Noradrenaline (NA) is an important neuromodulator involved in many aspects of behavior including wakefulness, response to novelty, attention, and stress.1–4 NA is primarily produced by the locus coeruleus (LC) and LC neurons project widely throughout the brain including the cortex.5 LC neurons exhibit state dependent firing patterns with low tonic firing rates during wakefulness punctuated by increased phasic activity during salient stimuli, decreased tonic activity during NREM sleep and near silence during REM sleep.6 Sleep is critical to maintain health7 and sleep deprivation leads to many negative health consequences. These include lowered immune function,8 metabolic dysregulation,9 and impaired cognitive performance,10 yet the mechanisms that lead to these deficits remain unclear.
In this issue of SLEEP, Bellesi and colleagues11 identify one potential mechanism by which prolonged wakefulness can lead to cognitive decline. This is the first study to measure dynamic changes in NA levels in the mouse cortex using in vivo micro-dialysis during prolonged wakefulness. Bellesi found that absolute levels of NA were higher in the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) versus primary motor cortex (M1). NA levels increased with prolonged wakefulness and decreased during sleep in both regions but the dynamics were faster in M1. In PFC, but not M1, NA levels started to decline after extended prolonged wakefulness. This suggests that LC neurons projecting to PFC are not able to maintain NA release for long periods of time. Synaptic fatigue may be due to NA synaptic vesicle depletion with sustained activity and is consistent with findings from Carter et al.,12 where extended LC firing at 10 Hz for 10 min resulted in decreased NA levels in the PFC. The differences in NE responses to sleep deprivation between PFC and M1 suggest that those projections originate from different populations of neurons within the LC and that individual populations of neurons within the LC are differentially susceptible to fatigue. The PFC projecting LC neurons susceptibility to fatigue may also be due to an inherent higher firing rate of PFC projecting LC neurons over M1 projecting LC neurons.5 Interestingly, M1 NA was correlated with both motor activity and the number of novel objects presented during sleep deprivation, whereas PFC NA was only correlated with the number of novel objects presented. These findings are consistent with the role of M1 in motor activity and PFC in attention.13 As the authors mention, these findings fit with observations that motor activity is the least susceptible to sleep deprivation, whereas cognitive functions are very susceptible to sleep deprivation.14
This study by Bellesi et al.11 represents an important first step at understanding neural fatigue with prolonged wakefulness. Two challenges of sleep deprivation methods include minimizing stress and controlling for increased locomotion.15 In this study, the two brain regions studied relate specifically to attention (PFC) and locomotor activity (M1) and are potentially sensitive to the method chosen for sleep deprivation. Baseline waking NA levels were collected while mice were on a slowly-rotating platform for 2 h, during which time NA levels were stable in both PFC and M1. Following these waking baseline measures, groups of mice were permitted spontaneous sleep-waking for 6 h or subjected to sleep deprivation for 6 h using introduction of novel objects. Prolonged wakefulness with novel objects induced an initial increase in NA in both PFC and M1 but only the PFC NA levels declined at the end of sleep deprivation. The authors suggest that the moving platform induced stereotyped behaviors leading to stable waking levels in NA, whereas introduction of novel objects lead to varying levels of attention thus increased NA. Therefore, the novel object method of sleep deprivation may preferentially activate LC to PFC neurons over LC to M1 neurons. Sleep used to be thought of as a whole brain phenomenon but recent evidence suggests that sleep can occur locally and is more intense in brain regions that experience greater use during wakefulness.16 Therefore, one explanation for the differential responses of NA levels in PFC versus M1 is that PFC was more active than M1 in the novel object sleep deprivation paradigm. It would be interesting to see if using other methods of prolonged sleep deprivation that do not specifically increase attention, like the moving platform, would produce the same increase in NE and selective fatigue of LC to PFC neurons.
Microdialysis is an excellent technique for long term monitoring of in vivo neurotransmitter levels but has low temporal resolution compared to electrochemical biosensors like cyclic voltammetry.17 Therefore, future studies using methods with faster sampling rates are needed to refine our understanding of the temporal resolution of NA changes across the short epoch duration and rapid transitions between sleep and wake observed in rodents. In the present study, NA did not correlate with slow wave activity, a measure of sleep pressure, in either brain region. This lack of correlation could be due to the long sampling rate of microdialysis compared to the faster sampling rate of the electroencephalogram. Additionally, new techniques like optogenetics18 that allow for selective activation of specific LC projections to either the PFC or M1 will help define their respective functions. For example, would activation of LC to PFC projections drive wakefulness or attention more or less than activation of LC to M1 projections?
Overall, Bellesi et al.11 identify one possible mechanism by which sleep deprivation leads to cognitive impairment. The decrease in PFC NA with prolonged wakefulness, presumably due to neural fatigue, may be one part of the complex network that leads to increased sleep pressure via withdrawal of an arousal signal. Interestingly, neural fatigue seems to be specific to PFC projecting LC neurons as compared to other projections of LC and other components of the arousal system like orexin,19 histamine,20 serotonin, or dopamine,21 which do not fatigue with prolonged wakefulness. Staying awake is so important evolutionarily, that there are multiple neural circuits that promote overall arousal. However, each individual brain region may contribute differentially to components of arousal. An exciting relevant application of this study's findings to clinical sleep diagnostics is the observation that pupillary size instability, which is controlled by the LC,22 increases with prolonged wakefulness,23 and may be a good biomarker of sleepiness.
CITATION
Van Dort CJ. Locus coeruleus neural fatigue: a potential mechanism for cognitive impairment during sleep deprivation. SLEEP 2016;39(1):11–12.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
Dr. Van Dort has indicated no financial conflicts of interest.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The author thanks Mohsen Hozan for helpful comments and discussions in preparing this manuscript.
REFERENCES
- 1.Saper CB, Fuller PM, Pedersen NP, Lu J, Scammell TE. Sleep state switching. Neuron. 2010;68:1023–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gompf HS, Mathai C, Fuller PM, et al. Locus ceruleus and anterior cingulate cortex sustain wakefulness in a novel environment. J Neurosci. 2010;30:14543–51. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3037-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Viggiano D, Ruocco LA, Arcieri S, Sadile AG. Involvement of norepinephrine in the control of activity and attentive processes in animal models of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neural Plast. 2004;11:133–49. doi: 10.1155/NP.2004.133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Morilak DA, Barrera G, Echevarria DJ, et al. Role of brain norepinephrine in the behavioral response to stress. Progr Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29:1214–24. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chandler DJ, Gao WJ, Waterhouse BD. Heterogeneous organization of the locus coeruleus projections to prefrontal and motor cortices. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111:6816–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320827111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Aston-Jones G, Bloom FE. Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. J Neurosci. 1981;1:876–86. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-08-00876.1981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Krueger JM, Frank MG, Wisor JP, Roy S. Sleep function: toward elucidating an enigma. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;28:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Irwin M, McClintick J, Costlow C, Fortner M, White J, Gillin JC. Partial night sleep deprivation reduces natural killer and cellular immune responses in humans. FASEB J. 1996;10:643–53. doi: 10.1096/fasebj.10.5.8621064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Knutson KL, Van Cauter E. Associations between sleep loss and increased risk of obesity and diabetes. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:287–304. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–26. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bellesi M, Tononi G, Cirelli C, Serra PA. Region-specific dissociation between cortical noradrenaline levels and the sleep/wake cycle. Sleep. 2016;39:143–54. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Carter ME, Yizhar O, Chikahisa S, et al. Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:1526–33. doi: 10.1038/nn.2682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Spencer RC, Devilbiss DM, Berridge CW. The cognition-enhancing effects of psychostimulants involve direct action in the prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:940–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Goel N, Rao H, Durmer JS, Dinges DF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol. 2009;29:320–39. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1237117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Leenaars CH, Dematteis M, Joosten RN, et al. A new automated method for rat sleep deprivation with minimal confounding effects on corticosterone and locomotor activity. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;196:107–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Krueger JM, Tononi G. Local use-dependent sleep; synthesis of the new paradigm. Curr Top Med Chem. 2011;11:2490–2. doi: 10.2174/156802611797470330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Park J, Takmakov P, Wightman RM. In vivo comparison of norepinephrine and dopamine release in rat brain by simultaneous measurements with fast-scan cyclic voltammetry. J Neurochem. 2011;119:932–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07494.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Mogri M, Deisseroth K. Optogenetics in neural systems. Neuron. 2011;71:9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Yoshida Y, Fujiki N, Nakajima T, et al. Fluctuation of extracellular hypocretin-1 (orexin A) levels in the rat in relation to the light-dark cycle and sleep-wake activities. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;14:1075–81. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Strecker RE, Nalwalk J, Dauphin LJ, et al. Extracellular histamine levels in the feline preoptic/anterior hypothalamic area during natural sleep-wakefulness and prolonged wakefulness: an in vivo microdialysis study. Neuroscience. 2002;113:663–70. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00158-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Zant JC, Leenaars CH, Kostin A, Van Someren EJ, Porkka-Heiskanen T. Increases in extracellular serotonin and dopamine metabolite levels in the basal forebrain during sleep deprivation. Brain Res. 2011;1399:40–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gilzenrat MS, Nieuwenhuis S, Jepma M, Cohen JD. Pupil diameter tracks changes in control state predicted by the adaptive gain theory of locus coeruleus function. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10:252–69. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Peters T, Gruner C, Durst W, Hutter C, Wilhelm B. Sleepiness in professional truck drivers measured with an objective alertness test during routine traffic controls. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2014;87:881–8. doi: 10.1007/s00420-014-0929-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]