Abstract
Coordinated sensory and motor system activity leads to efficient localization behaviours; but what neural dynamics enable object tracking and what are the underlying coding principles? Here we show that optimized distance estimation from motion-sensitive neurons underlies object tracking performance in weakly electric fish. First, a relationship is presented for determining the distance that maximizes the Fisher information of a neuron's response to object motion. When applied to our data, the theory correctly predicts the distance chosen by an electric fish engaged in a tracking behaviour, which is associated with a bifurcation between tonic and burst modes of spiking. Although object distance, size and velocity alter the neural response, the location of the Fisher information maximum remains invariant, demonstrating that the circuitry must actively adapt to maintain ‘focus' during relative motion.
Animals tracking objects can adapt their movements to optimise sensory coding. Using fish that sense objects as perturbations to an electric field, Clarke et al. reveal that the optimal object distance maintained by the fish is encoded within the firing properties of electrosensory neurons.
Neural systems that actively engage and track moving objects are faced with two major challenges: determining motion parameters and directing appropriate motor commands to maintain informative sensory input. Behavioural studies on tracking eye movements1, fly navigation2, electrosensory tracking3,4 and bat echolocation5 have together led to the hypothesis that active sensing can be directed to optimize re-afferent sensory processing. However, the conclusion that active sensing can be executed in a manner that directly benefits neural coding is premature, since it has not been shown that the sensory activity evoked by these motor outputs optimizes neural transmission and thus the resulting behaviour. To assess whether precise tracking performance relies on optimized stimulus estimation, we apply Fisher Information6 (IF) to sequences of action potentials (spikes) recorded from the motion-sensitive responses of electrosensory ON and OFF contrast-coding neurons. By finding where IF is maximal along the transverse distance axis (Fig. 1a), we seek to identify where a decoder of ON and OFF cell firing rates may, in theory, achieve the best possible estimation of changes in object position from the observed spiking activity.
The IF of a neuron whose spiking activity follows a Poisson distribution, with firing rate function λ, can be computed directly as follows (Supplementary Note 2)6,
This equation illustrates that the best possible estimate of a stimulus feature (x), over an interval of time (Δt), occurs when the square of the derivative of the firing rate with respect to x, divided by the firing rate, is maximal. In other words, stimulus-induced changes in firing rate are more readily estimated against lower levels of spiking activity. This estimation principle appears to be reflected in natural behaviour: during echolocation, bats cast their echo-beams off-axis from a target so that the maximum spatial slope, not the peak intensity, of the beam is reflected back to the animal5. Despite a weaker signal input, the benefit of this strategy is that small changes in relative distance result in large changes in reflected echo intensity, with putative benefits for motion processing. In an obvious parallel with bats, gymnotiform weakly electric fish track objects at distances that cause relatively small perturbations to their endogenously generated electric field, but with relatively large spatial derivatives (evoking large λ′)7,8,9,10. We propose that sensing objects at distances that cause weak signals is not simply a consequence of where the slope in the physical signal is maximal. To optimize estimation, a fine balance exists between sensing an object at a distance that evokes a relatively low firing rate in sensory neurons but where relative changes in object distance over time cause large changes in the neural response.
Although the Poisson case is conceptually important and a working approximation under static conditions, we are interested in dynamic stimuli (looming/receding motion) and the assumptions underlying equation (1) may no longer suffice. Therefore, we first sought to examine the statistical nature of pyramidal cell spiking more closely. We show that ON and OFF cell spiking is not a Poisson process under spontaneous or stimulus-driven conditions and then move on to more accurately investigate the idea that optimal estimation in sensory neural networks enables observed tracking behaviours.
Results
ON and OFF cell responses to motion
In vivo extracellular recordings of ON and OFF contrast-coding neurons—located in the primary electrosensory lobe (ELL) of gymnotiform electric fish11—were obtained from immobilized Apteronotidae, while spherical objects were moved towards (looming) and away (receding) from the animal along the transverse body axis (Fig. 1a). Importantly, our stimulus is a mimic of the type of motion experienced by the electrosense during a behavioural tracking task, the electromotor response7,8. Brass and plastic spheres were used in our experiments to cast both positive and negative local contrast patterns onto the cutaneous electroreceptors (object contrast is defined by its electrical conductivity relative to the background water). The electroreceptor afferents (EAs) project topographically to ON and OFF cell pairs in the ELL, forming direct excitatory contact with ON cells and indirect contact with the OFF cells through an inhibitory interneuron11. As a result, ON cells are selective for increases in stimulus intensity over time and OFF cells are selective for decreases in stimulus intensity, due to the sign inversion. Therefore, motion reversal (switching from looming to receding) changes the sign of the temporal derivative of input intensity, evoking switches in the activity of downstream ON and OFF cells, an event marked by prominent burst spiking12 (Fig. 1a). By combining ON and OFF cell outputs, a downstream decoder achieves a bidirectionally symmetric representation of motion. The reader should note that both ON and OFF cells generate looming and receding responses, depending on the sign of the stimulus contrast (only the plastic negative contrast case is presented in Fig. 1a; ON and OFF cell role reversal is observed for brass). In previous work12, we show that there is no significant difference between ON and OFF cell responses for looming and receding motion. Therefore, we pooled the data in this paper into looming (ON cell, brass; OFF cell, plastic) and receding (OFF cell, brass; ON cell, plastic) responses. The instantaneous firing rates of individual cell responses were averaged to form ON/OFF cell population firing rates as a function of distance for our eight different stimulus conditions.
ON and OFF cell spike train statistics
The dynamical transitions between quiescent, tonic and burst spiking states (Fig. 1a, raster plot) impose statistical structure onto the inter-spike interval (ISI) distributions (Fig. 1b). Although the pooled ISI probability densities of ON and OFF cell populations can be described by exponential distributions (with dead time <3 ms for the absolute refractory period), an individual neuron's ISI statistics are not Poisson distributed (Fig. 1b insets; Supplementary Note 1). In particular during motion processing there is strong departure from a Poisson process, serving as an immediate caveat for application of equation (1) to our data. Below we present a generalized formula for locating IF maxima, which is inclusive of the spiking distributions characteristic of ON and OFF cells.
In response to looming and receding motion, the average serial correlation of ON and OFF cell ISIs shows that the spiking statistics are strongly non-renewal, that is, the spatiotemporal contrast patterns caused by moving objects produce temporal correlations between successive spikes (Fig. 1c and Supplementary Fig. 1c). To rigorously demonstrate that individual ON and OFF cell spiking is non-Poisson during motion processing, we need to distinguish the intrinsic temporal dynamics of spike generation from those induced by our spatiotemporal stimulus. To this end, we applied the time-rescaling theorem13, also used in Fig. 1b (insets), to effectively remove the dependence of the ISIs on the distance of the object (Supplementary Note 1). After removing the stimulus-induced trend in the ISIs, no significant correlation remains, indicative of memoryless (renewal) spiking dynamics over the course of stimulation (see Supplementary Fig. 1b,c for receding motion). In other words, the motion-sensitive responses of ON and OFF cells depend only on the current stimulus value and the timing of the last spike, an important feature of instantaneous rate coding. Therefore, we generalize equation (1) to locate the IF maxima of neurons with renewal spiking statistics, free of the restrictive assumption that the observed spiking is described by a Poisson distribution. We expect this result will be relevant for other rate coding systems with irregular, bursty spiking, such as retinal ON and OFF ganglion cells14, hippocampal CA1 neurons15 and cortical pyramidal neurons16.
Stimulus estimation and Fisher information
An instantaneous population firing rate, which depends on object position (x), was determined by averaging individual firing rates obtained from both ON and OFF cells in response to 2 cm s−1 looming motion. This function was then substituted into the following formula, which, although not a direct expression for IF, does locate where IF is maximal as a function of x (see derivation in Supplementary Note 2):
where and Δt=10 ms, a short decoding time window. After repeating this procedure for three more natural looming speeds (1, 3 and 4 cm s−1), the mean distance and s.d. for the prominent IF maximum was determined to be 1.37±0.01 cm from the fish's body (Fig. 2a; Supplementary Fig. 2; see Methods section for the number of repeats and replicates associated with each case). In this vicinity, the variance associated with the estimation of object distance from the observed spiking activity can achieve its lowest possible value6, giving the best possible encoding. For comparison's sake, IF was also computed directly using the Poisson formula (1), resulting in poor identification of a maximum (Supplementary Fig. 2).
According to W. Heiligenberg's classic behavioural result8, the electromotor response (data reprinted in Fig. 2b), gymnotiform electric fish track sinusoidally moving rods optimally (gain near 1 and phase near 0) while maintaining an average distance of 1.34 cm from the nearest rod. Given the striking agreement between the predicted position at which IF is maximal and the position at which the animal best tracks the object, we suggest that the object distance that maximizes the IF of a neuron's tuned response is the neural basis of a sensory focal point.
ON and OFF cells are sensitive encoders of object speed, reflected in both the peak of λ(x, t) and its slope (Fig. 3a). Despite these strong speed dependencies, the position of maximal IF was found to be speed invariant over the range of 1–4 cm s−1, using equation (2). However, when applied to the even slower speed of 0.5 cm s−1, representative of the electromotor response behaviour, we encountered a methodological issue: the weaker spiking evoked by very slow changes in object position created much more response variability, requiring far greater numbers of trials to obtain sufficiently smooth firing rates. This experimental roadblock to identifying a clear IF maximum from a noisy equation (2) inspired us to investigate what aspect of ON and OFF cell spiking activity reflects maximal IF in vivo, as well as how the electrosensory circuits might decode the responses to produce tracking behaviours for very slow speeds.
Neuron bursting and invariant sensory focus
Both modelling and experimental studies of pyramidal cells have shown that input signal intensity controls a bifurcation between tonic and burst modes of spiking17. Since burst spiking is strongly dependent on input signal slope18, is known to be important for feature extraction19 and becomes prominent near the focal point (Fig. 1a), we sought to understand how bursting directly contributes to stimulus estimation. In particular, equation (2) shows that the relationship 1-Δt·λ(x, t) scales the Poisson IF relationship, suggesting that high-frequency burst spiking (large λ) is important for controlling the spike-likelihood within a small coding window (Δt). When a neuron is in the tonic spiking mode, this scale factor, 1-Δt·λ(x, t), is close to one because the chance of observing a spike in the next Δt≤10 ms is low. However, when the cell sharply transitions to the bursting state, there is a dramatic increase in the spike-likelihood and the scale factor drops nearly to 0. Therefore, bursting activity is expected to sharpen local IF maxima and enhance focal point acuity (even for very slow motion).
To investigate this idea, the number of burst spikes (3<ISI≤10 ms) divided by the total number of spikes (tonic and burst) was determined within 30 consecutive 2 mm intervals along the distance axis (6 cm trajectory) to compute burst fraction (BF) as a function of object distance (Fig. 3b; Supplementary Note 3). We found a clear increase in the relative proportion of burst spiking in the 24th interval, 1.25–1.45 cm, for looming speeds of 0.5–4 cm s−1, occurring very near a data-driven BF threshold of 0.3 (Fig. 3c). Within this interval there is a notable reduction in BF variance. From 1.25 cm onward, dramatic, speed-dependent increases in BF surpass this threshold (Fig. 3c). Note that the 0.3 threshold is a robust measure of burst activation as it sits more than 3 s.d. away from the closely related burst probability of ON and OFF cells under spontaneous conditions (previously reported as ON: 0.25±0.014; OFF: 0.22±0.016) (ref. 20).
The match between the BF measure, our theoretical IF maximum and the electromotor response behaviour is remarkable, particularly because the size of our stimulus is smaller than the object used by Heiligenberg for the data presented in Fig. 2b (d=1.21 cm versus d=2 cm). To illustrate the impact of object size and electrical contrast, input intensity curves were generated for our main object size (d=1.21 cm), and a significantly smaller object (d=0.64 cm) using an empirical model (Fig. 4a)9. The intensity of the contrast sensed at the receptive field (RF) of ON and OFF cells scales with the cube of a spherical object's radius. Furthermore, due to their distinct electrical conductivities relative to the surrounding water, a negative plastic stimulus produces a contrast intensity that is half of the intensity produced by the same-sized, positive brass stimulus9,12. These observations suggest that the location where IF attains a local maximum is also intensity invariant, consistent with recorded behavioural performance for a range of object sizes8. To test this hypothesis directly, we presented 2 cm s−1 looming motion using the smaller stimulus (d=0.64 cm), which is a lower bound for object size and electromotor response performance8. At the location of the IF maximum (x*=1.37 cm), the signal intensity for the smaller sphere is 23% that of the larger sphere. Yet, when using our BF measure we found that the focal point was still located in the 1.25–1.45 cm range (Fig. 4b, left; Supplementary Table 1).
Despite a bidirectional physical symmetry with the traces of Fig. 4a, receding stimuli evoke a skew in the EAs firing rate responses compared with the looming responses, which is caused by directionally selective spike-rate adaptation12. This asymmetric EA response causes an apparent disconnect between the feedforward sensory input, pronounced burst spiking and switches in ON and OFF cell coding12. Yet, the location of the IF maximum is preserved in the population (Fig. 2a, bottom; Fig. 4b, right), provided continuous motion occurs, as in the electromotor response protocol. Interestingly, if long pauses (looming, 7 s pause, receding and then 10 s pause) are inserted, there is no longer a notable reduction of the receding burst response. These pauses create a static object distance and are therefore unnatural for tracking behaviours; in reality continuously varying relative motion is experienced by the network. Under the pause circumstances the IF maximum is now found at 1.72 cm according to the theoretical approach. The BF measure also shifts further from the body to the 1.45–1.65 cm distance interval (Fig. 4b, right), which strongly supports the idea that burst onset is a major determinant of the focal point location (Supplementary Fig. 3).
Motion direction- and size-invariance are particularly surprising features of the focal point. An additional population-coding perspective is provided in Fig. 4c (left) for our 2-cm s−1 velocity cases (default-sized looming, default-sized receding and small looming), which shows that the maximum probability for a neuron in the population to transition to bursting occurs in the 24th interval (1.25, 1.45) cm. This peak corresponds to the cumulative probability that approximately one half of the neurons in the stimulated population have transitioned to bursting Fig. 4c (right) and could be viewed as a tipping point between two population activity states. Remarkably, these population distributions are preserved for both directions of motion and for markedly different stimulus intensities. The close agreement between the focal point location predicted by Fisher Information and BF (Fig. 4d; Supplementary Table 1) is even more convincing in light of these burst-probability distributions.
Discussion
We have demonstrated that the accuracy of the information conveyed by ON and OFF cells about the location of a transversely moving object is maximal between 1.3 and 1.4 cm from the fish's skin. This location is independent of the nature of the object (positive or negative contrast), its direction of motion, its speed and its size. Importantly, this location is precisely the one chosen by an electric fish as it tracks a transversely moving object in a classic behavioural paradigm. The methods we used to compute the location of the IF maximum are sophisticated, and, at first, it was not clear how a neural network could actually implement a focal point. ON and OFF cells burst in response to transverse motion and we discovered that a burst detector could, in principle, accurately read out the theoretically defined optimal location. Detailed studies have elucidated the ON/OFF cell burst mechanism21,22,23 and identified numerous potential modulators of bursting24,25. As discussed below, these may be the cellular and network bases by which the burst mechanism is controlled to give rise to an invariant focal point.
The fact that a stable IF maximum was maintained for both ON and OFF cell types under many stimulus conditions implies that the ELL network must actively adapt to the electrosensory input to dynamically control gain and burst onset in the ON and OFF cell population. This is a unique example of Barlow's efficient coding hypothesis26, which holds that sensory systems should transmit information optimally by adapting to natural stimuli. However, the manner by which electrosensory networks adapt to different objects and control focal point stability is unknown. The observed speed invariance of the IF maximum to looming motion may follow directly from two phenomena: a special computation performed by the EAs and the intrinsic bursting properties of ON and OFF cells. First, a scale-free (power law) form of spike-rate adaptation transforms EA firing rates to accurately encode changes in looming object distance, regardless of the stimulus timescales (that is, looming speed)27. Second, both modelling and experimental studies of ELL pyramidal cells have shown that feedforward input intensity controls a saddle-node bifurcation between tonic and burst modes of spiking16. As such, object distance will be directly encoded by the EA firing rates independent of the object's looming speed27 and could trigger a burst bifurcation in the downstream ON and OFF cells at a specific position, defined by the biophysical characteristics of the network.
Although the EAs are able to remove speed-dependent effects from their firing rates, the magnitude of their responses is strongly sensitive to different object sizes27. Therefore, assuming a hard-wired network response, it is reasonable to expect that the smaller object would trigger ON or OFF cell bursting much closer to the sensory surface, whereas larger objects would cause bursting to occur at distances further from the skin. However, this is not the case. The observed invariance of the IF maximum to object size firmly demonstrates that the burst onset can't simply trigger off of the feedforward input. As a further example, our analysis pooled ON cell responses to looming brass spheres with OFF cell responses to looming plastic spheres since they are indistinguishable12. Likewise, receding responses of ON cells to plastic and OFF cells to brass were pooled. This simple fact is actually surprising given the stimulus intensity of a brass object is twice greater than the same-sized object made of plastic (Fig. 4a). Clearly multiplicative24 and divisive28 gain control must be implemented by the network to influence bursting and stabilize the focal point. An understanding of such flexible gain control for dynamic stimuli is an exciting challenge for future work. Note that adjusting the overall gain based on object size and conductivity would allow for effective operation of the hypothesized speed invariance mechanism described above.
Receding motion further challenges our understanding of how burst spiking is controlled in the network. Despite the fact that the feedforward EA response to looming is considerably different than the receding response, downstream ON and OFF cell receding firing rates are nearly mirror-symmetrical versions of the looming responses12 and the focal point is preserved for continuous motion. In this scenario, the dynamical states are visited in the reverse order, but with the same result, illustrating that dynamical transitions can optimize signal estimation regardless of their temporal sequence (burst to quiescence or vice versa). Further modelling and experiments on balanced excitatory and inhibitory feedback pathways will be required to directly connect dynamic physiological control of the tonic-burst spiking bifurcation to both the observed behavioural responses and optimized stimulus estimation. A clue to potential mechanisms may lie in the robustness of the spiking activity patterns of single neurons in response to concerted changes of multiple conductances induced by neuromodulators29. The invariant location of the IF maximum would presumably require coordinated changes in synaptic conductances of the ELL network to control the location of the burst bifurcation in the face of varying electrosensory input.
The ON and OFF cells project to the midbrain (torus semicircularis) where there is an explosion in the number of cell types, indicative of early feature extraction11. Previously, directionally selective cells have been identified in the torus semicircularis that are highly sensitive to longitudinal motion; however, further work is required to characterize torus semicircularis cells that are selective for the transverse looming and receding motion. It has previously been suggested that the burst and tonic spikes of electrosensory ON and OFF cell spike trains can be segregated by facilitating and depressing synapses, respectively30; both forms of plasticity have been observed in torus semicircularis neurons31,32. Therefore, we propose that midbrain networks can processes tonic and burst spiking in parallel to compute BF and identify the location of optimized distance estimation.
Extensive neuroanatomical studies11 have demonstrated that a strong projection from torus semicircularis to the tectum is the only route from the electrosensory periphery to the motor system. The electromotor response behaviour presumably uses this pathway during active sensing; where stimulation of the EAs leads to motor outputs that simultaneously influence the re-afferent sensory processing. We hypothesize that sensory focus is not an open-loop, cascade-like computation but rather a closed sensorimotor loop that arises during active sensing behaviour—the fish finds a location that maximizes the information from its electrosensory input and then uses this optimal input to guide subsequent motor outputs.
The simultaneous activity of sensory and motor systems during tracking behaviour is expected to preserve the relative distance as follows. The animal should adjust its position such that the object distance triggers bursting in approximately half of the stimulated population (Fig. 4c). On the cusp of transitions between different dynamical states, a downstream decoder can optimally estimate object position based on the evoked spiking activity of ON and OFF pyramidal cell populations. In this region of space, better estimates of object distance are expected to permit more accurate estimates of speed, which are reflected in the temporal slope of the pyramidal cell firing rates (Fig. 3a). For a given object contrast, increases and decreases in signal intensity indicate changes in the relative distance and are expected to dictate appropriate orienting behaviours. On changes in motion direction, the prompt bursting of either the ON or OFF cell populations appears very well suited to guide compensatory motor commands to maintain object focus.
Methods
Surgical procedure
Surgery was performed on adult male and female gymnotiform fish, Apteronotus leptorhynchus (imported from natural habitats in South America), to expose the caudal cerebellum overlying the ELL. All surgical and experimental procedures were reviewed and approved by the Animal Care Committee at the University of Ottawa. Immediately following surgery, fish were immobilized with an injection of the paralytic pancuronium bromide (0.2% w/v), which has no effect on the neurogenic discharge of the electric organ that produces the fish's electric field (EOD)—the basis of the electrosense. The animal was then transferred into a large tank of water (27 °C; electrical conductivity between 100–150 μS cm−1) and a custom holder was used to stabilize the head during recordings. The tails were gently tethered in position with thread to avoid any potential displacement of the body due to the small hydro-mechanical effects caused by looming/receding motion. All fish were monitored for signs of stress and allowed to acclimatize before commencing stimulation protocols.
Neurophysiology
Extracellular recordings were taken from pyramidal cells of the centrolateral map of the ELL11. This map was chosen because its neurons respond strongly to object motion and have fairly large, easy-to-locate RFs33. Recordings were obtained from cells whose RFs were located 30–65% along the rostral-caudal body axis of the animal, as this region provides the flattest body surface and EOD isopotentials with low curvature that lay perpendicular to the looming/receding stimulus trajectories. Likewise, since the body curves away from the ‘sensory plane' on the belly and back, distance is harder to control for and only cells whose RFs were in the 25–75% range on the dorsal ventral axis were used. These restrictions were to ensure a consistent electric image that was not warped by body geometry or the field boundary effects occurring at the interface of tank water and air. Importantly, this range of the body surface includes the location where the gymnotiform fish Eigenmannia virescens align themselves during the electromotor response behaviour7,8. After finding a cell's RF centre using a local stimulus dipole, we classified it as ON or OFF based on its response to step increases and decreases in the local field potential. We then mapped out the RF centres, which yielded spatial spreads consistent with anatomical estimates for the centrolateral map33. The baseline firing rates of the recorded ON and OFF pyramidal cells (5.4–25.9 Hz; NE=15, NI=16) demonstrate that they are the superficial and intermediate types11. In some cases, we were able to simultaneously record from ON/OFF cell pairs and directly compare their differential responses to object motion. A particularly nice example of one such pair is displayed in Fig. 1a.
Using our total population of 31 cells (Supplementary Fig. 1a), we computed a representative s.d. as σ=17.06 × 0.95=16.21 Hz, to determine a sample size (n) as
for a 99% confidence level (z*=2.58) on a confidence interval width (W) of ±5 Hz. From this simple calculation, we see that we need at least 70 trials. However, when choosing a sample size for estimating mean firing rates in a population of neurons by averaging repeated trials, we need to consider the following. It is not clear that the use of the Z-statistic, which is related to the normal distribution, is correct since we are sampling different classes of ON and OFF cells whose spiking statistics are inhomogeneous and whose sampling cannot be treated as independent and identically distributed. There are a number of other complicating factors in determining our sample size, including the statistically non-stationary responses of ELL pyramidal cells to motion and the fact that there are two sources of variance: the variance of single cell's response to repeated trials, as well as the variance in the population spiking statistics. In the above calculation, we chose to use spike train statistics in the absence of a stimulus—although the variance grows larger when the stimulus is present, there are also more spikes present, which, when averaged, yield smoother firing rate estimates compared with baseline levels. The degree of smoothing is further influenced by the length of the smoothing kernel (although it was chosen to be a minimal 10 ms in the work presented here). Without a well-defined formula to compute an exact required sample size, we turned to practicality, averaging enough trials to get sufficiently smooth firing rates to readily discern the stimulus-induced response and address our hypotheses. We well exceeded the calculated population size for all of our data conditions, with the exception of 0.5 cm s−1, which takes 24 s to do a single trial, greatly diminishing the amount of data that can be collected. Our largest sample, N=463, yields 99% confidence on an interval of ±1.94 Hz. The number of replicates (N1) and repeats (N2) for each of our stimulus conditions are listed here and were used for both the theory approach in Fig. 2 and the BF measures of Figs 3 and 4. For the large looming sphere: 0.5 cm s−1, N1=9, N2=73; 1 cm s−1, N1=23, N2=255; 2 cm s−1, N1=31, N2=463; 3 cm s−1, N1=13, N2=120; 4 cm s−1, N1=15, N2=166. For the large receding sphere (2 cm s−1): continuous (0/0), N1=14, N2=168; discontinuous (7/10), N1=13, N2=147. Finally, the small looming sphere (2 cm s−1): N1=8, N2=104.
According to cell type, a plastic or brass sphere was connected to an electromechanical positioner, which was pre-programmed for the appropriate motion sequence and initiated by outputs from our data acquisition software (Spike2 v7.03; Cambridge Electronic Designs). The selected sphere was aligned with the cell's RF centre along the transverse (lateral) body axis and placed at the initial position, x0=0.25 cm from the skin. Note that mal-alignment with a degree of ±0.05 cm error was unavoidable when setting x0 by using 0.1 cm gradations to measure the position of the sphere's leading edge relative to the fish's body. Slight mal-alignment of the sphere's centre with the RF center is another small, but unavoidable source of error. For our large sphere, d=1.21 cm, the effect of mal-alignment on the electric image is negligible since the sphere easily saturated the RF. The sphere was then withdrawn and motion was initiated a few moments later. Stimulation consisted of consecutive repetitions of looming and receding sequences at a speed of 2 cm s−1, chosen as an intermediate value from studies of gymnotiform locomotion7,8,10. This stimulation protocol was then repeated with a sphere creating an electric image with the opposite contrast, as well as for varying speeds (0.5–4 cm s−1). Acceleration at the beginning and end of constant velocity looming/receding motion profiles was set to 150 cm s−2, so periods of non-uniform velocity were negligible (<0.05 cm in all cases). For 13 of the 31 pyramidal cells used in this study, the same loom/recede sequence was repeated with a 7 s pause at the skin before receding to the initial position and a 10 s pause before the next trial (as performed in a previous study12). Typically 10 trials were obtained per cell, but for a few cells it was as many as 25. Population-averaged firing rates were computed by convolving the individually recorded spike trains with a minimal, 10 ms exponential kernel and then averaging across all trials and all cells to obtain smooth response curves for each stimulus condition. The example firing rate curves displayed in Fig. 1a were generated using a smoothing technique employed in previous work12 (see reference within), however, this technique was not used for the analysis since the algorithm assumes that spiking can be treated as a Poisson process. Note that convolution of the spike train with a smoothing kernel results in a sharp rounding down of the firing rate at the very ends of the file, where the recorded spiking abruptly stops. As can be seen in Fig. 2, as well as in Supplementary Figs 2 and 3, we routinely clip 0.25 cm from one (receding) or both (looming) edges of the recordings to avoid this false curvature that causes large spurious jumps in equations (1) and (2). The firing rates were then further smoothed with a short-duration moving average filter of 250 ms, as shown in Fig. 2a (top). Notice that the smooth curve (blue) is still a very strong representation of the underlying firing rate. The curves resulting from equations (1) and (2) were then smoothed once more with a 100 ms moving average filter, enabling us to identify very clear maxima.
We collected data with two conventional sphere sizes, used previously by Chen et al.9 in detailed models of the electrosensory image: our default sphere, an intermediate size from Heiligenberg's studies with a diameter (d) of 1.21 cm, and a substantially smaller sphere (d=0.64 cm), which was used as an object size control. Heiligenberg8 (Fig. 10 within) showed that strong gain in the electromotor response performance begins to deteriorate for d≈0.8 cm and significantly so for the object diameters <0.6 cm, thus the small stimulus is expected to represent a meaningful lower bound for network processing during motion tracking. The smaller sphere creates very localized changes in the electric potential; in many cases it seemed we were on the edge of the RF, partially stimulating the surround as evidenced by the occasional total lack of response, only to see it appear again. For this reason, we only used data in which a clear, strong response was observed, omitting cases in which the firing rate was inconsistent from trial-to-trial and dropped dramatically on some trials.
In general, data trials were only excluded when external noise was present in a recording or if a cell showed pathological signs of activity, such as hyper-excitability or lack of response to the stimulus. This may occur due to cell fatigue, damage or anoxic stress.
All data analyses were performed using custom Matlab scripts, which can be made available to those interested on request to the corresponding author. Application of the time-rescaling theorem in particular is the most relevant; the remaining code is straight forward or intended to interact with our specific recording software.
Estimation of tracking distance from behavioural data
Heiligenberg's original electromotor response data8, presented in Fig. 2b of the main text, was imported into Matlab to identify the positions of the individual data points in the image relative to the axes; this allowed us to compute sample statistics for the obvious clusters, which were not provided explicitly in his paper. This was accomplished with open source code that can be found on the Matlab Central website (ReversePlot 2009, Jordi Palacin. All rights reserved.) For the top half of the plot (gain versus distance), this method yielded a mean and s.d. of 1.06±0.14 cm for the gain, occurring at distance of 1.37±0.11 cm. Repeating the analysis for the bottom half of the plot, we found a phase of −0.68±0.26 radians, occurring at a distance of 1.31±0.13 cm. The average of these two mean distances and their corresponding composite s.d. is 1.34±0.17 cm, which is overlaid as grey shading onto the behavioural data.
It is important to note that the electromotor response data sets were obtained using the weakly electric gymnotiform fish E. virescens. Like the closely related gymnotiform A. leptorhynchus, they produce a continuous, high-frequency EOD and have nearly identical electrosensory afferents and hindbrain circuitry34. Unfortunately, their electric organ is myogenic and the pancuronium bromide injections used for immobilization blocks the electric organ discharge. A mimic EOD can be generated by mounting an electric dipole onto the animal, with one end in the mouth and the other looped around the tail. Although this is suitable for studying broad global stimuli relating to social interactions with conspecifics, the resulting EODs are highly artificial and do not reflect the local spatial aspects of the field in vivo. Despite being useful as a behavioural species, Eigenmannia is not well suited to our neurophysiology protocols.
Additional information
How to cite this article: Clarke, S. E. et al. The neural dynamics of sensory focus. Nat. Commun. 6:8764 doi: 10.1038/ncomms9764 (2015).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
S.E.C. is supported by a Frederick Banting and Charles Best Canada Graduate Scholarship from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. A.L. and L.M. are supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research.
Footnotes
Author contributions S.E.C. and L.M. designed the experiments and methods of data analysis; S.E.C. performed theory, experiments and data analysis; S.E.C., L.M. and A.L. wrote the paper.
References
- Friston K., Adams R. A., Perrinet L. & Breakspear M. Perceptions as hypotheses: saccades as experiments. Front. Psychol. 3, 1–20 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louis M., Huber T., Benton R., Sakmar T. P. & Vosshall L. B. Bilateral olfactory sensory input enhances chemotaxis behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 187–199 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann V. et al. Motor patterns during active electrosensory acquisition. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 1–13 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stamper S. A., Roth E., Cowan N. J. & Fortune E. S. Active sensing via movement shapes spatiotemporal patterns of sensory feedback. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 1567–1574 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yovel Y., Falk B., Moss C. F. & Ulanovsky N. Optimal localization by pointing off axis. Science 327, 701–704 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P. & Abbott L. F. Theoretical Neuroscience: Computational and Mathematical Modeling of Neural Systems MIT Press (2001). [Google Scholar]
- Heiligenberg W. ‘Electromotor' response in the electric fish Eigenmannia (Rhamphichthyidae, Gymnotoidei). Nature 243, 301–302 (1973). [Google Scholar]
- Heiligenberg W. Electrolocation of objects in the electric fish Eigenmannia (Rhamphichthyidae, Gymnotoidei). J. Comp. Physiol. 87, 137–164 (1973). [Google Scholar]
- Chen L., House J. L., Krahe R. & Nelson M. E. Modeling signal and background components of electrosensory scenes. J. Comp. Physiol. A 191, 331–345 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maciver M. A., Sharabash N. M. & Nelson M. E. Prey-capture behavior in gymnotid electric fish: motion analysis and effects of water conductivity. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 543–557 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krahe R. & Maler L. Neural maps in the electrosensory system of weakly electric fish. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 24, 13–21 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke S. E., Longtin A. & Maler L. A neural code for looming and receding motion is distributed over a population of electrosensory ON and OFF contrast cells. J. Neurosci. 34, 5583–5594 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haslinger R., Pipa G. & Brown E. Discrete time rescaling theorem: determining goodness of fit for discrete time statistical models of neural spiking. Neural Comput. 22, 2477–2506 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillow J. W. in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS) 1473–1481MIT Press (2009). [Google Scholar]
- Barbieri R., Quirk M. C., Frank L. M., Wilson M. A. & Brown E. N. Construction and analysis of non-Poisson stimulus-response models of neural spiking activity. J. Neurosci. Methods 105, 25–37 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen M. N. & Newsome W. T. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J. Neurosci. 18, 3870–3896 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doiron B., Laing C., Longtin A. & Maler L. Ghostbursting: a novel neuronal burst mechanism. J. Comput. Neurosci. 12, 5–25 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kepecs A., Wang X. J. & Lisman J. Bursting neurons signal input slope. J. Neurosci. 22, 9053–9062 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krahe R. & Gabbiani F. Burst firing in sensory systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 13–23 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastian J. & Nguyenkim J. Dendritic modulation of burst-like firing in sensory neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 10–22 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemon N. & Turner R. W. Conditional spike backpropagation generates burst discharge in a sensory neuron. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1519–1530 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez F. R., Mehaffey W. H. & Turner R. W. Dendritic Na+ current inactivation can increase cell excitability by delaying a somatic depolarizing after-potential. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 3836–3848 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laing C. R. et al. Type I burst excitability. J. Comput. Neurosci. 14, 329–342 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehaffey W. H., Doiron B., Maler L. & Turner R. W. Deterministic multiplicative gain control with active dendrites. J. Neurosci. 25, 9968–9977 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehaffey W. H., Fernandez F. R., Maler L. & Turner R. W. Regulation of burst dynamics improves differential encoding of stimulus frequency by spike train segregation. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 939–951 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenblith W. A. Sensory Communication: Contributions MIT Press (1961). [Google Scholar]
- Clarke S. E., Naud R., Longtin A. & Maler L. Speed-invariant encoding of looming object distance requires power law spike rate adaptation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 13624–13629 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mejias J. F., Payeur A., Selin E., Maler L. & Longtin A. Subtractive, divisive and non-monotonic gain control in feedforward nets linearized by noise and delays. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 8, 1–15 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman M. S., Golowasch J., Marder E. & Abbott L. F. Global structure, robustness, and modulation of neuronal models. J. Neurosci. 21, 5229–5238 (2001). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton J., Yu N., Longtin A. & Maler L. Routing the flow of sensory signals using plastic responses to bursts and isolated spikes: experiment and theory. J. Neurosci. 31, 2461–2473 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortune E. S. & Rose G. J. Short-term synaptic plasticity contributes to the temporal filtering of electrosensory information. J. Neurosci. 20, 7122–7130 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fortune E. S. & Rose G. J. Short-term synaptic plasticity as a temporal filter. Trends Neurosci. 24, 381–385 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maler L. Receptive field organization across multiple electrosensory maps. I. Columnar organization and estimation of receptive field size. J. Comp. Neurol. 516, 376–393 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maler L. The posterior lateral line lobe of certain gymnotoid fish: quantitative light microscopy. J. Comp. Neurol. 183, 323–363 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.