Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Aug 21.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2013 Aug;25(3):643–652. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000072

Interests in high-functioning autism are more intense, interfering, and idiosyncratic, but not more circumscribed, than those in neurotypical development

Laura Gutermuth Anthony 1, Lauren Kenworthy 1, Benjamin E Yerys 1, Kathryn F Jankowski 1, Joette D James 1, Madeline B Harms 2, Alex Martin 2, Gregory L Wallace 2
PMCID: PMC4543385  NIHMSID: NIHMS715281  PMID: 23880382

Abstract

Though circumscribed interests are pathognomonic with autism, how these interests differ from those expressed during neutotypical (NT) development remains unknown. Using a novel measure, the Interests Scale (IS), this study compares interests between 76 NT individuals and 109 individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HF-ASD) matched group-wise on age, IQ and gender ratio. Participants and their parents/caregivers completed diagnostic measures (Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [ADI-R], Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule [ADOS]; HF-ASD only), cognitive tests (Wechsler IQ Scales), and questionnaires (the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised [RBS-R], the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function [BRIEF], and the Social Responsiveness Scale [SRS]), in addition to the IS. HF-ASD and NT individuals did not differ in number of interest areas, but the types of interests differed considerably. HF-ASD individuals had significantly more intense and interfering interests than NT individuals. Using only the intensity score, 77.5% of individuals were correctly classified (NT or HF-ASD) in a discriminant function analysis. Among individuals with HF-ASD, IS scores were significantly related to ADOS, BRIEF, RBS-R and SRS scores, but not to ADI-R scores, IQ, gender, age or psychotropic medication use. The type and intensity, but not the number, of interests distinguish high-functioning children with ASD from NT children.

Keywords: autism, circumscribed interest, restricted interest, executive function, repetitive behavior

Introduction

Interests and hobbies (e.g., sports participation or crafts) are an important feature in the development of typical children and adolescents (see McHale, Crouter, & Tucker, 2001 for a review), as they offer avenues for social connection (e.g., Werner, 1993). In contrast, the restricted and/or circumscribed interests (CIs) often seen in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders (HF-ASD), can have socially isolating effects due to their intensity or unusual nature. However, the quantitative and qualitative differences between interests/hobbies characteristic of typical development and those that are circumscribed, as in HF-ASD, have not been fully distinguished.

One of the defining symptoms of ASD is repetitive behaviors and restricted interests (APA, 2000). Because CIs are a defining symptom of ASD, the ability to distinguish the interests in HF-ASD from those in neurotypical (NT) individuals is critical. CIs, including topics of a non-social nature like fossils or vacuum cleaners (e.g., Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999; Klin et al., 2007), are pathognomonic with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), but understudied in high functioning individuals. The prevalence of CIs was recently estimated in 96 children with HF-ASD using an interview format and CIs were observed to be quite common (75% for preschool children and 88% for elementary school age children) and interfered with other activities in the child’s life (Klin et al., 2007). This prevalence is higher than that found in an ASD sample including both lower- and higher-functioning individuals, which reported lifetime prevalence estimates for CIs based on three items from the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (Szatmari et al., 2006): 56% for CIs, 60% for unusual preoccupations, and 49% for attachment to objects. These findings suggest that CIs are quite common in ASD, but are more common in HF-ASD.

Prior studies of children and adults with ASD show that stereotyped/repetitive behaviors and CIs diverge in function (e.g., Turner, 1999), developmental trajectory (e.g., Moore & Goodson, 2003), and familiality (e.g., Lam, Bodfish, & Piven, 2008). Additionally, Carcani-Rathwell, Rabe-Hasketh and Santosh (2006) suggest that the higher-order cognitive rigidity behaviors (including CIs) are unique to ASD, while lower-order repetitive behaviors relate to intellectual disability (ID) with or without ASD. Furthermore, Klin, Danovitch, Merz and Volkmar (2007) argue that CIs are a larger problem in HF-ASD than lower-functioning ASD, and contribute to difficulties in daily functioning and socialization (though note that CIs can also be used as motivators/reinforcers to increase socialization; see Baker, 2000; Baker et al., 1998; Boyd et al., 2007).

Clinicians may struggle in determining whether an interest meets diagnostic criteria because there is insufficient empirical evidence documenting the boundary between a typical interest and a CI. Accordingly, there has not been any previous research directly comparing interests in HF-ASD and NT individuals. The prevalence estimates of intense interests in NT children show these behaviors are common, particularly in boys, with a developmental peak occurring during the preschool years (DeLoache, Simcock, & Macari, 2007). Nevertheless, even at its peak, the rates of intense interests in NT development are not as high as those found in studies of individuals with ASD (Alexander, Johnson, Leibham, & Kelley, 2008; DeLoache, Simcock, & Macari, 2007; Johnson et al., 2004; Knickmeyer et al., 2005).

CIs may also relate to executive functioning (EF), which is another area of difficulty in ASD. EF consists of several sub-domains that include the abilities to: initiate and sustain attention/behavior, inhibit impulses, set goals and make realistic plans, organize information and problem-solve, manage information “on-line” in working memory, shift strategies flexibly, and monitor and regulate one’s own behavior (e.g., Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000; Kaplan, 1988; Welsh & Pennington, 1988). Just as CIs are costly to individuals with ASD (Klin et al., 2007), so has EF been consistently identified as an impairment in ASD (for review, see Hill, 2004; Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony & Wallace, 2008; O’Hearn, Asato, Ordaz, & Luna, 2008; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Sergeant, Geurts, & Oosterlaan, 2002). The relationship between EF and repetitive behavior more broadly has been explored, but without a focus on CIs specifically (e.g., Lopez, Lincoln, Ozonoff, & Lai, 2005; Kenworthy et al., 2009; Turner, 1997; Yerys et al., 2009; for review see Geurts, Corbett, & Solomon, 2009). In a notable exception, South, Ozonoff and McMahon (2007) explored, but did not find support for a relationship between CIs and EF in individuals with HF-ASD, though this study cannot be considered definitive, as there were limitations of sample size (ASD n=19) and EF measurement tools. Most prior studies, including South and colleagues, correlated performance on lab-based set-shifting measures, not ecologically-valid EF measures, with repetitive behavior (Kenworthy et al., 2008).

There is no widely accepted measure of CIs; most researchers use items from broader established scales. However, this approach presents challenges. Some have used the ADI (Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) for examination of CIs, but these instruments contain only a few questions that relate to CIs (Cuccaro, Shao & Grubber, 2003). In addition, using diagnostic data as the dependent variable introduces the risk of circular logic when examining ASD-NT group differences or attempting to correlate ASD symptoms with CIs. Diagnostic measures are further limiting because they are inappropriate for non-ASD children, do not provide continuous scores, and do not probe the content or intensity of the interests (Lam, Bodfish, & Piven, 2008). The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino & Gruber, 2005) is a widely used continuous measure of ASD-related behavior that is also appropriate for use with NT individuals; however, the SRS combines CIs with other repetitive behaviors on the Autistic Mannerisms subscale, but this subscale does not load separately from social-communicative behaviors in factor analyses (Constantino et al., 2004). Turner’s (1997) Repetitive Behavior Interview is fairly lengthy, and the time spent (2+ hours) on a single ASD symptom domain exceeds its clinical utility. The Yale Survey of Special Interests (Klin & Volkmar, 1996, as described in Klin et al., 2007) asks open-ended questions about the child’s top three interests that are then coded into categories, followed by quantitative interference ratings. We chose to use the Interests Scale (IS; Bodfish, 2004), a 40-item checklist that asks the parent or caregiver to rate each area of interest as being present currently or in the past. The parent is then asked several additional questions about the three primary interests, including how intense the interests are, how much they interfere with social interactions and flexibility, and the need for accommodation around those interests. The IS has the advantage of offering a qualitative examination of the content or type of interest as well as providing a quantitative rating of the intensity of the interests.

Though there has been a recent increase in studies related to CIs in HF-ASD, much remains unknown regarding their intensity and content, relationship to other symptoms and gender, and how best to distinguish typical interests from CIs in ASD. Given that CIs are part of the diagnostic criteria for ASD, and particularly prevalent and important in HF-ASD, we chose to conduct this study with HF-ASD individuals. In addition, for the purposes of this paper, an interest becomes a CI when it is intense and interferes with daily functioning.

The present study directly compares the number, type, and intensity of interests in groups of HF-ASD and NT individuals matched on age, IQ and gender ratio. Furthermore, we examine how the interests relate to social-communication symptoms/behaviors and EF. We chose social and communication behaviors because they constitute the remaining two components of the triad of impairment in ASD (APA, 2000). We chose EF because it has been linked with restricted or repetitive behaviors and interests (Kenworthy et al., 2009; Lopez, Lincoln, Ozonoff, & Lai, 2005; South, Ozonoff, & McMahon, 2007; Turner, 1997; Yerys et al., 2009).

Hypotheses

We hypothesized that (1) individuals with HF-ASD would have fewer interests but have higher intensity or interference ratings, than NT individuals; consistent with the notion of a restricted number of interests that are also interfering in daily activities in the HF-ASD group, (2) individuals with HF-ASD would have fewer socially-oriented interests and more solitary or object/sensory-focused interests than NT individuals, (3) males and females from both groups would differ in their areas of interest, and (4) the IS would be negatively correlated with IQ and age but positively correlated with measures of autistic symptomatology and repetitive behavior in the HF-ASD group.

Method

Participants

For all participants, informed consent was obtained from parents/caregivers when participants were under the age of 18, and informed assent was obtained from the participants in accordance with the requirements of the IRBs from Children’s National Medical Center and the National Institute of Mental Health. Informed consent was obtained from participants ages 18 and older.

Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)

One hundred and nine individuals with HF-ASD were recruited for this study through a hospital clinic specializing in ASD and through the community. Thirty-eight individuals (35%) were taking at least one psychotropic medication. Individuals in the ASD group received a clinical diagnosis of Autism, Asperger’s Syndrome, or Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified [PDD-NOS] based on DSM-IV-TR criteria (APA, 2000). All participants with ASD also met criteria for ‘broad ASD’ based on scores from the ADI or ADI-R (Le Couteur et al., 1989; Lord et al., 1994) and/or the ADOS (Lord et al., 2000), following criteria established by the NICHD/NIDCD Collaborative Programs for Excellence in Autism (Lainhart et al., 2006). A full-scale IQ score of at least 70 with either a Verbal/Verbal Comprehension or Performance/Perceptual Reasoning IQ of at least 80 was required for study entry and was derived from testing on a Wechsler instrument: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (n=85), Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-Third (n=3) or Fourth Editions (n=18) or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (n=3) (Wechsler 1991; 1997; 1999; 2003). Subjects were also excluded if they had any history of comorbid medical or genetic disorders that would affect brain development and/or cognitive functioning.

Neurotypical (NT) Individuals

Seventy-six NT individuals were recruited from the community via advertisements. Individuals enrolled in the NT group did not receive diagnostic measures, but were screened through a brief parent-completed interview and questionnaire. Participants were screened and excluded if they or a 1st degree relative were found to have developmental, learning, neurological, or psychiatric disorders, or if the NT individual used any type of psychotropic medication. As with the ASD group, a full-scale IQ score of at least 70 with either a Verbal/Verbal Comprehension or Performance/Perceptual Reasoning IQ of at least 80 was required for study entry and was derived from testing on a Wechsler instrument: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (n=74) or Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children- Fourth Edition (n=2) (Wechsler 1999; 2003). The ASD and NT groups did not differ in terms of gender ratio, age, IQ or SES (ps>.05; see Table 1)

Table 1.

Participant Demographics

NT (N=76) HF-ASD (N=109) Significance (two-tailed)
% Males 79% 85% .259
Age Mean (SD) 13.59 (3.85) 12.70 (3.79) .122
Range 7.22–24.25 7.13–22.92
Full Scale IQ Mean (SD) 115.03 (11.72) 111.56 (17.48) .133
Range 96–144 73–159
SES Mean (SD) 40.31(23.09) 36.99 (22.31) .354
Range 11–100 11–89

Note. SES calculated with Hollingshead’s (1975) coding; (Middle class range=28–43).

Measures

All participants were tested in a laboratory setting within the context of two separate, larger studies. Participation included a 3- to 4-hour testing session with a variety of neuropsychological, diagnostic and cognitive measures. Participants received monetary compensation for their time.

1. Interests Scale (IS; Bodfish, 2004)

The IS is an informant report of interests that consists of two sections. Part 1 is a checklist of 41 categories of interests (e.g., mechanical systems/actions, numbers/numerical information), and informants are asked to endorse all categories that constitute either a Current or a Past interest. In Part 2, informants are asked to identify the current primary, secondary and tertiary interests and are instructed to answer several questions designed to assess the intensity of each interest (e.g., frequency of specific activity related to interest, degree of interference with other activities, degree of resistance when interrupted). The IS yields a Total Number of Current Interests Endorsed score and a Total Intensity score. We use both scores in analyses, as well as examining item-level data. Higher scores indicate greater number or intensity of interests.

2. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF; Gioia et al., 2000)

The BRIEF is an informant report of EF in everyday situations. Its 86 items yield eight scales that are collapsed into a broad composite, the Global Executive Composite (GEC). Higher scores indicate greater impairment. We chose a parent report measure because it is more reliable at capturing EF problems seen in ASD (for review, see Geurts, Corbett, & Solomon, 2009; Kenworthy, Yerys, Anthony, & Wallace, 2008). For this study, we chose the GEC as the key dependent variable in order to limit the number of comparisons/correlations, and because CIs may be related to a combination of EF impairments as discussed above. Higher scores are associated with greater impairment.

3. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino & Gruber, 2005)

The SRS is an informant report, 65-item questionnaire that assesses behaviors characteristic of ASD, covering interpersonal, communicative and repetitive/stereotypic behaviors. We use Total T scores in our analyses because factor analyses consistently suggest a single factor solution (Constantino et al., 2004). Higher scores are associated with greater impairment.

4. Repetitive Behavior Scale- Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish, Symons, & Lewis, 1999)

The RBS-R is an informant report, 39-item questionnaire assessing various categories of repetitive behavior, including Stereotyped Behavior, Self-Injurious Behavior, Compulsive Behavior, Ritualistic Behavior and Sameness Behavior. Raters are asked to consider the frequency of the behavior, the individual’s resistance to interruption while engaging in the behavior and the extent to which the behavior interferes in daily functioning. We used the RBS-R Overall Score (sum of the Total subscale scores). Higher scores indicate more repetitive behaviors.

Data Analysis

Before hypothesis testing, basic psychometrics of the IS were evaluated for each group with descriptive statistics and internal consistency of the total scores (Current Interests Total and Intensity Score). Spearman or Pearson correlations were used to test our predictions that the IS total scores would be negatively correlated with IQ and age and positively correlated with ADOS, SRS, BRIEF and RBS-R scores. Multiple linear regressions were used to explore the relationships between IS total scores and age, gender and Full Scale IQ. These demographic variables were entered in the first step of the regression. Missing data reduced the combined sample size to 181; this reduced sample did not differ from the larger sample in any systematic way (e.g., age, IQ, gender ratio). To explore whether psychotropic medication usage was related to IS scores in this sample, a linear multiple regression was run for the HF-ASD group only.

We predicted that individuals with HF-ASD would have fewer interests and higher intensity or interference ratings than NT individuals. A discriminant function analysis was used to determine whether the IS scores differentiate HF-ASD from NT individuals. Both the IS Total Number of Interests and the IS Intensity Ratings were entered to predict diagnostic group membership, computing the prior probabilities from group sizes since the groups were unequal.

We also hypothesized that individuals with HF-ASD would have fewer socially-oriented interests and more solitary or object/sensory-focused interests than NT individuals and males with ASD would have different interest areas and more intense interests than females with ASD. To qualitatively assess interest areas, we tallied the top three most frequently reported primary interests by group (HFA vs. NT) and gender, and the most frequently reported primary interest by age (in three year increments, other than the youngest and oldest age groups: 7-0 to 8–11, 9-0 to 11-11, 12-0 to 14-11, 15-0 to 17-11, and 18-0 to 23) and gender. We also explored item-level differences on the IS between the HF-ASD and NT groups using independent samples t-tests.

Corrections for multiple comparisons were instituted throughout using the False Discovery Rate (FDR) set at q<.05 (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995).

Results

Psychometrics of the IS

Cronbach’s alpha for the 41 items on the IS was high (0.904). Cronbach’s alpha for the eight interference items was also strong (0.808). Table 2 presents the correlations between the IS and ADOS, RBS-R, BRIEF and SRS in the HF-ASD sample only (to avoid artificially increasing the correlation coefficients by including the very low scores on most of those measures in the NT individuals) after FDR correction for multiple comparisons. As expected, the IS scales were strongly positively correlated with the RBS-R. The number of current interests was correlated with the ADOS Stereotyped Behavior Scale. The Intensity of Interests was also positively correlated with the SRS and BRIEF. The relationships with the ADOS, SRS and RBS-R lend support for the IS’s construct validity.

Table 2.

Interests Scale Correlations for HF-ASD Participants Only

ADOS Comm/SocialS ADOS Stereotyped BehaviorS SRS Total T ScoresP RBS-S TotalP BRIEF GECP IS Sum of Current InterestsP
IS Correlation −.174 .229* .148 .403* .052
Sum of Current Sig (2-tailed) .088 .024 .132 <.001 .606 -
Interests N 97 100 105 107 100
IS Correlation .070 .039 .268* .285* .451* .092
Total Intensity Sig (2-tailed) .498 .707 .006 .003 <.001 .343
Rating N 96 98 103 105 98 107
*

Significant (p<.05) correlations (after FDR correction in bold [q<.05]).

S

Spearman correlation.

P

Pearson correlation.

Multiple regression analyses revealed that age, gender and Full Scale IQ do not significantly predict either Number of Interests (R2=.441; F=1.79, p=.54; with none of the covariates approaching significance) or Intensity of Interests (R2=.040; F=1.04, p=.67; with none of the covariates approaching significance) on the IS. Within the HF-ASD group, psychotropic medication use also did not predict IS scores (Number R2=.005; F=0.49, p=.49; Intensity R2=.009; F=0.06, p=.82).

We hypothesized that individuals with HF-ASD would have fewer interests, and that these interests would be more intense. To examine IS score differences between the groups, a discriminant function analysis was conducted. This analysis revealed that the Intensity Rating significantly discriminated between the two groups (Wilks’ Lambda .684, df=1, p<.001), though Number of Interests did not (Wilks’ Lambda .998). Using the IS Intensity Rating alone, 82% of the individuals with HF-ASD were correctly classified, and 71.2% of the NT individuals were correctly classified. This finding is striking, because the IS does not have any items assessing the other components of the ‘triad of impairment’ in autism (i.e., social reciprocity and social communication) per se.

To examine the second hypothesis that there will be differences in the foci of interests for NT individuals and those with ASD, t-tests were conducted on each IS item (0=Never had interest, 1=current interest, 2=current and past interest; past interests only were excluded). Item analyses with FDR correction for multiple comparisons suggest many similarities in areas of interests, with a few notable exceptions. Individuals with HF-ASD were more likely to show interest in: factual information, playing games alone, collecting/hoarding, cartoons, attachment to a particular object or item, and sensory seeking activities. NT individuals were more likely to be interested in people and sports. The two groups did not differ in many of the other non-social interests, such as computers, mechanical or building things, maps or astronomy, animals, calendars or math, though many people assume that these interests are more common in individuals with ASD. Please see Table 3 for details.

Table 3.

Group Differences on IS Items

Group Mean SD t (df=183) p
Machines/how things work NT 0.58 0.72 0.78 .436
HF-ASD 0.49 0.71
Mechanical systems/mechanical actions NT 0.34 0.62 1.29 .198
HF- ASD 0.23 0.55
Numbers/numerical information NT 0.26 0.55 −1.32 .190
HF-ASD 0.38 0.66
Strongly attached to a particular item or object NT 0.18 0.45 −3.31 .001**
HF-ASD 0.50 0.75
Seeks particular sensations or sensory experiences NT 0.09 0.33 −4.01 <.001**
HF-ASD 0.44 0.69
Factual information NT 0.25 0.59 −3.48 .001**
HF-ASD 0.61 0.77
Animals NT 0.46 0.74 −0.15 .879
HF-ASD 0.48 0.71
Insects NT 0.09 0.29 −0.98 .328
HF- ASD 0.16 0.51
Dinosaurs NT 0.12 0.43 −1.68 .095
HF-ASD 0.27 0.67
People NT 0.53 0.70 4.83 <.001**
HF-ASD 0.15 0.35
Collecting/hoarding things NT 0.29 0.56 −2.78 .006**
HF-ASD 0.59 0.81
Arts/crafts NT 0.47 0.68 0.95 .344
HF-ASD 0.38 0.69
Music NT 1.04 0.66 2.32 .021
HF-ASD 0.81 0.67
Television/movies NT 0.93 0.68 −1.49 .138
HF- ASD 1.10 0.79
Vehicles/transportation NT 0.37 0.63 −0.62 .537
HF-ASD 0.43 0.71
Building things/construction NT 0.66 1.30 0.40 .688
HF-ASD 0.60 0.77
Time NT 0.17 0.38 −1.52 .130
HF-ASD 0.27 0.51
Measuring/measurement NT 0.12 0.32 0.32 .746
HF-ASD 0.10 0.38
Geography/maps NT 0.20 0.40 −2.03 .044
HF-ASD 0.38 0.69
Schedules NT 0.06 0.25 −1.38 .169
HF-ASD 0.13 0.34
Calendars/dates NT 0.08 0.27 −1.75 .081
HF-ASD 0.19 0.52
Math/counting/calculating NT 0.21 0.44 −1.86 .064
HF-ASD 0.37 0.63
Computers/computing/computer science NT 0.50 0.58 −1.80 .074
HF-ASD 0.67 0.67
Sports NT 0.88 0.67 6.10 <.001**
HF-ASD 0.31 0.59
Astronomy/planets NT 0.21 0.59 −0.78 .436
HF-ASD 0.27 0.52
Reading/writing NT 0.70 0.67 0.17 .865
HF-ASD 0.68 0.76
Religion NT 0.49 0.70 1.94 .054
HF-ASD 0.30 0.58
Rocks/minerals/geology NT 0.18 0.51 −0.23 .816
HF-ASD 0.20 0.50
Politics/government NT 0.32 0.47 0.39 .696
HF-ASD 0.28 0.58
Physics NT 0.25 0.59 0.26 .795
HF-ASD 0.23 0.48
Psychology NT 0.14 0.35 0.61 .540
HF-ASD 0.11 0.39
History/philosophy NT 0.37 0.69 0.48 .631
HF-ASD 0.32 0.63
Language/linguistics NT 0.18 0.39 −0.99 .324
HF-ASD 0.26 0.55
Object or item motions NT 0.10 0.42 −2.17 .031
HF-ASD 0.26 0.50
Plants/gardening/farming NT 0.13 0.34 0.24 .813
HF-ASD 0.12 0.35
Toys NT 0.63 0.80 −0.52 .598
HF-ASD 0.70 0.85
Playing games with others NT 0.58 0.68 0.18 .853
HF-ASD 0.56 0.71
Playing games alone NT 0.76 0.65 −3.00 .003**
HF-ASD 1.10 0.82
Cartoons/animation NT 0.39 0.59 −4.15 <.001**
HF-ASD 0.85 0.83
Building/architecture NT 0.21 0.50 0.79 .430
HF-ASD 0.16 0.43
Other (not listed) NT 0.14 0.35 −2.66 .009**
HF-ASD 0.35 0.60
**

Group difference is significant after FDR correction (q<.05).

We further explored interest areas in a more qualitative way. At the end of the IS, the caregiver is asked to specify the individual’s primary interests. We compared the top three specific interests for males and females with and without HF-ASD (please see Table 4). It was compelling to observe that males from both groups shared an interest in video games, but there was no overlap in the primary interests of females from each group; however, this potential gender difference should be interpreted with caution because there were relatively few females in the present study. Age differences were explored in a similar way. It is notable that the primary interest across all ages in the HF-ASD group was video games, while there was a shift in the NT group from television in 7 and 8 year olds, to sports in 8–17 year olds, to religion in the young adults.

Table 4.

Primary Interest by Group and Gender

Group Males Percent Females Percent
NT Sports 26.7% Reading 18.8%
Reading 8.3% People 12.5%
Video games 6.7% Other (not listed) 6.3%
HF-ASD Video games 21.5% Numbers/math 18.8%
Legos 7.5% Music 12.5%
Playing games alone 4.3% Other (not listed) 6.3%

Discussion

We find that: (1) interests in HF-ASD individuals are more intense, but surprisingly not more limited in number/scope (i.e., not ‘restricted’) than those found in NT individuals, (2) fact-, object-, and sensory-oriented interests are greater in HF-ASD, while people- and sports-oriented interests are greater in NT individuals, (3) the areas of interest for males and females differ in both groups, and (4) intensity of interests are associated with other ASD-related behaviors (e.g., symptoms) and difficulties (e.g., executive functioning).

The present study shows that individuals with HF-ASD do not have fewer interests than NT individuals, contrary to our predictions. Rather, the intensity of their interests is greater, and their interest areas are less likely to be socially-oriented (e.g., interaction with others) and more likely to be object or sensory oriented, consistent with our predictions. This suggests that the DSM-IV criterion “encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that is abnormal either in intensity or focus” is the best way to distinguish the interests in HF-ASD from those in NT development, not merely the presence of an intense interest, nor a restriction in the number of interests (APA, 2000).

This study is also the first to directly compare the interest areas of NT and HF-ASD individuals. There were many intuitive, significant group differences, such as the HF-ASD group almost exclusively showing interests in a very particular item or object, sensory interests, collecting/hoarding things and interest in factual information, while interest in sports rarely occurred in HF-ASD. Qualitative analysis of the main interest areas broken down by gender and group (i.e., NT versus HF-ASD) revealed no overlap in interests in females, while males had an overlapping interest in video games (though they were especially common in HF-ASD). This could mean that females, as opposed to their male counterparts, with HF-ASD will have a more difficult time using their CIs to build friendships with NT females.

Furthermore, this study links everyday EF (BRIEF) with intensity of CIs (regardless of content area) and demonstrates an association between CIs and greater difficulties with social reciprocity and social communication (SRS). Whether intensity of CIs interferes with daily social interactions, or is just a proxy for more intensity of autism-related behavior regardless of domain is a question for future research. But these findings are consistent with Klin and colleagues’ (2007) suggestion that CIs are related to the social and repetitive core symptoms in HF-ASD. CIs may also cause significant family stress, as suggested by Bishop, Richler, Cain and Lord’s (2007) finding of increased parental stress with higher reports of restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests on the ADI-R. Therefore, treatment of CIs to increase adaptability, reduce social deficits, and reduce parental distress is an important clinical need for individuals with ASD (and their families). Moreover, the strong relationship we have identified between everyday EF and CIs in HF-ASD might offer an avenue for therapy: cognitive behavioral intervention could improve flexibility, inhibition, generativity and organization, and thereby reduce the intensity and interference of these behaviors. It is also possible that improved social engagement and pragmatic language skills result in a reduction of the intensity of CIs, as can happen in repetitive behaviors (e.g., Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992), though this is not necessarily supported in HF-ASD (Piven, Harper, Palmer, & Arndt, 1996).

The measure of interests used here, the IS, appears to be an excellent complementary assessment tool to the ADI/ADOS because it offers a detailed, continuous measure that can be used in both ASD and NT children and adults. This issue is particularly important when looking for measures to assess non-social phenotypes in family and/or genetic association studies. For example, a recent compelling study by Smith et al. (2009) identified a strong familial pattern of intense preoccupations, but used the ADI-R for the children with ASD and the Modified Personality Assessment Scale–Revised (Tyrer, 1988) for their biological parents. The IS would allow using the same instrument to assess this domain in the proband and first-degree relatives, whether siblings or parents. Additionally, the IS had strong positive correlations with scores on the ADOS and RBS-R in the subjects with HF-ASD. These findings lend support for the IS’s construct validity by revealing a strong relationship between intensity of CIs and other restricted and repetitive behaviors (RBS-R).

Contrary to previous research in ASD (Klin et al., 2007; Moore & Goodson, 2003), we did not find any significant relationships between number or intensity of CIs and age, gender, medication use or IQ in school age children and young adults. It would be especially important, however, to investigate this with the IS in a younger age range, since both the Moore and Goodson (2003) and Klin et al. (2007) studies showed greater prevalence of CIs in preschool children as compared to school-age children. The lack of association with IQ suggests that within the range of low average to above average IQ, CIs are not related to IQ as they are in samples that include individuals with ID. Inconsistent with our predictions, intensity and number of CIs did not correlate with IQ though our findings are consistent with a prior study of HF-ASD individuals (Bishop et al., 2006). CIs may be like “insistence on sameness” behaviors in that they are more prevalent in higher IQ individuals with ASD and unlike repetitive behaviors, which are more prevalent in lower IQ individuals with ASD (Richler et al., 2010).

The findings of this study, though maximizing ecological validity, are limited due to reliance upon parent-report measures of not only CIs, but also many of the correlates. Future research might include cross-informant and observational methods. Moreover, neuroimaging technologies could be used to assess neural underpinnings of intensity of CIs in HF-ASD and NT individuals, similar to prior investigations of interest areas in ASD (Grelotti et al., 2005). Longitudinal studies including preschool aged children could examine the changing nature of interests over time, look for distinguishing patterns in ASD and could explore the potential relationship between CIs and subsequent higher academic pursuits and/or employment opportunities/careers. Finally, our sample was purposely restricted to HF-ASD because of the higher prevalence of CIs in this group and the ability to have age matched controls, but future studies should also include the full autism spectrum.

In conclusion, consistent with ASD diagnostic criteria, we find more intense interests among individuals with HF-ASD versus NT using the IS. Intriguingly, the overall number of interests did not differ between HF-ASD and NT individuals, though NT individuals were more likely to show socially-oriented interests while individuals with HF-ASD were more likely to show factually-, object- or sensory-oriented interests. These findings have strong clinical implications regarding the importance of documenting intense, unusual interests as opposed to limited numbers of interests in diagnosing HF-ASDs. Finally, CIs were related to repetitive behaviors (from the RBS-R and ADOS), and autistic traits in general (from the SRS), which implies that CIs should be important targets of intervention. The association between CIs and everyday executive control difficulties (from the BRIEF) indicates that CI intervention strategies could include teaching EF skills. The BRIEF-CI association also has theoretical implications for our understanding of the link between executive dysfunction and the third aspect of the triad in ASD. Overall, these findings indicate that measures of CIs, such as the IS, may prove useful in future phenotyping and behavior genetic studies in HF-ASD.

Table 5.

Means and Primary Interest by Group and Age

Age Group N Primary Interest (%) IS Sum of Current Interests Mean (SD) IS Total Intensity Mean (SD)
7–8 NT 8 Television (25) 11.88 (4.45) 25.25 (4.77)
HF-ASD 25 Video games (17) 12.56 (5.90) 40.88 (11.92)
9–11 NT 22 Sports (22) 13.45 (9.70) 23.86 (5.57)
HF-ASD 18 Video games (20) 14.85 (6.81) 37.61 (10.39)
12–14 NT 19 Sports (20) 12.05 (6.44) 28.31 (8.37)
HF-ASD 23 Video games (29) 12.96 (5.27) 39.04 (11.67)
15–17 NT 17 Sports (39) 12.50 (6.40) 29.47 (6.01)
HF-ASD 27 Video games (33) 12.78 (6.73) 38.37 (8.86)
18–23 NT 6 Religion (38) 9.25 (4.27) 25.33 (6.28)
HF-ASD 11 Video games (18) 11.00 (7.22) 41.82 (13.53)

Acknowledgments

We thank the people with autism and their families who have educated us over the years. LGA and LK are supported by awards from the NIMH (R34 MH083053-01A2) and The Isadore and Bertha Gudelsky Family Foundation. LK and JDJ were supported by the Studies for the Advancement of Autism Research and Treatment (STAART: NIMH U54 MH066417). LK, MBH, AM, and GLW are supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, NIMH. BEY is supported by the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center (NIH IDDRC P30HD40677) and by an award from NIMH 5K23MH086111. BEY and KFJ were supported by the Frederick and Elizabeth Singer Foundation. The CNMC GCRC (NIH/NCRR) supported data collection.

Abbreviations

ADI-R

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised

ADOS

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ASD

autism spectrum disorder

BRIEF

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning

CIs

circumscribed interests

EF

executive functioning

FDR

False Discovery Rate

HF-ASD

high-functioning ASD

ID

Intellectual Disability

IS

Interests Scale

NT

neurotypical

RBS-R

Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised

SRS

Social Responsiveness Scale

WASI

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest

Lauren Kenworthy receives financial compensation for use of the BRIEF in this study. The other authors have no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Alexander JM, Johnson KE, Leibham ME, Kelley K. The development of conceptual interests in young children. Cognitive Development. 2004;23(2):324–334. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (fourth ed., text rev.) Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker MJ. Incorporating the thematic ritualistic behaviors of children with autism into games: Increasing social play interactions with siblings. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions. 2000;2(2):66–84. [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker MJ, Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Increasing the social behavior of young children with autism using their obsessive behaviors. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. 1998;23(4):300–308. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. ‘Obsessions’ in children with autism or Asperger Syndrome: a content analysis in terms of core domains of cognition. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;175:484–490. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.5.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological) 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bishop S, Richler J, Lord C. Restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section C) 2006;12(4–5):247–267. doi: 10.1080/09297040600630288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bishop SL, Richler J, Cain AC, Lord C. Predictors of perceived negative impact in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2007;112(6):450–461. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[450:POPNII]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bodfish JW. Western Carolina Center Research Reports. 2004. The interests scale. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Lewis MH. Western Carolina Center Research Reports. 1999. Repetitive behavior scale-revised. [Google Scholar]
  11. Boyd BA, Conroy MA, Mancil GR, Nakao PJ. Effects of circumscribed interests on the social behaviors of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(8):1550–1561. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0286-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carcani-Rathwell I, Rabe-Hasketh S, Santosh PJ. Repetitive and stereotyped behaviours in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:573–581. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01565.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Constantino JN. The social responsiveness scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  14. Constantino JN, Gruber CP, Davis S, Hayes S, Passanante N, Przybeck T. The factor structure of autistic traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(4):719–726. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00266.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cuccaro ML, Shao Y, Grubber J. Factor analysis of restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-R. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2003;34(1):3–17. doi: 10.1023/a:1025321707947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Deloache JS, Simcock G, Macari S. Planes, Trains, Automobiles - and Tea Sets: Extremely Intense Interests in Very Young Children. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:1579–1586. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  18. Geurts H, Corbett B, Solomon M. The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorders. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2009;13:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Grelotti DJ, Klin AJ, Gauthier I, Skudlarski P, Cohen DJ, Gore JC, Volkmar FR, Schultz RT. fMRI activation of the fusiform gyrus and amygdala to cartoon characters but not to faces in a boy with autism. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(3):373–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Johnson KE, Alexander JM, Spencer S, Leibham ME, Neitzel C. Factors associated with the early emergence of intense interests within conceptual domains. Cognitive Development. 2004;19(3):325–343. [Google Scholar]
  22. Kaplan E. A process approach to neuropsychological assessment. In: Boll T, Bryant BK, editors. Clinical Neuropsychology and Brain Function. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  23. Kenworthy L, Black DO, Harrison B, Della Rosa A, Wallace GL. Are Executive Control Functions Related to Autism Symptoms in High Functioning Children? Child Neuropsychology. 2009;15:425–440. doi: 10.1080/09297040802646983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kenworthy L, Yerys BE, Anthony LG, Wallace GL. Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychology Review. 2008;18(4):320–338. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9077-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Klin A, Danovitch JH, Merz AB, Volkmar FR. Circumscribed interests in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An exploratory study. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities. 2007;32(2):89–100. [Google Scholar]
  26. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K. Foetal testosterone, social relationships and restricted interests in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2005;46:198–210. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00349.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Hurley C, Frea WD. Improving social skills and disruptive behavior in children with autism through self-management. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1992;25(2):341–353. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1992.25-341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lainhart JE, Bigler ED, Bocian M, Coon H, Dinh E, Dawson G, Deutsch CK, Dunn M, Estes A, Tager-Flusberg H, Folstein S, Hepburn S, Hyman S, McMahon W, Minshew N, Munson J, Osann K, Ozonoff S, Rodier P, Rogers S, Sigman M, Spence MA, Stodgell CJ, Volkmar F. Head circumference and height in autism: A study by the collaborative program of excellence in autism. American Journal Medical Genetics. 2006;140A:2257–2274. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.31465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lam AU, Bodfish JW, Piven J. Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behavior in autism that differ in familiality and association with other symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49(11):1193–1200. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01944.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Le Couteur A, Rutter M, Lord C. Autism Diagnostic Interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders. 1989;19(3):363–387. doi: 10.1007/BF02212936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lopez BR, Lincoln AJ, Ozonoff S, Lai Z. Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of Autistic Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2005;35:445–460. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-5035-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule – generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30(3):205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview – revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24(5):659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McHale SM, Crouter AC, Tucker CJ. Free-time activities in middle school: Links with adjustment in early adolescence. Child Development. 2001;72(6):1764–1778. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Moore V, Goodson S. How well does early diagnosis of autism stand the test of time? Follow-up study of children assessed for autism at age 2 and development of an early diagnostic service. Autism. 2003;7(1):47–63. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007001005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. O’Hearn K, Asato M, Ordaz S, Luna B. Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20(4):1103–1132. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Piven J, Harper J, Palmer P, Arndt S. Course of behavioral change in autism: A retrospective study of high-IQ adolescents and adults. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35(4):523–529. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199604000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Richler J, Huerta M, Bishop SL, Lord C. Developmental trajectories of restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests in children with autism spectrum disorders. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22(1):55–69. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Oosterlaan J. How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioral and Brain Research. 2002;130:3–28. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00430-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Smith CJ, Lang CM, Kryzak L, Reichenberg A, Hollander E, Silverman JM. Familial associations of intense preoccupations, an empirical factor of the restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests domain of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50(8):982–990. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02060.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. South M, Ozonoff S, McMahon WM. The relationship between executive functioning, central coherence, and repetitive behaviors in the high-functioning autism spectrum. Autism. 2007;11(5):437–451. doi: 10.1177/1362361307079606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Bryson S, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Mahoney W, Goldberg J, Tuff L. Investigating the structure of the restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests domain of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):582–590. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01537.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Turner MA. Toward an executive dysfunction account of repetitive behavior in autism. In: Russell J, editor. Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  44. Turner M. Annotation: Repetitive behavior in autism: A review of psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40(6):839–849. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Tyrer P. Personality Assessment Schedule. In: Tyrer P, editor. Personality Disorders: Diagnosis, Management and Course. Butterworth; London: 1988. pp. 140–167. [Google Scholar]
  46. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children– Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  47. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  48. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  49. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  50. Welsh MC, Pennington BF. Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1988;4(3):199–230. [Google Scholar]
  51. Werner EE. Risk, resilience and recovery: Perspectives from the Kauai Longitudinal Study. Development and Psychopathology. 1993;5:503–515. [Google Scholar]
  52. Yerys BE, Wallace GL, Harrison B, Celano MJ, Giedd JN, Kenworthy L. Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders: Reversal shifting performance correlates with restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests. Autism. 2009;13:523–538. doi: 10.1177/1362361309335716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES