Abstract
Within the NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework, dimensions of behavior are investigated across diagnoses with the goal of developing a better understanding of their underlying neural substrates. Currently, this framework includes five domains: cognitive, social, arousal/regulatory, and negative and positive valence systems. We argue that the inclusion of a motor systems domain is sorely needed as well. Independent of medication, distinct areas of motor dysfunction (e.g., motor planning/inhibition/learning/coordination, involuntary movements) commonly appear across a number of mental disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, autism, ADHD, Alzheimer’s, depression) as well as neurological disorders accompanied by significant psychological symptoms (e.g., Parkinson’s disease). In addition, motor systems are amenable to study across multiple levels of analysis from the cellular molecular level focusing on cytoarchitechtonics and neurotransmitter systems, to networks and circuits measured using neuroimaging, and finally at the level of overt behavioral performance. Critically, the neural systems associated with motor performance have been relatively well defined, and different circuits have been linked to distinct aspects of motor behavior. As such, they may also be differentially associated with symptoms and motor dysfunction across diagnoses, and be uniquely informative about underlying etiology. Importantly, motor signs can change across stages of illness; they are also often present in the prodromal phases of disease and closely linked with course, suggesting that these behaviors represent a core feature reflective of pathogenic processes. The inclusion of a motor domain would allow researchers to better understand psychopathology more broadly, and may also reveal important contributions to disease processes across diagnoses.
The Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative was introduced by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to provide a cutting edge framework for the study of brain disorders. The principle goal of the RDoC initiative is to combine the study of behavior with neuroscience research, to better understand psychopathology and develop targeted treatment options (Sanislow et al. 2010; Cuthbert & Insel 2013). Within this framework, constructs are investigated dimensionally across diagnoses with the goal of developing a better understanding how respective neural substrates are involved. Ultimately, such an approach stands to increase our understanding of disease processes, particularly given the emphasis on the biological substrates from multiple levels of analysis (eg., cellular, genetic, brain, behavior). This relatively new initiative has already proved fruitful in providing new integrated frameworks (eg., Langenecker et al. 2014; Dillon et al. 2014), and as current research conducted as part of the RDoC initiative matures, mental health research stands to make great strides.
As it stands currently, there are five constructs included in the RDoC initiative: negative valence systems, positive valence systems, cognitive systems, systems for social processes, and arousal/regulatory systems. Noticeably missing however are motor systems. Consistent with the other 5 constructs in RDoC, motor behavior also varies greatly amongst individuals, and there is a plethora of evidence to indicate that there are motor deficits across psychiatric diagnoses including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depression, and Alzheimer’s disease, as well as in developmental psychopathology such as Autism spectrum disorders (ASD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and tic disorder. Further, given that a number of motor behaviors occur in clinical populations that we have a much more developed etiological conception for (e.g., Huntington’s, Parkinson’s, stroke), understanding these behaviors in psychiatric populations could potentially allow for us to tap into this sizeable work more directly, and make significant strides in conceptualizing and treating mental illness. Given that motor dysfunction is present in many psychiatric diagnoses across the lifespan (Quinn et al. 2001), inclusion of such a category stands to provide key insights into the underlying biology associated with psychopathology. Though the nature of the motor deficits varies to some degree across disorders, there are also cases of overlap across diagnoses with respect a particular motor behavior. Critically, in many cases motor systems dysfunction is present in the absence of medication (Caligiuri & Lohr 1994; Fenton et al. 1994), indicating that these deficits are not merely a side effect of pharmacological treatments. Furthermore, evidence also indicates that motor systems dysfunction is present prior to disease onset (e.g., Mittal et al. 2010). Together this suggests that it may be a trait feature of psychopathology, though it is likely that there are state effects with respect to the severity of this dysfunction associated with disease course, symptom severity, and medications.
Motor Dysfunction Across Disorders
Though beyond the scope of this commentary, it is known that multiple neural systems underlie and contribute to motor behaviors, including cerebello-thalamo-cortical and striatal-cortical networks, pre-frontal and pre-motor response selection contributions, and common across these respective systems, the final common pathway wherein a motor signal is sent from the primary motor cortex to initiate a movement. To illustrate the range of motor dysfunction seen across diagnoses, we provide a brief overview with respect to several categories of psychiatric disorder. Motor dysfunction as discussed here includes neuromotor and psychomotor deficits. The former refers to hyper- and dyskinetic movements, dysfunctional sensorimotor integration, neurological soft signs, and deficits in gait and posture. The latter refers to hypo-kinesia, psychomotor slowing, and catatonia. In addition, deficits in motor learning are included in this proposed cluster, and both neuromotor and psychomotor dysfunction can contribute to these deficits. Though motor learning paradigms may be confounded with cognitive function given higher order cognitive influences (e.g., Anguera et al. 2010), learning deficits above and beyond those due to cognitive decline may also be present given that cognitive factors only account for a portion of the variance in motor learning (Anguera et al. 2010). Motor learning paradigms allow investigators to tap into important motor cortical circuits, which may be impacted across diagnoses. Within a given disorder, motor dysfunction of both types may be present, and understanding the associated neural circuitry could allow us to better understand psychopathology, further supporting the case for a motor category within the RDoC framework.
Developmental Disorders
A wide range of motor deficits have been reported in ASD including catatonia (eg., Ghaziuddin et al. 2005), though these seem to primarily relate to sensorimotor integration as it relates to motor planning (reviewed in Gowen & Hamilton 2013). Recent work also suggests that there are deficits in both the feed-forward and feedback control mechanisms (neuromotor deficits) associated with cerebellar motor control in ASD (Mosconi et al. 2015). In all cases however, these motor deficits may be driving some of the symptoms seen in these individuals, particularly those with respect to social and language functions. In ADHD the deficits differ, but include neuromotor deficits such as postural control and gait, similar (though lesser in magnitude) to those seen in children with cerebellar lesions (Buderath et al. 2009). In addition fine motor control and coordination deficits have also been demonstrated in boys with ADHD (Piek et al. 1999). Finally, motor learning deficits have been reported in both populations (Mostofsky et al. 2000; Barnes et al. 2010; Izawa et al. 2012). Thus, across developmental disorders, motor deficits are present. In particular, in ASD, these seem to be a hallmark of the disease itself, as opposed to a side-effect of medications, though this is likely also the case in ADHD. Across these disorders, the motor dysfunction may be indicative of deficits in the underlying circuitry (eg. fronto-striatal; cerebello-thalamo-cortical) that are seen in other psychiatric disorders.
Psychosis and Psychosis Risk
A variety of neuromotor and psychomotor signs and symptoms have been reported in patients with schizophrenia, as well as in individuals at risk for developing psychosis (Bernard & Mittal 2014). Motor dysfunction in psychosis populations varies greatly and includes relatively diffuse neurological soft signs (Mittal et al. 2014), dyspraxia (Schiffman et al. 2015), postural control deficits (Marvel et al. 2004; Bernard et al. 2014), and both hypo- and hyper-kinesias (Mittal et al. 2010; Pappa & Dazzan 2009), catatonia and psychomotor slowing (Walther & Strik 2012), and motor learning deficits (Marvel et al. 2007). What is particularly interesting is that in psychosis risk populations where confounds such as drug abuse and medications are less prevalent, movement abnormalities have been useful in predicting symptom course and disease progression (Dean et al. 2015; Mittal et al. 2010) indicating that these are trait deficits, and they may serve as an easily quantifiable biomarker of disease course. However, striatally-based dyskinesias and cerebellar-mediated postural control also seem to be distinctly associated with positive and negative symptom course, respectively (Dean et al. 2015; Mittal et al. 2010). Thus, not only is motor dysfunction present in psychosis, but it also seems to be linked to the core symptomatology, and this may be insightful for other diagnostic groups that share symptoms and motor deficits.
Mood Disorders
Psychomotor symptoms, particularly slowing, are striking in mood disorders (eg., Cornell et al. 1984). In patients with major depression, movements are slowed, and this impacts speech, eye movements, facial affect, posture, and the movement of the limbs more generally (Buyukdura et al. 2011), and catatonia has been reported in up to 20% of patients (Starkstein et al. 1996). Both psychomotor and neuromotor deficits may be contributing to motor learning differences seen in patients with depression (Naismith et al. 2010). Though bipolar disorder shares some features with depression in that blunted affect and similar mood symptoms are present during periods of depression, these cycle with periods of mania. The motor deficits seen in this particular population also differ to some degree. In bipolar disorder, neuromotor dysfunction including postural sway deficits have been reported (Bolbecker et al. 2011), and neurological soft signs are also present (Goswami et al. 2006). Thus, at the surface at least in terms of motor dysfunction, bipolar disorder seems to have more in common with psychosis than it does with major depression. However, this remains an empirical question. Investigating these motor deficits across disorders however will allow for the direct investigation of this suggestion, and may also shed light on to key underlying neural dysfunction seen across disorders with similar motor deficits.
Aging and Alzheimer’s Disease
Even in healthy aging, individuals experience a decline in normal motor function (reviewed in Seidler et al. 2010). This includes general slowing of movements, declines in postural control, changes in force production and stability, and motor learning deficits (Seidler et al. 2010). However, in age-related pathologies such as Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment, motor systems dysfunction has also been reported. This includes neuromotor deficits in balance and gait, but also extends to dual-task motor paradigms (Pettersson et al. 2005). Furthermore, in both Parkinson’s disease (with and without dementia) and Lewy Body dementia, the biological underpinnings are well-known, and may aid in linking neural circuits and biology to motor signs and symptoms seen across psychopathology. Not only would the inclusion of a motor dimension allow for the investigation of similarities in symptoms and etiology across disorders, but this also dovetails with the recent suggestion from Casey and colleagues for the inclusion of a neurodevelopmental component to the RDoC framework (Casey et al. 2014).
Benefits of a Motor Dimension
Because different neural systems (e.g., cerebellar, basal ganglia, frontal action selection, corticospinal tract) subserve different aspects of motor control, they also may be differentially associated with symptoms and dysfunctional aspects of motor control across diagnoses. For example, dopaminergic dysfunction impacting the basal ganglia results in a variety of movement abnormalities. One can truly grasp the range of impact that this system has on movement when one considers that patients with Parkinson’s disease often experience difficulty in initiating internally generated movements, but with medications can also experience dyskinesias (Obeso et al. 2000). A similar range of dysfunction, from psychomotor slowing to dyskinetic movements can be seen when we investigate mood disorders as well as psychosis, and ADHD, pointing to a potential dopaminergic basal ganglia contribution. Indeed, the basal ganglia have been implicated in psychomotor slowing in depression (Buyukdura et al. 2011), as well as in dyskinesias seen in psychosis risk populations (Mittal et al. 2010). Paralleling this motor basal ganglia dysfunction, cerebellar motor circuits are also implicated across disorders including Autism spectrum disorders and psychosis, taking the form of motor learning deficits, timing dysfunction, and poor postural control. Finally, a recent investigation in major depression has found that there are white matter abnormalities in the corticospinal tract of patients relative to controls (Sacchet et al. 2014), implicating the final common pathway in psychopathology as well. In all cases, overt motor dysfunction points to several well-studied neural subsystems, and also may be related to differing symptom profiles. Finally, by looking at associations between different motor signs and symptoms, and their relationships to one another, as well as to cognition, we may gain further insight into the neural systems, and subsystems contributing uniquely, and in concert, across diagnoses and symptom profiles.
We suggest that the inclusion of a motor systems dimension to the RDoC initiative is useful for three primary reasons. First, motor dysfunction is primarily an overt behavior. Motor signs and symptoms are easily quantifiable using observer methods and movement coding (eg, Mittal et al. 2010), but can also be tested using a variety of validated instrumental measures. Second, differing motor subsystems are relatively well-mapped, and are relatively distinct. Interestingly though, when we consider both the basal ganglia and cerebellum, there are also non-motor regions within these larger structures. Thus, motor behaviors may point to a more general dysfunction that impacts non-motor domains, including cognition, emotion, motivation, personality, and symptomatology. By investigating this more overt domain, it may provide important insight into other critical domains as well. Third, consistent with the goal of the RDoC initiative, motor systems can indeed be studied from all levels of analysis. Investigations of differing cytoarchitectonics in the cortex and cerebellum may prove insightful, as may investigations of dopaminergic systems that govern movement, but many other functions as well. As noted, behavior can be quantified, and a variety of well-established paradigms have also been designed for use in the functional neuroimaging environment, as well as with animal models and pharmacological challenge paradigms. Finally, self-report and reports from family members regarding motor skills, developmental milestones (eg. walking, fine motor control) and general motor function can also be reliably collected.
Finally, as noted above with respect to psychosis-risk, cerebellar- and basal ganglia-mediated motor deficits seem to be distinctly associated with symptom progression across symptom types (Mittal et al. 2010; Dean et al. 2015), indicating that they are a trait feature of the disease, but are confounded by disease state. This example illustrates the potential utility of investigating different motor circuits and patterns of behavioral dysfunction across disorders, as these different motor systems may also extend across diagnostic boundaries, implicating specific neural circuits in the pathophysiology of multiple psychiatric diagnoses. A better understanding of the motor systems associated with deficits across disorders may allow for the development of new targets for treatment and remediation.
Conclusions
While the RDoC initiative stands to greatly improve and expand our understanding of psychopathology, the inclusion of a motor systems dimension would provide a more well-rounded picture of the behavioral deficits seen in normative populations and those with a variety of psychiatric diagnoses. Motor systems fit well with the pillars of study proposed as part of RDoC allowing for study across levels of analysis, and the easily quantifiable deficits associated with motor systems dysfunction may make for important biomarkers associated with disease. Together, the inclusion of motor systems as part of the RDoC framework stands to greatly improve our understanding of motor function across a range of abilities, as well as the role of motor systems in psychopathology across the lifespan.
Acknowledgments
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health grants R01MH094650 and R21/R33MH103231 to V.A.M. and F32MH102989-01 to J.A.B.
References
- Anguera JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Willingham DT, Seidler RD. Contributions of spatial working memory to visuomotor learning. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2010;22:1917–1930. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes KA, Howard JH, Howard DV, Kenealy L, Vaidya CJ. Two forms of implicit learning in childhood ADHD. Developmental neuropsychology. 2010;35:494–505. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard JA, Dean DJ, Kent JS, Orr JM, Pelletier-Baldelli A, Lunsford-Avery J, Gupta T, Mittal VA. Cerebellar Networks in Individuals at Ultra High-Risk of Psychosis: Impact on Postural Sway and Symptom Severity. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35:4064–4078. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard JA, Mittal VA. Cerebellar-motor dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychosis-risk: the importance of regional cerebellar analysis approaches. Frontiers in Psychiatry Schizophrenia. 2014;5:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolbecker AR, Hong SL, Kent JS, Klaunig MJ, O’Donnell BF, Hetrick WP. Postural control in bipolar disorder: increased sway area and decreased dynamical complexity. PloS One. 2011;6:e19824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buderath P, Gärtner K, Frings M, Christiansen H, Schoch B, Konczak J, Gizewski ER, Hebebrand J, Timmann D. Postural and gait performance in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Gait & posture. 2009;29:249–254. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buyukdura JS, McClintock SM, Croarkin PE. Psychomotor retardation in depression: Biological underpinnings, measurement, and treatment. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2011;35:395–409. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caligiuri MP, Lohr JB. A disturbance in the control of muscle force in neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Biological psychiatry. 1994;35:104–111. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91199-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Oliveri ME, Insel T. A neurodevelopmental perspective on the research domain criteria (RDoC) framework. Biological psychiatry. 2014;76:350–353. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornell DG, Suarez R, Berent S. Psychomotor retardation in melancholic and nonmelancholic depression: Cognitive and motor components. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1984;93:150–157. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.93.2.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC medicine. 2013;11:126. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean DJ, Kent JS, Bernard JA, Orr JM, Gupta T, Pelletier-Baldelli A, Carol EE, Mittal VA. Increased postural sway predicts negative symptom progression in youth at ultrahigh risk for psychosi. Schizophrenia Research. 2015:10–13. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dillon DG, Rosso IM, Pechtel P, Killgore WDS, Rauch SL, Pizzagalli Da. Peril and pleasure: an rdoc-inspired examination of threat responses and reward processing in anxiety and depression. Depression and anxiety. 2014;31:233–249. doi: 10.1002/da.22202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenton WS, Wyatt RJ, McGlashan TH. Risk Factors for Spontaneous Dyskinesia in Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:643–650. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950080055008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghaziuddin M, Quinlan P, Ghaziuddin N. Catatonia in autism: A distinct subtype? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2005;49:102–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00666.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U, Sharma A, Khagstigir U, Ferrier IN, Young AH, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Moore PB. Neuropsychological dysfunction, soft neurological signs and social disability in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;188:366–373. doi: 10.1192/bjp.188.4.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gowen E, Hamilton A. Motor abilities in autism: A review using a computational context. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43:323–344. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1574-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izawa J, Pekny SE, Marko MK, Haswell CC, Shadmehr R, Mostofsky SH. Motor learning relies on integrated sensory inputs in ADHD, but over-selectively on proprioception in autism spectrum conditions. Autism Research. 2012;5:124–136. doi: 10.1002/aur.1222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langenecker Sa, Jacobs RH, Passarotti AM. Current Neural and Behavioral Dimensional Constructs across Mood Disorders. Current behavioral neuroscience reports. 2014;1:144–153. doi: 10.1007/s40473-014-0018-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marvel CL, Schwartz BL, Rosse RB. A quantitative measure of postural sway deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2004;68:363–372. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marvel CL, Turner BM, O’Leary DS, Johnson HJ, Pierson RK, Boles Ponto LL, Andreasen NC. The neural correlates of implicit sequence learning in schizophrenia, Neuropsychology. 2007;21:761–777. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mittal VA, Walker EF, Bearden CE, Walder D, Trottman H, Daley M, Simone A, Cannon TD. Markers of basal ganglia dysfunction and conversion to psychosis: neurocognitive deficits and dyskinesias in the prodromal period. Biological psychiatry. 2010;68:93–99. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mittal VA, Dean DJ, Bernard JA, Orr JM, Pelletier-Baldelli A, Carol EE, Gupta T, Turner J, Leopold DR, Robustelli BL, Millman ZB. Neurological Soft Signs Predict Abnormal Cerebellar-Thalamic Tract Development and Negative Symptoms in Adolescents at High Risk for Psychosis: A Longitudinal Perspective. Schizophrenia bulletin. 2014;50:1204–1215. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosconi MW, Mohanty S, Greene RK, Cook EH, Vaillancourt DE, Sweeney JA. Feedforward and Feedback Motor Control Abnormalities Implicate Cerebellar Dysfunctions in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:2015–2025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2731-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Landa RJ, Denckla MB. Evidence for a deficit in procedural learning in children and adolescents with autism: implications for cerebellar contribution. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2000;6:752–759. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700677020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naismith SL, Lagopoulos J, Ward PB, Davey CG, Little C, Hickie IB. Fronto-striatal correlates of impaired implicit sequence learning in major depression: an fMRI study. Journal of affective disorders. 2010;125:256–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obeso JA, Olanow CW, Nutt JG. Levodopa motor complications in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2000;1931:0–5. doi: 10.1016/s1471-1931(00)00031-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pappa S, Dazzan P. Spontaneous movement disorders in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode psychoses: a systematic review. Psychological medicine. 2009;39:1065–1076. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettersson AF, Olsson E, Wahlund L-O. Motor function in subjects with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders. 2005;19:299–304. doi: 10.1159/000084555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piek JP, Pitcher TM, Hay DA. Motor coordination and kinaesthesis in boys with attention deficit – hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1999;41:159–165. doi: 10.1017/s0012162299000341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn J, Meagher D, Murphy P, Kinsella A, Mullaney J, Waddington JL. Vulnerability to involuntary movements over a lifetime trajectory of schizophrenia approaches 100%, in association with executive (frontal) dysfunction. Schizophrenia Research. 2001;49:79–87. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00220-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacchet MD, Prasad G, Foland-Ross LC, Joshi SH, Hamilton JP, Thompson PM, Gotlib IH. Structural abnormality of the corticospinal tract in major depressive disorder. Biology of mood & anxiety disorders. 2014;4:8. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-4-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanislow CA, Pine DS, Quinn KJ, Garvey MA. Developing constructs for psychopathology research: Research Domain Criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:631–639. doi: 10.1037/a0020909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffman J, Mittal V, Kline E, Mortensen EL, Michelsen N, Ekstrøm M, Millman ZB, Mednick S a, Sørensen HJ. Childhood dyspraxia predicts adult-onset nonaffective–psychosis-spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2015:1–8. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414001436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, Kwak Y, Lipps DB. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34:721–733. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starkstein SE, Petracca G, Tesón a, Chemerinski E, Merello M, Migliorelli R, Leiguarda R. Catatonia in depression: prevalence, clinical correlates, and validation of a scale. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1996;60:326–332. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.60.3.326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walther S, Strik W. Motor symptoms and schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology. 2012;66:77–92. doi: 10.1159/000339456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]