evans and Simmons 2008; Slatyer et al. 2012; Parker and Birkhead 2013; Pizzari and Wedell 2013). Although it appears straightforward to understand that males can increase their reproductive success by mating with multiple females, the components of selection that drive the evolution of female multiple mating are often considerably less obvious (Bateman 1948; Arnold and Duvall 1994; Parker and Birkhead 2013).
In some systems, for example, where males provide nuptial gifts, multiple mating can increase female fecundity, implying that polyandry is under positive direct selection (e.g., Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Fedorka and Mousseau 2002; Engqvist 2007; Alonzo and Pizzari 2010; Slatyer et al. 2012). However, in other cases polyandry seems more likely to experience negative direct selection (i.e., to be costly for females), for example, because it causes physiological harm or increases disease or predation risk (e.g., Rowe 1994; Thrall et al. 2000; Chapman et al. 2003; Orsetti and Rutowski 2003; Wigby and Chapman 2005), but provides no obvious direct fitness benefit. A key remaining challenge, therefore, is to explain why polyandry evolves or is maintained in systems where direct natural selection on multiple mating seems most likely to be negative (Byrne and Roberts 2000; Hosken et al. 2003; Arnqvist and Kirkpatrick 2005; Fisher et al. 2006; Sardell et al. 2012).
Many hypotheses that invoke different forms of indirect selection, inbreeding avoidance, bet-hedging, and fertility assurance, have been proposed (Halliday and Arnold 1987; Harvey and May 1989; Keller and Reeve 1995; Zeh and Zeh 1996, 2008; Yasui 1997, 2001; Petrie and Kempenaers 1998; Jennions and Petrie 2000; Cornell and Tregenza 2007; Kokko and Mappes 2013). Prominent among these is the broad hypothesis that polyandry evolves due to indirect selection, defined as selection stemming from genetic covariances between polyandry and other female or male traits that experience direct selection (Lynch and Walsh 1998; Jennions and Petrie 2000; Simmons 2005; Pizzari and Gardner 2012; Parker and Birkhead 2013). One particularly influential suggestion is the “sexually selected sperm” hypothesis (hereafter SSS hypothesis; Harvey and May 1989; Keller and Reeve 1995). This hypothesis states that if alleles underlying polyandry are initially present at low frequency, the resulting occasional multiple mating creates opportunity for postcopulatory male–male competition to fertilize polyandrous females’ eggs. Polyandrous females will therefore increase the probability that their eggs will be fertilized by a male with relatively high “fertilization efficiency,” defined as a male's ability to succeed in postcopulatory competition for paternity through some form of sperm competition, displacement, or inhibition (Keller and Reeve 1995). If there is additive genetic variance in male fertilization efficiency, then positive genetic covariance between polyandry and fertilization efficiency is hypothesized to arise. Such covariance might cause alleles underlying polyandry to increase in frequency due to indirect selection stemming from positive direct selection on fertilization efficiency, thereby causing ongoing evolution of polyandry (Keller and Reeve 1995).
The SSS hypothesis assumes that there is additive genetic variance in both fertilization efficiency and polyandry (Evans and Simmons 2008; Evans and Gasparini 2013). In its purest form, it makes no further assumption of any preexisting genetic covariance between fertilization efficiency and polyandry, or between either trait and any other components of male or female fitness. The SSS hypothesis is therefore particularly appealing because it explains how the key genetic covariance that causes indirect selection on polyandry might arise as direct consequence of the competition over fertilization that polyandry itself creates. It therefore obviates the need to invoke any preexisting covariance due to pleiotropy or any other form of linkage. The SSS hypothesis has consequently attracted substantial interest and sparked empirical tests, and is widely cited as one plausible evolutionary explanation for polyandry (Bernasconi and Keller 2001; Simmons 2003; Simmons and Kotiaho 2007; Evans and Simmons 2008; Evans and Gasparini 2013; Klemme et al. 2014; McNamara et al. 2014).
However, there is a surprising paucity of explicit quantitative theory or models that examine whether the SSS mechanism is, in principle, sufficient to cause evolution of polyandry, thereby validating the SSS hypothesis. Curtsinger (1991) formulated a deterministic model with two diallelic loci that determine female tendency for polyandry and male sperm competitive ability, respectively, and showed that the conditions under which sperm competition can cause evolution of polyandry are extremely restricted. Specifically, there must be tight physical linkage between the two loci, positive linkage disequilibrium, and no direct fitness cost of polyandry. Curtsinger concluded that, given his model assumptions, sperm competition is unlikely to cause the evolution of polyandry. However, as Curtsinger (1991) noted and Keller and Reeve (1995) reiterated, Curtsinger's model has a well-known general limitation of single locus, diallelic models (Kirkpatrick 1982; Reeve 2000; Mead and Arnold 2004): the genetic variation attainable is very limited. The “more competitive” male genotype quickly fixates, thereby eliminating any linkage disequilibrium with polyandry and terminating coevolution. It follows that if polyandry has any direct fitness cost then the underlying allele is rapidly eliminated.
Keller and Reeve (1995) proposed that, in contrast to Curtsinger's (1991) conclusion, the SSS mechanism could in fact drive evolution of polyandry. They drew an analogy with well-established models explaining the evolution of female mating preferences for male display traits via Fisherian sexual selection (“sexy-son” models; Weatherhead and Robertson 1979; Kokko et al. 2006). In these models, alleles underlying preference, which might initially be rare, become positively genetically correlated with alleles underlying the preferred display trait due to inevitable assortative reproduction. This can cause “runaway” evolution of display away from its naturally selected optimum, causing correlated evolution of preference due to indirect selection (Fisher 1915). Since the first quantitative model that demonstrated the principle of the “runaway” process (Lande 1981), multiple different models have reached similar conclusions (reviewed in Mead and Arnold 2004; Kokko et al. 2006; Kuijper et al. 2012). When there is direct selection against the display trait but not against preference, coevolution depends on the genetic covariance between the two (Cpd) and on the additive genetic variance in display (Vd). Traits coevolve along lines of equilibrium with slope equal to Cpd/Vd (Lande 1981). Runaway coevolution is predicted if Cpd > Vd, whereas if Cpd < Vd the display only evolves to match the preference (Mead and Arnold 2004; Kokko et al. 2006). However, these lines of equilibrium are structurally unstable. When there is any direct selection against preference (i.e., preference is costly), the equilibrium is broken and preference, and the consequent coevolution of display, are eliminated (Pomiankowski 1987b; Bulmer 1989b; Barton and Turelli 1991). A variety of mechanisms, such as negatively biased mutations on the display (Pomiankowski et al. 1991), spatial variation in display (Day 2000), “condition-dependence” (Rowe and Houle 1996; van Doorn and Weissing 2004), dispersal, and negative pleiotropy (Jennions and Petrie 2000), can potentially maintain sufficient additive genetic variance in the display to rescue indirect selection and hence maintain preference even when it is costly.
Keller and Reeve (1995) suggested that female tendency for polyandry and male fertilization efficiency can be considered analogous to preference and display, respectively. They suggested that positive linkage disequilibrium between alleles underlying polyandry and fertilization efficiency will arise in exactly the same way as linkage disequilibrium between preference and display, thereby causing coevolution. They argued that the same mechanisms that might maintain genetic variation in display (e.g., negatively biased mutations) might also maintain genetic variation in fertilization efficiency and thus maintain indirect selection on polyandry even when it is costly.
However, while the verbal analogy between the “sexy-son” and the SSS processes seems compelling, at closer examination it is not necessarily perfect. Preference and display are directly linked through assortative reproduction stemming from active female choice. This is not the case for polyandry where females actively mate multiply but, at least under the pure SSS hypothesis, are assumed not to directly choose males based on their fertilization efficiency. The stochasticity introduced by precopulatory mate choice unrelated to males’ fertilization efficiency might mean that any covariance arising between polyandry and fertilization efficiency is weaker than that arising between preference and display, thereby weakening indirect selection on polyandry. Moreover, other factors that influence the outcome of postcopulatory competition for paternity, including variation in the probability of fertilization associated with mating order (e.g., first or last male precedence) or mating frequency (Birkhead and Hunter 1990; Harshman and Clark 1998; Pischedda and Rice 2012), and the relationship between males’ relative fertilization efficiency and realized paternity share, might further prevent positive linkage disequilibrium between polyandry and fertilization efficiency from developing. Because of such intrinsic biological differences between female preference for male displays versus polyandry in relation to male fertilization efficiency, there is an as yet unfulfilled need for quantitative models that explicitly validate Keller and Reeve's (1995) SSS hypothesis and identify any restrictive conditions under which it might be expected to operate.
In summary, we are left with the questions of whether polyandry could, in principle, evolve via pure Fisherian indirect selection resulting from postcopulatory competition for paternity (Harvey and May 1989; Keller and Reeve 1995) as is widely hypothesized (Simmons 2005; Evans and Simmons 2008; Klemme et al. 2014; McNamara et al. 2014), or whether such evolution requires further conditions. To answer these questions we build and compare two analogous models: one considering coevolution of preference and display and one considering coevolution of polyandry and fertilization efficiency. We thereby formalize Keller and Reeve's (1995) verbal analogy between the “sexy-son” and SSS processes and test (1) the basic premise that runaway coevolution of polyandry and fertilization efficiency can in principle occur in an analogous fashion to runaway coevolution of preference and display in the absence of direct selection; (2) whether such coevolution can continue in the presence of negative direct selection (i.e., when both traits are costly); (3) whether negatively biased mutations on fertilization efficiency can rescue indirect selection on polyandry; (4) whether coevolution between polyandry and fertilization efficiency depends on the relationship between males’ relative fertilization efficiency and realized fertilization success; (5) whether coevolution still occurs when there is environmental variance in fertilization efficiency conceptualized as mating order effects on paternity (i.e., male precedence). We thereby identify conceptual similarities and differences between the “sexy-son” and SSS processes, and discuss the degree to which the latter process might potentially drive the evolution of polyandry in nature.
Methods
We used genetically explicit, individual-based models (e.g., Reeve 2000; Lorch et al. 2003; van Doorn and Weissing 2004; Fawcett et al. 2007; Kuijper et al. 2012) to compare coevolution of preference and display (hereafter “mate-choice model”) with coevolution of polyandry and fertilization efficiency (hereafter “SSS model”). The mate-choice model is built to recreate well-known results (Lande 1981; Pomiankowski et al. 1991; Mead and Arnold 2004; Kokko et al. 2006; Kuijper et al. 2012) within the same framework as our new SSS model, facilitating comparison between the “sexy-son” and SSS processes. The two models are analogous in every respect except for differences stemming from key biological characteristics of the different traits considered. Individual-based models do not require restrictive a priori assumptions regarding the distributions of genotypic values or the magnitude or direction of genetic covariances. Rather, they can allow these distributions and covariances to emerge, facilitating investigation of the magnitudes and dynamics of arising linkage disequilibria.
Genetic Architecture
Both models assume a diploid additive genetic system with two autosomal traits: female preference (P) and male display (D) in the mate-choice model, and female tendency for polyandry (Py) and male fertilization efficiency (F) in the SSS model. In each model, all individuals of both sexes carry L diploid physically unlinked loci underlying both traits, with sex-limited phenotypic expression. Any genetic covariance between female and male traits therefore results exclusively from linkage disequilibrium generated by assortative reproduction. We assumed a continuum-of-alleles model (Kimura 1965; Lande 1976; Reeve 2000) whereby the possible allelic distribution at each locus comprises an infinite number of alleles producing a continuous distribution of genetic effects. Each individual's genotypic value is calculated as the sum of all 2L allelic values underlying each trait (hereafter denoted gP, gD, gPy, and gF for the four traits, respectively). We did not model any explicit environmental variance, meaning that individuals’ phenotypic values (hereafter P, D, Py, and F) are primarily determined by their genotypes. However, because of stochastic processes affecting trait expression (see below), realized heritability is in some cases less than one.
Models examining mate choice evolution typically allow preference and display traits to assume any real number (e.g., Lande 1981; Pomiankowski et al. 1991; Pomiankowski and Iwasa 1998; Higashi et al. 1999; Day 2000). Although this is mathematically convenient and justifiable in some cases, allowing negative values does not make biological sense for polyandry because females can mate once or more times, but cannot mate a negative number of times. We therefore allowed the male phenotypes (D and F) to take any real value, but constrained the female phenotypes (P and Py) to be equal to or greater than zero by equating any negative genotypic value to zero. Preliminary simulations for our mate-choice model that did and did not constrain female phenotypes to be positive both quantitatively reproduced expectations from previous theory (Lande 1981; Mead and Arnold 2004; Kokko et al. 2006). Our constrained model is therefore appropriate to test the SSS hypothesis in relation to existing models of mate choice.
For each locus, offspring inherit single random alleles from their mother and father. Each allele has a mutation probability of μ per generation. When a mutation occurs, a mutational effect sampled from a normal distribution with mean m and variance σ2m [i.e., N(m, σ2m)] is added to the allele value (Table S1; Kimura 1965; Lande 1976). Negatively biased mutational effects on male traits are sampled from a normal distribution N(m′, σ2m) (Table S1).
Mating, Reproduction, and Survival
We modeled a single, freely mixing population with nonoverlapping generations, and a 1:1 primary sex ratio. Each generation starts with reproduction, split into mating, fertilization, and birth stages. After reproduction, all adults die and offspring survive to adulthood according to density dependence and viability selection.
The mating phase differs conceptually between the mate-choice and SSS models. In the mate-choice model, each female chooses a male according to the strength of her preference and sampled male displays. We implemented a best-of-N model (Seger 1985; Fawcett et al. 2007), where the female chooses from a random sample of NmalesI males. This constraint facilitates analogy with the SSS model, where polyandrous females typically mate with relatively few males randomly sampled from the population (see below). The best-of-N constraint introduces stochasticity into precopulatory mate choice, causing the heritability of preference to be less than one and reducing the correlation between preference and display (Benton and Evans 1998). This model is therefore conservative regarding possible runaway coevolution. Within the random sample of males, each male j has a probability (pcj) of being chosen by female i given by (Lande 1981; Pomiankowski et al. 1991; Higashi et al. 1999; Lorch et al. 2003; Fawcett et al. 2007):
(1) |
To avoid numerical errors, we imposed an arbitrary maximum phenotypic value of 60 for D, thereby curtailing ongoing runaway toward infinite values. Imposing this maximum does not hinder initial coevolution. Each female produces a number of offspring sampled from a Poisson distribution with mean R and all offspring are sired by the female's chosen male. Individual males can mate with unlimited females.
In the SSS model we assumed that males cannot directly advertise their fertilization efficiency, meaning that females cannot exert direct precopulatory choice (therefore conceptualizing the pure SSS hypothesis; Keller and Reeve 1995). Each female mates with a number of randomly selected males, NmalesII, given by
(2) |
All females therefore mate at least once (which is assumed to ensure full fertility), and the number of additional matings is positively correlated with a female's genotypic value for polyandry. This formulation relaxes the unrealistic assumption that polyandrous females mate with as many males as necessary to sample the population's entire sperm pool (as assumed by Curtsinger 1991). One assumption of the mate-choice model, that preference P is continuously distributed (e.g., Lande 1981; Pomiankowski et al. 1991; Pomiankowski and Iwasa 1998; Higashi et al. 1999; Day 2000), is not appropriate for polyandry Py because females mate a discrete number of times. Equation (2) translates continuous genetic variation in gPy into a discrete phenotype Py. This introduces some additional nongenetic variance in Py compared to P, constituting a conceptual difference between the mate-choice and SSS models. Using threshold models to translate continuous variation in gPy to discrete variation in Py yielded similar conclusions.
As in the mate-choice model, each female produces a number of offspring sampled from a Poisson distribution with mean R. After mating, all the female's mates compete for the paternity of each offspring (conceptually, through sperm competition and related mechanisms, Parker 1970; Keller and Reeve 1995). Some function relating each male's fertilization efficiency F to realized fertilization success and paternity is therefore required. Our primary model assumes a “fair-raffle” (Parker 1990) weighted by each male's relative F. The probability, pfj, for male j to fertilize each of female's eggs is:
(3) |
As for equation (1), we imposed an arbitrary maximum value of 60 for F. We used equation (3) for fertilization probability to facilitate analogy with the probability that a male would be chosen in relation to his display in the mate-choice model (eq. 1; Lande 1981). However, the two functions differ, reflecting an intrinsic difference between the pre- and postcopulatory processes. Specifically, while P and D are directly linked through precopulatory female choice, Py and F are only indirectly linked via the postcopulatory competition for paternity created by polyandry. Female preference is part of equation (1) because females exert direct choice among sampled males. By contrast, polyandry does not appear in equation (3) because females do not directly choose males based on their fertilization efficiency.
The offspring survival phase is identical in both models. Each offspring, i, has a survival probability, psi, determined by the population carrying capacity K and its individual viability, vi, weighted by the sum of the viabilities of all offspring (Noff) in the population (cf. Burton et al. 2010):
(4) |
When no direct fitness cost is applied to any of the four focal traits, vi = 1. Costs are applied through stabilizing selection toward a naturally selected optimum, θ, for the trait (Haldane 1954; Lande 1981; Bulmer 1989a). Generally, given nonzero cost of trait t, individual viability is:
(5) |
Here, t represents the phenotypic value of the trait (D or F for males and P or Poisson(gPy) for females), θt the trait's naturally selected optimum and ωt the strength of stabilizing selection, where higher values of ωt correspond to weaker selection (see Fig. S1).
Form of Fertilization
Fair-raffle fertilization, where males that mate with a particular female fertilize ova in approximate proportion to their relative F values, may be common in nature (Simmons 2014, see Discussion). However there are other conceivable outcomes, such as a “winner-takes-all” scenario where the male with the highest F fertilizes all of a female's ova irrespective of the relative F values of the female's other mates (e.g., Simmons and Siva-Jothy 1998; Wedell and Cook 1998; Simmons 2014). This scenario is conceptually more similar to female preference for male display, where all of a female's offspring are sired by the single preferred male. The operation of the SSS process might therefore depend on the relationship between a male's relative F and his realized fertilization success. To investigate this dependence we modified (3) as:
(6) |
Here, α determines the degree to which postcopulatory fertilization success is biased toward the male with the highest F value out of each female's mates. A value of α = 1 corresponds to the fair-raffle weighted by relative F (eq. 3), whereas α > 1 biases fertilization success more strongly toward the male with the highest F up to “winner-takes-all,” whereas α < 1 weakens the association between F and fertilization success, such that paternity is distributed more evenly than in the F-weighted fair-raffle.
Male Precedence
In nature, strong first or last male precedence is often observed, where the first or last male to mate with a female sires a disproportionate number of offspring (e.g., Parker 1970; Birkhead and Hunter 1990; Watson 1991; Lacey et al. 1997; Price et al. 1999; Kraaijeveld-Smit et al. 2002). When mating order is not itself genetically determined, male precedence implies strong environmental modulation of underlying genetic effects on fertilization. We therefore further investigated the degree to which such environmental effects and resulting precedence could modulate the SSS process. Here, a male's fertilization probability is determined not only by his F value but also by the rank order in which he mates with the female relative to her other mates, assuming first male precedence. Because mating order is random in our model, the results would be identical assuming last male precedence. The probability, pfj, for male j to fertilize each of female's eggs becomes:
(7) |
where β represents the strength of precedence and rankj is the rank order with which male j mated with female i (0 ≤ rankj ≤ NmalesII; see Fig. S2).
Simulations
We ran simulations that quantified coevolution of male and female traits (D and P in the mate-choice model and F and Py in the SSS model) with (1) no direct selection on either trait (i.e., no costs); (2) direct selection on both traits (i.e., both are costly); (3) negatively biased mutations on the male trait; (4) different relationships between relative F and realized fertilization; (5) nongenetic precedence in fertilization. When direct selection was applied, we varied the strength of selection, ω, on the female traits, but held selection on the male traits constant (ω2D = ω2F = 1.0). Conclusions remained similar when selection on the male traits was also varied. All model variables and parameter values are summarized in Table S1. Five hundred females and 500 males were initialized in each simulation. Initial allelic values for each trait were sampled from specified normal distributions (Table S1; Fig. S3). Thereafter, the distributions of genetic and phenotypic values for the four traits, and associated genetic (co)variances, emerged from the processes of drift, selection, and mutation encapsulated in the model and were otherwise unconstrained. All simulations were run for 10,000 generations, checked for equilibrium, and replicated 50 times. We report results for the first 5000 generations as all simulations had already equilibrated. We primarily present cross-sex genetic correlations instead of covariances to allow comparison between models (as correlations are unit-free variance-standardized covariances). Genetic variances and covariances are provided as Supporting Information. Genetic correlations and covariances arising within each generation of each simulated population were calculated across the within-individual genotypic values of male and female traits.
To quantify female trait values at mutation-selection balance, we additionally simulated “neutral” traits with no function (meaning that each female mated with one random male) but the same cost as the active female traits (i.e., P or Py). Finally, we quantified model sensitivity to the number of loci L, the mutation rate μ, and the mutational variance σ2m. Conclusions were not sensitive to the value of R.
Results
Basic Models
When there was no direct selection on preference or display (i.e., both traits were cost-free), the mate-choice model produced the expected Fisherian “runaway,” where both traits coevolved and increased in value across generations (up to the imposed numerical limit; Fig. 1A, C). Runaway coevolution occurred consistently across replicate simulations (Fig. 1A, C).
The basic SSS model, where polyandry and fertilization efficiency were cost-free with “fair-raffle” fertilization, produced qualitatively weaker coevolution (Fig. 1B, D). Generally, mean Py slowly increased or persisted around the initialization value while mean F increased. However, in some replicates polyandry did not evolve at all, but instead decreased to zero (Fig. 1B, D).
In both models, the expected positive genetic correlation between the female and male traits arose in the first few generations, but was larger in the mate-choice model than in the SSS model (Fig. 1E, F). The correlations peaked within the first five generations then gradually decreased to zero. The correlation between gP and gD decreased to zero only once the “runaway” was halted by the artificial maximum imposed on D (Fig. 1). However, the correlation between gPy and gF decreased to zero well before F reached the imposed maximum, effectively stopping coevolution of Py and F (Fig. 1). As expected, the genotypic variance for the male traits decreased quickly in both models (Fig. 1G, H). However, it decreased earlier in the mate-choice model, resulting from stronger sexual selection on display than on fertilization efficiency (Fig. S4).
Direct Selection (Costs)
As expected from previous theory (Pomiankowski 1987b; Bulmer 1989b; Barton and Turelli 1991), the mate-choice model showed that when both preference and display experienced direct selection (i.e., were costly), P eventually stabilized at the mean value expected under mutation-selection balance (Fig. 2A, C). For small costs, the slower decrease in P allowed D to be temporarily pulled away from mutation-selection balance but, as P decreased, D gradually decreased back to mutation-selection balance.
A similar pattern emerged for polyandry and fertilization efficiency when both were costly, in that neither persisted above mutation-selection balance (Fig. 2B, D). Therefore, as for P and D, coevolution of Py and F did not readily occur in the presence of opposing direct selection.
The initial mean genetic correlation between gPy and gF was less than half that between gP and gD, especially for lower costs on the respective female traits. Generally, in both models, the higher the cost on the female trait, the smaller the initial correlation between female and male traits and the faster the correlation decreased across generations. Genotypic variances and covariances are shown in Figure S5.
Biased Mutations
As expected (Pomiankowski et al. 1991), introducing negatively biased mutations on display D maintained some genetic variation and hence maintained preference away from mutation-selection balance (Figs. 3 and 4A, C, E). Stronger negatively biased mutations on D (i.e., more negative mean m′ of the distribution of mutational effects) and weaker direct selection on P resulted in higher equilibrium values of both P and D. The higher the cost on P, the stronger the negative mutation bias necessary to maintain P away from mutation-selection balance (Fig. 4).
In contrast, in the SSS model, imposing negatively biased mutation on fertilization efficiency failed to substantially rescue coevolution with polyandry (Figs. 3 and 4B, D, F). Even for small costs of polyandry, strong negatively biased mutations led to mean equilibrium values for Py that were very close to the expected mutation-selection balance (Figs. 3F and 4F).
In both models, negatively biased mutations had the expected effect of maintaining greater genetic variance in the male trait (Fig. S6). However, the magnitude of mutation bias hardly affected the magnitude of the genetic correlation between female and male traits (Fig. S7). For lower costs on the female traits, in the presence of negatively biased mutations, a slightly higher correlation was maintained between gP and gD, while the correlation between gPy and gF was not affected. Therefore, in our simulations with fair-raffle fertilization, Keller and Reeve's (1995) proposition that the SSS process, and resulting evolution of polyandry, could be rescued by negatively biased mutations in fertilization efficiency is not supported.
Form of Fertilization
Coevolution between polyandry and fertilization efficiency was influenced by the relationship between relative male F and realized fertilization success (Fig. 5). With no costs, fertilization scenarios that tended toward “winner-takes-all” (α > 1) lead to reliable runaway coevolution between Py and F (Fig. 5A, B) and the stochasticity observed with “fair-raffle” fertilization (α = 1) progressively disappeared (Fig. S8). In contrast, when paternity was shared more evenly than under an F-weighted fair-raffle (α < 1), coevolution was even weaker and more stochastic, and Py frequently decreased to zero or remained at the initialization value (Figs. 5A, B, and S8). Furthermore, even given “winner-takes-all” fertilization, coevolution did not occur when polyandry was costly; Py then equilibrated at mutation-selection balance (Fig. 5D, E). However, with a low cost on Py, strong negatively biased mutations on F “rescued” polyandry and maintained the population mean above mutation-selection balance (Figs. 5G, H, and S9). Indeed, the stronger the fertilization bias toward the male with the highest F the stronger the genetic correlation between Py and F. Genotypic variances and covariances are shown in Figure S10.
Male Precedence
Assuming “fair-raffle” fertilization, the occurrence of nongenetic male precedence in fertilization further reduced the genetic correlation between gPy and gF, causing even weaker coevolution than in the absence of any such precedence (Fig. 6C, F, I, N). Of course, the extreme case where the first male to mate fertilizes all the female's eggs (β = 10.0), eliminated the opportunity for selection on fertilization efficiency, causing F (and indirectly Py) to be effectively neutral in absence of costs (Fig. 6A–C). Stronger precedence then caused Py and F to evolve to lower mean values than without any precedence.
When both Py and F were costly, Py stabilized at mutation-selection balance (Fig. 6D–F), as with no precedence. Moreover, F equilibrated at lower values for higher degrees of precedence (Fig. 6D). Imposing negatively biased mutations on F (Fig. 6G–I) did not change this general pattern. The same was true when fertilization differed from “fair-raffle” (Fig. 6L–N). With high paternity bias (α = 5), low cost and negatively biased mutations, Py still evolved given some precedence. However, the genetic correlation between Py and F and the equilibrium value of Py decreased with increasing precedence. Genotypic variances and covariances are shown in Figure S11.
Sensitivity Analyses
In the absence of costs, both the mate-choice and SSS models were sensitive to the number of loci, L (Figs. 7A and S12A). The main difference was between L = 1 and L > 1. With L = 1, the genetic variances and correlation between the female and male traits rapidly tended toward zero (Figs. S13A, B and S14A, B). This effectively stopped runaway coevolution and the female trait evolved to lower values than in corresponding multilocus models. With costs, results were independent of L and the female traits equilibrated at mutation-selection balance (Fig. S12B–D; cf. Fig. 2). However, the combination of multiple loci and strong negatively biased mutations on the male trait resulted in higher mean equilibrium values of P and Py (Fig. 7B).
Both models were sensitive to mutation rate, μ, in the absence of costs (Figs. 7C and S15A) and, given costs, in the presence of strong negatively biased mutations on the male trait (Figs. 7D and S15C). Given weak direct selection on polyandry (ω2Py = 200.0), high mutation rate (μ = 0.01), and strong negatively biased mutations (m′ = −0.35) polyandry was, on average, maintained at or above its initialization values (Figs. 7D and S15C), although showed considerable stochasticity (Fig. 7D). This outcome was associated with the maintenance of high genetic variance in the female and male traits and consequent higher genetic covariance (Fig. S17).
Discussion
Following Keller and Reeve's (1995) compelling formulation, the SSS hypothesis, which proposes that polyandry (Py) evolves due to indirect selection stemming from positive genetic covariance with male fertilization efficiency (F), has been widely cited as one plausible evolutionary explanation for polyandry. However, there remains a surprising lack of quantitative models that formally validate the premise of the SSS hypothesis or identify conditions under which it might operate. We developed a genetically explicit model to test whether the SSS hypothesis is valid despite contrary conclusions drawn from simple diallelic models (Curtsinger 1991). Specifically, we considered whether indirect selection created by postcopulatory competition over fertilization could be sufficient to promote evolution of polyandry in an analogous way to the evolution of female preferences (P) through positive genetic covariance with male displays (D). We conclude that while it is possible, under certain conditions, for intrinsic genetic covariances to arise and cause or maintain some level of polyandry, these conditions are rather restricted. In the presence of even weak direct selection against polyandry (i.e., small costs), it appears unlikely that polyandry could generally evolve or be maintained exclusively due to intrinsic genetic covariance with fertilization efficiency resulting from assortative reproduction.
Evolution of Cost-Free Polyandry
In the absence of direct selection against polyandry and fertilization efficiency, polyandry did evolve due to intrinsic indirect selection as postulated (Harvey and May 1989; Keller and Reeve 1995). Such evolution occurred consistently when fertilization tended toward “winner-takes-all,” such that all of a female's offspring were sired by the male with whom she mated that had the highest F. However, when fertilization followed a “fair-raffle” weighted by the relative F values of each female's mates, evolution of polyandry was less reliable than evolution of female preference. The stochastic outcome of the SSS process under the “fair-raffle,” and the overall dependency on the relationship between relative F and realized fertilization success, suggests that the SSS process might not on its own provide a deterministic explanation for the widespread evolution of polyandry in nature, even when polyandry and high fertilization efficiency are both completely cost-free.
With “fair-raffle” fertilization, the emergent genetic correlation between Py and F was weaker than the analogous genetic correlation between P and D. The analogy between the “sexy-son” and SSS processes, on which the SSS hypothesis relies (Keller and Reeve 1995), is then only partly valid. This is due to inherent differences between the assumed biological processes of precopulatory female preference and postcopulatory male competition for fertilization. Key is the division of paternity across females’ offspring, which influences the reproductive and genetic associations between the female and male traits. With female preference, the preferred male is assumed to sire all the female's offspring, creating a direct reproductive and genetic association between P and D. By contrast, assuming that males cannot advertise their fertilization efficiency and “fair-raffle” fertilization, paternity is shared between the female's mates proportionally to their F values. Males with high F are then likely to sire at least some offspring of polyandrous females, thereby creating some positive genetic covariance between Py and F. However, as genetic variance in F is eroded paternity will likely be shared among the female's mates, thus reducing any genetic covariance between Py and F. Coevolution between polyandry and fertilization efficiency stemming from the SSS process is then weaker than the analogous coevolution between preference and display predicted by long-standing models of Fisherian runaway. When paternity is disproportionately biased toward males with higher F, the SSS process becomes more similar to the “sexy-son” process.
Direct Selection and Negatively Biased Mutations
The SSS hypothesis seems appealing because it suggests one means by which positive indirect selection on polyandry might arise, thereby explaining why polyandry might evolve and persist despite negative direct selection (Simmons and Kotiaho 2007; Evans and Simmons 2008; Evans and Gasparini 2013; McNamara et al. 2014). However, this logic is clearly invalid if the SSS mechanism that is postulated to cause positive indirect selection on polyandry does not work when there is negative direct selection (i.e., when polyandry is costly). Through their basic model formulations, Curtsinger (1991) and Keller and Reeve (1995) both concluded that the basic SSS process cannot work given any direct cost of polyandry. However, this point is rarely mentioned in subsequent literature, and the SSS hypothesis is often (inconsistently) invoked as an explanation for ongoing evolution or maintenance of polyandry in the face of putative direct costs (Evans and Simmons 2008; Klemme et al. 2014). Our model confirms that polyandry cannot evolve solely due to the pure SSS process when multiple mating is costly, irrespective of the form of fertilization and even if the cost is very small, without some additional force (just as costly preferences for displays cannot evolve through basic indirect selection).
Negatively biased mutations in fertilization efficiency have been hypothesized to maintain genetic variance and rescue the SSS process, thereby maintaining nonzero polyandry even when multiple mating is costly (Keller and Reeve 1995). Such rescue can occur for preference and display (Pomiankowski et al. 1991), as illustrated by our mate-choice model. However, our SSS model shows that, contrary to Keller and Reeve's (1995) key suggestion, the same rescue does not necessarily occur for polyandry and fertilization efficiency. Even with a very small cost of polyandry and strong negatively biased mutations in F, the mean value of polyandry maintained was often only marginally higher than mutation-selection balance.
Some degree of evolution and maintenance of costly polyandry arose given relatively extreme parameterization of our SSS model, such as “winner-takes-all” fertilization or very high mutation rates plus strong negatively biased mutations (Figs. 5G and 7C, D) and, to a lesser extent, numerous loci per trait (Fig. 7A, B), creating high standing genetic variation. There is therefore scope for some degree of costly polyandry to be maintained via indirect selection stemming from postcopulatory competition over fertilization, provided that high genetic variance in F is somehow maintained and the female's mate with the highest F gains highly disproportionate paternity. The question, therefore, is whether such high additive genetic variance in fertilization efficiency and/or disproportionate fertilization success exist in nature.
Although substantial additive genetic variance has been estimated in sperm traits that are hypothesized to influence sperm competitiveness, additive genetic variance and heritability in fertilization success itself have proved hard to detect (Simmons and Moore 2009; Tregenza et al. 2009; Dowling et al. 2010; but see Konior et al. 2005). Detection is impeded by the relative nature of fertilization success (given a random sample of male competitors), and by the discrepancy between observed paternity success and underlying fertilization success due to differential embryo viability caused by sire effects (García-González 2008a,b; García-González and Evans 2011) or male-by-female interactions (Evans and Marshall 2005; Droge-Young et al. 2012).
In many species, fertilization success is highly skewed and does appear to reflect a biased raffle (Sakaluk and Eggert 1996; Simmons and Siva-Jothy 1998; Wedell and Cook 1998; Parker and Pizzari 2010; Simmons 2014). However, “fair-raffle” fertilization also appears to be widespread (Gage and Morrow 2003; Engqvist et al. 2007; Manier et al. 2010; Parker and Pizzari 2010; Simmons 2014). Furthermore, many such examples concern insects with limited sperm storage capacity, and “fair-raffle” fertilization may be even more likely when sperm storage is less constrained, such as in many vertebrates and external fertilizers (Parker and Pizzari 2010). Our models suggest that the pure SSS process is unlikely to drive the evolution of polyandry in such systems.
Logic suggests that the SSS process cannot work if male fertilization efficiency is maternally inherited (Pizzari and Birkhead 2002; Evans and Simmons 2008). Our model suggests that the pure SSS process might not generally be a strong evolutionary force underlying polyandry even given simple biparental autosomal inheritance, at least if polyandry incurs any direct cost and if post-copulatory competition for fertilization results in some degree of shared paternity. Our model therefore concurs with the broader theoretical and empirical view that indirect selection on mating strategies might generally be weak, and hence play a relatively minor role in driving evolutionary dynamics (Kirkpatrick 1996; Kirkpatrick and Barton 1997; Cameron et al. 2003; Arnqvist and Kirkpatrick 2005; Jones and Ratterman 2009).
Model Assumptions and Extensions
Our SSS model was designed to capture the pure SSS process as proposed by Keller and Reeve (1995), and consequently makes some strong assumptions. Our basic model assumes that phenotypes are directly determined by entirely additive genetic effects with no explicit environmental components of phenotypic variance, apart from stochasticity introduced by random sampling of males and translation of continuously distributed genetic variation in female tendency for polyandry into a discrete number of mates. This is unrealistic; life-history traits often show high environmental variance and low heritability (Houle 1992). Introducing additional environmental variance would presumably further weaken the genetic correlation between female and male traits, further diminishing coevolution. Indeed, coevolution scarcely occurred in our SSS model that included male precedence in fertilization according to random mating order and corresponding nongenetic variance. Although fertilization is sometimes independent of mating order (e.g., Zeh and Zeh 1994; Engqvist et al. 2007), some form of precedence occurs widely in species with internal fertilization (Parker 1970; Birkhead and Hunter 1990; Watson 1991; Lacey et al. 1997; Price et al. 1999; Kraaijeveld-Smit et al. 2002; Pischedda and Rice 2012), further challenging the hypothesis that the pure SSS process could, in general, drive evolution of polyandry.
In our SSS model, the degree of precedence could not evolve. However, given the variety of forms of precedence observed in nature, different strategies are likely to have evolved to ensure precedence and/or favorable mating order. An interesting model extension would therefore be to allow mating order to have a genetic basis and hence to evolve, potentially encompassing trade-offs between male efficiency in ensuring favorable mating order and other factors influencing fertilization efficiency.
Keller and Reeve (1995) did not explicitly discuss the relationship between relative male fertilization efficiency and realized fertilization success achieved under postcopulatory competition, meaning that the sensitivity of the SSS process to this relationship has not been highlighted. Keller and Reeve (1995) implicitly suggest a “winner-takes-all” scenario in the context of a simple single-locus biallelic situation, but this might not be generally appropriate. The degree to which the form of fertilization could coevolve with polyandry also merits future attention. For example, our model implies that by ensuring “fair-raffle” rather than “winner-takes-all” fertilization, females could potentially impede runaway evolution of polyandry.
The pure SSS hypothesis also assumes no preexisting genetic covariances between fertilization efficiency and any other components of male or female fitness. Although this could be viewed as a strength, it is also a limitation if other covariances do in fact exist. Indirect selection on polyandry could potentially be facilitated (or hindered) by complex genetic covariances between multiple different traits. For example, we considered F as a single trait, but fertilization efficiency often results from multiple interacting traits and postcopulatory processes, including sperm quantity, viability, and displacement ability (Gomendio and Roldan 1993; Keller and Reeve 1995; Snook 2005; Pizzari and Parker 2009; Simmons and Moore 2009). These traits might trade-off and experience divergent selection, potentially helping to maintain genetic variance in overall fertilization efficiency and allowing for complex multiple genetic covariances and coevolutionary dynamics between different male traits and females strategies (Parker 1990; Parker and Pizzari 2010; Engqvist 2012; Alonzo and Pizzari 2013).
Various extensions and variations on the pure SSS hypothesis have been proposed that invoke additional genetic covariances. Most pertinently, the “good-sperm” hypothesis (Yasui 1997; Petrie and Kempenaers 1998), in analogy with the “good-genes” hypothesis for evolution of female preference (Pomiankowski 1987a; Iwasa et al. 1991; Rowe and Houle 1996; Mead and Arnold 2004; Kokko et al. 2006), assumes a priori existence of positive genetic covariance between fertilization efficiency and “viability.” Models suggest that such pleiotropy can facilitate evolution of polyandry (Yasui 1997), but do not explain why the underlying positive covariance exists in the first place. Indeed, empirical evidence of positive genetic covariance between fertilization efficiency and viability, or between polyandry and offspring fitness, remains scarce and contradictory (Simmons and Kotiaho 2002; Hosken et al. 2003; Simmons 2005; García-González and Simmons 2011; Reid and Sardell 2012; Sardell et al. 2012; Slatyer et al. 2012).
Furthermore, fertilization efficiency could positively covary with male attractiveness, especially if both traits were condition dependent, creating positive genetic covariance between pre- and postcopulatory male traits (Birkhead and Pizzari 2002; Kvarnemo and Simmons 2013). Available evidence is inconclusive: positive genetic correlations between male attractiveness and fertilization efficiency have sometimes been observed (Evans et al. 2003; Locatello et al. 2006; Janhunen et al. 2009; Navara et al. 2012), and sometimes not (Birkhead et al. 1997; Pizzari et al. 2004; Evans 2010; Simmons et al. 2010; Engqvist 2011; Mautz et al. 2013). In addition males, rather than solely females, might influence the degree of polyandry, and male and female tendencies for multiple mating might potentially be correlated (Halliday and Arnold 1987; Forstmeier et al. 2011). It therefore remains possible that the SSS process could act alongside other mechanisms of indirect selection. Genetically explicit quantitative theory that considers evolution of polyandry through indirect selection given multidimensional trait space is now required.
Acknowledgments
We thank A. B. Duthie, H. Kokko, S. Losdat, J. M. J. Travis, and M. E. Wolak for useful discussions and comments on the manuscript, and the European Research Council for funding. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for their insightful suggestions.
Data Archiving
The doi for our data is 10.5061/dryad.2tr22.
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher's website:
Literature Cited
- Alonzo SH, Pizzari T. Male fecundity stimulation: conflict and cooperation within and between the sexes: model analyses and coevolutionary dynamics. Am. Nat. 2010;175:174–185. doi: 10.1086/649596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selection on female remating interval is influenced by male sperm competition strategies and ejaculate characteristics. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2013;368:20120044. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold S, Duvall D. Animal mating systems: a synthesis based on selection theory. Am. Nat. 1994;143:317–348. [Google Scholar]
- Arnqvist G, Kirkpatrick M. The evolution of infidelity in socially monogamous passerines: the strength of direct and indirect selection on extrapair copulation behavior in females. Am. Nat. 2005;165:S26–37. doi: 10.1086/429350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnqvist G, Nilsson T. The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Anim. Behav. 2000;60:145–164. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barton NH, Turelli M. Natural and sexual selection on many loci. Genetics. 1991;127:229–55. doi: 10.1093/genetics/127.1.229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bateman AJ. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity. 1948;2:349–368. doi: 10.1038/hdy.1948.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benton TG, Evans MR. Measuring mate choice using correlation: the effect of female sampling behaviour. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1998;44:91–98. [Google Scholar]
- Bernasconi G, Keller L. Female polyandry affects their sons’ reproductive success in the red flour beetle Tribolium castaneum. J. Evol. Biol. 2001;14:186–193. doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00247.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birkhead TR, Hunter FM. Mechanisms of sperm competition. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1990;5:48–52. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(90)90047-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birkhead TR, Pizzari T. Postcopulatory sexual selection. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2002;3:262–273. doi: 10.1038/nrg774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birkhead TR, Buchanan KL, Devoogd TJ, Pellatt EJ, Székely T, Catchpole CK. Song, sperm quality and testes asymmetry in the sedge warbler. Anim. Behav. 1997;53:965–971. [Google Scholar]
- Bulmer M. Maintenance of genetic variability by mutation-selection balance: a child's guide through the jungle. Genome. 1989a;31:761–767. [Google Scholar]
- Structural instability of models of sexual selection. Theor. Popul. Biol. 1989b;35:195–206. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(89)90017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton OJ, Phillips BL, Travis JMJ. Trade-offs and the evolution of life-histories during range expansion. Ecol. Lett. 2010;13:1210–1220. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01505.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne PG, Roberts JD. Does multiple paternity improve fitness of the frog Crinia georgiana. Evolution. 2000;54:968–973. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00096.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron E, Day T, Rowe L. Sexual conflict and indirect benefits. J. Evol. Biol. 2003;16:1055–1060. doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00584.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2003;18:41–47. [Google Scholar]
- Cornell SJ, Tregenza T. A new theory for the evolution of polyandry as a means of inbreeding avoidance. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2007;274:2873–2879. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtsinger J. Sperm competition and the evolution of multiple mating. Am. Nat. 1991;138:93–102. [Google Scholar]
- Day T. Sexual selection and the evolution of costly female preferences: spatial effects. Evolution. 2000;54:715–730. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00074.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dowling DK, Nystrand M, Simmons LW. Maternal effects, but no good or compatible genes for sperm competitiveness in Australian crickets. Evolution. 2010;64:1257–1266. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00912.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Droge-Young EM, Manier MK, Lüpold S, Belote JM, Pitnick S. Covariance among premating, post-copulatory and viability fitness components in Drosophila melanogaster and their influence on paternity measurement. J. Evol. Biol. 2012;25:1555–1563. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02540.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engqvist L. Nuptial food gifts influence female egg production in the scorpionfly Panorpa cognata. Ecol. Entomol. 2007;32:327–332. [Google Scholar]
- Male attractiveness is negatively genetically associated with investment in copulations. Behav. Ecol. 2011;22:345–349. [Google Scholar]
- Evolutionary modeling predicts a decrease in postcopulatory sperm viability as a response to increasing levels of sperm competition. Am. Nat. 2012;179:667–677. doi: 10.1086/665000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engqvist L, Dekomien G, Lippmann T, Epplen JT, Sauer KP. Sperm transfer and paternity in the scorpionfly Panorpa cognata: large variance in traits favoured by post-copulatory episodes of sexual selection. Evol. Ecol. 2007;21:801–816. [Google Scholar]
- Evans JP. Quantitative genetic evidence that males trade attractiveness for ejaculate quality in guppies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2010;277:3195–3201. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans JP, Gasparini C. The genetic basis of female multiple mating in a polyandrous livebearing fish. Ecol. Evol. 2013;3:61–66. doi: 10.1002/ece3.435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans JP, Marshall DJ. Male-by-female interactions influence fertilization success and mediate the benefits of polyandry in the sea urchin Heliocidaris erythrogramma. Evolution. 2005;59:106–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans JP, Simmons LW. The genetic basis of traits regulating sperm competition and polyandry: can selection favour the evolution of good- and sexy-sperm. Genetica. 2008;134:5–19. doi: 10.1007/s10709-007-9162-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans J, Zane L, Francescato S, Pilastro A. Directional postcopulatory sexual selection revealed by artificial insemination. Nature. 2003;421:25–28. doi: 10.1038/nature01367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fawcett TW, Kuijper B, Pen I, Weissing FJ. Should attractive males have more sons. Behav. Ecol. 2007;18:71–80. [Google Scholar]
- Fedorka KM, Mousseau TA. Material and genetic benefits of female multiple mating and polyandry. Anim. Behav. 2002;64:361–367. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher RA. The evolution of sexual preference. Eugen. Rev. 1915;7:184–192. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher DO, Double MC, Blomberg SP, Jennions MD, Cockburn A. Post-mating sexual selection increases lifetime fitness of polyandrous females in the wild. Nature. 2006;444:89–92. doi: 10.1038/nature05206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forstmeier W, Martin K, Bolund E, Schielzeth H, Kempenaers B. Female extrapair mating behavior can evolve via indirect selection on males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:10608–10613. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103195108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gage M, Morrow E. Experimental evidence for the evolution of numerous, tiny sperm via sperm competition. Curr. Biol. 2003;13:754–757. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00282-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-González F. Male genetic quality and the inequality between paternity success and fertilization success: consequences for studies of sperm competition and the evolution of polyandry. Evolution. 2008a;62:1653–1665. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00362.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The relative nature of fertilization success: implications for the study of post-copulatory sexual selection. BMC Evol. Biol. 2008b;8:140. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-8-140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-González F, Evans JP. Fertilization success and the estimation of genetic variance in sperm competitiveness. Evolution. 2011;65:746–756. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García-González F, Simmons LW. Good genes and sexual selection in dung beetles (Onthophagus taurus): genetic variance in egg-to-adult and adult viability. PLoS One. 2011;6:e16233. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomendio M, Roldan ER. Mechanisms of sperm competition: linking physiology and behavioural ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1993;8:95–100. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(93)90059-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haldane J. The measurement of natural selection. In: Montalenti G, Chiarugi A, editors. Proceedings of the 9th International Congress of Genetics. 1954. pp. 480–487. Caryologia, Firenze. [Google Scholar]
- Halliday T, Arnold S. Multiple mating by females: a perspective from quantitative genetics. Anim. Behav. 1987;35:939–941. [Google Scholar]
- Harshman L, Clark A. Inference of sperm competition from broods of field-caught Drosophila. Evolution. 1998;52:1334–1341. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1998.tb02015.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey PH, May RM. Copulation dynamics. Out for the sperm count. Nature. 1989;337:508–509. doi: 10.1038/337508a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higashi M, Takimoto G, Yamamura N. Sympatric speciation by sexual selection. Nature. 1999;402:523–526. doi: 10.1038/990087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holman L, Kokko H. The consequences of polyandry for population viability, extinction risk and conservation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2013;368:20120053. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hosken DJ, Garner TWJ, Tregenza T, Wedell N, Ward PI. Superior sperm competitors sire higher-quality young. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2003;270:1933–1938. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houle D. Comparing evolvability and variability of quantitative traits. Genetics. 1992;130:195–204. doi: 10.1093/genetics/130.1.195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwasa Y, Pomiankowski A, Nee S. The evolution of costly mate preferences II. The ‘handicap’ principle. Evolution. 1991;45:1431–1442. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb02646.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janhunen M, Rudolfsen G, Kekäläinen J, Figenschou L, Peuhkuri N, Kortet R. Spawning coloration and sperm quality in a large lake population of Arctic charr (Salmonidae: Salvelinus alpinus L.) Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2009;98:794–802. [Google Scholar]
- Jennions MD, Petrie M. Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biol. Rev. 2000;75:21–64. doi: 10.1017/s0006323199005423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones AG, Ratterman NL. Mate choice and sexual selection: what have we learned since Darwin. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:10001–10008. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901129106. and (Suppl): [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller L, Reeve H. Why do females mate with multiple males? The sexually selected sperm hypothesis. Adv. Study Behav. 1995;24:291–315. [Google Scholar]
- Kimura M. A stochastic model concerning the maintenance of genetic variability in quantitative characters. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1965;54:731–736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.54.3.731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick M. Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution. 1982;36:1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1982.tb05003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Good genes and direct selection in the evolution of mating preferences. Evolution. 1996;50:2125–2140. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1996.tb03603.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkpatrick M, Barton NH. The strength of indirect selection on female mating preferences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1997;94:1282–1286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.4.1282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klemme I, Bäumer J, Eccard JA, Ylönen H. Polyandrous females produce sons that are successful at post-copulatory competition. J. Evol. Biol. 2014;27:457–465. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kokko H, Mappes J. Multiple mating by females is a natural outcome of a null model of mate encounters. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 2013;146:26–37. [Google Scholar]
- Kokko H, Jennions M, Brooks R. Unifying and testing models of sexual selection. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2006;37:43–66. [Google Scholar]
- Konior M, Keller L, Radwan J. Effect of inbreeding and heritability of sperm competition success in the bulb mite Rhizoglyphus robini. Heredity. 2005;94:577–581. doi: 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraaijeveld-Smit F, Ward S, Temple-Smith P, Paetkau D. Factors influencing paternity success in Antechinus agilis: Last-male sperm precedence, timing of mating and genetic compatibility. J. Evol. Biol. 2002;15:100–107. [Google Scholar]
- Kuijper B, Pen I, Weissing FJ. A guide to sexual selection theory. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2012;43:287–311. [Google Scholar]
- Kvarnemo C, Simmons L. Polyandry as a mediator of sexual selection before and after mating. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2013;368:20120042. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lacey E, Wieczorek J, Tucker P. Male mating behaviour and patterns of sperm precedence in Arctic ground squirrels. Anim. Behav. 1997;53:767–779. [Google Scholar]
- Lande R. The maintenance of genetic variability by mutation in a polygenic character with linked loci. Genet. Res. 1976;26:221–235. doi: 10.1017/s0016672300016037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1981;78:3721–3725. doi: 10.1073/pnas.78.6.3721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locatello L, Rasotto MB, Evans JP, Pilastro A. Colourful male guppies produce faster and more viable sperm. J. Evol. Biol. 2006;19:1595–1602. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorch PD, Proulx S, Rowe L, Day T. Condition-dependent sexual selection can accelerate adaptation. Evol. Ecol. Res. 2003;5:867–881. [Google Scholar]
- Lynch M, Walsh B. Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA. 1998 [Google Scholar]
- Manier MK, Belote JM, Berben KS, Novikov D, Stuart WT, Pitnick S. Resolving mechanisms of competitive fertilization success in Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2010;328:354–357. doi: 10.1126/science.1187096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mautz BS, Møller AP, Jennions MD. Do male secondary sexual characters signal ejaculate quality? A meta-analysis. Biol. Rev. 2013;88:669–682. doi: 10.1111/brv.12022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNamara KB, van Lieshout E, Simmons LW. A test of the sexy-sperm and good-sperm hypotheses for the evolution of polyandry. Behav. Ecol. 2014;00:1–7. [Google Scholar]
- Mead LS, Arnold SJ. Quantitative genetic models of sexual selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2004;19:264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navara KJ, Anderson EM, Edwards ML. Comb size and color relate to sperm quality: a test of the phenotype-linked fertility hypothesis. Behav. Ecol. 2012;23:1036–1041. [Google Scholar]
- Orsetti DM, Rutowski RL. No material benefits, and a fertilization cost, for multiple mating by female leaf beetles. Anim. Behav. 2003;66:477–484. [Google Scholar]
- Parker GA. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol. Rev. 1970;45:525–567. [Google Scholar]
- Sperm competition games: raffles and roles. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1990;242:120–126. [Google Scholar]
- Parker G, Birkhead T. Polyandry: the history of a revolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2013;368:20120335. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker GA, Pizzari T. Sperm competition and ejaculate economics. Biol. Rev. 2010;85:897–934. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00140.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrie M, Kempenaers B. Extra-pair paternity in birds: explaining variation between species and populations. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1998;13:52–58. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(97)01232-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pischedda A, Rice WR. Partitioning sexual selection into its mating success and fertilization success components. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:2049–2053. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1110841109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzari T, Birkhead TR. The sexually-selected sperm hypothesis: sex-biased inheritance and sexual antagonism. Biol. Rev. 2002;77:183–209. doi: 10.1017/s1464793101005863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzari T, Gardner A. The sociobiology of sex: inclusive fitness consequences of inter-sexual interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2012;367:2314–2323. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzari T. Sperm competition and sperm phenotype. In: Pitnick S, Parker G, editors; Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, editors. Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective. Elsevier, Burlington, MA; 2009. pp. 207–245. [Google Scholar]
- Pizzari T, Wedell N. The polyandry revolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2013;368:20120041. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzari T, Jensen P, Cornwallis CK. A novel test of the phenotype-linked fertility hypothesis reveals independent components of fertility. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2004;271:51–58. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomiankowski A. Sexual selection: the handicap principle does work—sometimes. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1987a;231:123–145. [Google Scholar]
- The costs of choice in sexual selection. J. Theor. Biol. 1987b;128:195–218. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5193(87)80169-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomiankowski A, Iwasa Y. Runaway ornament diversity caused by Fisherian sexual selection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1998;95:5106–5111. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomiankowski A, Iwasa Y, Nee S. The evolution of costly mate preferences I. Fisher and biased mutation. Evolution. 1991;45:1422–1430. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb02645.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price CS, Dyer KA, Coyne JA. Sperm competition between Drosophila males involves both displacement and incapacitation. Nature. 1999;400:449–452. doi: 10.1038/22755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reeve JP. Predicting long-term response to selection. Genet. Res. 2000;75:83–94. doi: 10.1017/s0016672399004140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid JM, Sardell RJ. Indirect selection on female extra-pair reproduction? Comparing the additive genetic value of maternal half-sib extra-pair and within-pair offspring. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2012;279:1700–1708. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.2230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe L. The costs of mating and mate choice in water striders. Anim. Behav. 1994;48:1049–1056. [Google Scholar]
- Rowe L, Houle D. The lek paradox and the capture of genetic variance by condition dependent traits. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1996;263:1415–1421. [Google Scholar]
- Sakaluk S, Eggert A. Female control of sperm transfer and intraspecific variation in sperm precedence: antecedents to the evolution of a courtship food gift. Evolution. 1996;50:694–703. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1996.tb03879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sardell RJ, Arcese P, Keller LF, Reid JM. Are there indirect fitness benefits of female extra-pair reproduction? Lifetime reproductive success of within-pair and extra-pair offspring. Am. Nat. 2012;179:779–793. doi: 10.1086/665665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seger J. Unifying genetic models for the evolution of female choice. Evolution. 1985;39:1185–1193. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1985.tb05685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons LW. The evolution of polyandry: patterns of genotypic variation in female mating frequency, male fertilization success and a test of the sexy-sperm hypothesis. J. Evol. Biol. 2003;16:624–634. doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00572.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The evolution of polyandry: sperm competition, sperm selection, and offspring viability. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2005;36:125–146. [Google Scholar]
- Shuker D, Simmons L, editors. The evolution of insect mating systems. Oxford Univ. Press; 2014. Sperm competition; pp. 181–203. Oxford, U.K. [Google Scholar]
- Simmons LW, Kotiaho JS. Evolution of ejaculates: patterns of phenotypic and genotypic variation and condition dependence in sperm competition traits. Evolution. 2002;56:1622–1631. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb01474.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quantitative genetic correlation between trait and preference supports a sexually selected sperm process. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:16604–16608. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704871104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmons L. Evolutionary quantitative genetics of sperm. In: Pitnick S, Moore A, editors; Birkhead TR, Hosken DJ, editors. Sperm biology: an evolutionary perspective. Elsevier, Burlington, MA; 2009. pp. 405–434. [Google Scholar]
- Simmons LW. Sperm competition in insects: mechanisms and the potential for selection. In: Birkhead T, Moller AP, Siva-Jothy MT, editors. Sperm competition and sexual selection. Lond: Academic Press; 1998. pp. 341–434. [Google Scholar]
- Simmons LW, Tinghitella RM, Zuk M. Quantitative genetic variation in courtship song and its covariation with immune function and sperm quality in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behav. Ecol. 2010;21:1330–1336. [Google Scholar]
- Slatyer RA, Mautz BS, Backwell PRY, Jennions MD. Estimating genetic benefits of polyandry from experimental studies: a meta-analysis. Biol. Rev. 2012;87:1–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00182.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snook RR. Sperm in competition: not playing by the numbers. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2005;20:46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor ML, Price TAR, Wedell N. Polyandry in nature: a global analysis. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2014;29:376–383. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2014.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thrall PH, Antonovics J, Dobson AP. Sexually transmitted diseases in polygynous mating systems: prevalence and impact on reproductive success. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 2000;267:1555–1563. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tregenza T, Attia F, Bushaiba SS. Repeatability and heritability of sperm competition outcomes in males and females of Tribolium castaneum. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2009;63:817–823. [Google Scholar]
- Van Doorn GS, Weissing FJ. The evolution of female preferences for multiple indicators of quality. Am. Nat. 2004;164:173–186. doi: 10.1086/422203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson PJ. Multiple paternity and first mate sperm precedence in the sierra dome spider, Linyphia litigiosa Keyserling (Linyphiidae) Anim. Behav. 1991;41:135–148. [Google Scholar]
- Weatherhead P, Robertson R. Offspring quality and the polygyny threshold: “the sexy son hypothesis. Am. Nat. 1979;113:201–208. [Google Scholar]
- Wedell N, Cook PA. Determinants of paternity in a butterfly. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1998;265:625–630. [Google Scholar]
- Wigby S, Chapman T. Sex peptide causes mating costs in female Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:316–321. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yasui Y. A “good-sperm” model can explain the evolution of costly multiple mating by females. Am. Nat. 1997;149:573–584. [Google Scholar]
- Female multiple mating as a genetic bet-hedging strategy. Ecol. Res. 2001;16:605–616. [Google Scholar]
- Zeh JA, Zeh DW. Last-male sperm precedence breaks down when females mate with three males. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1994;257:287–292. [Google Scholar]
- The evolution of polyandry I: intragenomic conflict and genetic incompatibility. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 1996;263:1711–1717. [Google Scholar]
- Maternal inheritance, epigenetics and the evolution of polyandry. Genetica. 2008;134:45–54. doi: 10.1007/s10709-007-9192-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.