Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Apr 10.
Published in final edited form as: Med Decis Making. 2010 Nov-Dec;30(6):625–638. doi: 10.1177/0272989X10363479

Estimation and Validation of a Multi-Attribute Model of Alzheimer's Disease Progression

Eric Stallard 1, Bruce Kinosian 2, Arthur S Zbrozek 3, Anatoliy I Yashin 4, Henry A Glick 5, Yaakov Stern 6
PMCID: PMC4392765  NIHMSID: NIHMS677522  PMID: 21183754

Abstract

Objectives

To estimate and validate a multi-attribute model of the clinical course of Alzheimer's Disease (AD) from mild AD to death in a high-quality prospective cohort study; to estimate the impact of hypothetical modifications to AD progression rates on costs associated with Medicare and Medicaid services.

Data and Methods

We estimated sex-specific longitudinal Grade of Membership (GoM) models for AD patients (103 males; 149 females) in the initial cohort of the Predictors Study (1989–2001) based on 80 individual measures obtained every six months for 10 years. We replicated these models for AD patients (106 males; 148 females) in the second Predictors Study cohort (1997–2007). Model validation required that the disease-specific transition parameters be identical for both Predictors Study cohorts. Medicare costs were estimated from the National Long Term Care Survey.

Results

Sex-specific models were validated using the second Predictors Study cohort with the GoM transition parameters constrained to the values estimated for the first Predictors Study cohort; 57–61 of the 80 individual measures contributed significantly to the GoM models. Simulated, cost-free interventions in the rate of progression of AD indicated that large potential cost offsets could occur for patients at the earliest stages of AD.

Conclusions

AD progression is characterized by a small number of parameters governing changes in large numbers of correlated indicators of AD severity. The analysis confirmed that the progression of AD represents a complex multidimensional physiological process that is similar across different study cohorts. The estimates suggested that there could be large cost offsets to Medicare and Medicaid from the slowing of AD progression among patients with mild AD. The methodology appears generally applicable in AD modeling.

Keywords: Clinical assessment, outcomes, staging of dementia

INTRODUCTION

Modeling the clinical course of Alzheimer's Disease (AD) is essential for accurate, reliable, and valid medical decisions for the care and treatment of AD patients and for estimating cost offsets for proposed medical and pharmaceutical interventions. In addressing these issues, decision makers have increasingly relied on Markov transition models to form the core components of their decision analyses.1

Markov transition models are typically based on three assumptions: (A1) that each patient is always in one of a small number of discrete health states; (A2) that the transitions from one health state to the next are independent of the prior states and timings of prior transitions; and (A3) that the patient population in each state is homogeneous with respect to the risk of subsequent transitions. Although such assumptions are often used in modeling the clinical course of AD,2 it is recognized that each assumption is only an approximation that is violated to some degree.3,4

Analyses based on Cox's proportional hazards model have demonstrated that individual variability in transition rates is substantial for AD patients, which violates assumption A3.3,5 Caro and colleagues6 dealt with this violation in their Assessment of Health Economics in Alzheimer's Disease (AHEAD) model by conducting long-term forecasts for a 3-state Markov model at the individual-patient level and by basing transitions on Cox regression parameters for extrapyramidal signs, psychotic symptoms, cognitive function, duration of illness, current age, age at onset of disease, and gender that were derived from the Predictors Study.5 This approach allowed the transitions to depend on the time in the current state, thereby resolving potential violations of assumption A2. This model was used to develop cholinesterase inhibitor guidance for the National Health Service (U.K), although Caro and colleagues disagreed with this application of their model.7

While Caro's specification of the model transitions at the individual-patient level resolved some important violations of the assumptions of the Markov model, it was not fully satisfactory for generating long-term forecasts. Two issues remain to be resolved.

First, the Cox regression model implicitly assumes that the predictors are fixed for individual patients. Actually, five of the seven predictors (i.e., extrapyramidal signs, psychotic symptoms, cognitive function, duration of illness, and current age) change over the course of the disease, with the first three being significant markers of the stage of the disease. These changes are not addressed by using the Cox regression model nor are they addressed elsewhere in Caro's model. Adequate resolution of this issue must also deal with the right-censoring problems typically encountered in survival analysis.

Second, it is not clear that the Caro model's use of three states – (1) not needing full-time care (FTC), (2) needing FTC, operationalized as nursing home (NH) institutionalization, and (3) death - are adequate for characterizing the progression of AD. There are several options for defining the number and nature of such states which can be based on any of several instruments for the staging of the disease, including the 7-state Global Deterioration Scale (GDS)8 or the 3-state Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scale,9 with extensions to 4, 5, or 6 states to represent “questionable”, “profound,” and “terminal”stages.10

Eisdorfer and colleagues found that the Global Deterioration Scale incorrectly predicted the timing of psychiatric symptoms and functional impairments.11 They recommended separate measures for cognitive, clinical, and functional status, and the development of multidimensional scales.

Bolstering Eisdorfer's recommendations, Stern and colleagues used longitudinal data from the Predictors Study to establish that the progression of AD occurs in three dimensions, with different and distinct nonlinear changes on measures of cognition, activities of daily living (ADLs), and instrumental ADLs.12

These results invalidate assumption A1 of the Markov model: it is not true that each patient is always in one of a small number of discrete health states. The health states are multidimensional; the multiplicity of available scales indicates that the states are not discrete. The outcome categories of the multiple attributes used to inform the staging models are discrete, but they are so numerous that any attempt to represent them as a single dimensional scale with 3–7 stages necessarily involves substantial simplification and distortion of the underlying process.

This paper takes up Eisdorfer's challenge to develop a multidimensional multi-attribute approach for modeling the progression of AD, thereby resolving the limitations of the Markov transition model identified above. The approach responds to Caro and colleagues' recent critique of the AHEAD model and call for the development of models that “…incorporate individual patient characteristics and history…” and “…allow proper handling of competing risks and treatment persistence and compliance.”7 The approach also responds to Green's recent call for “more appropriate methods for the modeling of AD progression…” using “…multi-attribute health states using a combination of cognitive function, functional ability, and behavior and mood.”13

The fundamental assumption is that the multiple measures of individual patient attributes are symptoms of AD, not direct measures of the biological characteristics of AD itself. The latter are currently unavailable and, hence, unobserved; they are assumed to be the underlying drivers of the disease and are the missing factors that account for the observed symptoms, as evidenced by ongoing research targeted on discovery of AD biomarkers.14 Moreover, the observed symptoms are assumed to be only probabilistically determined by the unobserved biological characteristics of the disease. This allows patients with the same unobserved biological characteristics to exhibit different patterns of symptoms, including occasional “reversals” in symptoms even as disease progression continues.

Under this approach, we achieve parsimony and transparency by using a large number of factors to identify a low-dimensional process that describes AD progression. In the remainder of this paper, we describe and report results from such a model.

METHODS

Model

The analyses used a longitudinal form of the Grade of Membership (GoM) model.15,16 GoM provides a statistically optimized summarization of large amounts of data on individual AD patients by use of a small number of distinct variables that represent the most salient characteristics of the AD process as it develops over time.17,18

Longitudinal GoM is a multidimensional state-space model that is based on three assumptions:

  • A1

    That each patient is always located at some point (the “state vector”) in an unobserved low-dimensional continuous bounded state space that accurately represents the biological characteristics of AD.

  • A2

    That the changes in the state vector during the interval from one observation time to the next can be completely determined by an upper-triangular transition matrix that characterizes the progression of AD for that observation interval, with the axes of the coordinate system ordered by increasing AD severity.

  • A3

    That the observed symptoms are random variables that are conditionally independent, given the state vector, with the symptom probabilities being functionally dependent on the elements of the state vector; there is no explicit upper limit to the number of such symptoms.

To specify this model mathematically, we denote the categorical data array * for the observable variables as {xijt}, where

  • i = index for I individual AD patients

  • j = index for J discrete variables in the study

  • l = index for Lj symptom indicators (response levels) within variable j

  • m = index for M combinations (j, l)

  • t = index for time since intake examination.

The fundamental equation expresses the probability of each possible outcome as a time-varying linear function of the GoM scores:

Prob(xijt=l)=gi{α=0t1Uα}λmjl=giVtλmjl, (1)

where gi denotes the transpose of gi, the K-element column vector of GoM scores for individual i indicating his or her initial location in the postulated state space of dimensionality D = K−1 the elements are non-negative and sum to 1 over the range of the index k, k = 1,…, K. The K elements define a set of K latent states, classes, or “pure types.” Ut is the upper-triangular K×K state-space transition matrix governing the AD progression over the interval (t, t + 1); the elements in each row are non-negative and sum to 1. Vt is the K×K matrix containing the cumulative product of the t state-space transition matrices governing the AD progression over the interval (0, t). By convention, V0 = I, a K×K identity matrix. λmjl is the K-element column vector of probabilities for symptom (response) m; the elements are non-negative and, for fixed indexes (j, k), the elements λkmjl sum to 1 over the range of the index l, l = 1, …, Lj.

It follows from assumption A3 that the likelihood is the product over i, j, l, and t of the probabilities in eqn. (1):

LIK=ijlt(gi{α=0t1Uα}λmjl)yijlt, (2)

where yijlt = 1 if xijt = l, and yijlt = 0 if xijtl. Maximum likelihood estimation (MLE) of the parameters is described in Stallard.16

For the special case of K = 1, defining a 0-dimensional [0-D] state space, the right side of eqn. (1) is a scalar quantity that is independent of i and t; and the right side of eqn. (2) is a composite function formed from the product of J multinomial likelihood functions with MLE values equal, respectively, to the observed relative frequencies of each response to each of the J variables. The 0-D model is the null model for statistical model selection.

For any specified value of K, the representation of the right side of eqn. (2) as a product over J variables implies that the J variables are assumed to be statistically independent. For the 0-D model, this condition implies marginal independence. For all other cases, the independence is conditioned on the state vector (assumption A3).

Eqn. (2) readily accommodates planned missing data due to death and various forms of questionnaire “skip patterns” and unplanned randomly missing data due to drop-out and sporadic missing items.16

Erosheva19 used a geometric approach to establish the connections between the basic nonlongitudinal GoM model and the Rasch model, demonstrating that the GoM model may be viewed as a specific form of item response theory (IRT) model. Erosheva19 further demonstrated that GoM scores differ from Rasch ability parameters in that only the former are “intrinsic” to the response probability manifold, a characterization that allows GoM scores to be described as “natural measures” of latent traits with certain invariance properties defined by Ramsay.20 Thus, the 1-D GoM model can describe multivariate dichotomous categorical data within an IRT framework with extensions to polytomous categorical data and to 2-D, 3-D, or higher dimensional models readily implemented.

Selection of the best model from among several competing (e.g., 1-D, 2-D, 3-D) models is based on identifying the model with the smallest value of the Bayesian information criterion (BIC),21,22 computed for each model as follows:

BIC=2×ln(LIK)+df×ln(N), (3)

where df is the number of independently adjusted parameters in the model and N is the effective sample size.

N can be calculated in two ways: (1) N = N*, the weighted geometric mean number of responses for the J variables, with the weight for each variable equal to the df (denoted dfj) for the corresponding λ-parameters (BIC1); and (2) N = N**, the geometric mean number of additive terms in the formulas for the diagonal elements of the df×df Hessian matrix of the log-likelihood function (BIC2).

N** approximates the dfth root of the ratio of: (1) the determinant of the expected Fisher information matrix for all observations; to: (2) the determinant of the expected Fisher information matrix for one observation – the approximation recommended by Raftery as most accurate.22 N* is equivalent to the geometric mean number of additive terms in the formulas for the diagonal elements of the partition of the Hessian matrix corresponding to the λ-parameters, which excludes the diagonal elements corresponding to the g- and u-parameters; hence N = N* is expected to be less accurate.

For comparison, we also calculated Akaike's information criterion (AIC)23 and Bozdogan's asymptotically consistent form of AIC (CAIC) using N = N*.24 For ln(N*) > 2 (i.e., for 8 or more observations), the following inequality holds: AIC < BIC1 < CAIC; indicating that model selection decisions based on BIC1 will be intermediate to those based on AIC and CAIC.

We hypothesized that the transition matrices, {Vt}, governing the changes in the state vectors are fundamental parameters of the disease process that are constant from one patient to the next, within sex, implying that the transition matrices estimated from any one database should fit any other. Application of these matrices to the initial vector of GoM scores, gi, yields the vectors of time-varying GoM scores, git, as follows:

git=giVt. (4)

We tested this hypothesis by applying the BIC selection procedures to the second Predictors Study cohort with the transition matrices constrained to the values estimated for the first Predictors Study cohort.

Data

The Predictors Study was specifically designed to investigate the natural history of AD in order to develop improved models for the management of the disease.25 Case selection was based on the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria for probable AD, criteria which were confirmed in up to 96% of postmortem diagnostic evaluations (Zhu et al., 2006).26 The study comprises two distinct cohorts, designated Predictors 1 and Predictors 2, respectively.

Predictors 1 consists of longitudinal follow-up on 103 males and 149 females; Predictors 2 consists of longitudinal follow-up on 106 males and 148 females. All cases were determined to have probable AD at the time of recruitment into the study, with the severity of dementia determined to be mild at that time (generally based on a modified Mini Mental Status (mMMS)27 score of 30 or above in Predictors 1; or 16+ on the standard Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE) in Predictors 2).

The analyses of Predictors 1 were based on the first 21 waves of follow-up which occurred approximately every 6 months over the period 1989–2001. The use of exactly 21 waves was motivated, in part, by the fact that the total resulting follow-up time was 10 years. Beyond the 21st wave, the sample sizes became too small.

The analyses of Predictors 2 were based on the first 16 waves of follow-up, occurring approximately every 6 months beginning in 1997, continuing through early-2007. Beyond the 16th wave (7.5 years follow-up), the sample sizes became too small.

The longitudinal GoM model was estimated using 79 (female) or 80 (male) variables from Predictors 1 (Myocardial Infarction was deleted for females due to no events), and was validated using a closely matched set of variables from Predictors 2. The variables were representative of measures likely to be collected in many AD databases, but they were not an exhaustive compilation of all variables available in one or the other of the Predictor Study cohorts. They included cognition (mMMS, 6 items and total score), functional capacity (Part 1 of the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale [BDRS],28 11 items and total score; Dependence Scale,29 13 items, total score, and equivalent institutional care30 levels), behaviors (5 items), psychopathological symptoms (3 items), motor signs (1 item), seizures (3 items), vision, CVD risk factors/signs (6 items), alcohol use (4 items), occupation, citizenship, education, spoken language, demographic factors, neurologist's estimation of AD duration, and 6-month survival.

The average age (standard deviation) at intake examination was 71.4 (9.4) years for males and 74.5 (9.0) years for females in Predictors 1. The corresponding ages were 75.4 (7.5) and 77.3 (8.2) years, respectively, in Predictors 2. The estimated average duration (standard deviation) of AD at intake was 4.8 (2.7) years for males and 4.3 (2.4) years for females in Predictors 1. The corresponding average durations were 4.6 (2.3) years and 4.3 (2.3) years, respectively, in Predictors 2. On average, the Predictors 2 cohort was 3–4 years older at intake. The average AD durations in the two cohorts ranged from 4.3 to 4.8 years at intake.

We used the National Long Term Care Survey (NLTCS) data in supplementary analyses to generate Medicare cost parameters for each of the GoM pure types in the NLTCS model in a form that was matched to each of the GoM pure types in the Predictors 1 model.

Predictors 2 introduced measures of the cost of medical care which were not available in Predictors 1 and which were used in the supplementary analyses to validate the relative cost differentials for Medicare costs among the GoM pure types in the NLTCS model.

Medicaid NH costs were obtained from Grabowski et al.31 These costs were assumed to depend only on the fact of institutionalization, independent of the individual GoM scores.

All costs were converted to 2007 dollars using the CPI-U Medical Care series.

RESULTS

Sex-specific 1-D, 2-D, and 3-D models of AD progression were estimated from Predictors 1 for 103 males and 149 females. Predictors 2 was used in subsequent analyses to validate the results obtained from Predictors 1. Predictors 2 and the NLTCS were further used in supplementary analyses to estimate the costs associated with Medicare-reimbursed medical interventions and Medicaid-reimbursed NH stays, and the cost offsets associated with hypothetical modifications to AD progression rates.

The 1-D and 3-D models were chosen to reflect plausible alternative models of AD progression consistent with the review of the literature provided above. Briefly, standard specifications of the Markov transition model and the existing global assessment scales (e.g., GDS, CDR) both imply a 1-D model of AD progression. Alternatively, analyses by Eisdorfer, Stern, and others indicated that AD progression may be better modeled as a 3-D process.11,12 However, these prior reports did not indicate how this might be done, nor how to compare the results of such a 3-D model with 1-D models.

The analyses were stratified by sex because prior GoM analyses reported substantial differences between men and women with respect to the estimated AD pure types and AD-related care measures.15,18

Predictors 1 Estimation

For each sex-specific model, a total of 79 or 80 variables (female; male) were employed in estimation. Under the Bayesian information criteria (both BIC1 and BIC2), the 3-D models provided better fits for both sexes to Predictors 1 than the 1-D and 2-D models; hence the 3-D models were selected as the best models.§

Tables 1 and 2 display the sex-specific λ-parameters (i.e., response/symptom probabilities) by pure type for the 1-D and 3-D models for 10 variables. Three of the 10 variables were summary scores for another 30 items not included in the two tables: mMMS (6 items), Dependence Scale (13 items), and BDRS (Part 1; 11 items).

Table 1.

Response Probabilities by Pure Type for the 1- and 3-Dimensional Models of AD Progression in the Predictors 1 Data, Males

1-Dimensional Model
3-Dimensional Model
Pure Type
Pure Type
Variable Response No. of Obs. Observed Predicted I II ΔBICj 0–1 &
Rank1
Predicted I II III IV ΔBICj 0–3 &
Rank2
Dependence Scale 0: Independent 11 0.012 0.012 0.015 0.008 474.29
3
0.012 0.049 0.010 0.000 0.000 881.54
1
1: Occasional Reminders 12 0.013 0.013 0.024 0.000 0.013 0.051 0.012 0.000 0.000
2: More Frequent Reminders 330 0.359 0.358 0.647 0.000 0.354 0.761 0.808 0.059 0.000
3: Needs Supervision 227 0.247 0.247 0.304 0.176 0.233 0.136 0.169 0.615 0.000
4: Active ADL Help 101 0.110 0.110 0.010 0.234 0.104 0.003 0.000 0.326 0.052
5: Physical Dependence 238 0.259 0.260 0.000 0.582 0.283 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.948

Equivalent Institutional Care Limited Home Care 201 0.219 0.210 0.379 0.000 469.07
4
0.228 0.631 0.439 0.000 0.000 746.19
3
Adult Home Care 364 0.396 0.384 0.621 0.091 0.420 0.365 0.561 0.803 0.000
Health Related Facility (FTC) 354 0.385 0.406 0.000 0.909 0.352 0.004 0.000 0.197 1.000

BDRS (Parti) Score 0–5 74 0.081 0.080 0.145 0.000 401.22
10
0.083 0.255 0.136 0.000 0.000 708.70
5
6–10 256 0.280 0.278 0.500 0.000 0.288 0.676 0.549 0.092 0.000
11–15 255 0.279 0.278 0.355 0.182 0.293 0.068 0.314 0.723 0.034
16–27 330 0.361 0.363 0.000 0.818 0.336 0.000 0.000 0.185 0.966

Overall mMMS Response No answers 410 0.358 0.365 0.091 0.680 182.09
26
0.362 0.113 0.124 0.171 0.810 270.92
28
Any answers 736 0.642 0.635 0.909 0.320 0.638 0.887 0.876 0.829 0.190

mMMS Score 0–19 215 0.294 0.280 0.000 0.761 266.02
19
0.277 0.000 0.000 0.295 0.971 528.27
16
20–29 132 0.180 0.181 0.203 0.144 0.190 0.000 0.221 0.439 0.000
30–39 203 0.277 0.283 0.392 0.096 0.285 0.251 0.508 0.267 0.029
40–57 182 0.249 0.256 0.405 0.000 0.248 0.749 0.270 0.000 0.000

Residence Status Home 650 0.707 0.680 0.983 0.305 312.81
17
0.703 0.924 0.994 0.984 0.078 469.04
19
Retirement Home 8 0.009 0.008 0.013 0.003 0.009 0.018 0.006 0.014 0.000
Nursing Home 224 0.244 0.268 0.000 0.599 0.248 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.827
Hospital 5 0.005 0.006 0.000 0.013 0.006 0.001 0.000 0.002 0.015
Rehabilitation Center 18 0.020 0.022 0.000 0.048 0.020 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.066
Other 14 0.015 0.016 0.004 0.031 0.015 0.057 0.000 0.000 0.013

Moderate Extra-pyramidal Signs Absent 436 0.740 0.736 0.949 0.301 109.37
31
0.740 0.980 0.879 0.775 0.040 151.61
32
Present 153 0.260 0.264 0.051 0.699 0.260 0.020 0.121 0.225 0.960

Delusions Absent 574 0.627 0.630 0.511 0.781 18.90
41
0.634 0.910 0.528 0.159 0.966 175.80
31
Present 341 0.373 0.370 0.489 0.219 0.366 0.090 0.472 0.841 0.034

Hallucinations Absent 792 0.866 0.866 0.916 0.803 1.04
57
0.865 1.000 0.894 0.718 0.885 35.13
42
Present 123 0.134 0.134 0.084 0.197 0.135 0.000 0.106 0.282 0.115

Prospective 6-Month Survival Died 70 0.065 0.064 0.017 0.121 12.95
46
0.064 0.009 0.018 0.038 0.152 17.04
52
Survived 1,015 0.935 0.936 0.983 0.879 0.936 0.991 0.982 0.962 0.848

Notes

1

ΔBICj, 0–1 is the difference in the BIC statistics between the 0-D and 1-D models (i.e. the null model and the 1-D alternative) for the indicated variable. Positive values indicate that the higher dimensional model performs better statistically, with higher positive values indicating greater significance. The rankings indicate the relative significance among the 80 variables in the male model.

2

ΔBICj, 0–3 is the difference in the BIC statistics between the 0-D and 3-D models (i.e. the null model and the 3-D alternative) for the indicated variable, which is interpreted like ΔBICj, 0–1.

Table 2.

Response Probabilities by Pure Type for the 1- and 3-Dimensional Models of AD Progression in the Predictors 1 Data, Females

1-Dimensional Model
3-Dimensional Model
Pure Type
Pure Type
Variable Response No. of Obs. Observed Predicted I II ΔBICj 0–1 &
Rank1
Predicted I II III IV ΔBICj 0–3 &
Rank2
Dependence Scale 0: Independent 14 0.010 0.010 0.014 0.006 871.21
3
0.010 0.032 0.010 0.000 0.005 1,342.45
1
1: Occasional Reminders 12 0.008 0.008 0.017 0.000 0.009 0.044 0.003 0.000 0.000
2: More Frequent Reminders 271 0.191 0.189 0.390 0.000 0.195 0.854 0.169 0.000 0.000
3: Needs Supervision 420 0.296 0.293 0.556 0.047 0.303 0.070 0.662 0.645 0.000
4: Active ADL Help 260 0.183 0.185 0.022 0.338 0.183 0.000 0.155 0.355 0.210
5: Physical Dependence 442 0.311 0.315 0.000 0.610 0.301 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.786

Equivalent Institutional Care Limited Home Care 215 0.152 0.167 0.346 0.000 658.93
12
0.147 0.699 0.091 0.000 0.000 1,070.89
3
Adult Home Care 442 0.312 0.337 0.630 0.063 0.320 0.301 0.874 0.125 0.012
Health Related Facility (FTC) 758 0.536 0.495 0.024 0.937 0.533 0.000 0.035 0.875 0.988

BDRS (Part 1) Score 0–5 108 0.076 0.077 0.158 0.000 696.28
9
0.077 0.348 0.058 0.000 0.000 973.40
8
6–10 358 0.253 0.254 0.512 0.012 0.258 0.609 0.318 0.380 0.000
11–15 410 0.290 0.290 0.329 0.253 0.296 0.044 0.590 0.511 0.105
16–27 538 0.380 0.379 0.000 0.735 0.369 0.000 0.034 0.109 0.895

Overall mMMS Response No answers 660 0.375 0.387 0.095 0.645 304.21
26
0.384 0.170 0.114 0.091 0.769 416.21
29
Any answers 1,098 0.625 0.613 0.905 0.355 0.616 0.830 0.886 0.909 0.231

mMMS Score 0–19 331 0.304 0.303 0.000 0.728 490.86
16
0.273 0.000 0.042 0.000 1.000 825.18
14
20–29 275 0.253 0.253 0.261 0.242 0.267 0.067 0.591 0.322 0.000
30–39 353 0.324 0.325 0.536 0.030 0.338 0.465 0.351 0.619 0.000
40–57 129 0.119 0.119 0.204 0.000 0.122 0.468 0.017 0.059 0.000

Residence Status Home 841 0.593 0.586 0.937 0.257 387.50
20
0.573 0.999 0.934 0.180 0.289 549.49
23
Retirement Home 30 0.021 0.021 0.029 0.013 0.020 0.000 0.055 0.022 0.004
Nursing Home 468 0.330 0.337 0.016 0.638 0.348 0.000 0.000 0.663 0.622
Hospital 2 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.002 0.001 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.003
Rehabilitation Center 44 0.031 0.032 0.000 0.061 0.033 0.000 0.000 0.036 0.070
Other 33 0.023 0.023 0.017 0.029 0.024 0.000 0.011 0.099 0.013

Moderate Extrapyramidal Signs Absent 648 0.695 0.695 0.915 0.360 133.97
31
0.693 1.000 0.858 0.564 0.240 189.81
34
Present 285 0.305 0.305 0.085 0.640 0.307 0.000 0.142 0.436 0.760

Delusions Absent 841 0.603 0.607 0.463 0.745 44.39
40
0.607 0.736 0.277 0.498 0.839 119.70
37
Present 554 0.397 0.393 0.537 0.255 0.393 0.264 0.723 0.502 0.161

Hallucinations Absent 1,254 0.899 0.899 0.909 0.890 −6.87
71
0.899 0.998 0.757 0.949 0.934 36.68
47
Present 141 0.101 0.101 0.091 0.110 0.101 0.002 0.243 0.051 0.066

Prospective 6-Month Survival Died 92 0.055 0.055 0.012 0.094 20.60
48
0.055 0.004 0.001 0.094 0.099 27.83
51
Survived 1,581 0.945 0.945 0.988 0.906 0.945 0.996 0.999 0.906 0.901

Notes

1

ΔBICj 0–1 is the difference in the BIC statistics between the 0-D and 1-D models (i.e. the null model and the 1-D alternative) for the indicated variable. Positive values indicate that the higher dimensional model performs better statistically, with higher positive values indicating greater significance. The rankings indicate the relative significance among the 80 variables in the male model.

2

ΔBICj 0–3 is the difference in the BIC statistics between the 0-D and 3-D models (i.e. the null model and the 3-D alternative) for the indicated variable, which is interpreted like ΔBICj 0–1.

The remaining 7 variables were selected to display other important aspects of AD progression. Residence Status indicates the current place of residence of the patient; on average (under the heading “Observed” in column 4), 24.4% of males and 33.0% of females resided in a NH. Equivalent Institutional Care was derived as an adjunct to the Dependence Scale; on average, 38.5% of males and 53.6% of females needed full time care (FTC) equivalent to that provided in a health related facility. These differences are consistent with prior reports that rated FTC risk was greater than actual NH risk, which justifies keeping both sets of measures in the model.6 Overall mMMS Response represents the probability that the mMMS questions would be attempted at the current examination; the average attempt rate was 64.2% for males and 62.5% for females. Moderate Extrapyramidal Signs indicate the presence/absence of non-drug induced motor signs using a Parkinson's disease rating scale; 26.0% of males and 30.5% of females exhibited such signs. Delusions and Hallucinations separately indicate the presence/absence of two important psychopathological features of AD; 37.3% of males and 39.7% of females had delusions, but only 13.4% of males and 10.1% of females had hallucinations. Prospective 6-Month Survival represents the risk of death for individual patients from one examination to the next; the average death probability was 6.5% for males and 5.5% for females.

Columns 5–8 display the parameters, the △BICj statistics, and their rankings (among the full set of J = 79 or 80 variables) for the 1-D model; columns 9–14 display the corresponding parameters, △BICj statistics, and rankings for the 3-D model. The △BICj statistics in columns 8 and 14 are the differences between the BICj statistics for the 0-D model and the BICj statistics for the 1-D and 3-D models, respectively. The BIC1 statistics were computed by restricting eqn. (3) to the data for the jth variable with dfj set equal to the number of free parameters for that variable, i.e., the initial GoM scores and transition parameters were assumed to be “fixed” for these calculations, and Nj was set equal to the corresponding number of observed responses.

Because the △BICj statistics account for differences in sample size and number of parameters, they can be used to assess the relative influence of the different variables. Positive values indicate that the 1-D or 3-D model is favored over the 0-D model, which is true for all comparisons except Hallucinations for the female 1-D model. Each △BICj statistic for the 3-D model is larger than the corresponding value for the 1-D model, indicating that the 3-D model is favored over the 1-D model for all ten variables. Overall, the △BICj statistics were positive for 57–61 of the 79 or 80 variables in each sex-specific 1-D or 3-D GoM model.**

The predicted values in columns 5 and 9 are the marginal probabilities for the 1-D and 3-D models. They can be compared with the observed values in column 4 where the differences were generally in the range ±0.020, indicating that both models closely reproduced the observed distributions of outcomes in the sex-specific study data. For both sexes, the Dependence Scale exhibited the highest ranked △BICj statistics for both models. Equivalent Institutional Care ranked second for three of the four comparisons, the exception being the female comparison of 0-D with 1-D, with BDRS (Part 1) Score moving up to second.

The pure type probabilities in columns 6–7 and 10–13 are the MLEs of the λmjl parameters for the 1-D and 3-D models, respectively. They can be compared across models and with the observed values for the 0-D model in column 4. These comparisons are the key to understanding the substantive meaning of the model.

Consider the pure type probabilities for the 1-D model in columns 6–7. For both sexes, the estimates for the “mild” pure type (Type I) generally indicated a higher than average (column 4) probability of a favorable response and a lower than average probability of an unfavorable response, whereas the reverse held for the “severe” pure type (Type II).

For Equivalent Institutional Care for males, the average probability of FTC was 38.5%, which dropped to 0.0% for Type I and increased to 90.9% for Type II. For Residence Status, the average probability of residing in a NH was 24.4% for males, which dropped to 0.0% for Type I and increased to 59.9% for Type II.

For the Dependence Scale for males, the average probability of a rating within Levels 4–5 was 36.9%, which dropped to 1.0% for Type I and increased to 81.6% for Type II. Level 4 included persons who had to be dressed, washed, and groomed; taken to the toilet regularly; or fed. Level 5 included persons who had to be turned, moved, or transferred; assisted with a diaper or catheter; or tube fed.

An important exception to the above generalization was the higher than average occurrence of Delusions for Type I, with probabilities of 48.9% for males and 53.7% for females, compared to the respective average probabilities of 37.3% and 39.7%. This pattern is consistent with prior reports from the Predictors Study that the prevalence of delusions peaked at the second year and then dropped.32 Note, however, that the 1-D model provides no mechanism for delusions to be predictive of a faster rate of progression of AD, despite reports of such effects, 33 since the rate of progression for all patients is constrained to that shown below in Web Figures 1–3.

Two other observations can be made with respect to differences between the sex-specific estimates for Type I in the 1-D model. For the Dependence Scale, the mode occurred at Level 2 for males and Level 3 for females, indicating that Type I females were more likely to need supervision. For the mMMS Score, the mode occurred at 40–57 for males and 30–39 for females, indicating that Type I females had poorer cognitive functioning.

The pure type probabilities for the 3-D model in columns 10–13 indicate, for both sexes, that the “mildest” pure type (Type I) generally had a higher than average (column 4) probability of a favorable response and a lower than average probability of an unfavorable response, whereas the reverse held for the “severest” pure type (Type IV).

The response probabilities for Types II and III were less extreme than for Types I and IV, consistent with the assumption that higher numbered pure types exhibited greater AD severity.

Comparisons with the corresponding results from the 1-D model in columns 6–7 show that the Type I results from the 3-D model were generally more favorable than the Type I results from the 1-D model; conversely, the Type IV results from the 3-D model were generally less favorable than the Type II results from the 1-D model. Thus, the 3-D model had a broader range of possible outcomes between the mildest and severest states than the 1-D model. This was important because it provided “room” in the state space to better represent the individual differences among individuals who were classified as “mild” in the 1-D model.

Trajectories of AD Progression

Web Figure 1 displays the estimated deterioration in AD health status as a function of time for the 1-D model, for persons who were initially at the highest level of health status among the Predictors 1 cohort (i.e., with a GoM score of 1 on Type I). The points on the plots are the leading diagonal elements of the Vt matrices, which quantify the cumulative progression of AD at each 6-month observation time. Females deteriorate more rapidly than males but the timing of the start and end of the decline in AD health status is similar. At 5 years, the AD health status score for females is less than half that for males.

Web Figures 2 and 3 present the individual trajectories of AD progression for the 1-D model, where each point is the first element of the corresponding GoM score vector, git (see eqn. 4). The plots in Web Figures 2 and 3 are bounded above by the sex-specific plots shown in Web Figure 1. The plots show that there was substantial heterogeneity in each study cohort at intake to the study (year 0) even though all of the participants were determined to have mild severity of AD at that time. The individual trajectories maintain constant proportionality with respect to each other over the entire duration of the process. This is the primary constraint imposed by the 1-D model.

Web Figures 4 and 5 present the individual trajectories of AD progression for the 3-D model, where each point is the sum of the first three elements of the corresponding GoM score vector, git. The plots show that there was less heterogeneity in each study cohort at intake to the study (year 0) in the 3-D than in the 1-D model (Figs. 2 and 3). The individual trajectories no longer maintain constant proportionality with respect to each other over the entire duration of the process. Instead, there is substantial heterogeneity in the rates of progression with some individuals reaching the most severe state in 2.5 years while others take up to 10+ years.

Predictors 2 Validation

The Vt matrices estimated from Predictors 1 were preferable to the Vt matrices estimated from Predictors 2 for both sexes for the 3-D model under the BIC criteria, using the following Model Forms:

  • F1

    Fix the λ- and u-parameters at the values estimated from Predictors 1; GoM scores were estimated from Predictors 2.

  • F2

    Fix the u-parameters at the values estimated from Predictors 1; GoM scores and λ-parameters were estimated from Predictors 2 (this is the preferred Model Form).

  • F3

    All parameters were independently estimated from Predictors 2.

The differences in log-likelihood function values between Model Forms 2 and 3 were 96.75 for males and 65.00 for females (90 df each). Based on these differences, both sets of BIC statistics strongly favored fixing the transition matrices at the values estimated from Predictors 1 for both sexes.††

The BIC comparisons between Model Forms 1 and 2 indicated that Form 2 was preferable. This means that the λ-parameters from Predictors 1 cannot be used for Predictors 2. Nonetheless, the ΔBICj statistics for 32/80 variables for males and 44/79 variables for females were negative in value, indicating that the Predictors 1 values would be acceptable for Predictors 2 in these cases.

Medicare Cost Estimates

Table 3 compares the direct medical care cost estimates derived from the Predictors 2 data with the Medicare cost estimates derived from the NLTCS using the transition parameters from Predictors 1. Predictors 1 provided no cost data, necessitating the use of some set of auxiliary procedures to obtain cost estimates like those in Table 3.

Table 3.

Cost Estimates (Average Cost per Year in Constant 2007 Dollars) from Predictors 2 and NLTCS based on Alternative Models Using Predictors 1 and/or Predictors 2 Transition Parameters

Pure Type
Correlations of Cost Vectors
Cost Item/Type Data and Transition Parameters N Observed Predicted I II III IV Chi-Squared df 1 2
Males
1. Direct Medical Care Predictors 2 Alone: Form 3 265 12,083 12,105 7,471 10,681 14,985 17,380 22.50 15 1.000 0.989 0.957
2. Direct Medical Care Predictors 2 with Predictors 1 Transitions: Form 2 265 12,083 12,115 7,432 10,228 14,472 18,797 23.16 15 0.989 1.000 0.96S
3. Medicare Payments NLTCS with Predictors 1 Transitions 583 11,387 11,451 5,674 10,832 12,269 17,549 119.58 51 0.957 0.969 1.00C
Females
1. Direct Medical Care Predictors 2 Alone: Form 3 349 10,662 10,660 6,641 6,308 12,483 20,589 45.49 15 1.000 0.999 0.79E
2. Direct Medical Care Predictors 2 with Predictors 1 Transitions: Form 2 349 10,662 10,645 6,929 6,024 12,331 21,545 46.12 15 0.999 1.000 0.764
3. Medicare Payments NLTCS with Predictors 1 Transitions 1,595 10,569 10,593 4,275 8,749 13,258 13,572 111.12 51 0.795 0.764 1.00C

Notes

Direct medical care included hospitalization, outpatient treatment and procedures, assistive devices, and medications. Costs were based on units of direct medical care services using nationally representative average payment rates for the various services, originally expressed in constant 2004 dollars using the medical care component of the Consumer Price Index, and converted to constant 2007 dollars using the same CPI series.

Medicare services included home health care, hospice care, skilled nursing facility care, and acute care and other services provided under Parts A and B of Medicare. Costs include the Medicare program payments and exclude deductibles and copayments.

The Predictors 2 estimates with and without use of the transition parameters from Predictors 1 were highly correlated (r = 0.99) across the four pure types, supporting the use of the Predictors 1 transitions to characterize the AD process in the NLTCS cost estimates.

The NLTCS costs for males were highly correlated (r = 0.96 each) with the Predictors 2 costs, but the costs for females were substantially less highly correlated (r = 0.76 and 0.80). For males, Type I had the lowest costs among the four pure types. For females, Type I had the lowest costs for the Medicare estimates but the second lowest for the direct medical care estimates obtained from the Predictors 2 data. This difference accounts for the lower female correlations between Medicare and Predictors 2 costs.

Applications

Our second objective was to employ the clinical model to estimate the impact of hypothetical modifications to progression rates on costs associated with Medicare and Medicaid services. This was done in two steps:

  • S1

    The transition parameters, ‡‡ probabilities of death (Tables 1 and 2), and cost estimates (Table 3) were used to project survival and costs over a 10-year period corresponding to the 10-year follow-up in Predictors 1. Table 4 displays the summary results for the 4 pure types.

  • S2
    The modifications to the AD progression rates were specified as delays in the start of the deterioration process. A delay was reasonably consistent with the patterns of deterioration shown in Web Figures 4 and 5. Two delays were considered:
    • A 3-year delay to approximate the largest gaps between the plots in Figs. 4 and 5.
    • A 9-month delay to approximate the size of delays that could be clinically significant.

    Tables 5 and 6 display the simulated interventions by sex.

Table 4.

Sex-Specific Baseline Projections of 10-Year Medicare Costs and Nursing Home (NH) Utilization Rates and Costs (in 2007 Dollars), 3-D GoM Model

Initial Pure Type Total Years Lived Years Lived in NH Discounted Medicare Costs Discounted Medicare Cost per Year Lived Discounted NH Costs Discounted NH Cost per Year Lived
Males
I 8.54 0.99 58,199 6,817 40,971 4,799
II 6.23 1.70 76,906 12,351 74,679 11,993
III 4.33 2.15 63,752 14,737 100,682 23,275
IV 2.93 2.42 51,949 17,724 119,027 40,610
Females
I 8.38 1.79 65,001 7,760 76,587 9,143
II 6.05 2.53 66,392 10,970 116,206 19,201
III 4.36 2.87 55,446 12,725 137,806 31,626
IV 4.20 2.61 54,845 13,055 125,584 29,893

Note: NH costs are fixed at the average Medicaid daily rate of $145 in 2007 dollars; costs are discounted at 3% per year.

Table 5.

Simulated Cost-Free Intervention Effects on 10-Year Medicare Costs and Nursing Home (NH) Utilization Rates and Costs (in 2007 Dollars), 3-D GoM Model, 9- Month and 36-Month Delays, Males

Initial Pure Type Total Years Lived Years Lived in NH Discounted Medicare Costs Discounted Medicare Cost per Year Lived Discounted NH Costs Discounted NH Cost per Year Lived
9-Month Delay
I 0.23 −0.29 −2,560 −471 −12,145 −1,511
II 0.45 −0.15 2,612 −440 −7,562 −1,940
III 0.43 −0.16 3,395 −618 −9,430 −4,086
IV 0.00 0.00 0 0 0 0
36-Month Delay
I 0.57 −0.87 −10,015 −1,526 −36,165 −4,271
II 1.53 −0.74 6,940 −1,537 −34,640 −6,829
III 1.51 −0.66 11,189 −1,906 −35,866 −12,177
IV 0.00 0.00 0 0 0 0

Note: NH costs are fixed at the average Medicaid daily rate of $145 in 2007 dollars; costs are discounted at 3% per year.

Table 6.

Simulated Cost-Free Intervention Effects on 10-Year Medicare Costs and Nursing Home (NH) Utilization Rates and Costs (in 2007 Dollars), 3-D GoM Model, 9- Month and 36-Month Delays, Females

Initial Pure Type Total Years Lived Years Lived in NH Discounted Medicare Costs Discounted Medicare Cost per Year Lived Discounted NH Costs Discounted NH Cost per Year Lived
9-Month Delay
I 0.36 −0.22 −2,173 −566 −10,184 −1,540
II 0.58 −0.10 3,252 −465 −6,655 −2,676
III 0.00 0.01 −1 −10 330 52
IV 0.00 0.00 0 0 0 0
36-Month Delay
I 1.09 −1.03 −11,543 −2,110 −45,644 −5,873
II 2.13 −0.53 10,758 −1,540 −29,871 −8,648
III 0.01 0.02 −10 −27 908 147
IV 0.00 0.00 0 0 0 0

Note: NH costs are fixed at the average Medicaid daily rate of $145 in 2007 dollars; costs are discounted at 3% per year.

The results indicated that large potential offsets for Medicare costs could occur for patients at the earliest stages of AD (Type I):

  • A 3-year delay in initial disease progression produced 10-year cumulative (discounted at 3%) Medicare cost offsets of $10,015 for males and $11,543 for females, and corresponding average annual offsets of $1,526 (males) and $2,110 (females).

  • A 9-month delay produced 10-year cost offsets of $2,560 (males) and $2,173 (females), and annual offsets of $471 (males) and $566 (females).

The results also indicated that large potential offsets for Medicaid NH costs could occur for patients at several stages of AD (Types I–III for males; Types I–II for females). For Type I:

  • A 3-year delay produced 10-year NH cost reductions of $36,165 (males) and $45,644 (females), and annual reductions of $4,271 (males) and $5,873 (females).

  • A 9-month delay produced 10-year NH cost reductions of $12,145 (males) and $10,184 (females), and annual reductions of $1,511 (males) and $1,540 (females).

The actual Federal Medicaid NH cost offsets would be smaller, because:

  • Approximately 50% of AD patients rely on Medicaid to pay all or part of their NH costs; 34 the Federal Government (CMS) pays about 60% of these costs with individual states paying varying balances in the range 24–50%35 and average costs within individual states ranging from 30% below to 40% above the national average cost.31

  • Thus, no more than 60% of the NH cost reductions could offset Federal Medicaid payments for AD patients on Medicaid, assuming that all such reductions would first apply to the Medicaid share of the NH payments. In this case, the marginal offsets for all AD patients would be close to 30% of the NH cost reductions in Tables 5 and 6.

Even with these downward adjustments, the Federal Medicaid NH cost offsets would still be comparable to the Medicare cost offsets (Type I).

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

The analysis has both substantive and methodological implications.

Substantively, the analysis provided new estimates of the clinical course of AD that accounted for initial heterogeneity of the patient population at the start of follow-up and differential patterns of deterioration of health status over the course of follow-up.

The analysis successfully incorporated multi-attribute measures of cognition, function, and behavior in a low-dimensional representation of AD progression.

The analysis ranked the top predictors in the following order: Dependence Scale, Equivalent Institutional Care, Blessed Dementia Rating Scale (Part 1), Residence Status, and mMMS.

The estimates suggested that there could be large cost offsets to Medicare from the slowing of disease progression among patients with mild AD and substantial cost offsets to Federal Medicaid payments for NH care from the slowing of disease progression among patients with both mild and moderate AD.

Methodologically, the longitudinal GoM model meets Eisdorfer's11 and Green's13 criteria that the model can represent combinations of multiple attributes including measures of cognitive functioning, functional ability, behavior and mood, and that it do so in a transparent way.

The approach represents a viable alternative to the standard Markov transition model – with simpler assumptions that are more closely satisfied. It differs from prior applications of the GoM model to cross-sectional AD data18 in that the longitudinal changes among individual AD patients are fully integrated into the model. Rather than representing individual AD patients as (random) points in a high-dimensional state space, the approach represents them as (random) trajectories in a low-dimensional state space.

The use of a low-dimensional state space in GoM was recommended by Wachter.36 Our innovation extended Wachter's recommendation to the low-dimensional state-space trajectories of longitudinal GoM with the 3-D dimensionality validated using two forms of the Bayesian information criterion, and with the transition parameters validated using a second, independent dataset (Predictors 2). The methodology appears applicable to the modeling of existing AD datasets. It may be sufficiently flexible to incorporate future AD-progression predictors, such as biomarkers and brain imaging technologies.

Our study had several limitations. The 506 cases in Predictors 1 and 2 were recruited at three sites in the northeastern U.S. using specific inclusion/exclusion criteria25 that may influence the generalizability of the results to other AD patients. Sex differences in the transition matrices and outcome probabilities were identified but not modeled further. For example, the use of nursing homes and other paid LTC services was higher for females than males, in part, because of the higher probability of lack of a spouse to provide care for widowed females. There are other important fixed variables that are already in (e.g., demographics) or could be added to (e.g., APOE genotype) the model that need to be further evaluated. The transition matrices in the current application were estimated separately for each observation interval, creating “jumps” in the trajectories that could be eliminated by smoothing the trajectories or graduating the transition matrices.

Our study was both exploratory and confirmatory. We successfully described AD progression as a 3-D process, validated that description on an independent dataset, and provided strong evidence that AD is not a 1-D or 2-D process, but we did not prove that AD is truly a 3-D process. Although the biological mechanisms underlying AD progression should be consistent with a 3-D process, better understanding of those mechanisms may reveal a substantially more complex process.

Supplementary Material

01

Acknowledgments

Sources of Support The primary financial support for this study was provided by a grant jointly funded by Elan Pharmaceuticals and Wyeth Pharmaceuticals. Funding for supplemental analysis of the National Long Term Care Survey was provided by the National Institute on Aging, through Grant Nos. R01-AG028259 and R01-AG030612. The Predictors Study is supported by a grant to Dr. Stern from the National Institute on Aging: Grant No. R01-AG07370.

The funding agreements ensured the authors' independence in designing the study, interpreting the data, writing, and publishing the report.

The following author is employed by a primary sponsor: A.S. Zbrozek, Wyeth Pharmaceuticals.

The helpful comments of Trent McLaughlin, PhD, Elan Pharmaceuticals, on this manuscript are acknowledged. David L. Straley provided programming support at Duke University.

Footnotes

*

For simplicity, all continuous variables are assumed to be recoded to discrete categorical variables prior to the analysis.

In fact, BIC1 and BIC2 yielded identical model selection decisions for all analyses in this paper.

16 cases in Predictors 1 had an initial mMMS score in the range 21–29; 10 cases in Predictors 2 had an initial MMSE score in the range 9–15. These cases were retained in the analysis because GoM generates scores for each

§

Supplementary tables with log-likelihood values from eqn. (2), corresponding AIC, BIC1, BIC2, and CAIC statistics, and extensive sets of parameter estimates are provided online in a web-only format for interested readers.

**

See Table A2 in the supplementary online material.

††

See Table A5 in the supplementary online material.

‡‡

The transition matrices for the sex-specific 3-D models are reported in Tables A6 and A7 in the supplementary online material where they were combined with the probabilities of death to generate 10-year life tables for Type I.

Life tables for the other pure types were computed similarly.

individual independently.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Sonnenberg FA, Beck JR. Markov models in medical decision making: A practical guide. Med Decis Making. 1993;13(4):322–338. doi: 10.1177/0272989X9301300409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Neumann PJ, Hermann RC, Kuntz KM, et al. Cost-effectiveness of donepezil in the treatment of mild or moderate Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1999;52(6):1138–1146. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.6.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Neumann PJ, Araki SS, Arcelus A, et al. Measuring Alzheimer's disease progression with transition probabilities: Estimates from CERAD. Neurology. 2001;57(6):957–964. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.6.957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kuntz KM, Goldie SJ. Assessing the sensitivity of decision-analytic results to unobserved markers of risk: Defining the effects of heterogeneity bias. Med Decis Making. 2002;22(3):218–227. doi: 10.1177/0272989X0202200310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stern Y, Tang MX, Albert MS, et al. Predicting time to nursing home care and death in individuals with Alzheimer's disease. JAMA. 1997;277(10):806–812. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Caro JJ, Getsios D, Migliaccio-Walle K, Raggio G, Ward A. Assessment of health economics in Alzheimer's disease (AHEAD) based on need for full-time care. Neurology. 2001;57(6):964–971. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.6.964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Getsios D, Migliaccio-Walle K, Caro JJ. NICE Cost-Effectiveness Appraisal of Cholinesterase Inhibitors: Was the Right Question Posed? Were the Best Tools Used? Pharmacoeconomics. 2007;25(12):997–1006. doi: 10.2165/00019053-200725120-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Crook T. The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139(9):1136–1139. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.9.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dooneief G, Marder K, Tang MX, Stern Y. The Clinical Dementia Rating scale: Community-based validation of `profound' and `terminal' stages. Neurology. 1996;46:1746–1749. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.6.1746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Eisdorfer C, Cohen D, Paveza GJ, et al. An empirical evaluation of the Global Deterioration Scale for staging Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149(2):190–194. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stern Y, Liu X, Albert M, et al. Application of a growth curve approach to modeling the progression of Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontol Med Sci. 1996;51A(4):M179–M184. doi: 10.1093/gerona/51a.4.m179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Green C. Modelling disease progression in Alzheimer's disease: A review of modelling methods used for cost-effectiveness analysis. Pharmacoeconomics. 2007;25(9):735–750. doi: 10.2165/00019053-200725090-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Alzheimer's Association . ICAD 2008 Press Release. Chicago: Jul 29, 2008. Markers in blood and spinal fluid, and a new imaging agent, show promise for early detection of Alzheimer's. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kinosian BP, Stallard E, Lee JH, Woodbury MA, Zbrozek AS, Glick HA. Predicting 10-year care requirements for older people with suspected Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(6):631–638. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb04720.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stallard E. Trajectories of morbidity, disability, and mortality among the U.S. elderly population: Evidence from the 1984–1999 NLTCS. North American Actuarial Journal. 2007;11(3):16–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Woodbury MA, Clive J. Clinical pure types as a fuzzy partition. Journal of Cybernetics. 1974;4(3):111–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fillenbaum GG, Woodbury MA. Typology of Alzheimer's disease: Findings from CERAD data. Aging & Mental Health. 1998;2(2):105–127. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Erosheva EA. Comparing latent structures of the Grade of Membership, Rasch, and Latent Class models. Psychometrika. 2005;70(4):619–628. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ramsay JO. A geometrical approach to item response theory. Behaviormetrika. 1996;23(1):3–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics. 1978;6(2):461–464. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Raftery A. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:111–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Akaike H. Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In: Petrov BN, Csaki F, editors. Second International Symposium on Information Theory. Akademiai Kiado; Budapest: 1973. pp. 267–281. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bozdogan H. Model selection and Akaike's information criterion (AIC): The general theory and its analytical extensions. Psychometrika. 1987;52(3):345–370. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Stern Y, Folstein M, Albert M, et al. Multicenter study of predictors of disease course in Alzheimer disease (the “Predictors Study”). I. Study design, cohort description, and intersite comparisons. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1993;7(1):3–21. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199307010-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Zhu CW, Scarmeas N, Torgan R, et al. Clinical features associated with costs in early AD: Baseline data from the Predictors Study. Neurology. 2006;66(7):1021–1028. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000204189.18698.c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stern Y, Sano M, Paulson J, Mayeux R. Modified mini-mental state examination: Validity and reliability. Neurology. 1987;37(Suppl 1):179. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M. The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry. 1968;114(512):797–811. doi: 10.1192/bjp.114.512.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Stern Y, Albert SM, Sano M, et al. Assessing patient dependence in Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontol. 1994;49(5):M216–M222. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.5.m216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sarazin M, Stern Y, Berr C, et al. Neuropsychological predictors of dependency in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 2005;64(6):1027–1031. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000154529.53488.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Grabowski DC, Feng Z, Intrator O, Mor V. Recent trends in state nursing home payment policies. Health Affairs Suppl Web Exclusives. 2004;W4:363–373. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.w4.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Holtzer R, Tang MX, Devanand DP, et al. Psychopathological features in Alzheimer's disease: Course and relationship with cognitive status. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(7):953–960. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2389.2003.51308.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Scarmeas N, Brandt J, Albert, et al. Delusions and hallucinations are associated with worse outcome in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2005;62(10):1601–1608. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.10.1601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Alzheimer's Association . 2008 Alzheimer's Disease Facts and Figures. Alzheimer's Association; Washington, DC: 2008. p. 27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services . Medicaid At-a-Glance 2005. Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services; Baltimore, MD: 2005. Pub No. CMS-11024-05. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wachter KW. Grade of Membership models in low dimensions. Statistical Papers. 1999;40(4):439–457. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES