Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychooncology. 2015 Mar 9;24(12):1774–1783. doi: 10.1002/pon.3793

Concealment of Lung Cancer Diagnosis: Prevalence and Correlates

Brian D Gonzalez 1, Heather S L Jim 1, Julie M Cessna 1,2, Brent J Small 3, Steven K Sutton 4, Paul B Jacobsen 1
PMCID: PMC4564357  NIHMSID: NIHMS672598  PMID: 25753772

Abstract

Objective

Lung cancer has a commonly-understood behavioral etiology. Thus, lung cancer patients are often blamed for their illness and may seek to avoid this blame by concealing their diagnosis from others. This study sought to determine the prevalence of concealment and identify demographic, clinical, and psychosocial correlates of concealment among lung cancer patients.

Methods

A sample of 117 lung cancer patients receiving chemotherapy for non-small cell or small cell lung cancer was recruited and completed self-report demographic questionnaires, a measure of diagnosis concealment designed and pilot-tested for this study, and standard measures of psychosocial variables. Clinical factors were assessed via a medical chart review.

Results

Thirty participants (26%) reported concealing their diagnosis in the previous month, most frequently from casual friends and close friends. Reported reasons for concealment largely reflected concern for others. Univariate analyses indicated that those concealed their lung cancer diagnosis reported more internalized shame related to their illness and use of positive reappraisal as a coping strategy (ps ≤ .02). In addition, those who concealed were more likely to have used alcohol in the previous month and have a more recent recurrence, among those who had a recurrence (ps ≤ .04). Multivariate analyses indicated that internalized shame and use of positive reappraisal accounted for significant unique variance in concealment above and beyond that accounted for by use of alcohol (ps < .05).

Conclusions

Future research should aim to replicate and extend these findings with longitudinal designs to elucidate the directionality of the associations observed in this study.

Keywords: lung cancer, concealment, disclosure, stigma, shame

Background

Unlike many other forms of cancer, lung cancer is associated with a behavioral etiology, and individuals with lung cancer often perceive that they are stigmatized for their illness [13]. However, the extent to which patients conceal stigmatizing diseases, such as lung cancer, remains largely unstudied. The current study sought to identify the prevalence and correlates of concealment of lung cancer diagnosis and to determine whether concealment is associated with negative affect, behavior, and self-evaluation.

Studies have shown that lung cancer patients are more likely to be blamed for their illness than patients with other cancers [4]. Moreover, lung cancer patients blame themselves for their illness more than their primary caregivers [5]. Thus, individuals with lung cancer can be considered at higher risk of stigmatization than individuals with many other forms of cancer due to perceptions of blame. Moreover, studies have shown that illness-related perceived stigma is associated with worse social support, dyadic adjustment, depression, anxiety, and quality of life [68].

Although the motivation to conceal one’s lung cancer diagnosis is widely understood, no published quantitative studies have examined diagnosis concealment or its predictors among individuals with cancer. Several qualitative studies have suggested that men with cancer are particularly resistant to discussing or disclosing their diagnosis, citing gender expectations of them as being stoical men [9] or the need to protect others [10] as a rationale for their as rationale for concealment. Evidence in non-cancer populations suggests that greater introversion and trait social anxiety are associated with greater tendencies to conceal personal characteristics likely to be perceived as negative [11, 12]. In contrast, greater social support may have the opposite effect. Among individuals with HIV, those who reported greater social support were less likely to conceal their serostatus, and among those who disclosed their diagnosis the desire for support was the most often endorsed reason for disclosure [13]. Together, these findings suggest that men, more introverted patients, those with greater social anxiety, those with less social support, and those reporting less use of seeking support may be more likely to conceal their stigmatizing diagnoses.

In identifying potential consequences of concealment, the Cognitive-Affective-Behavioral Model of Concealable Stigma is instructive [14]. This model posits that concealment of potentially stigmatizing conditions often has negative self-evaluative, affective, and behavioral consequences. Research in non-cancer populations suggests that individuals who conceal a stigma come to appraise the stigma as more shameful, view themselves more negatively, and experience greater psychological distress than those who disclose the stigma [14]. In addition, individuals concealing a stigma may become more isolated in order to avoid the distress and cognitive demands associated with concealing a stigma from others.

Data from non-cancer populations support this conceptual model. For example, in one study college students who reported characteristics that might be considered concealable and stigmatizing reported greater anxiety and depression as well as lower self-esteem than students with potentially stigmatizing characteristics that are less concealable [15]. Another study found that in pregnant women who were planning to have an elective abortion, secrecy regarding the abortion at baseline was associated with greater distress two years later; this association was mediated by intrusive thoughts and suppression of thoughts about their abortion [16]. These findings suggest that concealment of a lung cancer diagnosis may be associated with negative affective implications and are consistent with current theoretical understanding of the consequences of concealing a concealable stigma [14].

Given the stigmatizing nature of lung cancer and the lack of data on lung cancer concealment, the present study sought to examine the prevalence as well as demographic, clinical, and psychosocial correlates of diagnosis concealment among lung cancer patients. We anticipated that a subset of participants would report concealing their lung cancer diagnosis in the previous month. We also expected that most of those who concealed would endorse concealing their diagnosis for fear of negative consequences, such as stigmatization. Demographic, clinical, and psychosocial correlates were examined for their associations with concealment. We hypothesized that diagnosis concealment would be associated with male gender, greater introversion, greater social anxiety, less social support, less use of seeking support as a coping strategy, as well as greater anxiety, depression, cancer-specific distress, and perceived stigma.

Methods

Participant Eligibility and Recruitment

Eligible patients for this study were: 1) receiving chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy for treatment of non-small cell lung cancer (NSCLC) or small cell lung cancer (SCLC), 2) free of any history of other cancers with the exception of non-melanoma skin cancers, 3) ≥18 years of age, 4) able to read English, and 5) able to provide informed consent. With Institutional Review Board approval, patients were recruited between February and December, 2012. Patients were approached by study staff at a routine outpatient visit or via mail for those not scheduled for a routine outpatient visit within the 3 weeks after they were identified as potentially eligible. All patients completed assessments at this time; participants were not compensated for their study participation.

Measures

Demographics and background information were collected using a self-report form assessing age, sex, race, ethnicity, education, income, marital status, and smoking history, which was used to classify participants as never smokers (<100 cigarettes in lifetime), former smokers, or current smokers.

A review of patients’ medical records assessed the following: date of lung cancer diagnosis, disease stage, disease type (small cell vs. non-small cell), previous lung cancer treatment, and ECOG performance status (a measure of overall well-being) [17].

Diagnosis concealment was assessed with a brief self-report measure that was designed and pilot-tested for this study. Respondents were asked to indicate whether or not they had chosen to conceal their lung cancer diagnosis (yes/no) from anyone within certain specified groups (i.e., family, friends, and coworkers) within the previous month. Those who indicated they had chosen to conceal their lung cancer diagnosis were asked to indicate their reason(s) for concealing by choosing from a specified list of reasons derived from pilot-testing of this questionnaire.

Coping strategies were assessed using the Coping Responses Inventory (CRI; [18]), a 48-item instrument which assesses specific coping responses to their cancer and treatment for cancer via eight subscales. Four subscales assess approach coping styles: seeking guidance and support (attempting to seek support from others), problem solving (taking action to deal with the problem), logical analysis (attempting to understand and prepare for the problem), and positive reappraisal (attempting to construe the problem in a positive way). Four subscales assess avoidant coping styles: seeking alternative rewards (engaging in alternative sources of satisfaction), emotional discharge (reducing tension by expressing negative feelings), cognitive avoidance (avoiding thinking realistically about the problem), and acceptance or resignation (reacting to the problem by accepting it). Higher scores indicate greater use of each coping style. The eight subscales of the CRI have demonstrated validity and reliability in cancer patients [18, 19]. Internal consistency reliability for this scale ranged from .44 – .70 in the current sample.

Extroversion was assessed using the 12-item Extroversion subscale of the NEO–Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; [20]). Higher scores indicate more extroversion. Sample items include “I like to have a lot of people around me” and “I really enjoy talking to people.” The NEO-FFI has demonstrated acceptable reliability and validity in the general population as well as in individuals with cancer [20, 21] and demonstrated internal consistency reliability in the current sample of .79.

Trait social anxiety and social avoidance were assessed using the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS; [22]), a 24-item measure which asks respondents to rate the fear/anxiety they experience during certain social situations (e.g., speaking up at a meeting). Respondents are then asked to indicate the degree to which they would avoid these social interactions. The trait social anxiety and social avoidance subscales are coded such that higher scores indicate greater social anxiety and avoidance. Both subscales have demonstrated acceptable reliability and validity [22, 23] and demonstrated internal consistency reliability in the current sample of .89 – .92.

Social support was assessed using the ENRICHD Social Support Instrument (ESSI; [24]), a 5-item instrument designed to assess emotional support. The ESSI has been shown to have acceptable reliability and validity, and higher scores indicate greater perceived social support [24]. Sample questions include “Is there someone available to give you good advice about a problem?” and “Is there someone available to you who shows you love and affection?” [24]. This scale has demonstrated validity and reliability among medically-ill populations, including lung cancer patients [8, 24, 25]. Internal consistency reliability in the current sample was .92.

Anxiety and depression were assessed using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; [26]). Higher scores indicate worse symptoms of anxiety and depression. This scale has demonstrated acceptable validity and reliability, has been used extensively in studies of patients with cancer [27, 28], and demonstrated internal consistency reliability in the current sample of .78.

Cancer-specific distress was assessed using the 22-item intrusion subscale of the Impact of Events Scale – Revised (IES-R; [29]). Higher scores on this scale indicate worse cancer-specific distress. Sample intrusion subscale items include “Other things kept making me think about it” and “Pictures about it popped into my head.” Respondents were instructed to rate items with regard to the diagnosis and treatment of their lung cancer. This scale, which has demonstrated acceptable reliability and validity, has been extensively used in the cancer population as a measure of cancer-specific distress [29, 30] and demonstrated internal consistency reliability in the current sample of .87.

Self-esteem was assessed using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; [31]), a 10-item Likert-type scale. Higher scores on this scale indicate greater self-esteem. This scale has demonstrated adequate reliability and validity and has been used with numerous populations, including with individuals with cancer [31, 32]. Internal consistency reliability in the current sample was .86.

Perceived lung cancer-related stigma was assessed using the Social Impact Scale (SIS; [33]), a 24-item scale which measures the extent to which individuals with an illness believe they are experiencing social rejection, financial insecurity, internalized shame, and social isolation as a result of their illness. Social rejection refers to the feeling of being discriminated against by others. Financial insecurity refers to the financial consequences of one’s stigmatizing condition. Internalized shame assesses the degree to which individuals feel ashamed due to their stigmatizing condition. Social isolation refers to the degree to which feel that their stigmatizing condition causes them to feel set apart from others. In addition to a total score, the measure yields subscale scores for the four aspects of experienced stigma described above. Higher scores on the total score and each subscale indicate greater perceived lung cancer-related stigma. These four subscales have been shown to have strong internal consistency [33]. The SIS has demonstrated validity and reliability among cancer patients [8, 33]. Internal consistency reliability in the current sample was .92.

Statistical Analyses

The frequencies of participants who concealed from various groups people and the reported reasons for concealment were calculated. Diagnosis concealment was dichotomized to compare individuals who did not conceal their diagnosis from anyone in the previous month to those who did conceal their diagnosis from anyone in the previous month. Independent samples t-tests and chi-square tests were conducted to identify demographic and clinical correlates of concealment. Effect sizes for differences between those who concealed and those who did not conceal were calculated using Cohen’s d [34]. Univariate logistic regression analyses were used to determine the association between diagnosis concealment (categorical dependent variable) and potential psychosocial correlates of concealment. The Holm-Bonferroni correction was used to control Type I error rate in analyses of psychosocial correlates of concealment [35]. In addition, a hierarchical logistic regression analysis examined whether psychosocial factors accounted for unique variance in diagnosis concealment above and beyond that contributed by associated demographic and clinical variables.

Results

Participants

Participant flow is shown in Supplementary Figure 1. One hundred ninety-six patients were approached for participation. A total of 157 (80%) signed consent. The patients who agreed to participate in the study did not differ in terms of age, gender, or race from those who declined to participate, ps ≥ .24. Four participants who agreed to participate were found to be ineligible before consent, three were found to be ineligible after consent, four withdrew from the study, and 29 did not complete the study measures. Analyses are based on the 117 participants who completed the study measures.

Participant demographic and clinical characteristics

Participants ranged in age from 36 to 85 years (see Table 1). The majority of the participants were high school graduates (70%), married (62%), White (82%), and previous smokers or current smokers (78%). On average, participants were 20.56 months (SD = 27.92) from their original lung cancer diagnosis. Twenty-seven participants (23%) had a recurrence of their lung cancer, thirty-seven (32%) had a surgical resection of this cancer, and 50 (43%) had been treated with radiation therapy.

Table 1.

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Sample (N = 117)

  Variable Total Sample (N = 117) Did Conceal (n = 30) Did Not Conceal (n = 87) p

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Age, years 64.22 (9.66) 61.47 (9.93) 65.17 (9.40) .07
Pack years of tobacco usea 42.52 (29.96) 46.48 (33.62) 40.22 (27.67) .39
Body mass index 26.33 (5.19) 25.79 (4.97) 26.52 (5.27) .51
Months since original diagnosis 20.56 (27.92) 22.03 (30.94) 20.05 (26.97) .74
Months since recurrenceb 9.74 (9.53) 4.22 (2.73) 12.50 (10.55) .03
Months since resectionc 26.46 (32.75) 31.17 (34.51) 24.20 (32.35) .55
Months since radiationd 13.40 (21.70) 19.83 (36.35) 11.37 (15.00) .24

  Variable n (%) n (%) n (%)

Gender .67
 Males 58 (50%) 17 (57%) 41 (47%)
 Females 59 (50%) 13 (43%) 46 (53%)
Education .36
 ≤High school graduate 35 (30%) 7 (23%) 28 (32%)
 > High school graduate 82 (70%) 23 (77%) 59 (68%)
Race .15
 White 96 (82%) 22 (73%) 74 (85%)
 Non-white 21 (18%) 8 (27%) 13 (15%)
Ethnicity .45
 Hispanic 5 (4%) 2 (7%) 3 (3%)
 Non-hispanic 112 (96%) 28 (93%) 84 (97%)
Marital Status .90
 Currently married 73 (62%) 19 (63%) 54 (62%)
 Not married 44 (38%) 11 (37%) 33 (38%)
Total Household Income .61
 < $40,000 29 (25%) 6 (20%) 23 (26%)
 ≥ $40,000 69 (59%) 20 (67%) 49 (56%)
 Declined to answer 19 (16%) 4 (13%) 15 (17%)
Alcohol Use in Previous Month .04
 No 69 (59%) 13 (43%) 56 (64%)
 Yes 48 (41%) 17 (57%) 31 (36%)
Cigarette Use .24
 Never 26 (22%) 9 (30%) 17 (20%)
 Previous & Current 91 (78%) 21 (70%) 70 (80%)
Type of Lung Cancer .07
 NSCLC 104 (89%) 24 (80%) 80 (92%)
 SCLC 13 (11%) 6 (20%) 7 (8%)
NSCLC Disease Stagee .91
 I – II 14 (12%) 4 (13%) 10 (11%)
 III 17 (15%) 3 (10%) 14 (16%)
 IV 73 (62%) 17 (57%) 56 (64%)
SCLC Disease Stagef .91
 Limited stage SCLC 2 (2%) 1 (3%) 1 (1%)
 Extensive stage SCLC 11 (9%) 5 (17%) 6 (7%)
ECOG Performance Status .39
 0 23 (20%) 9 (30%) 14 (16%)
 1 84 (72%) 19 (63%) 65 (75%)
 2 – 4 10 (9%) 2 (7%) 8 (9%)
Taking antidepressants .76
 No 92 (79%) 23 (77%) 69 (79%)
 Yes 25 (21%) 7 (23%) 18 (21%)
Taking sedative medication .23
 No 67 (57%) 20 (67%) 47 (54%)
 Yes 50 (43%) 10 (33%) 40 (46%)
History of lung cancer recurrence .30
 No 90 (77%) 21 (70%) 69 (79%)
 Yes 27 (23%) 9 (30%) 18 (21%)
History of resection .25
 No 80 (68%) 18 (60%) 62 (71%)
 Yes 37 (32%) 12 (40%) 25 (29%)
Treated with radiation .94
 Never 67 (57%) 18 (60%) 49 (56%)
 Finished radiation before consent 42 (36%) 10 (33%) 32 (37%)
 Receiving radiation when consented 8 (7%) 2 (7%) 6 (7%)
Received chemotherapy .56
 Never 1 (1%) 0 (0%) 1 (1%)
 Receiving chemotherapy 116 (99%) 30 (100%) 86 (99%)

Note. ECOG = Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group. NSCLC = non-small cell lung cancer. SCLC = small cell lung cancer.

a

Among only past smokers and current smokers (n = 91).

b

Among only those with a recurrence (n = 27).

c

Among only those with a resection (n = 37).

d

Among only those with radiation (n = 50).

e

Among only those with NSCLC (n = 104).

f

Among only those with SCLC (n = 13).

Prevalence of and reasons for concealment

Thirty participants (26%) reported concealing in the previous month. Frequencies of concealment from various groups, among those who reported concealing their diagnosis, as well as reasons for concealment are presented in Table 2. Among those who concealed their diagnosis in the month prior to study participation, they most frequently concealed from casual friends (67%), close friends (40%), and family members they did not consider close relatives (27%). Of those who concealed in the previous month, 10 (33%) concealed from people in more than one category. Within most categories of concealment from groups of people, the majority of participants reported cited concern for others as reasons for concealment of their lung cancer diagnosis.

Table 2.

Frequencies of Concealment From Various Groups and Reasons for Concealment for Patients Who Reported Concealment (n = 30)

Concealment From n (%) n (%)
Within Category
Close Family 5 (17%)
 I didn’t want to overburden them 2 (40%)
 I didn’t want them to worry about me 1 (20%)
 Other 1 (20%)
 Missing 1 (20%)

Other Family 8 (27%)
 I didn’t want them to worry about me 5 (63%)
 I didn’t want them to take pity on me 2 (25%)
 I didn’t want to overburden them 1 (13%)

Work Supervisor 0 (0%)

Co-Worker 4 (13%)
 Other 3 (75%)
 I was concerned that they might judge me 1 (25%)

Close Friend 12 (40%)
 I didn’t want them to worry about me 7 (58%)
 I didn’t want them to take pity on me 3 (25%)
 I didn’t want to overburden them 1 (8%)
 Other 1 (8%)

Casual Friend 20 (67%)
 Other 10 (53%)
 I didn’t want them to take pity on me 6 (32%)
 I didn’t want them to worry about me 2 (11%)
 I didn’t want to overburden them 1 (4%)

Identifying demographics and clinical correlates of concealment

Comparisons were made between those who concealed and those who did not on demographic measures (see Table 1). Contrary to expectations, gender was not associated with concealment (p = .67). Exploratory analyses indicated that those who concealed their diagnosis were more likely to report drinking alcohol in the previous month (57%) than those who did not conceal their diagnosis (36%, p = .04). There was also a non-significant trend towards an association between age and concealment, such that those who concealed were younger than those who did not conceal, p = .07. History of smoking, and marital status were not associated with concealment (ps ≥ .24).

Regarding potential clinical correlates of concealment, exploratory analyses indicated that among patients with a recurrence of lung cancer, those who recurred more recently were more likely to report concealing their diagnosis than others (p = .03). There was a non-significant trend towards an association between type of lung cancer (e.g., NSCLC vs. SCLC) and concealment, such that patients with SCLC (40%) were more likely to conceal their diagnosis than patients with NSCLC (23%, p = .07). Body mass index, time since diagnosis, time since resection, time since radiation, disease stage, ECOG performance status, use of antidepressants, and use of sedatives were not associated with concealment (ps ≥ .23). Similarly, no associations were found between concealment and whether participants had a recurrence, a surgical resection of their lung cancer, radiotherapy, or chemotherapy (ps ≥ .25).

Psychosocial correlates of diagnosis concealment

As hypothesized, those who concealed their diagnosis reported greater internalized shame related to the diagnosis of lung cancer than those who did not conceal their diagnosis (p < .01) (see Table 3). Those who reported greater use of positive reappraisal as a coping strategy were also more likely to report concealing their diagnosis in the previous month (p = .02). Contrary to expectations, extroversion, social anxiety, social support, other coping strategies, anxiety, depression, cancer-specific distress, social avoidance, and self-esteem were not associated with concealment (ps ≥ .15). In addition, perceived social rejection, financial insecurity, social isolation, and total perceived stigma were not associated with diagnosis concealment (ps ≥ .33).

Table 3.

Psychological Differences Between Participants Who Did Conceal (n = 30) vs. Did Not Conceal (n = 87)

Did Conceal (n = 30) Did Not Conceal (n = 87)

Variable M (SD) M (SD) d t
Extroversion 26.59 (6.39) 28.64 (6.58) 0.31 1.47
Social Anxiety 12.17 (10.72) 13.74 (12.42) 0.13 0.62
Social Avoidance 17.17 (10.55) 17.89 (13.06) 0.06 0.27
Social Support 22.14 (3.93) 22.29 (4.16) 0.10 0.18
Coping Responses
 Logical Analysis 46.26 (8.14) 45.09 (15.27) 0.10 −0.37
 Positive Reappraisal 56.94 (4.64) 52.70 (8.39) 0.63 −3.31**
 Seeking Guidance & Support 53.87 (8.16) 53.33 (8.50) 0.06 −0.29
 Problem Solving 52.66 (5.88) 52.27 (8.72) 0.05 −0.20
 Cognitive Avoidance 49.97 (8.63) 50.78 (8.61) 0.09 0.42
 Acceptance or Resignation 50.03 (10.34) 49.64 (8.48) 0.04 −0.19
 Seeking Alternative Rewards 52.35 (9.92) 53.22 (9.26) 0.09 0.41
 Emotional Discharge 53.18 (8.24) 51.78 (8.55) 0.17 −0.73
Anxiety 7.60 (2.93) 8.49 (3.42) 0.28 1.28
Depression 6.90 (3.10) 6.89 (2.76) 0.00 −0.03
Cancer-Specific Distress 0.81 (0.76) 0.89 (0.72) 0.11 0.48
Self-Esteem 23.10 (5.07) 22.70 (5.37) 0.08 −0.35
Perceived Stigma
 Social Rejection 13.60 (3.32) 13.28 (4.22) 0.08 −0.38
 Financial Insecurity 4.50 (2.13) 4.71 (2.32) 0.09 0.43
 Internalized Shame 9.93 (2.59) 8.34 (2.42) 0.63 −3.05**
 Social Isolation 13.93 (3.37) 13.47 (4.51) 0.12 −0.52
 Total Perceived Stigma 41.97 (8.42) 39.79 (11.03) 0.22 −0.98
**

p < .01

A hierarchical logistic regression indicated that positive reappraisal and internalized shame together accounted for an additional 16% of the variance in concealment above and beyond that accounted for by use of alcohol (see Table 4). Controlling for the effect of use of alcohol, one-point increases in the positive reappraisal or internalized shame scales were associated with an increase in the odds of concealing one’s diagnosis by 10% and 31%, respectively.

Table 4.

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Concealment (n = 117)

Step Predictors B (SE) Odds Ratio 95% C.I. ΔR2
1 Alcohol use in previous month (yes) −0.99* (0.49) 0.37 (0.14–0.97) .07
2 Use of positive reappraisal for coping 0.10* (0.04) 1.10 (1.07–1.62) .16
Internalized shame 0.27* (0.11) 1.31 (1.02–1.19)

Note. Step 1 model fit, −2 log likelihood = 117.68, Nagelkerke R2 = .07; Step 2 model fit, −2 log likelihood = 104.31, Nagelkerke R2 = .23.

*

p < .05

Conclusions

The present study was the first to examine the prevalence and correlates of diagnosis concealment in lung cancer patients. Of a sample of 117 lung cancer patients, 26% reported concealing their diagnosis in the previous month. Participants predominantly reported concealing from casual friends.

Patients in this study often endorsed reasons for concealing their diagnosis that reflected concern for others, including not wanting to overburden others and concern that they would overburden others with their troubles. These were the most commonly endorsed reasons among those who reported concealing from close family, extended family, and close friends. This is contrary to our expectations that most patients who concealed their diagnosis would report doing so for fear of judgment or social isolation from others. These concerns were only endorsed by one of four patients who concealed from co-workers and one of twenty participants who reported concealed from casual friends. In addition, use of alcohol and a more recent lung cancer recurrence (for those whose lung cancer recurred) were associated with diagnosis concealment. Patients who reported greater use of positive reappraisal as a coping strategy and more lung cancer-related internalized shame were also more likely to conceal their diagnosis. These differences reflect medium effect sizes (i.e., d = .63).

Theoretical implications

Contrary to the Cognitive-Affective-Behavioral Model of Concealable Stigma [14] hypotheses regarding negative affective, behavioral, and self-evaluative correlates of concealment were largely not supported. Anxiety, depression, cancer-specific distress, social avoidance, and self-esteem were not associated with diagnosis concealment. However, the finding that patients who concealed their lung cancer diagnoses reported greater internalized shame is consistent with the hypothesized impact of concealment on affective outcomes. This pattern of results raises questions about whether the theorized links between concealment and its consequences apply to lung cancer patients. It is possible that the negative consequences of concealing one’s lung cancer diagnosis were not significant enough to impact participants’ overall well-being or were not detectable by this study’s measures. That is, concealing a stigma such as lung cancer may result in negative consequences, but these negative consequences may be short-lived and go unnoticed in retrospective studies such as the present study.

Previous studies in potentially stigmatizing diseases have focused on the rate of disclosure rather than concealment. For example, in a sample of breast cancer patients 23% and 30% reported they disclosed their diagnosis “not at all” or “a little” to family and friends, respectively [36]. Similarly, a study of HIV-positive individuals referred for psychiatric evaluation reported that 53% were open about their serostatus “some of the time” or less often [37]. In contrast, the current study focuses on the rate at which and reasons for which lung cancer patients deliberately chose not to share their diagnosis with others. Whereas disclosing requires an active, conscious decision to share one’s diagnosis, concealment can be a more passive act.

Another possible explanation for the lack of support for the study’s hypotheses involves the patients’ reasons for concealment. Many reported concealing for reasons having to do with concern for others (e.g., “I didn’t want them to worry about me”). Thus, it is possible that the potential negative consequence of disclosure (i.e., their loved one’s excessive worry about them) may not have been sufficiently negative to elicit the adverse affective, behavioral, and self-evaluative consequences hypothesized. However, the reasons reported for concealment are at odds with the significant relationship between concealment and internalized shame. Patients who reported these reasons for concealment may have been rationalizing shame-based concealment. In addition, patients who experienced greater internalized shame may have been using concealment as an adaptive coping strategy to avoid disclosing to unsupportive members of their social network. Lastly, the greater use of positive reappraisal among patients who concealed their diagnosis may have served to reduce their distress.

Clinical implications

With regard to clinical implications, these findings suggest that some patients may wish to conceal their illnesses from others, including close family members. Thus, providers should be particularly cautions not to accidentally disclose patients’ diagnoses of lung cancer or other potentially stigmatizing conditions. Accordingly, providers may wish to discuss plans for ensuring patients’ privacy with their lung cancer patients. In addition, the findings suggest that mental healthcare professionals treating individuals with cancer consider discussing concealment or disclosure of their diagnosis with their patients. Those who experience more internalized shame regarding their lung cancer may be more likely to conceal their illness.

Limitations and future directions

The cross-sectional nature of this study’s data collection limits the conclusions that can be drawn from its findings. Although the data can be interpreted as suggesting that use of positive reappraisal as a coping strategy increases the likelihood that lung cancer patients will conceal their diagnoses and that concealment contributes to greater internalized shame, the possibility of reverse relationships between these measures cannot be ruled out. Future studies should test this theoretical model using in vivo studies to examine the impact of concealment vs. disclosure of one’s diagnosis to confederate strangers. Such longitudinal studies could also clarify the directionality of relationships between concealment and the correlates identified in this study.

The sample’s homogeneity with regard to race, ethnicity, and receipt of treatment limits the generalizability of the study’s findings to the broader population of individuals with lung cancer. Future studies should aim to recruit more diverse samples of individuals with lung cancer. For example, concerns over the ability to conceal a lung cancer diagnosis may be less salient for patients finished with therapy for lung cancer.

This study used a generic measure of perceived stigma that may not capture the self-blame associated with lung cancer. Future studies in this area should consider stigma measures specific to lung cancer patients, such as the Cataldo Lung Cancer Stigma Scale [38]. This scale may better assess association between smoking and stigma in lung cancer patients, which has been previously demonstrated [39].

Prescription of antidepressant and sedative medications were not related to concealment in this study. However, participants’ use of psychotherapy and related services was not assessed. Thus, the potential buffering effect of these services could not be ascertained.

This study dichotomized concealment of one’s lung cancer diagnosis, grouping together those who concealed from only one casual friend with those who concealed from close family members. However, concealment may be better conceptualized as a continuous variable to reflect the varying degrees to which individuals conceal their diagnoses from others. Dichotomizing concealment may have artificially increased the error in the measurement of concealment, thereby reducing the study’s power to identify significant effects. Future studies should examine the frequency of concealment or study concealment as a continuous variable by measuring the varying degrees of concealment from others. In addition, this study did not assess the degree to which patients disclosed their diagnoses to others. Concealment and disclosure are complementary constructs; however, disclosure is a more discreet variable that merits examination in future studies.

Another limitation is the low internal consistency reliabilities for some of the Coping Responses Inventory subscales. These low internal consistency reliabilities may partially explain the lack of associations between concealment and the coping strategies assessed. Thus, the lack of associations between concealment and some of the coping strategies should be interpreted with caution.

Lastly, this study’s limited sample size precluded the examination of the impact of concealing from various groups of individuals, such as close family vs. extended family members. Larger studies could also explore the degree to which reasons for concealment vary depending upon the person from whom the diagnosis is concealed (e.g., close family vs. close friends).

Acknowledgments

Funding sources: This work was supported by a grant from the National Cancer Institute: R25CA090314 (PI: Jacobsen)

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  • 1.Chapple A, Ziebland S, McPherson A. Stigma, shame, and blame experienced by patients with lung cancer: Qualitative study. British Medical Journal. 2004;328:1470. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38111.639734.7C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Holland JC, Kelly BJ, Weinberger MI. Why psychosocial care is difficult to integrate into routine cancer care: Stigma is the elephant in the room. Journal of the National Comprehensive Cancer Network. 2010;8:362–366. doi: 10.6004/jnccn.2010.0028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schönfeld N, Timsit JF. Overcoming a stigma: The lung cancer patient in the intensive care unit. European Respiratory Journal. 2008;31:3–5. doi: 10.1183/09031936.00126307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Marlow LA, Waller J, Wardle J. Variation in blame attributions across different cancer types. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention. 2010;19:1799–1805. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-09-1298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lobchuk MM, Murdoch T, McClement SE, et al. A dyadic affair: Who is to blame for causing and controlling the patient’s lung cancer? Cancer Nursing. 2008;31:435–443. doi: 10.1097/01.NCC.0000339253.68324.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cataldo JK, Brodsky JL. Lung cancer stigma, anxiety, depression and symptom severity. Oncology. 2013;85:33–40. doi: 10.1159/000350834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cataldo JK, Jahan TM, Pongquan VL. Lung cancer stigma, depression, and quality of life among ever and never smokers. European Journal of Oncology Nursing. 2012;16:264–269. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2011.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gonzalez BD, Jacobsen PB. Depression in lung cancer patients: the role of perceived stigma. Psycho-Oncology. 2012;21:239–246. doi: 10.1002/pon.1882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hilton S, Emslie C, Hunt K, et al. Disclosing a cancer diagnosis to friends and family: A gendered analysis of young men’s and women’s experiences. Qualitative Health Research. 2009;19:744–754. doi: 10.1177/1049732309334737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Maliski SL, Rivera S, Connor S, et al. Renegotiating masculine identity after prostrate cancer treatment. Qualitative Health Research. 2008;18:1609–1620. doi: 10.1177/1049732308326813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Endler NS, Flett GL, Macrodimitris SD, et al. Separation, self-disclosure, and social evaluation anxiety as facets of trait social anxiety. European journal of personality. 2002;16:239–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wismeijer A, van Assen M. Do neuroticism and extraversion explain the negative association between self-concealment and subjective well-being? Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:345–349. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Petrak JA, Doyle AM, Smith A, et al. Factors associated with selfdisclosure of HIV serostatus to significant others. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2001;6:69–79. doi: 10.1348/135910701169061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pachankis JE. The psychological implications of concealing a stigma: A cognitive-affective-behavioral model. Psychological bulletin. 2007;133:328–345. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Frable DE, Platt L, Hoey S. Concealable stigmas and positive self-perceptions: Feeling better around similar others. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1998;74:909. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.4.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Major B, Gramzow RH. Abortion as stigma: Cognitive and emotional implications of concealment. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1999;77:735. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.77.4.735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Oken MM, Creech RH, Tormey DC, et al. Toxicity and response criteria of the Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group. American journal of clinical oncology. 1982;5:649–656. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Moos RH. Coping Responses Inventory: Adult Form: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Chan Y, Ng T, Lee PW, et al. Symptoms, coping strategies, and timing of presentations in patients with newly diagnosed ovarian cancer. Gynecologic oncology. 2003;90:651–656. doi: 10.1016/s0090-8258(03)00394-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Costa PT, McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Montgomery GH, Kangas M, David D, et al. Fatigue during breast cancer radiotherapy: An initial randomized study of cognitive–behavioral therapy plus hypnosis. Health Psychology. 2009;28:317. doi: 10.1037/a0013582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Liebowitz MR. Social phobia. Modern problems of pharmacopsychiatry. 1987;22:141–173. doi: 10.1159/000414022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Heimberg RG, Horner K, Juster H, et al. Psychometric properties of the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. Psychological medicine. 1999;29:199–212. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mitchell PH, Powell L, Blumenthal J, et al. A short social support measure for patients recovering from myocardial infarction: The ENRICHD Social Support Inventory. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation and Prevention. 2003;23:398–403. doi: 10.1097/00008483-200311000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Vaglio J, Conard M, Poston W, et al. Testing the performance of the ENRICHD Social Support Instrument in cardiac patients. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes. 2004;2:24–28. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-2-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1983;67:361–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ibbotson T, Maguire P, Selby P, et al. Screening for anxiety and depression in cancer patients: The effects of disease and treatment. European journal of cancer. 1994;30:37–40. doi: 10.1016/s0959-8049(05)80015-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jacobsen PB, Jim HS. Psychosocial interventions for anxiety and depression in adult cancer patients: Achievements and challenges. CA: a cancer journal for clinicians. 2008;58:214–230. doi: 10.3322/CA.2008.0003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Weiss DS, Marmar CR. The Impact of Event Scale—Revised. In: Wilson J, Keane T, editors. Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. New York: Guilford Press; 1997. pp. 399–411. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Floyd A, Dedert E, Ghate S, et al. Depression may mediate the relationship between sense of coherence and quality of life in lung cancer patients. Journal of health psychology. 2011;16:249–257. doi: 10.1177/1359105310371856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rosenberg M. Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Greenfield DM, Walters SJ, Coleman RE, et al. Quality of life, self-esteem, fatigue, and sexual function in young men after cancer. Cancer. 2010;116:1592–1601. doi: 10.1002/cncr.24898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fife BL, Wright ER. The dimensionality of stigma: A comparison of its impact on the self of persons with HIV/AIDS and cancer. Journal of health and social behavior. 2000:50–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian journal of statistics. 1979:65–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Henderson B, Davison K, Pennebaker J, et al. Disease disclosure patterns among breast cancer patients. Psychology and Health. 2002;17:51–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Strachan ED, Bennett WRM, Russo J, et al. Disclosure of HIV status and sexual orientation independently predicts increased absolute CD4 cell counts over time for psychiatric patients. Psychosomatic medicine. 2007;69:74–80. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000249900.34885.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Cataldo JK, Slaughter R, Jahan TM, et al. Measuring stigma in people with lung cancer: Psychometric testing of the Cataldo Lung Cancer Stigma Scale. Oncology Nursing Forum. doi: 10.1188/11.ONF.E46-E54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.LoConte NK, Else-Quest NM, Eickhoff J, et al. Assessment of guilt and shame in patients with non–small-cell lung cancer compared with patients with breast and prostate cancer. Clinical lung cancer. 2008;9:171–178. doi: 10.3816/CLC.2008.n.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES