Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 May 20.
Published in final edited form as: Cell Rep. 2015 May 7;11(7):1043–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.020

GABA-A Inhibition Shapes the Spatial and Temporal Response Properties of Purkinje Cells in the Macaque Cerebellum

Pablo M Blazquez 1, Tatyana A Yakusheva 1
PMCID: PMC4439296  NIHMSID: NIHMS680992  PMID: 25959822

SUMMARY

Data from in-vitro and anesthetized preparations indicate that inhibition plays a major role in cerebellar cortex function. We investigated the role of GABA-A inhibition in the macaque cerebellar ventral-paraflocculus while animals performed oculomotor behaviors that are known to engage the circuit. We recorded Purkinje cell responses to these behaviors with and without application of gabazine, a GABA-A receptor antagonist, near the recorded neuron. Gabazine increased the neuronal responsiveness to saccades in all directions, and the neuronal gain to VOR cancellation and pursuit, most significantly the eye and head velocity sensitivity. L-glutamate application indicated that these changes were not the consequence of increases in baseline firing rate. Importantly, gabazine did not affect behavior or efference copy suggesting that only local computations were disrupted. Our data, collected while the cerebellum performs behaviorally relevant computations, indicate that inhibition is a potent regulatory mechanism to control input-output gain and spatial tuning in cerebellar cortex.

Keywords: Motor control, cerebellum, floccular complex, GABA, GABA-A receptors


graphic file with name nihms680992f8.jpg

INTRODUCTION

Inhibition is ubiquitous in the cerebellar cortex, where all interneurons with the exception of granule and unipolar brush cells are inhibitory. Evidence from in-vitro and anesthetized preparations suggest that GABAergic inhibition plays a key role in the computations carried out by the cerebellar cortex. For example, application of bicuculline, a potent GABA-A antagonist, disrupts the normal pattern of activation in cerebellar cortex following electrical stimulation of parallel fibers or the vibrissal pads (Gao et al. 2006). Tonic and spillover inhibition regulates the number of granule cells responsive to mossy fiber inputs and modulates the gain (slope of input-output relationship) of granule cells (Mitchell and Silver, 2003).

A critical aspect of cerebellar cortex function is its role in motor learning (Eccles et al. 1967). Traditionally cerebellar learning has been associated with LTD and LTP at the parallel fiber to Purkinje cell (PC) synapse (De Zeeuw et al. 1998; Schonewille et al. 2010), however emerging evidence indicate that local inhibitory interneurons are also capable of LTP and LTD (Jirenheld et al. 2013; Jorntell and Ekerot 2002). Indeed, the current view is that cerebellar learning involves plasticity at multiple sites within the cerebellar network (D’Angelo 2014). For instance, changes in eye and head velocity sensitivity of PCs in the ventral paraflocculus (VPFL) following vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) learning (Lisberger 1994) may be partially attributed to changes in the excitability of inhibitory interneurons.

To understand the role of inhibition in cerebellar cortex function it is imperative to study inhibition in the behaving animal; i.e., while the cerebellar circuit performs behaviorally relevant computations. Here, we studied the effect of blocking GABA-A receptors on the response of PCs in the macaque VPFL during oculomotor behaviors. The VPFL participates in the generation of eye movements during visual-vestibular stimulation (Rambold et al. 2002). VPFL mossy fiber inputs arriving from the brainstem carry efference copy, sensory information (retinal slip and vestibular), and perhaps proprioceptor information from extraocular muscles (Donaldson 2000; Lisberger 1994). VPFL PCs project to premotor neurons (Langer et al. 1985, Escudero et al. 1996; Lisberger 1994). The strong efferent copy input to VPFL creates a powerful feedback loop (cerebellum-brainstem) that is responsible for the maintenance of pursuit behavior (Lisberger 1994). Here, we blocked GABA-A receptors with SR95531 (gabazine), a potent GABA-A antagonist. Our results strongly suggest that GABA-A inhibition is necessary to confer the spatial response tuning and response gain of PCs, and that regulation of inhibition could be a potent mechanism for cerebellar learning.

RESULTS

We investigated the role of inhibition in cerebellar cortex function by making minute injections of gabazine near a PC and evaluating how it affected the responses of the PC during oculomotor behavior. We first quantified the spread of drug in nervous tissue.

Spread of gabazine in tissue (anesthetized mouse)

Figure 1A shows our experimental approach. We injected GABA pulses at the recording site, while measuring the neuronal response (i.e., decrease in activity following each GABA application). Next, we injected gabazine at a certain distance from the recording site. If the spread of gabazine overlaps with the region affected by GABA (Figure 1A-left), the neuronal responses to GABA pulses would decrease during gabazine application. However, if there is no overlap (Figure 1A-right), there would be little or no effect on the neuronal response to GABA. Importantly, GABA does not spread far in tissue when using standard iontophoretic techniques (<20 µm, Herz et al. 1969).

Figure 1. Spread of gabazine in the cerebellar cortex of mice.

Figure 1

(A) Experimental approach used to measure the spread of drug in tissue. An injecting electrode delivered pulses of GABA at the recording site. Next, gabazine was injected using constant current through a second electrode located far from the recording site. Left and right panels show cartoons and the expected effects on firing rate (F.rate) when there was overlap between the spread of GABA and gabazine (left), and when there was no overlap (right).

(B) Example cells (left and right) recorded using the electrode configurations shown in pictures (insets on top). The distance between the gabazine injection and recording electrode tips was 40 and 440 µm for the example cell 1 and 2, respectively. In center, raster plot of spikes showing the neuronal response to pulses of GABA (+40 nA, 5 s duration). Time 0 on the X axis represents the onset of each GABA pulse, and time zero on the Y axes the onset of Gabazine application. The red box indicates the period when gabazine was injected (+50 and +100 nA for example cell 1 and 2, respectively). For clarity, only one of every 10 trials and one of every 5 trials are shown for example cells 1 and 2, respectively. In bottom, average response to GABA pulses (includes all trials) before (blue), during (red), and after application of gabazine (cyan). Blue, red and cyan lines indicate the periods before, during and after gabazine application, respectively.

(C) Quantification of the response to pulses of GABA for the cells shown in B. Each point represents the neuronal response to GABA in each of the trials shown in the raster: 0% corresponds to no changes in firing rate following GABA application, and 100% corresponds to full inhibition following GABA application (no spikes). Blue, red and cyan lines indicate the periods before, during and after gabazine application, respectively.

(D) Population summary. Each dot represents data from a single neuron. Top, average response to GABA (%) during the last minute of gabazine injection (includes several GABA pulses, see C) vs. distance between recording and gabazine injection site. Bottom, time to reach 80% of gabazine effect, calculated using a fitting function over the data shown in C (see methods), vs. distance between recording and gabazine injection sites. Filled circles and black fitting line represent the data obtained using +50 nA injection of gabazine, and empty circles and gray fitting line represent data obtained using +100 nA injection of gabazine.

(E) Cartoon that illustrates the area affected by gabazine application in the macaque VPFL. Purkinje cell (PC), molecular layer interneurons, Golgi cells and granule cells are in red, blue, green and black, respectively. GL, granular layer; ML, molecular layer; PL, PC layer. The thickness of ML and GL were estimated using the atlas of Paxinos et al. 2000.

We built electrode assemblies consisting of a single capillary glass glued to a three barrel carbon fiber electrode (see photographs in Figure 1B). The capillary glass contained gabazine (10 mM, in 0.16 M NaCl, pH 4.0). The three barrel electrode contained a carbon fiber (5µm thick) in one barrel, GABA (200 mM in distilled water) in a second barrel, and NaCl (160 mM) for current compensation in the third barrel. Figure 1B-left and Figure 1C-left show the response of a cerebellar cortex neuron recorded with an electrode assembly built with a separation of 40 µm between the tip of the multibarrel electrode and the tip of the capillary glass. For this neuron, injection of gabazine (+50 nA) cancelled the neuronal response to GABA within the first minute of gabazine injection. The neuron shown in Figure 1B-right and Figure 1C-right, which was recorded using an electrode assembly with separation of 440 µm, did not modify its response to GABA pulses during gabazine application (+100 nA). This indicates that the volume of tissue affected by gabazine did not overlap with the region near the recording electrode.

We recorded 31 neurons from the cerebellar cortex of 8 adult mice using various assembly spacings. The effect of gabazine application on the neuronal responses to GABA decreased as the separation between the recording site and the gabazine injecting site increased (Figure 1D, top). Injection currents of +100 nA produced larger spread of gabazine than injection currents of +50 nA (filled and empty symbols in Figure 1D, top). Gabazine had an almost immediate effect on the neuronal response to GABA for separation distances smaller than 50 µm. For larger separation distances, the effect, if any, could take up to 4 minutes before reaching near asymptotic values (>80% of full effect on responses to GABA pulses, Figure 2D, bottom). The effective spread of gabazine in brain tissue using our delivery method was reduced by more than 80% for separation distances larger than 230 and 420 µm when using gabazine injection current injection of +50 and +100nA, respectively. Additionally, for any given cell that showed changes in its response to GABA application during gabazine injection, the effect of gabazine approached plateau (>90%) within the first 4 minutes of injection.

Figure 2. Response of VPFL PCs during saccades with and without gabazine application.

Figure 2

(A) Response of three example PCs to saccade before and during gabazine application. Each panel shows the average eye position (deg) and neuronal response (spk/s) during upwards (top), downwards (bottom), leftwards (left) and rightwards (right) saccades. Blue traces show data collected before drug application (retention period), red traces, data collected during gabazine application (injection period), and cyan trace, data collected after gabazine application (recovery period). Note that all types of PCs: OFF (cell1), OFF/ON (cell2) and ON (cell 3) become ON-only after gabazine injections (see also Figure S2 and S3).

(B) Distribution of saccade response types in VPFL PCs during retention and injection periods.

(C) Changes in the directional tuning of individual PCs with gabazine application. Only cells recorded during at least two periods are illustrated. Following the color code in A, blue, red and cyan shown data collected during the retention (Ret), injection (Gbz) and recovery (Rec) periods, respectively. Thin black lines join data collected from the same neuron and think gray lines show the average from the corresponding neurons.

(D) Histogram showing the absolute differences in preferred direction of PCs recorded with and without drug application.

See also Figure S2

Next, we present data collected in the macaque VPFL using injection currents of +50 nA. We use the above data obtained in mouse cerebellar tissue as proxy for the spread of gabazine in our macaque experiments because: 1-Our macaque electrodes had tip sizes similar to those used to measure the spread of gabazine in mice brain tissue (about 1–3 µm opening, see Inagaki et. al. 2009); 2- The three layers of the cerebellar cortex are morphological and neurochemically identical in mammals (Eccles et al., 1967); 3- The effective spread of gabazine was lower than the average thickness of the molecular and granular layers of mouse lobe IV–VI (0–1 mm lateral to the midline, mean of 300 and 270 µm, respectively; atlas of Franklin and Paxinos 2007) and monkey VPFL (mean of 345 and 428 µm, respectively; atlas of Paxinos, Huang & Toga, 2000). For all the above we argue that the effective spread of gabazine in our macaque experiments covered about two thirds of the molecular layer and about half of the granule cell layer (Figure 1E).

Gabazine application increases PC burst responses to saccades, and eliminates or inverts inhibitory responses

VPFL PCs respond to saccadic eye movements with increases in firing rate, decreases in firing rate, or both (named here as ON responses, OFF responses and ON/OFF responses, respectively [see Experimental Procedures]). In our control PC population recorded using tungsten electrodes (n=70), most PCs were ON/OFF neurons (63%, n=44/70, Figure S1), and the average maximum and minimum saccade response amplitude was 70.8 and −32.4 spk/s, respectively. This indicates that, as a population, VPFL PCs show ON/OFF saccade responses.

Figure 2A shows the response of three example PCs recorded during retention (blue traces) and injection (red traces) of gabazine (see also Figure S2). These PCs were classified as OFF (left), ON/OFF (center) and ON (right) neurons based on their response to saccades during the retention period. Interestingly, during gabazine application all three neurons changed to ON neurons. Perhaps, the most remarkable change is that of example cells 1 and 2, where saccade directions that generated clear OFF responses during the retention period switched to strong ON responses during gabazine application. Furthermore, all three example neurons changed their directional preference with gabazine application (compare the direction of the blue and red arrows at the center graph). The cell shown to the right was also recorded during the recovery period (cyan). During the recovery period the burst associated with saccades decreased, although it remained larger than that observed during the retention period.

Most PCs recorded during the retention period were ON/OFF neurons (72%, 13/18, Figure 2B top pie chart), however most PCs recorded during the injection period were ON neurons (92%, 22/24, Figure 2C bottom pie chart). The maximum neuronal response increased about 61.4% during gabazine application (mean, 119 vs. 192.1 spk/s for retention vs. injection periods, respectively), while the minimal response amplitude took positive values during the injection period (−35.1 vs. 94.7 spk/s for retention vs. injection periods, respectively). The directional tuning of the neuronal response decreased during gabazine application for all neurons classified as ON/OFF during the retention period (n=9, p=0.003, Wilcoxon sign rank test, Figure 2C left), and tended to increase during the recovery period (n=2). The decrease in the neuronal tuning was accompanied by an increase in the tuning width (mean, 36.2 vs. 110.2 spk/s for retention and injection periods, respectively; p=0.0099, Wilcoxon sign rank test, see also methods and Figure S1). We found no changes, or increases, in spatial tuning for ON and OFF neurons (Figure 2D right, albeit the low n). Lastly, as shown in Figure 2A for the example neurons, gabazine application changed the saccade preferred direction of PCs; often the preferred direction shifted near 180 degrees (Figure 2D and see center polar graphs in A), but has no consistent effect in the neuronal response latency (p=0.34, Wilcoxon rank test).

Importantly, microinjections of gabazine at the current and concentration used in this study had no effect on saccade latencies, and position errors (Figure S3, ANOVA p>>0.05).

Gabazine increases PC responses to pursuit and VOR cancellation

The canonical VPFL PC carries ipsilateral or down eye velocity information, and ipsilateral or down head velocity information (Blazquez et a. 2003; Lisberger 1994). Figure 3 shows an example PC recorded during sinusoidal pursuit (A), and VOR cancellation (B) before, during and after gabazine application (blue, red and cyan, respectively). Gabazine injection increased the amplitude of modulation for both pursuit and VOR cancellation (from 40.4 to 64 spk/s for pursuit, and from 11.6 to 27.8 spk/s for VOR cancellation), with small changes in phase (from 24.4 to 15.8 deg for pursuit with respect to eye velocity, and from −8 to +10.9 deg for VOR cancellation with respect to head velocity). During the recovery period the neuronal modulation during pursuit decreased toward preinjection values (49 spk/s).

Figure 3. Response of VPFL PCs during sinusoidal pursuit and VOR cancellation with and without gabazine application.

Figure 3

(A) Response to pursuit of an example VPFL PCs recorded before (blue), during (red) and after (cyan) gabazine application. Left panel, top traces show the eye position and bottom traces the neuronal response. Right panel, firing rate (F.rate, spk/s) is plotted versus eye velocity (Eye Vel., deg/s).

(B) Response to VOR cancellation of an example VPFL PCs recorded before (blue) and during (red) gabazine application. Left panel, top traces show the behavioral response and table position (black dashed line). Left panel, bottom traces show the neuronal response. Right panel, firing rate (spk/s) is plotted versus head velocity (Head Vel., deg/s).

(C) Left, bar graph illustrating the population data recorded during sinusoidal pursuit. Right, single cell data collected during more than one period. Retention (blue, Ret), injection (red, Gbz) and recovery (cyan, Rec) periods are shown. Thin black lines join data collected from the same neuron and think gray lines show the average from the corresponding neurons. Asterisks indicate significance (*0.05<p<0.01 and **p<0.01). Data are represented as mean ± STD. Abbreviations: Tungs, population recorded with tungsten electrodes; Ret, Gbz and Rec, population recorded with multibarrels electrodes during the retention, injection and recovery periods, respectively.

(D) Same as C but for VOR cancellation.

(E) Comparison of the amplitude of modulation of PCs during sinusoidal pursuit in the preferred (left) and non-preferred (right) orientations. Only neurons recorded during vertical and horizontal sinusoidal pursuit with and without gabazine application are shown. Black thin lines connect data from the same neuron. Think gray lines show the average from the corresponding neurons. Numbers 1 and 2 indicate the example neurons shown in Figure S5.

See also Figure S5.

Figure 3C and 3D illustrate the population data. The neuronal response amplitude for pursuit was larger during the injection period than during the retention period (mean ± STD, 64.9 ± 24 vs. 42.2 ± 14 spk/s, p=0.01, Mann Whitney U test), and larger than that found with tungsten electrode recordings (31.46 ± 12.6 spk/s, p=0.00003, Mann Whitney U test) (Figure 3C, left). This change was significant in cell-by-cell comparisons (p=0.025, Wilcoxon sign rank test; Figure 3C, right). Similar results were found for PCs recorded during VOR cancellation. PCs increased their amplitude of modulation with gabazine application (17.2 ± 13.4 [n=31], 13.4 ± 8.9 [n=8], and 25.4 ± 12.7 [n=6] spk/s for the tungsten, retention, and injection population, respectively). These changes showed marginal significance, perhaps due to the low n (p=0.043 and p=0.046 for gabazine vs. tungsten and gabazine vs. retention population, respectively, Mann Whitney U test) (Figure 3D left). Moreover, the four cells recorded during both periods, retention and gabazine injection, increased the modulation with gabazine application (Figure 3D right). In summary, we found that gabazine application increased the amplitude of modulation during pursuit and VOR cancellation by 66.5% (n=8) and 70.6% (n=4), respectively. When modulation is expressed as sensitivities, we observed an increase in eye velocity sensitivity (mean, 1.8 vs. 2.7 spk/s/deg/s [retention vs. gabazine], p=0.036, Wilcoxon rank test), and, marginally, head velocity sensitivity (mean, 0.77 vs. 0.21 spk/s/deg/s [retention vs. gabazine], p=0.068, probably because the low n, Wilcoxon rank test). There were no changes in the neuronal response phase (p>0.24, Wilcoxon rank test) (Figure S4). Remarkably, although the neuronal responses to pursuit in the preferred orientation (i.e., horizontal or vertical) increased, the neuronal responses to pursuit in the non-preferred orientation (i.e., vertical or horizontal) were not affected by gabazine application (Figure 3E; n=7, p=0.75, Wilcoxon rank test) (Figure S5).

To measure eye position and eye acceleration related neuronal discharge we studied the neuronal responses to step ramp pursuit towards the neuronal preferred direction. The two example neurons in Figure 4A showed a clear increase in their response during gabazine application, which could be described in terms of changes in neuronal sensitivities to eye movement parameters. Individual neurons recorded with gabazine application tended to increase their eye position, eye velocity and eye acceleration sensitivities, although this was significant only for the eye velocity component (Figure 4B bottom, p<0.003, Wilcoxon sign rank test). The same was true at the population level; eye position, velocity and acceleration sensitivities increased with gabazine application, but only increases in eye velocity sensitivity were significant (p<0.0002, Mann Whitney U test; Figure 4B).

Figure 4. Response of VPFL PCs during step ramp pursuit with and without gabazine application.

Figure 4

(A) Response of two example VPFL PCs recorded before (blue), during (red) and after (cyan in example cell 1) gabazine application. Top traces show the behavioral responses (deg) and bottom traces the neuronal responses (spk/s).

(B) Top panels show bar graphs illustrating the changes in eye position, velocity, and acceleration sensitivity of the population data (left, center and right, respectively). Bottom panels show data from individual cells. Retention (blue, Ret), injection (red, Gbz) and recovery (cyan, Rec) periods are shown. Thin black lines join data collected from the same neuron, and think gray lines show the average from the corresponding neurons. Asterisk indicates significance (* 0.05<p<0.01).

See also Figure S6.

Microinjections of gabazine at the current and concentration used in this study had no effect on pursuit behavior (latency and velocity error, Figure S6, ANOVA p>>0.05).

Gabazine has no effect on the efference copy of motor commands

A large proportion of mossy fibers entering the VPFL provide an efference copy the motor command to the cerebellar cortex. This signal is used to help maintain pursuit and generate predictions of the current motor state (Ghasia et al. 2008). To investigate if gabazine injections affect the efference copy signal, we compared the responses of eye movement related mossy fibers, which are thought to carry the efference copy signal of the eye movement, with and without gabazine application. Figure 5A shows an example mossy fiber recorded during spontaneous eye movements (see methods and Heiney et al, 2010; Laurens et al, 2013, for description of how to identify mossy fibers). The eye position sensitivity of this mossy fiber, calculated as the slope of the fitting line relating mean eye position during fixation and mean firing rate, did not change during gabazine injection (slopes of blue vs. red lines in Figure 5A, right). This was true for all recorded mossy fibers (n=4, Figure 5B), suggesting that the efference copy pathway was not affected by our experimental manipulation. Note that during pursuit of a laser in the dark the efferent copy information is arguably the major signal driving the response of VPFL PCs because retinal slip is minimal, and the macaque oculomotor system notably lacks of propiorecptors typically found in skeletal musculature (i.e., muscle spindles, Ruskell, 1999). Moreover, the response profile of the mossy fibers shown here resembles that of prepositus hipoclossi neurons, a major source of efferent copy signal to VPFL (Escudero et al. 1996).

Figure 5. Gabazine application does not affect the efference copy information.

Figure 5

(A) Example of instantaneous mossy fibers discharge (IFrate) during spontaneous eye movements (Eye, eye position). Top and bottom show the response of the same mossy fibers before and during gabazine injection, respectively. Right panel shows the method used to calculate the sensitivity of mossy fibers to eye position. The sensitivity corresponds to the slope of the fitting line (blue for data collected before gabazine application, and red for data collected during gabazine application).

(B) Sensitivity of each recorded mossy fiber (n=4) before and during gabazine application.

Gabazine increases PC simple spike discharge but does not affect the spike regularity or the complex spike discharge

We found no differences in DC firing rate, calculated as the mean firing rate during center fixation, between PCs recorded with tungsten electrodes (n=120) and multibarrel electrodes during the retention period (n=13, p>0.084, Mann–Whitney U test; Figure 6A left). However, DC firing rate increased during gabazine injection (n=13 retention vs. n=11 injection, mean ± STD, 74 ± 26.3 vs. 102.6 ± 30.7 spk/s, p=0.0075, Mann-Whitney U test; Figure 6A left). This difference became more pronounced when comparisons were made in a cell by cell basis (p=0.0037, Wilcoxon sign rank test; Figure 6A right). During the recovery period, the DC firing rate decreased relative to the injection period (126 ± 26 and 101 ± 18 spk/s, respectively, p=0.043 Wilcoxon sign rank test; Figure 6A right). The CV2 was not affected by gabazine injection (p=0.155, Wilcoxon sign rank test; Figure 6B), indicating that there was no effect on spike regularity.

Figure 6. Effects of gabazine application on the discharge properties of VPFL PCs.

Figure 6

(A) Left, comparison of simple spikes DC firing rate of VPFL PCs recorded with tungsten electrodes (black, Tungs), with multibarrel electrodes during the retention period (blue, Ret), injection period (red, Gbz) and recovery period (cyan, Rec). Right, data from single neurons recorded with multibarrel electrodes during the retention, injection or recovery period. Only data recorded during at least two periods is shown. Thin black lines join data collected from the same neuron and think gray lines show the average from the corresponding neurons. Asterisks indicate significance (* 0.05<p<0.01 and ** p<0.01).

(B) Same as A for CV2.

(C–D) Same as A and B for complex spikes.

Injection of gabazine did not affect the DC firing rate or CV2 of complex spike discharge (Figure 6C and D; p=0.86, and p=0.62 for DC firing rate and CV2, respectively, Mann–Whitney U test; Figure 6C and 7D left panels). This was also true at the individual cell level (p=0.74, and p=0.49 for DC firing rate and CV2, respectively, Wilcoxon sign rank test; Figure 6C and 6D right panels).

Figure 7. Effect of increasing DC firing with L-gluatmate on the neuronal response to sinusoidal pursuit.

Figure 7

(A) Top, eye position. Bottom, neuronal firing rate. Example cell 1 (left) shows the response of a neuron while retaining (lower trace, +20 nA) and injecting (upper trace, −25 nA) L-glutamate. Example cell 2 (right) shows the response of a neuron while retaining (lower trace, +20 nA), and injecting (middle and upper trace, −20 and −60 nA, respectively) L-glutamate.

(B) Amplitude of modulation to sinusoidal pursuit vs. DC firing rate for the six PCs recorded with L-glutamate injection. Different DC firing rate values were obtained for different L-Glutamate current injections for each cell. Different symbols show data collected from different neurons. Dashed lines connected data from the same neuron. Straight lines show the fitting lines to data collected from individual neurons. Numbers 1 and 2 indicate example neurons 1 and 2, respectively.

See also Figure S7

Changes in PC responses during gabazine application are not due to changes in baseline firing rate

We used two methods to investigate whether the changes observed in PC response to pursuit and VOR cancellation during gabazine application could be a direct consequence of increasing the neuronal DC firing rate. First, we divided the PC population recorded with tungsten electrodes into three groups based on their DC firing rate (Group I <70 spk/s; Group II 70 – 90 spk/s; and Group III >90 spk/s). In all groups, the dominant type of saccade response corresponded to the ON/OFF type (58%, 66% and 80% for Groups I, II and III, respectively), and there was no relation between amplitude of modulation during sinusoidal pursuit or VOR cancellation and DC firing rate (Figure S7A and S7B; p>0.24, and p>0.85 for pursuit and VOR cancellation, respectively, Mann Whitney U test).

We recorded the responses of 6 PCs to sinusoidal pursuit before and during tonic application of L-glutamate. L-glutamate increased the DC firing rate in all six PCs (the more L-Glutamate we injected the larger the DC firing rate) but caused no consistent changes in the amplitude of modulation during Pursuit (Figure 7A and B, see fitting lines for individual cells). Our data indicate that the response changes observed during gabazine application are not the result of increases in DC firing rate.

DISCUSSION

We investigated the role of GABA-A receptor mediated inhibition in the computations performed by the macaque VPFL using a finely targeted pharmacological and neurophysiological approach. Specifically, we compared the responses of VPFL PCs in alert primates during oculomotor behaviors before and during application of minute amounts of gabazine. Our drug application only affected processes or signal transformations taking place within a small volume of the cerebellar cortex because 1) The effective spread of the drug was less than 250 µm (Figure 1) and 2) The efference copy of the eye movement, a major input signal to VPFL, did not change with drug application. We found that gabazine increased the response amplitude of PCs to saccades, changed their directional preference, and decreased their spatial tuning. We also found that gabazine increased the neuronal response to pursuit and VOR cancellation. Our results suggest that GABA-A inhibition is an important mechanism to regulate the gain and directional preference of cerebellar cortical output neurons.

Characteristics of the experimental manipulation

The effective spread of gabazine was less than the average thickness of the VPFL molecular and granular layers, and corresponds to less than 1% of the total volume of the structure (6–9 mm3 folia V-X of the flocculus complex) (Paxino et al. 2000, Rambold et al. 2002). Functionally, the VPFL is divided into 3 sagittal zones, two related to vertical eye movements and one related to horizontal eye movements (Sato and Kawasaki 1990). These zones project to different areas of the vestibular nuclei and receive inputs from different portions of the inferior olive (Sato and Kawasaki 1991). Our gabazine application did not modify the information carried by mossy fibers (efference copy and proprioceptive pathways), nor the activity of the olivocerebellar pathway (complex spikes), suggesting that we affected only local computations.

Because our drug application does not target specific cell types our data cannot provide insights into the mechanisms or neuronal types responsible for the changes in PC responses demonstrated here. However, the data demonstrate that regulation of inhibition shapes PC responses in the alert animal.

Potential Role of GABA-A inhibition in VPFL function: Saccades

Saccade related signals are strong in the VPFL (Miles et al. 1980), and can be used to maintain saccade accuracy (Noda and Suzuki, 1979) and to update internal models of the eye movement (Ghasia et al. 2008). Gabazine changed the saccade response of PCs from only-OFF and ON/OFF to only-ON responses, suggesting that PCs receive omnidirectional ON saccade responses through the parallel fiber system (their only excitatory input), and that the directional preference of PCs is controlled by selective inhibition (Figure S7C). This view contrasts with computational modeling architectures that assume that VPFL PCs in the vertical and horizontal zones receive only vertical and horizontal eye related information, respectively (Blazquez et al. 2003; Lisberger 1994). However, our data are in agreement with the anatomy of the circuitry, where parasagitally organized parallel fibers functionally link different zones of the VPFL (Eccles et al. 1967). Selective inhibitory control would be best carried out by nearby molecular layer interneurons (stellate and basket cells) because they receive inputs from the same parallel fibers than the recorded PC (Eccles et al. 1967). A large portion of the inhibitory control to PCs from molecular layer interneurons is carried out by GABA transmission, although ephaptic transmission from basket cells to PC at the pinceau have also been described (Blot and Barbour, 2014). In addition, gabazine can have an indirect effect over the gain of PCs by changing the gain of their input elements (i.e., granule cells). In support, Duguid and colleagues (2012) showed that reduction of tonic inhibition increases the gain and saliency in granule cell responses to sensory stimulation. However, because VPFL mossy fibers contacting a single granule cell likely have similar preferred orientations as the PCs above (Cerminara et al, 2013; Pijpers et al. 2006; Sato an Kawasaki 1990, 1991), it is unlikely that changes in granule responsiveness can cause the omnidirectional response of PCs to saccades.

Potential role of GABA-A inhibition in VPFL function: Pursuit and VOR cancellation

Gabazine increased the response of PCs to pursuit and VOR cancellation, suggesting that inhibition regulates the input/output relationship in cerebellar cortex. These increases are not due to changes in DC firing rate, as demonstrated by our injections of L-glutamate. We argue that gabazine increased PC gain by increasing the input/output relationship in granule cells (Duguid et a., 2012; Mitchell and Silver, 2003). Thus, GABA-A inhibition may work in conjunction with LTP/LTD in parallel fiber-PC synapses to support motor learning (Schonewille et al. 2010; Hansel et al. 2006 Jirenheld et al. 2013; Liu et al. 2008). Indeed, the parameters that underwent the larger changes with gabazine application were eye and head velocity sensitivities, which are the components believed to drive VOR motor learning (Blazquez et al. 2003; Lisberger 1994). Regulation of inhibition could also explain the observation that mice lacking LTD at parallel fiber to PC synapses can adapt their VOR gain, albeit they require longer training times (van Alphen and De Zeeuw, 2002). Interestingly, gabazine increased the neuronal responsiveness to smooth pursuit only in the preferred orientation of the neuron. This can be explained by a reduction of inhibition in nearby granule cells, because, as mentioned above, reduction of inhibition in granule cells increases their input-output gain and the granule cells affected by gabazine would most likely have a similar preferred orientation as the recorded PC (Cerminara et al, 2013; Pijpers et al. 2006) (Figure S7D).

How can we reconcile the finding that gabazine changed the spatial response tuning of PCs during saccades, but not during pursuit? PCs receive saccade and smooth eye movement related inputs through the same set of mossy fibers (with a burst tonic response type, e.g., from prepositus hypoglossi) (Miles et al. 1980; Escudero et al. 1996); hence the effect must be specific to the type of neuronal activity associated with these behaviors. Saccades generate a powerful burst of activity in mossy fibers, while pursuit generates smaller changes in firing rate that build over longer time scales. It is possible that the subset of parallel fibers that form functional connections with each VPFL PC determine its eye movement preferred orientation, while the rest of mossy fibers form synapses with very high activation threshold (i.e. almost silent synapses [Isope and Barbour 2002]). Gabazine application may reduce the activation threshold just enough for these synapses to allow the high burst of activity associated with saccades to generate EPSCs in PCs, but not the smaller activity associated with pursuit. Alternatively, fast inhibitory transmission (e.g. GABA-A) could be responsible for the spatial tuning of PCs during saccades, while other forms of synaptic transmission (e.g., through GABA-B receptors) play the major role in shaping the directional tuning of PCs during pursuit. Perhaps, the different effect of gabazine on PC responses to saccade and pursuit eye movements may reflect different cerebellar strategies to control ballistic (e.g. saccades) and smooth (e.g. pursuit) movements.

In conclusion, our experiments show evidence for a role of GABA-A inhibition in the spatio-temporal signal transformations carried out by the cerebellar cortex while the structure performs behaviorally relevant computations. Excitation is likely the main driver of PC responses during pursuit and VOR cancellation because gabazine did not remove but rather increased the eye velocity sensitivity. However, inhibition is a strong mechanism to regulate saccade responses because it can overpower the excitatory drive arriving through parallel fibers. These results can serve as a bridge to link the remarkable advances in our understanding of cerebellar physiology from in-vitro and anesthetized preparations with the available data in the alert animal.

EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES

Animal preparation and recording setup

Mouse

Eight adult C57BL/6 mice were anesthetized with Xylazine (13 mg/kg every 2 hours). Following, a midline incision was made on the scalp to expose the bone surface. We removed 3 mm2 of the occipital bone posterior to the lambdoid suture, and the underlying dura mater to expose lobules IV, V, and VI of the cerebellar cortex, through which we run our electrode tracks. Animals were euthanized after the experiment with sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg). Surgical and experimental protocols were in accordance with the National Institute of Health guidelines and approved by the Washington University Committee on Animal Care.

The mouse recording setup consisted of an AC differential amplifier (BAK Electronics), a hydraulic microdrive (Narishige), and a Neurophore BH-2 iontophoretic pump system (Medical Systems Corp). Data was acquired using a Power 1401 (Cambridge Electronic Design) connected to a PC computer (Spike2 software, Cambridge Electronic Design).

Monkey

We used 3 macaques (M1, M2 and M3) of 5–7 years of age and 6–11 kg for neuronal recording in the VPFL. We used standard surgical procedures performed under isoflurane anesthesia and aseptic conditions in a fully equipped surgical suite (Heine et al. 2010). In a first surgery, we implanted a stainless steel head post for head fixation and an eye coil to monitor horizontal and vertical eye position. Two weeks later we implanted a recording chamber aimed to the left VPFL. Surgical and experimental protocols were in accordance with the National Institute of Health guidelines and approved by the Washington University Committee on Animal Care.

The macaque recording setup consisted of an AC differential amplifier (BAK Electronics), a hydraulic microdrive (Trent Wells, Inc.), a Neurophore BH-2 iontophoretic pump system (Medical Systems Corp), and a search coil eye movement detector (C.N.C. Engineering, Seattle, WA). A Power 1401 (Cambridge Electronic Design) connected to a PC computer (Spike2 software, Cambridge Electronic Design) was used for data acquisition and stimulus presentation. A red laser projected on a white screen placed in front of the animal (48 cm) served as our main visual stimulus.

Training and Behavioral paradigm

Macaques were trained to maintain their gaze on the laser using standard water restriction protocols. We used five tasks: i) Spontaneous eye movements in the light. This protocol was used to analyze the neuronal response to saccade eye movement when visually guided saccade data were not available; ii) Visually guided saccades to four cardinal directions. After an initial central fixation (1–1.7 s) the laser target was stepped 15 or 20 deg in one of four cardinal directions, where it remained stationary for 1 s. iii) Horizontal and vertical sinusoidal pursuit. The laser was moved sinusoidally around the center fixation at 0.4 Hz and 10 deg amplitude. iv) Vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) cancellation. Animals fixated a stationary target (red laser) while they were passively rotated along the earth vertical axis (yaw rotation) at 0.4 Hz and 10 deg amplitude; animals were rewarded with water each 1–1.5 s. v) Step ramp pursuit to four cardinal directions. After an initial central fixation (1–1.5 s), the laser was moved toward the endpoint (10 deg from center) at a constant velocity (10 or 20 deg/s, for 1.5 or 3 s, respectively). At the onset of pursuit, the target stepped back a few degrees to avoid catch up saccades (Heine et al. 2010).

Carbon fiber Multibarrel Electrode preparation, unit recording and drug application

Mouse

Electrode assemblies consisted of a single capillary glass electrode attached to a three barrel carbon fiber electrode. Both the single capillary glass electrode and the three barrel carbon fiber electrode were pulled using a horizontal puller (PML-107L, MicroData Instruments, NJ). The tip of the carbon fiber was etched by passing electric current through a saline bridge until it was reduced to about 10–15 µm (Inagaki et al. 2009). Next, we glued the multibarrel electrode to the single capillary glass electrode using dental cement with a separation between the tip of the capillary glass and the three barrel electrode of 0.04 to 0.8 mm (see Figure 1, pictures on top right). Electrodes were inspected before and after recording to confirm that the integrity of the assembly and the size of the electrode tips were not compromised. One barrel of the three barrel electrode was filled with GABA (500 mM in 0.165 M NaCl pH 5.0), a second barrel with 0.165 M NaCl for current compensation, and a third barrel contained the carbon fiber (5 µm). The capillary glass electrode was filled with gabazine (10 mM in 0.165 M NaCl, pH 3.5).

Our electrode penetrations were limited to 2 mm deep from the surface of the cerebellum (throughout vermis lobules IV–VI). Once a spontaneously firing neuron was isolated we tested the effect of GABA injection (+15 to +50 nA) while retaining gabazine (−50 to −100 nA). GABA was injected intermittently using pulses of 2–5 s duration every 10–30 s. The exact pulse parameters for GABA injection were chosen online, and were tailored to each recorded neuron to consistently generate large decreases in firing rate (near full pauses), but allowing the cell to recover in between pulses. The first 3–10 pulses of GABA (before gabazine injection) were used as control responses. Following this, we injected gabazine (+50 or +100 nA constant current) for up to 10 minutes. Note that we continued delivering pulses of GABA while injecting gabazine (see Figure 1A).

Monkey

We used tungsten electrodes (FHC, 3–8 MOhms) and carbon fiber multibarrel electrodes. Carbon fiber multibarrel electrodes were made using procedures described elsewhere (Inagaki et al., 2009). Briefly, a carbon fiber (5–7 µm) was inserted into one barrel of a three or four barrel capillary glass, the glass ensemble was then pulled (PML 107L, MicroData Instruments, NJ), and the remaining barrels were filled with solution. One of the barrels was filled with 2 (3 carboxypropyl) 3 amino 6 methoxyphenyl pyridazinium bromide (GABAZINE, 10mM in 0.165 M NaCl at pH 3, Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) or L-glutamate (20 mM, in 32 mM NaOH, Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO). Another barrel was filled with 0.165 mM NaCl solution for balance compensation. The values used for current retention and injection of gabazine were −50 to −75 nA (retention), and +50 to +75 nA (injection). For glutamate these values were, +15 to +75 nA (retention), and −10 to −50 nA (injection). Neuronal responses to gabazine application were measured after 30 seconds of the onset of gabazine application to guarantee that drug was present in the extracellular space. Similarly, neuronal responses during the recovery period were measured after 30 seconds of ending gabazine application. Often this period was not sufficient to get full recovery as indicated by the fact that the DC firing rate was still higher than preinjection levels.

We identified the three layers of the VPFL and their neuronal elements based on their characteristic neuronal activity (Heine et al. 2010; Laurens et al. 2013). PCs were identified by the presence of simple and complex spikes. Often, complex spikes could be heard through the entire recording but it was difficult to maintain isolation of both simple and complex spikes simultaneously for long periods. Mossy fibers were identified in the granular layer as units with narrow spikes (<0.25 ms duration) and monophasic profiles that could not be isolated for long distances (Laurens et al. 2013).

Analysis methods

Data analysis was performed offline in Matlab 2007 (MathWorks).

Mouse

Neuronal responses to GABA during gabazine application were compared to those during the control period (before gabazine) to quantify the effective spread of drug in tissue. We built peristimulus time histograms (PSTH) of the neuronal firing rate aligned with the onset of pulses of GABA during the control period (see Figure 1B, blue lines). We used these PSTHs to manually select for each cell a time period that showed clear responses to GABA (decreases in firing rate); this is the response period. The same time period was used as the test period during injection of gabazine. Neuronal response to GABA (%) was measured for each pulse of GABA as:

Response toGABA(%)=100*(1FRRPFRCP) (Eq. 1)

Where FRRP is the mean firing rate during the response period, and FRCP is the mean firing rate during the control period. The control period extended from five seconds before the onset of each GABA pulse until the onset of each GABA pulse. Therefore, the FRCP was calculated independently for each pulse. Lastly, we fit the changes in “Response to GABA (%)” with the decay curve below to calculate the time necessary to reach 80% of the gabazine effect.

Response toGABA(%)(t)=A+Be(rt) (Eq. 2)

Where Response to GABA(%)(t) corresponds to the predicted Response to GABA(%) at time t, A is the asymptotic value, B the maximum change, and r determines the rate of change.

Monkey

Saccade and pursuit data was sorted based on the direction of eye movement (up and ispilateral were considered positive, down and contralateral negative). Data collected during the spontaneous saccade task were also sorted into 4 groups corresponding to ipsilateral (−45 to 45 deg), contralateral (135 to 225 deg), upwards (45 to 135 deg) and downwards (225 to −45 deg). PSTHs were constructed from the sorted behavioral and neuronal responses using 2 ms bin size, and 17 points moving average smoothing. A 20 deg/s eye velocity threshold was used to determine the onset and offset of each saccade. Saccade gain was calculated as the ratio between saccade amplitude and target movement amplitude. A 30 deg/s2 acceleration threshold was used to detect the onset of pursuit; this was then manually inspected. Pursuit gain was measured as the ratio between plateau eye velocity (mean desaccaded eye velocity 300–350 ms after pursuit onset) and target eye velocity.

We used a Wilcoxon rank-sum test over the averaged PC data (PSTH, constructed using 2 ms bin size with 17 points moving average) to determine whether or not PCs were responsive to pursuit and saccades. Specifically, we compared the PC firing rate during the control period to that during the response period using a 70 ms moving window that slides in 2 ms steps from the beginning to the end of the response period (Blazquez et al. 2002). A PC was considered responsive if we found significant changes (p<0.05) in firing rate in six consecutive windows. For those eye movement directions where a neuron showed significant response, we quantified PC responses as follows:

PC response to saccades was calculated as the maximum change in firing rate during the response period (−10 to 180 ms after saccade onset) with respect to the mean firing rate during the control period (100 to 500 ms before saccade onset). PC responses to saccades took positive values for increases in firing rate, and negative values for decreases in firing rate. PCs with positive responses for one or more directions and no negative responses were called ON neurons. PCs with negative responses for one or more directions and no positive responses were called OFF neurons. PCs with positive and negative responses were called ON/OFF neurons. Next, the increase in firing rate (incFR) was plotted against each saccade direction (counterclockwise, right= 0; up=90; left=180; down=270) and the data was fitted with a cosine function of the form:

Saccade response(direction)=A+B*cos(direction) (Eq. 3)

Where A is the baseline of the cosine function, which corresponds to the tuning width, and B is the response amplitude (see insets in Figure SF 1). This function estimates the preferred direction (direction of maximum response), directional tuning (B/A), and tuning width (A).

Sinusoidal pursuit and VOR cancellation data was fitted by a sine function. Neuronal phase was calculated with respect to eye velocity for pursuit, and head velocity for VOR cancellation. During VOR cancellation our monkeys generated minimal eye movements. Only in few cases, were the amplitude of eye movements >2 deg/s, we subtracted from the PC response the component attributed to eye movement (calculated during pursuit, Lisberger et al. 1994). PC sensitivities to eye and head velocity during sinusoidal pursuit were calculated as the slope of the fitting line describing the relation between PC firing rate and eye or head velocity. PC responses to step ramp pursuit were quantified using standard methods, specifically the multiple linear regression approach expressed in equation 4.

f(t)=αË(t)+βĖ(t)+γE(t)+δ+ε(t) (Eq 4)

Where, f(t) is the PC firing rate at time t. α, β and correspond to the PC sensitivities to eye acceleration (Ë), eye velocity (Ė) and eye position (E), respectively, δ is the PC baseline firing rate and is the error term (Blazquez et al. 2003).

Supplementary Material

01

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We thank: Drs. Stephen Highstein, Dora Angelaki and Harold Barton for equipment and support; Dr. Yutaka Hirata and Shane Heiney for helping editing the manuscript; Fanetta Hampton, Valentin Militchin, Krystal Henderson and Darryl Craig for technical support and animal care; Keith Graham and Buster Tipton of TORAY Carbon Fiber for supplying the carbon fibers. This work was funded by grants NINDS R01-NS065099 (PMB), the McDonnell Center for Higher Brain Function (PMB), and NIDCD R03-DC011142 (TAY)

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION

PMB was involved in all aspects of the project, including, neuronal recordings, data analysis, and manuscript preparation. TAY helped with neuronal recordings, interpretation of the data, and manuscript preparation.

BIBLIOGRAPHY

  1. Blazquez PM, Fujii N, Kojima J, Graybiel AM. A network representation of response probability in the striatum. Neuron. 2002;33(6):973–982. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00627-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Blazquez PM, Hirata Y, Heiney SA, Green AM, Highstein SM. Cerebellar signatures of vestibulo-ocular reflex motor learning. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:9742–9751. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-30-09742.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cerminara NL, Aoki H, Loft M, Sugihara I, Apps R. Structural basis of cerebellar microcircuits in the rat. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:16427–16442. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0861-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. D’Angelo E. The organization of plasticity in the cerebellar cortex: from synapses to control. Prog. Brain Res. 2014;210:31–58. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00002-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. De Zeeuw CI, Hansel C, Bian F, Koekkoek SK, van Alphen AM, Linden DJ, Oberdick J. Expression of a protein kinase C inhibitor in Purkinje cells blocks cerebellar LTD and adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Neuron. 1998;20:495–508. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80990-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Donaldson IM. The functions of the proprioceptors of the eye muscles. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000;355:1685–1754. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Duguid I, Branco T, London M, Chadderton P, Häusser M. Tonic inhibition enhances fidelity of sensory information transmission in the cerebellar cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32:11132–11143. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0460-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dugue GP, Brunel N, Hakim V, Schwartz E, Chat M, Levesque M, Courtemanche R, Lena C, Dieudonne S. Electrical coupling mediates tunable low-frequency oscillations and resonance in the cerebellar Golgi cell network. Neuron. 2009;61:126–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Eccles J, Ito M, Szentagothai J. The cerebellum as a Neuronal Machine. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  10. Escudero M, Cheron G, Godaux E. Discharge properties of brain stem neurons projecting to the flocculus in the alert cat. II.Prepositus hypoglossal nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76(3):1775–1785. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.1775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York: Elsevier Science; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gao W, Chen G, Reinert KC, Ebner TJ. Cerebellar cortical molecular layer inhibition is organized in parasagittal zones. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:8377–8387. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2434-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ghasia FF, Meng H, Angelaki DE. Neural correlates of forward and inverse models for eye movements: evidence from three-dimensional kinematics. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:5082–5087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0513-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Heine SA, Highstein SM, Blazquez PM. Golgi cells operate as state-specific temporal filters at the input stage of the cerebellar cortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:17004–17014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3513-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Herz A, Zieglgansberger W, Farber G. Microelectrophoretic studies concerning the spread of glutamic acid and GABA in brain tissue. Exp. Brain Res. 1969;9:221–235. doi: 10.1007/BF00234456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Isope P, Barbour B. Properties of unitary granule-cell-->Purkinje cell synapses in adult rat cerebellar slices. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:9668–9678. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-22-09668.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Inagaki K, Heiney SA, Blazquez PM. Method for the construction and use of carbon fiber multibarrel electrodes for deep brain recordings in the alert animal. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2009;178:255–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jirenhed DA, Bengtsson F, Jörntell H. Parrallel fiber and climbing fiber responses in rat cerebellar cortical neurons in vivo. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013 May;17:7–16. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Jorntell H, Ekerot CF. Reciprocal bidirectional plasticity of parallel fiber receptive fields in cerebellar Purkinje cells and their afferent interneurons. Neuron. 2002;34:797–806. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00713-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Laurens J, Heiney SA, Kim G, Blazquez PM. Cerebellar cortex granular layer interneurons in the macaque monkey are functionally driven by mossy fiber pathways through net excitation or inhibition. PLoS One. 2013;8(12) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0082239. 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lisberger SG. Neural basis for motor learning in the vestibuloocular reflex of primates. III. Computational and behavioral analysis of the sites of learning. J. Neurophysiol. 1994;72:974–998. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.2.974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Liu SJ, Lachamp P, Liu Y, Savtchouk I, Sun L. Long-term synaptic plasticity in the cerebellar stellate cells. Cerebellum. 2008;7(4):559–562. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0057-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Miles FA, Fuller JH, Braitman DJ, Dow BM. Long-term adaptive changes in primate vestibuloocular reflex. III. Electrophysiological observations in flocculus of normal monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 1980;43:1437–1476. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.5.1437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mitchell SJ, Silver RA. Shunting inhibition modulates neuronal gain during synaptic excitation. Neuron. 2003;38:433–445. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00200-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Noda H, Suzuki DA. The role of the flocculus of the monkey in saccadic eye movements. J Physiol. 1979;294:317–334. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Paxinos G, Huang X-F, Toga AW. The rhesus monkey brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Accademic Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  27. Pijpers A, Apps R, Pardoe J, Voogd J, Ruigrok TJ. Precise spatial relationships between mossy fibers and climbing fibers in rat cerebellar cortical zones. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12067–12080. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2905-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rambold H, Churchland A, Selig Y, Jasmin L, Lisberger SG. Partial ablations of the flocculus and ventral paraflocculus in monkeys cause linked deficits in smooth pursuit eye movements and adaptive modification of the VOR. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;87:912–924. doi: 10.1152/jn.00768.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ruskell GL. Extraocular muscle proprioceptors and proprioception. Prog Retin Eye Res. 1999;18(3):269–291. doi: 10.1016/s1350-9462(98)00029-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Sato Y, Kawasaki T. Operational unit responsible for plane-specific control of eye movement by cerebellar flocculus in cat. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;64:551–564. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sato Y, Kawasaki T. Identification of the Purkinje cell/climbing fiber zone and its target neurons responsible for eye-movement control by the cerebellar flocculus. Brain Res. Rev. 1991;16:39–64. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(91)90019-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Schonewille M, Belmeguenai A, Koekkoek SK, Houtman SH, Boele HJ, van Beugen BJ, Gao Z, Badura A, Ohtsuki G, Amerika WE, Hosy E, Hoebeek FE, Elgersma Y, Hansel C, De Zeeuw CI. Purkinje cell-specific knockout of the protein phosphatase PP2B impairs potentiation and cerebellar motor learning. Neuron. 2010;67:618–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. van Alphen AM1, De Zeeuw CI. Cerebellar LTD facilitates but is not essential for long-term adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2002;16(3):486–490. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02094.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES