INTRODUCTION
Researchers in the field of animal behavior have recently placed increasing focus and attention on differences in behavior expressed among individuals within a species. While traditional studies of animal behavior generally sought to characterize typical or modal patterns of behavior within a given species, investigators are now providing detailed empirical descriptions and extensive theoretical analysis of individual variation (Reale et al. 2007; Smith and Blumstein 2008), its potential proximate causes (Rogers et al. 2013; Hopkins et al. 2014; Reale et al. 2010; Barr et al. 2003) and its long-term ecological and evolutionary consequences (Wolf and Weissing 2012; Tung et al. 2012). The concept of personality (sometimes referred to as temperament) provides an important organizing framework for the study of behavioral differences among individuals within a given animal species, as well as differences among species. Personality is often defined as a pattern or consistency of behavior expressed by a specific individual across various situations and contexts (Gosling 2001; Briffa and Weiss 2010). The central finding of this line of research is that these patterns often show stable differences among individuals, and therefore any given animal can be described as displaying one or more consistent personality traits, such as aggressiveness, anxiousness, agreeableness and others (Briffa and Weiss 2010; Morton et al. 2013; Fox et al. 2008; Kinnally et al. 2008; Capitanio 2004).
Given their cognitive and social complexity, developmental plasticity, and phylogenetic proximity to humans, nonhuman primates are interesting and productive subjects in the study of personality (Rogers et al. 2013; Weiss et al. 2011; Capitanio et al. 1999). Much of the work on personality in nonhuman primates has used observer ratings in which experienced observers having extensive familiarity with a particular set of animals assess their personalities using standardized questionnaires (Morton et al. 2013; Capitanio et al. 1999; Freeman et al. 2013; Hopper et al. 2014; Weiss et al. 2009). The alternative approach, also used in studies of primates and other vertebrate species, is based on structured observation and quantification of expressed behaviors recorded during specific observation periods, either in natural circumstances (Seyfarth et al. 2012; Silk et al. 2009) or during standardized behavioral testing of captive animals (Kalin and Shelton 2003; Fairbanks et al. 2004; Oler et al. 2010). Differences in temperament or personality among animals can have significant correlations with a number of important life-history outcomes, such as predation risk, access to preferred foods, reproductive success or dispersal (Wolf and Weissing 2012; Silk et al. 2009). This means that personality can have meaningful influences on fitness and thus broader evolutionary processes (Smith and Blumstein 2008; Wolf and Weissing 2012).
Personality is of course also a central and fundamental concept in human psychology, and differences in human personality have been studied extensively for many years. Among humans, personality is correlated with the likelihood of developing specific psychiatric disorders such as anxiety disorders (Brandes and Bienvenu 2006; Clark et al. 1994), major depression (Kendler et al. 1993; Hirschfeld et al. 1989) or alcoholism (Cloninger et al. 1988; Wills et al. 1994), as well as with risk for other medical problems (Denollet et al. 1996; Cole et al. 2003). Due to their close genetic, physiological and neurobiological similarities with humans, nonhuman primates are valuable biomedical models for the study of the relationships among personality, underlying neurobiological or genetic causes, and downstream consequences for health (Rogers et al. 2013; Barr et al. 2003; Capitanio et al. 1999; Fawcett et al. 2014; Birn et al. 2014; Roseboom et al. 2013). Thus, two lines of research (ethologically-oriented studies of behavioral variation within animal species and biomedically-focused investigation of behavioral variation and its health-related correlates in model organisms) are providing increasing information about personality within and among nonhuman primate species.
Baboons (genus Papio) are one of the most intensively studied nonhuman primate groups. The genus Papio is now generally considered to consist of six closely related parapatric species that differ in pelage and other morphological traits, but which often form natural hybrid zones where they come into contact in the wild (Keller et al. 2010; Jolly et al. 2011; Jolly 2001). The behavior of these six species differs in several ways, including aspects of diet and ranging that likely reflect adaptation to local environments. However, baboon species also differ in several aspects of social behavior, resulting in important divergences in social organization and social structure (Henzi and Barrett 2003; Jolly 1993) which generate fascinating complexity within hybrid zones (Jolly 2001; Bergman et al. 2008). Furthermore, researchers find significant differences in social relationships, social interactions and other aspects of behavior among individuals within taxonomically homogeneous (non-hybrid) baboon populations (Seyfarth et al. 2012; Silk et al. 2010; Gesquiere et al. 2011; Moscovice et al. 2009; Smuts 1985).
One aspect of primate behavior that can be readily quantified and may impact an individual’s ability to cope with complex ecological and social challenges is response to novelty, often described as varying from “shy” to “bold.” This “shy-bold” continuum has been investigated in many vertebrate species, but may be particularly important for a geographically widespread and omnivorous group such as the baboons, which live successfully in a variety of ecological habitats. “Boldness” and related measures of one type or another has been studied in several other nonhuman primates (Morton et al. 2013; Kinnally et al. 2008; Hopper et al. 2014; Weiss et al. 2009; Fairbanks et al. 2004; Fawcett et al. 2014; Weiss et al. 2006; Weiss et al. 2013; Fairbanks et al. 1999). Previous studies have described individual differences among wild chacma baboons in reaction to novel food items (Carter et al. 2012). Thus baboons, like other nonhuman primates, exhibit personality differences related to boldness and response to novelty.
One fundamental question in animal personality research is the nature of the proximate mechanisms that generate the observed individual variation. Genetic differences among individuals likely account for some proportion of the observed variation within many species. In chimpanzees (Hopkins et al. 2014), rhesus macaques (Oler et al. 2010; Rogers et al. 2008; Fawcett et al. 2014) and vervet monkeys (Fairbanks et al. 2004), additive genetic variation influences response to novelty, anxiety or other cognitive/behavioral traits. Specific genes have been associated with aspects of nonhuman primate personality and particular behaviors in rhesus macaques (Rogers et al. 2013; Barr et al. 2004; Chen et al. 2010; Trefilov et al. 2000) and chimpanzees (Hong et al. 2011; Hopkins et al. 2012). Thus, genetic variation accounts for a proportion of the variation in personality in several primate species. Despite decades of extensive behavioral study, little is known about the genetic basis of behavioral variation among Papio baboons. The goals of this study are: a) to explore individual variation in personality, as indexed by response to novel objects and a mirror, among a large pedigreed population of baboons, b) to investigate quantitative genetic differences among individuals as a potential proximate cause of that variation, and c) to attempt to identify specific genes that influence individual behavioral differences.
METHODS
Study Subjects
The study subjects were 578 olive baboons (P. anubis), yellow baboons (P. cynocephalus) and their offspring maintained at the Southwest National Primate Research Center, San Antonio, Texas. Some of the founding members of the colony were likely trapped in or near a hybrid zone between yellow and olive baboons, so this population consists of olive baboons, yellow baboons and individuals with varying degrees of admixture. Taxonomy is somewhat controversial in Papio baboons, but most experts now divide Papio baboons into six species (Jolly et al. 2011; Zinner et al. 2013). Regardless of classification scheme, natural populations of olive and yellow baboons hybridize in the wild (Alberts and Altmann 2001; Charpentier et al. 2012), as well as in the captive population at the Southwest National Primate Research Center, producing healthy fertile offspring. All subjects were adult at the time of testing (mean age of 15.8 years, range 8.0 to 29.8 yrs). At the time of testing, all subjects were living in social groups of 15–40 individuals, in large outdoor cages (27–47 m2) with heated indoor shelters. All study procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Texas Biomedical Research Institute, an AAALAC, Int. accredited institution.
The 578 subjects for which phenotypes were measured can be connected into a single four generation pedigree. This pedigree must also include 87 additional baboons that were not phenotyped but are needed to link the phenotyped animals into a suitable unbroken pedigree configuration. The frequency of pairwise kinship coefficients between the 578 subjects that did undergo phenotyping is presented in Supplemental Table 1. Importantly, a small number of relationship pairs reveal minor levels of inbreeding within the population. These relationship pairs are the result of directed breeding plans designed to facilitate specific unrelated genetic studies. As can be seen in Supplemental Table 1, the overall the population shows little inbreeding.
Assessment of Response to Novelty
To assess response to novelty, sets of eight animals were removed from their home social groups, placed in individual cages (0.75 m2) in an indoor facility, and a small metal tray was attached to the outside of each cage to habituate the subject to its presence. The next morning two inanimate, novel objects (first a plastic truck and then a plastic bear, Figure S1) were placed separately on the tray for 5 minutes each. After these trials a 0.09 m2 mirror was placed on the tray facing the animal for two minutes. Only a single observer (investigator) was present during testing, and while there were generally other baboons housed in the same room, none of the other subjects could see the baboon being tested or the toys being used.
Each baboon’s behavioral responses to each novel object were recorded using an ethogram. Measures of frequency and duration of 73 individual behaviors related to locomotion, aggression, submission, object interaction, and other relevant variables were scored. Additionally, location information within the cage and the timing of each event were recorded. The ethogram used was based on responses to novel objects observed during a preliminary study employing a separate set of baboons. A laptop computer and Observer™ software were used to record data. Table 1 presents the specific behaviors scored, and the categories used to group them into classes. Several observers were involved in the study, with all achieving at least 85% inter-observer reliability with L.B. prior to performing testing. Analyses presented here are based on data from each of the 578 subjects. Forty-three subjects underwent testing for two of the novel objects (truck and bear) on two occasions, with an average of 2.76 (SD = 0.58) years between tests.
Table 1.
Category | Data Type | Behavior(s) |
---|---|---|
Abnormal | Frequency Duration | clasp self, feces paint, head toss, pace, pull/eat hair, regurgitate, rock, self aggression, other stereotyped movement, suck self |
Activity | Frequency | changes in position (front sit, front stand, back sit, back stand, other location, shun) |
Aggression | Frequency Duration | aversive to object, brow raise, cage slap, lunge, open mouth threat, stare, teeth grind, yawn |
Arousal | Frequency | muzzle wipe, piloerection, penis erection, urinate/defectate, scratch self, mantle shake |
Avert | Frequency Duration | look intently away from stimulus object (usually upward) with fixed gaze |
Front | Duration | time spent in front of cage (front sit and front stand) |
Latency to touch | Duration | latency to first touch object |
Locomotion | Duration | locomote, including walk, jump, climb |
Object contact | Frequency Duration | bite object, manipulate object, smell object, watch object |
Self | Frequency Duration | groom self, manipulate self, masturbate, scratch self |
Slap cage | Frequency | cage slap |
Submissive | Frequency Duration | ambivalent (displaying submissive and other behavior at same time), avert, clasp self, present, ear flatten, fear grimace, jump back from object, lipsmack, present, scream |
Vigilance | Frequency Duration | intent monitoring area away from cage, stimulus and observer |
Watch Observer | Frequency Duration | watch observer |
Yawn | Frequency Duration | open mouth fully to expose teeth |
Behavioral Data Summarization
All files were summarized in the Observer™ program (Noldus, Leesburg, VA) as the frequency and duration of each behavior for each subject on each trial (object). Individual behaviors were grouped into exclusive categories for analysis (see Table 1), and more common individual behaviors (e.g., slap cage, vigilance, watch observer, yawn) were also analyzed separately. All variables are listed as frequency or duration per 5-minute or 2-minute observation. The Systat® statistical program was used for initial evaluation of results. Upon initial inspection of the univariate plots of the data for each trial and each behavior category, significant skewness and nonhomogeneity of variances were apparent in most distributions. A square root transformation was thus applied to all data prior to analysis to reduce skewness.
Measurement of monoamine metabolites
The second morning after novel object testing the animals were sedated with RAAK (rompum, acepromazine, atropine, and ketamine) and a spinal tap into the cisterna magna was performed to obtain a sample of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) from each study baboon. The CSF sample was collected within 30 minutes of animal sedation and immediately placed on wet ice. The samples were centrifuged to pellet contaminates and placed at -80°C within 90 minutes of collection. Homovanillic acid (HVA), 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) and 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG) levels were measured in each CSF sample by High Performance Liquid Chromatography (HPLC). A measured aliquot of each sample was mixed with an equal volume of cold mobile phase. The mixture was then filtered at centrifugation and part of the filtrate transferred to a 300-FL microinjection insert. This material was then analyzed using HPLC with electrical detection, allowing simultaneous measurement of HVA, 5-HIAA, and MHPG.
Heritability Analysis
Quantitative genetic theory establishes that within a pedigree, total phenotypic variance of a trait (σ2p) can be decomposed into an additive genetic component (σ2g), an environmental component (σ2e), and any number of covariates (σ2c) so that σ2p = σ2g + σ2e + σ2c (Falconer 1981). Using variance components methods implemented in SOLAR (Almasy and Blangero 1998; Blangero et al. 2001), we investigated the statistical significance of additive genetic variation as well as specific co-variates (age, sex and their interaction) as contributing factors to phenotypic variance in behavioral traits.
Genetic Correlations and Factor Analysis
To identify multivariate factors or dimensions of personality, the heritability of each individual behavior was quantified as described above. The estimated heritability of a behavior is the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by additive genetic variation, while the genetic correlation between two behaviors is the proportion of that genetic variance that is shared between the two. Any behavior phenotype without a significant heritability in our initial analysis was discarded from the subsequent factor analysis. Pair-wise phenotypic correlations were calculated for all remaining behaviors with significant estimates of heritability. All highly correlated behaviors and behavior scores that did not have continuous distribution were also discarded. Pair-wise genetic correlations were estimated among the remaining behaviors using SOLAR to measure genetic variance shared by any two behaviors. This analysis of genetic correlations identified 31 behaviors that served as our genetic correlation matrix. Factor analysis using Statistica version 6.1 was performed using both varimax and promax oblique rotation in an attempt to simplify data structure.
Follow-up observations in home cages
After the assessment of response to novelty among the study baboons, we designed a follow-up analysis to test the validity and relevance of our novel object testing for broader aspects of baboon behavior. One of us (J.R.) selected ten individuals that scored high on levels of aggression in response to the truck stimulus, and ten baboons that scored low on this trait. The list of 20 animal IDs was randomized and provided to S.R., who was therefore blinded to the scores of these animals on the previous novel object testing. S.R. conducted 10 hours of observations on each of these 20 animals in their home social groups, 20 observations of 30 minutes each. During these sessions, S.R. recorded both social and non-social behavior, using a standardized ethogram and the Observer program.
QTL scans
To perform whole genome scans for Quantitative Trait Loci (QTL), the following model was used:
where Πi is a matrix of Identity by Descent (IBD) allele sharing among family members at marker i, is the additive genetic variance at marker I, Φ is a matrix of kinship values, is the residual additive genetic variance, is the individual-specific environmental variance, and I is an identity matrix. A maximum likelihood analysis was used in which was estimated and the likelihood of that model compared to a model in which is constrained to zero, thus testing the null hypothesis that is not significantly greater than zero. The log10 difference between the two models is a LOD score. This analysis was performed using the software suite SOLAR (Almasy and Blangero 1998). Traditionally in human genetics a LOD score of 3.0 is considered strong evidence of linkage and accounts for issues of multiple testing across the genome. However within this baboon pedigree structure, and due to the density and location of markers in this microsatellite linkage map, a LOD score of 2.73 is genome-wide significant and adequately accounts for multiple testing, while a LOD of 1.5 is suggestive evidence of a QTL in this baboon population (Feingold et al. 1993).
Microsatellite design
Following initial QTL scans using the baboon linkage map (Rogers et al. 2000), we conducted additional analyses to refine positive QTL mapping results. New baboon microsatellites were identified through two processes. Baboon and human chromosomes were aligned using the original baboon linkage map, and both the deCODE (Kong et al. 2002) and Marshfield (Broman et al. 1998) human linkage maps were searched for human microsatellite markers predicted to fall in baboon chromosome regions of interest. Each identified microsatellite was tested for amplification and polymorphism in a set of eight male baboons. Microsatellites with four or more alleles and that were heterozygous in at least half of these eight individuals were then genotyped in the full animal set. The second method employed was to search the rhesus macaque whole genome DNA sequence (Gibbs et al. 2007) to identify microsatellites in that species. The rhesus–baboon relationship in much closer than the human-baboon, so any microsatellites identified in rhesus have a higher probability of being present and polymorphic in baboons than microsatellites identified in humans (Raveendran et al. 2006). Primer pairs were designed using Primer3 software (http://frodo.wi.mit.edu/) to flank these repeats and then tested and genotyped in all study baboons.
Microsatellite genotyping
Genotyping was performed using panels of four to eight microsatellites. Amplification reactions were performed on ABI 9700 thermal cyclers, and PCR product sizes analyzed using an ABI 3730 Genetic Analyzer and Genemapper 4.0 software. The raw genotypes were checked for Mendelian discrepancies with the PEDSYS software package (Dyke 1996). Errors were individually examined, and changes made when appropriate. In cases where discrepancies still existed, allele frequencies and patterns of allele distribution within the pedigree were evaluated using the Preswalk routine within PEDSYS, and the least likely genotypes blanked. The resulting genotype data were then used to generate first pass linkage maps. Recombination distances were determined using MultiMap (Matise et al. 1994) following the method described previously (Rogers et al. 2000; Cox et al. 2006). These data are then checked for double recombinants using Preswalk, and finally mapped again using MultiMap.
Sequencing analysis of candidate gene SNAP25
To discover and investigate variation in the baboon SNAP25 locus, sequencing primers were designed using the whole genome DNA sequence of the rhesus macaque (Gibbs et al. 2007) and Primer3 (http://frodo.wi.mit.edu). A series of study baboons were sequenced across the SNAP25 gene to identify SNPs. PCR was performed using standard amplification reactions on ABI 9700 thermal cyclers, and products sequenced on an ABI 3730. Once identified, each SNP was genotyped in the baboon population using one of three methods: ABI Snplex, Taqman, or Illumina golden gate assays following each respective protocol.
SNP association analysis
To estimate the influence of genetic differences in SNAP25 on phenotypic variance among animals, we performed pedigree-based genetic association analysis. The initial heritability analyses were performed using age, sex and their interaction as covariates with the following model σ2p = σ2age + σ2sex + σ2age × sex + σ2g + σ2e. To measure the effect of any individual SNP on phenotype, we modify this model to σ2p = σ2age + σ2sex + σ2age × sex + σ2g + σ2e + σ2SNP1, where σ2SNP1 is the variance in the phenotype due to the genotype of SNP1. If the variance attributed to SNP1 is statistically significantly different from zero, based on likelihood comparisons as described above, we then conclude that either SNP1, or an unknown polymorphism in linkage disequilibrium with SNP1, is responsible for a portion of the total phenotypic variance of the trait (Rogers et al. 2013; Rogers et al. 1999).
RESULTS
Initial Analysis of Phenotypic Variation
This population of baboons exhibited substantial individual variation in a number of behavioral responses to novelty. Table 1 presents the specific behaviors scored, and the categories used to group them into classes. In response to the toys, study subjects varied in their duration of locomotion, position within the cage relative to the novel object (i.e. front or back), and frequency of specific behaviors such as cage slapping, object contact and aggression. In response to the mirror, the study baboons differed again in levels of aggressive behavior, various locomotor behaviors, location within the cage and arousal reactions. Among 43 randomly chosen subjects tested on two separate occasions using both the truck and bear, we found a high degree of individual consistency in response (Supplemental Table 2). A wide range of individual behaviors, including measures of activity, arousal, object contact and others show significant correlations between exposures over 2 years apart.
Our measures of behavioral response to novelty differ by sex and age of the study subject in several but not all categories. These results are presented in Tables 2 and 3. Females tended to score higher for activity frequency, locomotion duration, submissive frequency and both watch observer frequency and watch observer duration. Females also had a longer latency to touch the novel object than did males. Males spent significantly more time in the front of the cage and had longer durations of self-directed behavior.
Table 2.
A) Truck | |||
---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Heritability | Covariates | p value |
Back Stand | 0.27 | Sex | 0.000025 |
Locomotion | 0.25 | age/sex | 0.000015 |
Mantle Shake | 0.21 | Sex | 0.0016 |
Latency to Touch | 0.29 | Sex | 9.4E-06 |
Other Location | 0.28 | age/sex | 0.000029 |
Self Scratch | 0.13 | Sex | 0.0082 |
Cage Slap | 0.39 | None | 7.3 × 10−9 |
Watch Object | 0.16 | age/sex | 0.0019 |
Watch Observer | 0.18 | age/sex | 0.00078 |
B) Bear | |||
---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Heritability | Covariates | p value |
Aggression | 0.19 | None | 0.00011 |
Avert | 0.33 | age/sex | 0.000001 |
Back Stand | 0.21 | Sex | 0.000059 |
Locomotion | 0.51 | age/sex | 5.0 × 10−12 |
Mantle Shake | 0.17 | age/sex | 0.0024 |
Object Interaction | 0.17 | Sex | 0.000022 |
Other Location | 0.3 | age/sex | 6E-07 |
Self Scratch | 0.27 | age/sex | 1E-07 |
Cage Slap | 0.33 | age/sex | 8.4 × 10−11 |
C) Mirror | |||
---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Heritability | Covariates | p value |
Aggression | 0.24 | age | 0.0023 |
Avert | 0.11 | age/sex | 3.5 × 10−15 |
Back Stand | 0.12 | sex | 0.003 |
Front of cage | 0.15 | age/sex | 0.007 |
Half Yawn | 0.12 | Sex | 0.011 |
Lip Smack | 0.27 | None | 0.0012 |
Locomotion | 0.22 | age/sex | 0.00031 |
Other Location | 0.21 | Sex | 0.002 |
Self Scratch | 0.16 | age/sex | 0.018 |
Cage Slap | 0.27 | None | 0.00027 |
Watch Observer | 0.22 | Sex | 0.00027 |
Yawn | 0.16 | None | 0.0031 |
Table 3.
A) Truck | |||
---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Heritability | Covariates | p value |
Aggression | 0.24 | Sex | 0.0000063 |
Back Stand | 0.32 | age/sex | 0.0000028 |
Abnormal Behav | 0.38 | age/sex | 4.88 × 10−10 |
Front Stand | 0.18 | Sex | 0.002 |
Locomotion | 0.28 | age/sex | 0.000051 |
Mantle Shake | 0.14 | Sex | 0.011 |
Object interaction | 0.19 | age/sex | 0.0018 |
Other Location | 0.30 | age/sex | 0.000006 |
Passive | 0.29 | age/sex | 0.0000029 |
Self Scratch | 0.13 | age/sex | 0.0097 |
Cage Slap | 0.36 | Sex | 0.000001 |
Watch Object | 0.30 | age/sex | 0.0000005 |
Watch Observer | 0.38 | age/sex | 1.00 × 10−9 |
Yawn | 0.12 | Sex | 0.011 |
B) Bear | |||
---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Heritability | Covariates | p value |
Aggression | 0.28 | age/sex | 0.0000001 |
Avert | 0.32 | age/sex | 0.000009 |
Back Sit | 0.15 | None | 0.0038 |
Back Stand | 0.30 | Sex | 0.000006 |
Abnormal Behav | 0.29 | None | 0.0000011 |
Locomation | 0.50 | age/sex | 3.54 × 10−12 |
Other Location | 0.28 | age/sex | 0.0000013 |
Passive | 0.22 | age/sex | 0.000033 |
Self Scratch | 0.32 | Age | 2.19 × 10−8 |
Cage Slap | 0.37 | age/sex | 4.19 × 10−10 |
Submissive | 0.19 | None | 0.00077 |
Watch Observer | 0.31 | age/sex | 1.89 × 10−8 |
C) Mirror | |||
---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Heritability | Covariates | p value |
Aggression | 0.24 | None | 0.0021 |
Watch Observer | 0.27 | None | 0.00052 |
Front Sit | 0.20 | age/sex | 0.0047 |
Half Yawn | 0.24 | Sex | 0.00037 |
Lips Smack | 0.21 | None | 0.0058 |
Locomotion | 0.23 | age/sex | 0.00065 |
Other Location | 0.20 | Sex | 0.00034 |
Self-Scratch | 0.14 | age/sex | 0.031 |
Cage Slap | 0.27 | None | 0.00055 |
Yawn | 0.22 | Sex | 0.000066 |
Estimates of Heritability
We found that many behaviors expressed in response to the three stimuli exhibit significant individual trait heritabilities (additive genetic heritability). Genetic results are also presented in Tables 2 and 3. The average heritability of individual behaviors in response to the bear was h2 (mean) = 0.251 for frequency measures and h2 (mean) = 0.242 for duration measures. For the truck, those two means are h2 = 0.227 and h2 = 0.213 respectively. Average heritability was slightly lower for the mirror test, h2 (mean) = 0.171 for frequency and h2 (mean) = 0.184 for duration. Heritability estimates greater than these average values were observed for watch object frequency, locomote frequency and duration, cage slap frequency, vigilance duration and measures of location within the cage (front, back, other) in response to the toys. In response to the mirror, we observed above average heritability for aggression frequency, submission frequency, watch object frequency and duration, and locomote duration.
Factor Analysis
Our subsequent factor analyses identified three orthogonal factors that account for 77.7% of the variation among the 31 individual behaviors included. Factor 1 is composed of 21 individual behaviors (Table 4). These behaviors may reflect generalized arousal and possibly fear- or anxiety-related reactivity. While there are risks in assigning labels to personality factors, because those labels may over-simplify the nature of the behavioral variation, and similar labels assigned to dimensions of personality in different species or research studies can lead to incorrect assumptions about comparability (Carter et al. 2012), we nevertheless see Factor 1 as reflecting variation similar to what has been called “Boldness” in other studies. Factor 1 (Boldness) accounts for 46.8% of the variation within the population, and has a high heritability (h2 = 0.59, p = 2.3 × 10−17). Factor 2 is composed of 14 behaviors, and those with heaviest loadings are in general related to interaction with the novel object itself (Table 4). Factor 2 (Engagement with Object) accounts for 18.8% of the variation, and has a lower but still substantial heritability (h2 = 0.33, p = 1 × 10−7), which is nevertheless higher than the average values for heritability across the two toys or mirror. The third factor included only a single behavior, accounts for a small proportion of the variance, and therefore was not further analyzed.
Table 4.
Factor | 1(Boldness) | 2(Engagement with Object) | |
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Object | Behavior | ||
Truck | Aggressive Freq | 0.57 | |
Back Stand | 0.53 | ||
Latency to touch | −0.40 | ||
Locomotion | 0.47 | −0.31 | |
Mantle shake | −0.43 | 0.37 | |
Other Location | 0.51 | −0.34 | |
Passive Freq | 0.61 | ||
Touch Object (Freq) | 0.69 | ||
Watch Object (Freq) | 0.36 | 0.58 | |
Watch Observer (Freq) | 0.73 | 0.30 | |
Bear | Aggressive Freq | 0.53 | |
Avert | −0.32 | ||
Back Stand | 0.47 | ||
Locomotion | 0.48 | ||
Other Location | 0.55 | −0.33 | |
Passive Freq | 0.46 | 0.38 | |
Watch Observer (Dur) | 0.37 | ||
Watch Observer (Freq) | 0.65 | 0.46 | |
Mirror | Aggressive Freq | 0.32 | −0.33 |
Half Yawn | 0.30 | ||
Locomotion | 0.30 | ||
Other Location | 0.37 | −0.35 | |
Watch Observer (Dur) | 0.33 | ||
Watch Observer (Freq) | 0.54 | 0.34 | |
Yawn | 0.30 |
Follow-up Validation
Our results indicate that the behavioral reactions of the study baboons to these test stimuli are influenced by age, sex and genetic variation. However, evaluating the significance and implications of these observations for understanding baboon behavior outside the artificial test situation required further analyses. Our follow-up studies showed that the experimental assessments of temperament conducted individually in a novel environment are positively correlated with social behavior exhibited months after the study subjects were returned to their home cages and social groups. Ten baboons that exhibited above average rates of aggression to the truck also displayed higher rates of aggression to their home cage-mates than did the ten animals that scored low on aggression to the truck (p = 0.008). In addition, the same highly aggressive animals received more submissive behavior from their cage mates than did the other ten animals (p = 0.018). These results extend our findings and suggest that our novel object test exposes individual variation that does have predictive value regarding normal social behavior in an animal’s home social environment.
Scan for Quantitative Trait Loci
Whole genome QTL linkage scans for each individual behavior, factor scores and CSF monoamine metabolite levels (HVA, MHPG and 5HIAA) using the available baboon microsatellite linkage map (Rogers et al. 2000; Cox et al. 2006) identified suggestive, but not definitive peak LOD scores on baboon chromosome 10 for temperament factor 2 (TF2, Engagement with Object), CSF level of HVA, CSF levels of 5-HIAA and the behavior labeled “avert.” After the identification and genotyping of an additional 11 microsatellite loci in the region, subsequent QTL analyses revealed peak LOD scores of 1.9 for TF2 (Engagement with Object), 1.6 for avert, 1.5 for HVA and 0.8 for 5-HIAA, all within the same area of PHA10. All of these LOD scores are below the genome-wide threshold to establish definitive linkage (LOD = 2.78), but the scores for TF2 (Engagement with Object), HVA and avert are at or above the threshold for suggestive linkage (LOD = 1.5). Locations of peak LOD score can be seen in supplemental table 3.
Candidate Gene SNP analysis
Given the nature of the phenotypes with overlapping QTL results, we hypothesized that differences among animals in monoamine neurotransmitter levels may be partly responsible for differences in observed behavior and derived personality factor scores. Consequently, we evaluated all genes within the QTL interval as potential candidates, and chose SNAP25 as the most likely positional candidate because of its known role in exocytosis and neurotransmitter function and its suggested role in bipolar depression, neuroticism and attention deficit hyperactivity (Etain et al. 2010; Brophy et al. 2002; Terracciano et al. 2010). Sequencing of founder individuals in the baboon pedigree identified 15 SNPs within the exons, at exon/intron borders, and across the putative promoter region of SNAP25. We genotyped all study animals for the newly discovered SNPs, and tested association between each polymorphism and our phenotypes (TF2, monoamine metabolite levels and avert). Specific polymorphisms are significantly associated with TF2 (Engagement with Object) (p=0.015), avert behavior (p=.024), HVA levels (p=.035), MHPG levels (p=.050). The amount of variation in the respective phenotypes explained by these SNPs is modest: 1.6%, 2.2%, 1.4%, and 0.8% respectively (supplemental table 4).
DISCUSSION
The increasing use of the concept of temperament or personality in animal behavior research is producing several important benefits (Reale et al. 2010; Weiss et al. 2011; Freeman et al. 2013; Nettle and Penke 2010). One of these is the growth of analyses of the proximate causes of individual variation in particular behaviors and broader dimensions of personality. Clearly, researchers have for many years measured and discussed the factors that lead to differences in expressed behavior among individuals within a population or species. But the increasing focus on identifying and probing patterns of correlated behaviors that are stably expressed by specific individuals in various situations, and that differ among conspecifics has encouraged new approaches to behavioral description and new theory (Reale et al. 2010; Briffa and Weiss 2010; Capitanio 2004; Capitanio et al. 1999; Seyfarth et al. 2012).
Our study of baboons was designed to investigate the causes of individual variation in behavioral responses to novel objects and a mirror. The data clearly show that sex is one factor significantly influencing the behavioral responses of these baboons to our test challenge. Females tended to score higher for activity (e.g. more locomotion) and anxiety, fear or withdrawal (indexed by higher frequency of submission and longer latency to touch a novel object). Males exhibited average responses that can be characterized as bolder and less inhibited. This is not unexpected given prior studies. Humans exhibit consistent sex differences in personality (Costa et al. 2001; Del Giudice et al. 2012). Moreover, consistent with field observations, personality ratings of captive chimpanzees score males as more impulsive and more aggressive than females (King et al 2008). Macaque males are scored as more confident and more active than females (Stevenson-Hinde 1978). Like other primates (human and nonhuman), there are consistent behavioral differences among male and female baboons, and this current finding that males and females respond differently to the novel object challenge is concordant with expectations.
The primary focus of our study was the assessment of genetic differences among individuals as a proximate cause for individual variation in behavioral responses and personality. We found significant heritability for multiple behaviors in response to both novel objects and the mirror. In addition, we determined that the estimated heritability for both of the multivariate factor scores we derived was higher than the average heritability of individual behavioral measurements. This is encouraging, suggesting that these factor scores captured the shared information common to multiple individual behaviors. This approach may reduce the random variation and noise present in scores for individual behaviors in individual animals. Furthermore, we were able to demonstrate that these heritable responses to the experimental procedures were predictive of patterns of social behavior months later in the home social group. These follow-up studies help to establish the broader ecological, ethological and evolutionary relevance of our experimental results.
Recent studies have begun to investigate differences in personality expressed among wild baboons in natural habitats. Silk and colleagues have documented stable individual differences in patterns of social behavior and social interaction among female yellow baboons (Silk et al. 2003) and chacma baboons (Silk et al. 2009; Silk et al. 2010). Seyfarth et al. (2012) examined variation among wild female chacma baboons in the time they spend alone and their rates of affiliative and aggressive interactions. Seyfarth and colleagues identified three dimensions of personality, and found these are predictive of levels of stress and other aspects of behavior (Seyfarth et al. 2012). The social stimulus we used here, a mirror, obviously constitutes a different type of test, and we would not expect the factors we identified to correspond to the factors identified in strictly observational studies of wild baboons. However, our finding of significant heritability for several behaviors expressed by the baboons to the mirror challenge, including rates of affiliative and aggressive behaviors, suggests that the dimensions of personality identified by Seyfarth et al (2012) may also have some degree of heritability. The factors those authors identified were not accounted for by differences in dominance rank or availability of kin, and therefore some additional proximate cause for these stable patterns of behavioral variation is needed.
Individual differences in response to novelty may also have ecological consequences or correlates. Chacma baboons were found to exhibit more interest in and more exploration of a series of novel objects than did phylogenetically close gelada baboons, genus Theropithecus (Bergman and Kitchen 2009). These authors suggest this may be related to the generalist diet of Papio baboons, as compared to that of geladas, which feed predominantly on grass. As a group, Papio baboons may exhibit greater interest in novel objects than geladas, and if so this too would raise the question of evolutionary mechanism. Our results indicate that there is genetic variability among Papio baboons (in our case olive and yellow baboons, rather than chacma baboons) accounting for differences in behavioral reaction to novel objects. This type of genetic variation would be required in order for different species to evolve different patterns of reactivity as a result of natural selection.
However, it is critical to remain cautious and conservative when comparing results across studies that investigate different environmental contexts, which use different measures of behavior or different analytical approaches. Of necessity, researchers apply labels to identified dimensions of personality (e.g. “extraversion” or “anxiousness” or “impulsivity”) to convey their interpretation of these analytically derived factors, but a priori ideas concerning the equivalence of such patterns of behavior across situations or species, such as the inference that two personality dimensions in different studies have the same underlying proximate causes, may be problematic. Carter et al. (2012) showed that among 57 wild chacma baboons, variation in “boldness” as measured by response to a model snake was not correlated with variation in “boldness” as measured by reaction to a novel food item (Carter et al. 2012). There is tremendous power in the study of personality among nonhuman primates, but caution in drawing conclusions about the identity of the proximate mechanisms that lead to apparently similar behaviors expressed in diverse circumstances is certainly warranted.
QTL analysis of several independently measured phenotypes including personality factor 2 (Engagement with Object), the avert behavior, and CSF levels of HVA and 5-HIAA identified a single region of baboon chromosome 10 (PHA10) with a suggestive LOD score. While each of these results alone is interesting, their mapping to overlapping locations makes these results more compelling. Furthermore, the region of human chromosome 20 (HSA20) that is homologous to the segment of PHA10 containing this baboon QTL has been implicated in conditions such as restless legs syndrome (Levchenko et al. 2006), migraine (Oedegaard et al. 2010), and bi-polar disorder (Etain et al. 2010). While the connection between these conditions and our baboon personality phenotypes may not be immediately obvious, dysregulation of the dopaminergic system is a suspected influence on each human phenotype. There are numerous protein coding genes in this region of PHA10, but we selected SNAP 25 as the most likely positional candidate gene because of (a) its role in exocytosis and neurotransmitter function and (b) its prior association with bipolar depression, neuroticism and attention deficit hyperactivity (Etain et al. 2010; Brophy et al. 2002; Terracciano et al. 2010). SNAP25 is involved in SNARE complex formation through interaction with snare proteins present on the vesicles (v-snares) and target membranes (t-snares) of neurons (Hodel 1998; Rizo and Sudhof 2002). The interaction of these proteins (v-snares, t-snares and the SNAP25 protein) is thought to localize synaptic vesicles to the plasma membrane in anticipation of exocytosis, with SNAP25 directly involved in triggering that exocytosis (Hodel 1998; Vilinsky et al. 2002). In mice, homozygous knockouts are lethal, but heterozygous deletion of the SNAP25 gene produces a phenotype resembling attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Feng et al. 2005; Hess et al. 1992). In addition these heterozygous null mice also show decreased extracellular levels of both dopamine and serotonin within the brain, suggesting that SNAP25 expression may be associated with deficiency in neurotransmitter release. Therefore, our association results are supported by independent data suggesting that SNAP25 can influence neurotransmitter release. In that way, sequence variation in SNAP25 among baboons may influence monoamine levels and downstream expressed behavior.
As the study of individual variation in personality within nonhuman primate species develops, efforts will be made to answer fundamental questions, including the nature and relative impact of various proximate causes of variation. Our results provide additional information concerning behavioral reactivity and personality among baboons. The behavior of baboons, like all nonhuman primates, is influenced by a wide array of inputs. These results from controlled studies of response to novelty demonstrate that sex, age and genetic differences exert significant influences on those traits among baboons, and raise the possibly that sequence differences in the SNAP25 locus may also be among the inputs that determine individual variation in those behaviors.
Supplementary Material
References
- Reale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2007;82(2):291–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BR, Blumstein DT. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology. 2008:19448–455. [Google Scholar]
- Rogers J, Raveendran M, Fawcett GL, Fox AS, Shelton SE, Oler JA, Cheverud J, Muzny DM, Gibbs RA, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH. CRHR1 genotypes, neural circuits and the diathesis for anxiety and depression. Molecular psychiatry. 2013;18(6):700–707. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Schaeffer J. Chimpanzee intelligence is heritable. Current biology : CB. 2014;24(14):1649–1652. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reale D, Dingemanse NJ, Kazem AJ, Wright J. Evolutionary and ecological approaches to the study of personality. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2010;365(1560):3937–3946. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr CS, Newman TK, Becker ML, Parker CC, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Goldman D, Suomi SJ, Higley JD. The utility of the non-human primate; model for studying gene by environment interactions in behavioral research. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2(6):336–340. doi: 10.1046/j.1601-1848.2003.00051.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf M, Weissing FJ. Animal personalities: consequences for ecology and evolution. Trends in ecology & evolution. 2012;27(8):452–461. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tung J, Charpentier MJ, Mukherjee S, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Genetic effects on mating success and partner choice in a social mammal. The American naturalist. 2012;180(1):113–129. doi: 10.1086/665993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gosling SD. From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychological bulletin. 2001;127(1):45–86. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briffa M, Weiss A. Animal personality. Current biology : CB. 2010;20(21):R912–914. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morton FB, Lee PC, Buchanan-Smith HM, Brosnan SF, Thierry B, Paukner A, de Waal FB, Widness J, Essler JL, Weiss A. Personality structure in brown capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella): comparisons with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), orangutans (Pongo spp.), and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) Journal of comparative psychology. 2013;127(3):282–298. doi: 10.1037/a0031723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox AS, Shelton SE, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH. Trait-like brain activity during adolescence predicts anxious temperament in primates. PloS one. 2008;3(7):e2570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinnally EL, Whiteman HJ, Mason WA, Mendoza SP, Capitanio JP. Dimensions of response to novelty are associated with social engagement and aggression in adult male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) Journal of comparative psychology. 2008;122(2):195–203. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.122.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capitanio J. Personality factors between and within species. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W, editors. Macaque Societies: A Model for the Study of Social Organization. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2004. pp. 13–33. [Google Scholar]
- Weiss A, Adams MJ, Widdig A, Gerald MS. Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) as living fossils of hominoid personality and subjective well-being. Journal of comparative psychology. 2011;125(1):72–83. doi: 10.1037/a0021187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capitanio JP, Mendoza SP, Baroncelli S. The relationship of personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques to progression of simian immunodeficiency virus disease. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 1999;13(2):138–154. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1998.0540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman HD, Brosnan SF, Hopper LM, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Gosling SD. Developing a comprehensive and comparative questionnaire for measuring personality in chimpanzees using a simultaneous top-down/bottom-up design. American journal of primatology. 2013;75(10):1042–1053. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopper LM, Price SA, Freeman HD, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Kendal RL. Influence of personality, age, sex, and estrous state on chimpanzee problem-solving success. Animal cognition. 2014;17(4):835–847. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0715-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss A, Inoue-Murayama M, Hong KW, Inoue E, Udono T, Ochiai T, Matsuzawa T, Hirata S, King JE. Assessing chimpanzee personality and subjective well-being in Japan. American journal of primatology. 2009;71(4):283–292. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seyfarth RM, Silk JB, Cheney DL. Variation in personality and fitness in wild female baboons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(42):16980–16985. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210780109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. The benefits of social capital: close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 2009;276(1670):3099–3104. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalin NH, Shelton SE. Nonhuman primate models to study anxiety, emotion regulation, and psychopathology. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003:1008189–200. doi: 10.1196/annals.1301.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairbanks LA, Newman TK, Bailey JN, Jorgensen MJ, Breidenthal SE, Ophoff RA, Comuzzie AG, Martin LJ, Rogers J. Genetic contributions to social impulsivity and aggressiveness in vervet monkeys. Biological psychiatry. 2004;55(6):642–647. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oler JA, Fox AS, Shelton SE, Rogers J, Dyer TD, Davidson RJ, Shelledy W, Oakes TR, Blangero J, Kalin NH. Amygdalar and hippocampal substrates of anxious temperament differ in their heritability. Nature. 2010;466(7308):864–868. doi: 10.1038/nature09282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandes M, Bienvenu OJ. Personality and anxiety disorders. Current psychiatry reports. 2006;8(4):263–269. doi: 10.1007/s11920-006-0061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S. Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1994;103(1):103–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. A longitudinal twin study of personality and major depression in women. Archives of general psychiatry. 1993;50(11):853–862. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820230023002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL, Lavori P, Keller MB, Griffith P, Coryell W. Premorbid personality assessments of first onset of major depression. Archives of general psychiatry. 1989;46(4):345–350. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810040051008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, Bohman M. Childhood personality predicts alcohol abuse in young adults. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1988;12(4):494–505. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1988.tb00232.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Vaccaro D, McNamara G. Novelty seeking, risk taking, and related constructs as predictors of adolescent substance use: an application of Cloninger’s theory. Journal of substance abuse. 1994;6(1):1–20. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(94)90039-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denollet J, Sys SU, Stroobant N, Rombouts H, Gillebert TC, Brutsaert DL. Personality as independent predictor of long-term mortality in patients with coronary heart disease. Lancet. 1996;347(8999):417–421. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)90007-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole SW, Kemeny ME, Fahey JL, Zack JA, Naliboff BD. Psychological risk factors for HIV pathogenesis: Mediation by the autonomic nervous system. Biological psychiatry. 2003;54(12):1444–1456. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01888-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fawcett GL, Dettmer AM, Kay D, Raveendran M, Higley JD, Ryan ND, Cameron JL, Rogers J. Quantitative Genetics of Response to Novelty and Other Stimuli by Infant Rhesus Macaques () Across Three Behavioral Assessments. International journal of primatology. 2014;35(1):325–339. doi: 10.1007/s10764-014-9750-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birn RM, Shackman AJ, Oler JA, Williams LE, McFarlin DR, Rogers GM, Shelton SE, Alexander AL, Pine DS, Slattery MJ, Davidson RJ, Fox AS, Kalin NH. Evolutionarily conserved prefrontal-amygdalar dysfunction in early-life anxiety. Molecular psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roseboom PH, Nanda SA, Fox AS, Oler JA, Shackman AJ, Shelton SE, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH. Neuropeptide Y Receptor Gene Expression in the Primate Amygdala Predicts Anxious Temperament and Brain Metabolism. Biological psychiatry. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller C, Roos C, Groeneveld LF, Fischer J, Zinner D. Introgressive hybridization in southern African baboons shapes patterns of mtDNA variation. American journal of physical anthropology. 2010;142(1):125–136. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jolly CJ, Burrell AS, Phillips-Conroy JE, Bergey C, Rogers J. Kinda baboons (Papio kindae) and grayfoot chacma baboons (P. ursinus griseipes) hybridize in the Kafue river valley, Zambia. American journal of primatology. 2011;73(3):291–303. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jolly CJ. A proper study for mankind: Analogies from the Papionin monkeys and their implications for human evolution. American journal of physical anthropology Suppl. 2001:33177–204. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henzi SP, Barrett L. Evolutionary ecology, sexual conflict and behavioral differentiation among baboon populations. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2003:12217–230. [Google Scholar]
- Jolly CJ. Species, subspecies and baboon systematics. In: Kimbel WH, Martin LB, editors. Species, Species Concepts and Primate Evolution. Plenum Press; New York, NY: 1993. pp. 67–107. [Google Scholar]
- Bergman TJ, Phillips-Conroy JE, Jolly CJ. Behavioral variation and reproductive success of male baboons (Papio anubis x Papio hamadryas) in a hybrid social group. American journal of primatology. 2008;70(2):136–147. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current biology : CB. 2010;20(15):1359–1361. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gesquiere LR, Learn NH, Simao MC, Onyango PO, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Life at the top: rank and stress in wild male baboons. Science. 2011;333(6040):357–360. doi: 10.1126/science.1207120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moscovice LR, Heesen M, Di Fiore A, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Paternity alone does not predict long-term investment in juveniles by male baboons. Behavioral ecology and sociobiology. 2009;63(10):1471–1482. doi: 10.1007/s00265-009-0781-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smuts BB. Sex and Friendship in Baboons. Aldine Publishing Co; New York, NY: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Weiss A, King JE, Perkins L. Personality and subjective well-being in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii) Journal of personality and social psychology. 2006;90(3):501–511. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss A, Gartner MC, Gold KC, Stoinski TS. Extraversion predicts longer survival in gorillas: an 18-year longitudinal study. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 2013;280(1752):20122231. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairbanks LA, Fontenot MB, Phillips-Conroy JE, Jolly CJ, Kaplan JR, Mann JJ. CSF monoamines, age and impulsivity in wild grivet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops aethiops) Brain, behavior and evolution. 1999;53(5–6):305–312. doi: 10.1159/000006601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter AJ, Marshall HH, Heinsohn R, Cowlishaw G. How not to measure boldness: novel object and antipredator responses are not the same in wild baboons. Animal Behaviour. 2012:84603–609. [Google Scholar]
- Rogers J, Shelton SE, Shelledy W, Garcia R, Kalin NH. Genetic influences on behavioral inhibition and anxiety in juvenile rhesus macaques. Genes, brain, and behavior. 2008;7(4):463–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00381.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fawcett GL, Dettmer AM, Kay D, Raveendran M, Higley JD, Ryan ND, Cameron JL, Rogers J. Quantitative Genetics of Response to Novelty and Other Stimuli by Infant Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) Across Three Behavioral Assessments. International journal of primatology. 2014;35(1):325–339. doi: 10.1007/s10764-014-9750-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr CS, Newman TK, Lindell S, Shannon C, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Suomi SJ, Goldman D, Higley JD. Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variation and rearing condition in alcohol preference and consumption in female primates. Archives of general psychiatry. 2004;61(11):1146–1152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen GL, Novak MA, Meyer JS, Kelly BJ, Vallender EJ, Miller GM. The effect of rearing experience and TPH2 genotype on HPA axis function and aggression in rhesus monkeys: a retrospective analysis. Hormones and behavior. 2010;57(2):184–191. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trefilov A, Berard J, Krawczak M, Schmidtke J. Natal dispersal in rhesus macaques is related to serotonin transporter gene promoter variation. Behavior genetics. 2000;30(4):295–301. doi: 10.1023/a:1026597300525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong KW, Weiss A, Morimura N, Udono T, Hayasaka I, Humle T, Murayama Y, Ito S, Inoue-Murayama M. Polymorphism of the tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2) gene is associated with chimpanzee neuroticism. PloS one. 2011;6(7):e22144. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins WD, Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. A polymorphic indel containing the RS3 microsatellite in the 5′ flanking region of the vasopressin V1a receptor gene is associated with chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11(5):552–558. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00799.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zinner D, Wertheimer J, Liedigk R, Groeneveld LF, Roos C. Baboon phylogeny as inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes. American journal of physical anthropology. 2013;150(1):133–140. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alberts SC, Altmann J. Immigration and hybridization patterns of yellow and anubis baboons in and around Amboseli, Kenya. American journal of primatology. 2001;53(4):139–154. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charpentier MJ, Fontaine MC, Cherel E, Renoult JP, Jenkins T, Benoit L, Barthes N, Alberts SC, Tung J. Genetic structure in a dynamic baboon hybrid zone corroborates behavioural observations in a hybrid population. Molecular ecology. 2012;21(3):715–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05302.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falconer DS. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. Longmans; London, New York: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Almasy L, Blangero J. Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees. Am J Hum Genet. 1998;62(5):1198–1211. doi: 10.1086/301844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blangero J, Williams JT, Almasy L. Variance component methods for detecting complex trait loci. Adv Genet. 2001:42151–181. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2660(01)42021-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feingold E, Brown PO, Siegmund D. Gaussian models for genetic linkage analysis using complete high-resolution maps of identity by descent. Am J Hum Genet. 1993;53(1):234–251. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers J, Mahaney MC, Witte SM, Nair S, Newman D, Wedel S, Rodriguez LA, Rice KS, Slifer SH, Perelygin A, Slifer M, Palladino-Negro P, Newman T, Chambers K, Joslyn G, Parry P, Morin PA. A genetic linkage map of the baboon (Papio hamadryas) genome based on human microsatellite polymorphisms. Genomics. 2000;67(3):237–247. doi: 10.1006/geno.2000.6245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong A, Gudbjartsson DF, Sainz J, Jonsdottir GM, Gudjonsson SA, Richardsson B, Sigurdardottir S, Barnard J, Hallbeck B, Masson G, Shlien A, Palsson ST, Frigge ML, Thorgeirsson TE, Gulcher JR, Stefansson K. A high-resolution recombination map of the human genome. Nature genetics. 2002;31(3):241–247. doi: 10.1038/ng917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broman KW, Murray JC, Sheffield VC, White RL, Weber JL. Comprehensive human genetic maps: individual and sex-specific variation in recombination. Am J Hum Genet. 1998;63(3):861–869. doi: 10.1086/302011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RA, Rogers J, Katze MG, Bumgarner R, Weinstock GM, Mardis ER, Remington KA, Strausberg RL, Venter JC, Wilson RK, Batzer MA, Bustamante CD, Eichler EE, Hahn MW, Hardison RC, Makova KD, Miller W, Milosavljevic A, Palermo RE, Siepel A, Sikela JM, Attaway T, Bell S, Bernard KE, Buhay CJ, Chandrabose MN, Dao M, Davis C, Delehaunty KD, Ding Y, Dinh HH, Dugan-Rocha S, Fulton LA, Gabisi RA, Garner TT, Godfrey J, Hawes AC, Hernandez J, Hines S, Holder M, Hume J, Jhangiani SN, Joshi V, Khan ZM, Kirkness EF, Cree A, Fowler RG, Lee S, Lewis LR, Li Z, Liu YS, Moore SM, Muzny D, Nazareth LV, Ngo DN, Okwuonu GO, Pai G, Parker D, Paul HA, Pfannkoch C, Pohl CS, Rogers YH, Ruiz SJ, Sabo A, Santibanez J, Schneider BW, Smith SM, Sodergren E, Svatek AF, Utterback TR, Vattathil S, Warren W, White CS, Chinwalla AT, Feng Y, Halpern AL, Hillier LW, Huang X, Minx P, Nelson JO, Pepin KH, Qin X, Sutton GG, Venter E, Walenz BP, Wallis JW, Worley KC, Yang SP, Jones SM, Marra MA, Rocchi M, Schein JE, Baertsch R, Clarke L, Csuros M, Glasscock J, Harris RA, Havlak P, Jackson AR, Jiang H, Liu Y, Messina DN, Shen Y, Song HX, Wylie T, Zhang L, Birney E, Han K, Konkel MK, Lee J, Smit AF, Ullmer B, Wang H, Xing J, Burhans R, Cheng Z, Karro JE, Ma J, Raney B, She X, Cox MJ, Demuth JP, Dumas LJ, Han SG, Hopkins J, Karimpour-Fard A, Kim YH, Pollack JR, Vinar T, Addo-Quaye C, Degenhardt J, Denby A, Hubisz MJ, Indap A, Kosiol C, Lahn BT, Lawson HA, Marklein A, Nielsen R, Vallender EJ, Clark AG, Ferguson B, Hernandez RD, Hirani K, Kehrer-Sawatzki H, Kolb J, Patil S, Pu LL, Ren Y, Smith DG, Wheeler DA, Schenck I, Ball EV, Chen R, Cooper DN, Giardine B, Hsu F, Kent WJ, Lesk A, Nelson DL, O’Brien WE, Prufer K, Stenson PD, Wallace JC, Ke H, Liu XM, Wang P, Xiang AP, Yang F, Barber GP, Haussler D, Karolchik D, Kern AD, Kuhn RM, Smith KE, Zwieg AS. Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome. Science. 2007;316(5822):222–234. doi: 10.1126/science.1139247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raveendran M, Harris RA, Milosavljevic A, Johnson Z, Shelledy W, Cameron J, Rogers J. Designing new microsatellite markers for linkage and population genetic analyses in rhesus macaques and other nonhuman primates. Genomics. 2006;88(6):706–710. doi: 10.1016/j.ygeno.2006.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dyke B. Tech rep 2. Population Genetics Laboratory, Department of Genetics, Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research; San Antonio: 1996. PEDSYS: a pedigree data management system. [Google Scholar]
- Matise TC, Perlin M, Chakravarti A. Automated construction of genetic linkage maps using an expert system (MultiMap): a human genome linkage map. Nature genetics. 1994;6(4):384–390. doi: 10.1038/ng0494-384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox LA, Mahaney MC, Vandeberg JL, Rogers J. A second-generation genetic linkage map of the baboon (Papio hamadryas) genome. Genomics. 2006;88(3):274–281. doi: 10.1016/j.ygeno.2006.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers J, Mahaney MC, Almasy L, Comuzzie AG, Blangero J. Quantitative trait linkage mapping in anthropology. American journal of physical anthropology Suppl. 1999:29127–151. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8644(1999)110:29+<127::aid-ajpa5>3.0.co;2-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etain B, Dumaine A, Mathieu F, Chevalier F, Henry C, Kahn JP, Deshommes J, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Jamain S. A SNAP25 promoter variant is associated with early-onset bipolar disorder and a high expression level in brain. Molecular psychiatry. 2010;15(7):748–755. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brophy K, Hawi Z, Kirley A, Fitzgerald M, Gill M. Synaptosomal-associated protein 25 (SNAP-25) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): evidence of linkage and association in the Irish population. Molecular psychiatry. 2002;7(8):913–917. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terracciano A, Sanna S, Uda M, Deiana B, Usala G, Busonero F, Maschio A, Scally M, Patriciu N, Chen WM, Distel MA, Slagboom EP, Boomsma DI, Villafuerte S, Sliwerska E, Burmeister M, Amin N, Janssens AC, van Duijn CM, Schlessinger D, Abecasis GR, Costa PT., Jr Genome-wide association scan for five major dimensions of personality. Molecular psychiatry. 2010;15(6):647–656. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D, Penke L. Personality: bridging the literatures from human psychology and behavioural ecology. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences. 2010;365(1560):4043–4050. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa PT, Jr, Terracciano A, McCrae RR. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2001;81(2):322–331. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Giudice M, Booth T, Irwing P. The Distance Between Mars and Venus: Measuring Global Sex Differences in Personality. PloS one. 2012;7(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science. 2003;302(5648):1231–1234. doi: 10.1126/science.1088580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergman TJ, Kitchen DM. Comparing responses to novel objects in wild baboons (Papio ursinus) and geladas (Theropithecus gelada) Animal cognition. 2009;12(1):63–73. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0171-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levchenko A, Provost S, Montplaisir JY, Xiong L, St-Onge J, Thibodeau P, Riviere JB, Desautels A, Turecki G, Dube MP, Rouleau GA. A novel autosomal dominant restless legs syndrome locus maps to chromosome 20p13. Neurology. 2006;67(5):900–901. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000233991.20410.b6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oedegaard KJ, Greenwood TA, Lunde A, Fasmer OB, Akiskal HS, Kelsoe JR. A genome-wide linkage study of bipolar disorder and co-morbid migraine: replication of migraine linkage on chromosome 4q24, and suggestion of an overlapping susceptibility region for both disorders on chromosome 20p11. Journal of affective disorders. 2010;122(1–2):14–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodel A. Snap-25. Int J Biochem Cell Biol. 1998;30(10):1069–1073. doi: 10.1016/s1357-2725(98)00079-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizo J, Sudhof TC. Snares and Munc18 in synaptic vesicle fusion. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(8):641–653. doi: 10.1038/nrn898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilinsky I, Stewart BA, Drummond J, Robinson I, Deitcher DL. A Drosophila SNAP-25 null mutant reveals context-dependent redundancy with SNAP-24 in neurotransmission. Genetics. 2002;162(1):259–271. doi: 10.1093/genetics/162.1.259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng Y, Crosbie J, Wigg K, Pathare T, Ickowicz A, Schachar R, Tannock R, Roberts W, Malone M, Swanson J, Kennedy JL, Barr CL. The SNAP25 gene as a susceptibility gene contributing to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular psychiatry. 2005;10(11):998–1005. 1973. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess EJ, Jinnah HA, Kozak CA, Wilson MC. Spontaneous locomotor hyperactivity in a mouse mutant with a deletion including the Snap gene on chromosome 2. J Neurosci. 1992;12(7):2865–2874. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-07-02865.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.