Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2016 Feb 4;4:638. Originally published 2015 Aug 27. [Version 2] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.6953.2

Double-Blinding and Bias in Medication and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy Trials for Major Depressive Disorder

Douglas Berger 1,a
PMCID: PMC4732552  PMID: 26870318

Version Changes

Revised. Amendments from Version 1

The sentence, "The unblinded trial makes an unbiased evaluation of the efficacy of these pills impossible." after Figure 1 was deleted because it was left-over from a prior pre-publication draft and did not fit with the text. Hyphens were inserted into the words "self-esteem" and "make-up" in the text.

Abstract

While double-blinding is a crucial aspect of study design in an interventional clinical trial of medication for a disorder with subjective endpoints such as major depressive disorder, psychotherapy clinical trials, particularly cognitive-behavioral therapy trials, cannot be double-blinded. This paper highlights the evidence-based medicine problem of double-blinding in the outcome research of a psychotherapy and opines that psychotherapy clinical trials should be called, “partially-controlled clinical data” because they are not double-blinded. The implications for practice are, 1. For practitioners to be clear with patients the level of rigor to which interventions have been studied, 2. For authors of psychotherapy outcome studies to be clear that the problem in the inability to blind a psychotherapy trial severely restricts the validity of any conclusions that can be drawn, and 3. To petition National Health Insurance plans to use caution in approving interventions studied without double-blinded confirmatory trials as they may lead patients to avoid other treatments shown to be effective in double-blinded trials.

Keywords: psychotherapy, cognitive-behavioral therapy, outcome studies, blinding, clinical trials


Psychotherapy clinical outcome trials for major depressive disorder (MDD) are often described as “randomized”, “controlled”, “single-blind”, etc. These words may not adequately describe the level of methodologic rigor of the design of a trial for MDD because the endpoints are subjective symptom ratings 1, and the inability to double-blind MDD psychotherapy outcome trials is a crucial problem in the methodology of these trials 2. Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) is a widely-used type of psychotherapy in the treatment of MDD, however, CBT is very difficult if not impossible to double-blind because the subjects are actively involved in the therapy 3. While clinical trials of CBT are often called “single-blind” because the raters are blind to treatment allocation, “single-blind” in a clinical trial is actually defined as a case where the subjects are blind, not the raters 2, 4.

The evaluation of MDD efficacy is more complex in some ways than that for objective endpoints of, say, tumor size, cholesterol level, or survival years. MDD may be diagnosed in a variety of persons, some with more psychological distress, and some with more neurovegetative symptoms. The symptoms of MDD are measured on rating scales whose scores become the endpoints of the study. All of the items on these rating scales are subjective, and some items like hopelessness and low self-esteem are likely to improve with non-specific aspects of receiving care that include the hope and expectation inherent in belief in the treatment, compared with other symptoms such as lethargy and insomnia 5.

It is well known that non-effective drugs and placebo pills will both show an average 30% improvement in depression scores from baseline, not just due to spontaneous improvement from waiting 6. This “placebo effect” is thought to be due to hope and expectation of improvement on the part of the patient 7.

We are also concerned that the term “evidence-based” is used in descriptions of the validity of a specific therapy without being clearly defined. While not foolproof, a double-blind design to control for expectations in antidepressant confirmatory studies is crucial in order to decrease potential bias 2, 8.

Non-experimental comparative designs may also be used to make clinical inferences, however, this requires that studies include a number of conditions including: that the study subjects need to provide valid observations for the biological question under study; and the effect of the treatment must be large compared with random error and bias 2. These conditions are extremely hard to meet in MDD where symptom reports are subjective 1.

We think that using the term “evidence-based” for both double-blinded clinical drug trials as well as for unblinded psychotherapy trials confuses a consistent definition for “evidence-based”. We opine that the most valid definition of “evidence-based” is that of evidence garnered from the results of confirmatory trials of antidepressants that require double-blinding ( http://cpnp.org/resource/mhc/2014/01/antidepressant-medications-fda-approval-process-and-need-updates) 9. In this way, clinical trial designs with the strongest control level would be the standard for “evidence-based” data, although we acknowledge that double-blind clinical trials may also have various design and/or operational problems leading to invalid results.

We would like to illustrate how the combination of the placebo effect, along with the inability to double-blind a psychotherapy trial, can lead to bias in the results.

Figure 1 illustrates the effect hope and expectation vs. pharmacological effect may have on improved depression scores. Ratings of depressive symptoms are subjective, some symptoms of which thus may be amenable to a subjective sense of improvement with the hope and/or expectation of entering a trial. Both subjects and ‘treaters’ are blind to the content of the pill received. Group B, is given a known antidepressant, but the subjects are blind to the nature of the pill. Blinding allows the study to show any unbiased antidepressant effect additional to hope and expectation.

Figure 1. Double-blind randomized drug trial.

Figure 1.

Even more than just saying a study was “blinded”, however, absolute concealment of what treatment was allocated is crucial in order to avoid bias 10. The study should clearly describe how they maintained the blind and employ an “exit analysis” to confirm that subjects were not aware of their treatment allocation.

Figure 2 illustrates a non-blinded psychotherapy efficacy trial, using a “discussion” group and a “CBT” group. Because subjects are openly receive the intervention given in a psychotherapy, it is essentially impossible to blind a psychotherapeutic intervention. Any type of psychotherapy could be used in this model, but CBT is an instructive case because there are workbooks and specific tasks given to subjects making it clear to subject that they are in the CBT group. The subjects and therapists are both told of the type of therapy received.

Figure 2. Unblinded randomized psychotherapy trial.

Figure 2.

General Discussion refers to a non-directive, non-supportive discussion that is generally assumed to have no effect on MDD and represents a group that would have no expectation or hope of receiving a specific and directive therapy like CBT for the purposes of demonstrating the effect of unblinding for these groups. Assuming that “General Discussion” should not be effective in MDD, the discussion group’s improvement would then be similar to the placebo effect seen in a drug trial, and psychological placebos may also be as effective as accepted psychotherapies in MDD 11.

A third person who rates the degree of depression throughout the study should not know the therapy received (called a “masked rater”), but any bias on the part of the subject will just be part of the ratings reported by the subject to, and recorded by, the rater. The study is open so that it is not really possible to assess how much of each group’s improvement is due to the placebo effect, actual efficacy, or a bias towards CBT in knowing one is receiving CBT and/or hope from the act of actively trying to decrease negative thoughts as is done in CBT.

The importance of blinding in CBT interventions for psychiatric disorders was supported by a large meta analysis. Controlling for placebo and blindedness, a meta-analysis of data from published trials of CBT that showed CBT fared no better than non-specific control interventions in the treatment of schizophrenia and did not improve relapse rates, CBT showed no effect in prevention of bipolar disorder episodes, and only small treatment effects were seen in studies of MDD 12.

In our location in Japan, the Japanese National Health Insurance (NHI) system added CBT as a reimbursable procedure for MDD in 2010 13. It is concerning to us for a National Health Insurance system to provide reimbursement for a treatment of MDD that does not have the same scientific rigor that a double-blind study of an antidepressant would have. This is similar to the NHI situation in the UK as discussed by Lynch 12.

The suicide rate in Japan is among the highest in the world ( http://www8.cao.go.jp/jisatsutaisaku/whitepaper/en/w-2013/pdf/chap1-1_p2-3.pdf, Accessed on December 4th, 2014.) 14, and it is possible that treatments for MDD that do not have confirmatory double-blind clinical trials may lead patients with serious depression away from other approved treatments that do have this confirmation. The fact that the organizational relationship of the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare (MHLW) who funded the CBT studies also decides on the make-up of the committees at an organization (called the Chuikyo) that determines approval for reimbursement by the NHI is also of concern ( http://www.japantimes.co.jp/life/2006/03/14/lifestyle/who-is-paying-the-price-of-health-care/#.VIAGhMlWrkc, Accessed March 20th, 2015) 15.

Conclusions

The conclusions of the rationale presented in this paper would be that for MDD:

  • 1.

    Pill placebos show considerable positive effect on disorders with subjective endpoints such as those used to rate MDD 57.

  • 2.

    Psychological placebos may be as effective as accepted psychotherapies 11.

  • 3.

    Psychotherapy clinical trials are non-blinded studies, and cannot effectively be double-blinded. Calling these studies “single-blind” obfuscates the non-blinded nature of these studies and is not in line with the definition of “single-blind” in a clinical trial 4.

  • 4.

    It is imperative that any intervention for a disorder with subjective endpoints such as MDD requires the same rigor in double-blinding in order to conclude that the results show “efficacy” or are “evidence-based”. This paper proposes to use the term, “partially-controlled clinical data” in place of “evidence-based clinical data” for results obtained from unblinded studies.

The implications for practice are, 1. For practitioners to be clear with patients the level of rigor to which interventions have been studied, 2. For authors of psychotherapy outcome studies to be clear that the problem in the inability to blind a psychotherapy trial severely restricts the validity of any conclusions that can be drawn, and 3. To petition National Health Insurance Plans to use caution in approving interventions studied without double-blinded confirmatory trials as they may lead patients to avoid other treatments shown to be effective in double-blinded trials.

The limitations of this paper are that the lack of double-blinding does also not prove that the psychotherapy intervention is not helpful in some way to the indication being treated. Clinical opinion and consensus may guide how a psychotherapy will be used in practice.

We hope this paper can stimulate more research related to problems in blinding of psychotherapy outcome studies, the potential economic and clinical costs of providing or not-providing private or national health reimbursement for psychotherapeutic interventions, and further discussion on how our official professional organizations and national research centers will define “evidence-based” in relation to interventions for major depressive disorder.

Acknowledgements

The author thanks Mr. Burton Berger for help in preparation of the Figures originally done on Microsoft Excel.

Funding Statement

The author(s) declared that no grants were involved in supporting this work.

[version 2; referees: 2 approved]

References

  • 1. Wood L, Egger M, Gluud LL, et al. : Empirical evidence of bias in treatment effect estimates in controlled trials with different interventions and outcomes: meta-epidemiological study. BMJ. 2008;336(7644):601–605. 10.1136/bmj.39465.451748.AD [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Piantadosi S: Clinical Trials: A Methodologic Perspective, 2nd ed. New York: Wiley-Interscience;2005. 10.1002/0471740136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, et al. : Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press;1979. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Friedman LM, Furgerg CD, DeMets DL: Fundamentals of Clinical Trials, Third Edition. Springer;1998. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Cuijpers P, Driessen E, Hollon SD, et al. : The Efficacy of non-directive supportive therapy for adult depression: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32(4):280–291. 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Marchesi C, De Panfilis C, Matteo T, et al. : Is placebo useful in the treatment of major depression in clinical practice? Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2013;9:915–920. 10.2147/NDT.S44519 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Rutherford BR, Mori S, Sneed JR, et al. : Contribution of spontaneous improvement to placebo response in depression: a meta-analytic review. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(6):697–702. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Naudet F, Millet B, Reymann JM, et al. : Improving study design for antidepressant effectiveness assessment. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2013;22(3):217–31. 10.1002/mpr.1391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Hanrahan C, New JP: Antidepressant Medications: The FDA-Approval Process and the Need for Updates. Ment Health Clin. 2014;4(1):11–16. 10.9740/mhc.n186950 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Schulz KF, Grimes DA: Blinding in randomised trials: hiding who got what. Lancet. 2002;359(9307):696–700. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07816-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Wampold BE, Minami T, Tierney SC, et al. : The placebo is powerful: estimating placebo effects in medicine and psychotherapy from randomized clinical trials. J Clin Psychol. 2005;61(7):835–54. 10.1002/jclp.20129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Lynch D, Laws KR, McKenna PJ: Cognitive behavioural therapy for major psychiatric disorder: does it really work? A meta-analytical review of well-controlled trials. Psychol Med. 2010;40(1):9–24. 10.1017/S003329170900590X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Ono Y, Furukawa TA, Shimizu E, et al. : Current status of research on cognitive therapy/cognitive behavior therapy in Japan. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;65(2):121–129. 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2010.02182.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Cabinet Office, Government of Japan. White paper on Suicide Prevention in Japan.2013. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Otake T: Who is paying the price of health care? Committee slow to bring transparency to matters of national insurance. The Japan Times,2006. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2016 Jan 25. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.7488.r11959

Referee response for version 1

Keith R Laws 1

I have a few comments on Dr Berger's manuscript, which raises interesting discussion points

  1. Dr Berger states that “Psychotherapy clinical outcome trials for major depressive disorder (MDD) are often described as “randomized”, “controlled”, “singleblind”, etc. These words may not adequately describe the level of methodologic rigor of the design of a trial for MDD because the endpoints are subjective symptom ratings” (my italics).

    This latter point might benefit from some expanding and clarifying. All current psychiatric ratings are subjective to some extent (in pharma trials as well), but there are a variety of subjectives. For example, differences in terms of whether such 'subjective' assessment occurs using a self-rating tool (like the Beck Depression Inventory: BDI) or a clinician-rating scale (e.g. Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression). This is especially pertinent in depression trials as self-rating tools like BDI are frequently used and in effect, this renders trials 'open'.

    As Dr Berger states, meta-analyses show quite clearly that open psychotherapy trials can produce highly inflated effect sizes (see also Jauhar et al. 2014). The latter meta-analysis proves two points a) that compared with even single-blind trials, open trials of CBT can inflate effect sizes hugely and b) that contrary to what some say, psychotherapy trials can be very effectively blinded to assessors. 

  2. I agree that the term ‘evidence-based’ in psychotherapy has been rendered somewhat meaningless and glosses over -what could be - important variability in terms of what this phrase covers. In some instances, paradoxically, the phrase is offered as an bland substitute for providing actual evidence...not unlike the often repeated “as recommended by NICE” - a shorthand way of avoiding the presentation and evaluation of actual evidence ... as if these phrases were chiselled in stone atop Mount Sinai.

  3. Study quality is undoubtedly the key factor and conflating evidence from double-blind, single-blind and open trials under the rubric of ‘evidence-based’ is unhelpful - especially to patients who require guidance on the efficacy of prospective psychotherapy as Dr Berger argues. Indeed, it would be much more helpful to patients and clinicians if bodies such as NICE incorporate study quality directly into their analysis of the evidence-base and hence, their recommendations.

  4. Its worth noting in passing that even when meta-analyses might reveal the same numerical effect size for a drug and a psychological intervention, this does not indicate equivalence of efficacy. Again, it depends crucially upon the quality of the data underpinning each effect size - so claims that CBT and anti-depressants have comparable effects really depends on the assumption that designs are equivalent (which they clearly are not). As Dr Berger suggests, drug trials are better controlled and so, any equivalence is more apparent than real.

  5. The proposal to use “partially-controlled clinical data” in place of “evidence-based clinical data” certainly highlights a key difference, though I guess it all depends upon the extent to which psychological therapy can be squeezed into the 'RCT template' that has served drug evaluation so well…RCTs of psych therapies are necessarily at best single blind or at worst (and still very often) ‘open’. Some psychotherapy advocates would of course make a special case for psychotherapy - that it is simply not amenable to the same paradigms as drug studies and perhaps Dr Berger could expand on how he views the validity of such counter-arguments.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

References

  • 1. Jauhar S, McKenna PJ, Radua J, Fung E, Salvador R, Laws KR: Cognitive-behavioural therapy for the symptoms of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis with examination of potential bias. Br J Psychiatry.2014;204(1) : 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116285 20-9 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2015 Oct 23. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.7488.r10898

Referee response for version 1

Gordon Parker 1

A straightforward paper, well written and taking the reader through the difficulties of undertaking and interpreting psychotherapies when no placebo control or equivalent control strategy is available and therefore worthy of indexing.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.


Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

RESOURCES