Abstract
This article reviews 95 publications (based on 21 independent samples) that have examined children at family risk of reading disorders. We report that children at family risk of dyslexia experience delayed language development as infants and toddlers. In the preschool period, they have significant difficulties in phonological processes as well as with broader language skills and in acquiring the foundations of decoding skill (letter knowledge, phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming [RAN]). Findings are mixed with regard to auditory and visual perception: they do not appear subject to slow motor development, but lack of control for comorbidities confounds interpretation. Longitudinal studies of outcomes show that children at family risk who go on to fulfil criteria for dyslexia have more severe impairments in preschool language than those who are defined as normal readers, but the latter group do less well than controls. Similarly at school age, family risk of dyslexia is associated with significantly poor phonological awareness and literacy skills. Although there is no strong evidence that children at family risk are brought up in an environment that differs significantly from that of controls, their parents tend to have lower educational levels and read less frequently to themselves. Together, the findings suggest that a phonological processing deficit can be conceptualized as an endophenotype of dyslexia that increases the continuous risk of reading difficulties; in turn its impact may be moderated by protective factors.
Keywords: dyslexia, endophenotype, family risk of dyslexia, language impairment, multiple risk factors
Literacy skills are the key to educational attainments and these in turn promote access to career opportunities and employment. However, even in developed educational systems, a substantial number of children fail to learn to read to age-expected levels. A specific barrier to progress is difficulty in decoding print—the first step to reading with understanding; such difficulties with the development of decoding skills are referred to as dyslexia. Dyslexia is a common condition, thought to affect some 3–7% of the English-speaking population, with more boys than girls affected (Rutter et al., 2004).
The definition of dyslexia has undergone many changes over the years, not least because there are no clear diagnostic criteria (Rose, 2009). Rather the distribution of reading skills in the population is continuous and the “cut-offs” used to define a person as “dyslexic” are arbitrary (e.g., Pennington, 2006). Moreover, dyslexia tends to co-occur with other disorders, including specific language impairment (SLI; e.g., Bishop & Snowling, 2004; McArthur, Hogben, Edwards, Heath, & Mengler, 2000; Melby-Lervåg & Lervåg, 2012), speech sound disorder (e.g., Pennington & Bishop, 2009), and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; e.g., McGrath et al., 2011; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). Consistent with this, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Association (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders–Fifth Edition, DSM–5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) groups together reading disorders (dyslexia), mathematical disorders, and disorders of written expression under a single overarching diagnosis of Specific Learning Disorder within the broader category of Neurodevelopmental Disorders.
It has been known for many years that dyslexia, like other neurodevelopmental disorders, runs in families and studies of large twin samples demonstrate that reading and the phonological skills that underpin it are highly heritable (Olson, 2011, for a review). However, the genetic mechanisms involved are complex and depend on the combined influence of many genes of small effect and gene—gene interactions (e.g., Newbury, Monaco, & Paracchini, 2014). In addition, “generalist” genes, thought to code for general cognitive abilities, contribute to the etiology of dyslexia (Plomin & Kovas, 2005). Arguably, progress in the neurobiology of dyslexia requires a refined understanding of the phenotype of dyslexia at the cognitive level which clarifies its key developmental features and its relationship to other disorders. The current review contributes to that understanding.
In this article we provide a comprehensive review of studies that, assuming the heritability of dyslexia, have compared children at family risk (FR) of dyslexia with children with no such risk (low risk, control) on behavioral measures of perception, language, and cognition. Such studies have typically been designed to test out causal hypotheses regarding the developmental course of dyslexia. The methodology has distinct advantages over the more standard case-control approach; first it is free of clinical bias because children are recruited before they enter formal reading instruction; second, it allows us to disentangle possible causes of dyslexia from its consequences; third, and arguably most importantly, it is the only methodology that can allow the identification of risk factors in the preschool period, together with likely protective factors. Although such longitudinal prediction can also be conducted using a general population sample, the high-risk method is much more efficient; for instance, in a general population sample with a rate of dyslexia at 10%, one would need to follow 500 children to obtain a sample of 50 dyslexics; in a high-risk sample with a rate of dyslexia around 30%, a sample of 150 will yield the same number of affected cases.
The current review is organized around the main questions that family risk studies have addressed: the prevalence of dyslexia in children with a first-degree-affected relative, the nature of the home literacy environment in dyslexic families and the possible causes of dyslexia. We use the findings to address two further important issues for theory and practice. First, the relationship between dyslexia (diagnosed when written language skills fail to develop adequately) and language impairment (diagnosed when spoken language skills fail to develop adequately; Bishop & Snowling, 2004; Ramus et al., 2012). Second, evidence regarding what works to prevent or ameliorate dyslexia in children at high risk of the disorder. We begin with a short review of the research that guided our hypotheses before outlining the methodology for the current study and the constructs that were evaluated in the meta-analysis.
Individual Differences in Reading Development
To consider the nature and developmental course of dyslexia, it is important to highlight the resource demands of learning to read. Universally, reading is a process of mapping between the visual symbols on the page (orthography) and the spoken language. However, the nature of the symbols and the units of spoken language to which they connect differ across languages (e.g., Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). In an alphabetic language such as English, the foundation of literacy is a system of mappings between letters and phonemes (the smallest speech sounds of words) and a challenge for the child is to abstract the mapping principle (Byrne, 1998). In contrast, Chinese is nonalphabetic; the orthographic symbols are characters comprising semantic and phonetic radicals that correspond to morphemes (units of meaning). The semantic radical provides information about the meaning and the phonetic radical provides a cue to the pronunciation of the word (Shu & Anderson, 1997).
Orthographies also differ in their “transparency”—that is the regularity of the mappings between symbols and sounds. Among European languages, English has the most inconsistent writing system, embodying many exceptions; German and Dutch have relatively few inconsistencies and Finnish is the most consistent. In general, it has been shown that regular languages pose fewer challenges for the beginning learner than irregular languages (Seymour, 2005) but arguably, this is a simplistic view. Languages also differ in the way in which they convey the grammar in writing and differences in morphological structure may moderate the ease of learning (Seidenberg, 2011).
Nonetheless, it is clear that the specific demands of learning to read will differ across languages. Irrespective of this, the child needs to learn the symbol set and how that set maps to the language and this requires explicit awareness of the structure of the language. Once the basic mappings have been established, reading practice (“print exposure”) is required to achieve reading fluency.
Among alphabetic languages it is well established that the predictors of individual differences in decoding (the skill that is impaired in dyslexia) are the same regardless of the transparency of the language: these are, letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and rapid automatized naming (Caravolas et al., 2012; Ziegler et al., 2010a). However, reading development proceeds faster in the more transparent orthographies than in opaque orthographies (Caravolas et al., 2013). The process of learning is more protracted in languages that have large symbol sets and in nonalphabetic languages where mappings are to meaning rather than sound (Nag, Caravolas, & Snowling, 2011). Nonetheless, a similar set of skills appear to predict progress in these languages—namely symbol knowledge, metalinguistic awareness, and rapid automatized naming; the corollary is that deficits in these skills compromise decoding in dyslexia.
The Role of Environmental Factors
The majority of research on dyslexia has focused on its proximal cognitive causes; however, within a developmental framework it is important also to consider distal risk factors. Given the differences between orthographies, one extrinsic factor which affects the rate of reading development is the language of learning. Thus, it might be expected that the prevalence of dyslexia will depend on the transparency of the language; indeed it has sometimes been speculated that the difficulty of the English language could be a cause of dyslexia. However, data from more regular orthographies provide little evidence in support of this contention (for Dutch: Blomert, 2005; for German: Fischbach et al., 2013).
Social-demographic factors can also affect reading attainments. For example, higher rates of reading difficulty have been reported in inner-city samples than in rural areas (except in developing countries where the opposite trend is seen) and it is well-known that there is a social gradient in reading attainment such that reading is poorer in disadvantaged groups (e.g., Rutter & Maughan, 2005). Among the factors that could account for demographic variation are differences in parental education level (Phillips & Lonigan, 2005, for a review) or in quality of schooling, although whether such differences are truly environmental, rather than reflecting genetic factors, is a moot point (Friend, DeFries, & Olson, 2008). The quality of the home literacy environment (e.g., Frijters, Barron, & Brunello, 2000) is one factor that likely reflects the correlated effects of genes and environment (ge correlation). Moreover, evidence suggests that the home literacy environment may at least partially mediate the influence of socioeconomic status (SES) on children’s literacy outcomes (e.g., Chazan-Cohen et al., 2009).
Home literacy environment is defined by a range of factors concerning parents’ and children’s attitudes and dispositions toward reading, including how long parents spend reading with their children and parents’ own reading behavior. An important component is “shared book reading” that has a significant impact on children’s oral language development (Bus, van Ijzendoorn, & Pellegrini, 1995). In addition, direct teaching of literacy concepts is observed in some families and this strengthens the foundational skills for reading. More important, differences in parental attitude to literacy appear to affect how children’s reading skills develop: while direct instructional practices focusing on print concepts facilitate the early development of decoding skills, shared language experiences around books have a greater impact on reading comprehension (Senechal & LeFevre, 2001). An important question that arises from this research is how the literacy environment in families in which there is a parent (or indeed a child) with dyslexia differs from that in a family where family members are free of literacy problems. Family risk studies provide the opportunity to do this and to track possible changes in both child- and parental attitudes as reading develops over time.
Perceptual and Cognitive Deficits in Dyslexia
A major thrust of research on dyslexia has been to specify the underlying deficits that are candidate causes of the condition. Such impairments (that may be cognitive and/or have their origins in basic perceptual processes) mediate the impact of heritable, brain-based differences on behavior (Morton & Frith, 1995; Pennington, 2002; Ramus, 2003, 2004).
Within this approach, the predominant view for many years was that dyslexia could be traced to deficits within the phonological system of language (Melby-Lervåg, Lyster, & Hulme, 2012; Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004). As we have seen, phonological skills are critical foundations for learning to read in alphabetic systems. More generally, phonological deficits have been reported to characterize dyslexia in logographic Chinese (Hanley, 2005; Ho, Chan, Lee, Tsang, & Luan, 2004; Ho, Chan, Tsang, & Lee, 2002) and poor readers of alphasyllabic scripts (Nag & Snowling, 2011). However, a problem in assessing the causal status of phonological deficits is that performance on phonological tasks (such as phoneme awareness and nonword repetition) is influenced by reading skill (Morais & Kolinsky, 2005 for a review). It follows that deficits in these processes could be correlates (rather than causes) of poor reading. An advantage of family risk studies is that phonological processing can be measured before the onset of literacy. As we shall see, all family risk studies have assessed the development of phonological skills.
At a more fine-grained level, research has pursued the causes of the phonological deficits in dyslexia. In now classic work, Tallal (1980) proposed that dyslexia is caused by a problem with the rapid temporal processing of auditory information needed for the perception of speech sounds, leading to a cascade of difficulty from auditory processing through speech perception to phonological skills. Family risk studies are well placed to test such causal chains from the early stages of language development. Accordingly many of these studies draw on research suggesting deficits in speech perception in dyslexia (e.g., Adlard & Hazan, 1998; Nittrouer, 1999; Serniclaes, Van Heghe, Mousty, Carré, & Sprenger-Charolles, 2004; Ziegler, Pech-Georgel, George, & Lorenzi, 2009) or in basic auditory processing (Hämäläinen, Salminen, & Leppänen, 2012 for a review).
A separate line of investigation has focused on possible causes of difficulties with the letter-by-letter structure of words (orthographic deficits) in dyslexia. One theory is that spatial coding deficits affect ocular motor control (Boden & Giaschi, 2007; Kevan & Pammer, 2008; Vidyasagar & Pammer, 2010). Alternatively, problems in the system of visual attention could affect the left-to-right extraction of orthographic information critical for parsing letter strings before decoding (Facoetti, Paganoni, Turatto, Marzola, & Mascetti, 2000; Valdois, Bosse, & Tainturier, 2004). In addition, there are modality—general theories that aim not to explain particular features of dyslexia but rather seek overarching explanations (e.g., Ahissar, Lubin, Putter-Katz, & Bani, 2006; Nicolson & Fawcett, 1990; Vicari, Marotta, Menghini, Molinari, & Petrosini, 2003). Whereas such theories may hold promise for understanding how dyslexia relates to other co-occurring disorders (comorbidity, e.g., Rochelle & Talcott, 2006) they do not explain why dyslexia can and sometimes does occur in the absence of any other cognitive deficits.
Nevertheless, as a long history of the search for subtypes of dyslexia attests, these causal hypotheses are not mutually exclusive and it is important to recognize that dyslexia is a heterogeneous condition (e.g., Ramus et al., 2003). As Pennington (2006) has argued, the etiology of complex disorders like dyslexia is multifactorial and involves the interactions of risk and protective factors. Longitudinal studies of children at family risk of dyslexia that follow children from early childhood to formal schooling can reveal the risk factors associated with a dyslexia outcome. In addition, because dyslexia is a dimensional disorder, the study of unaffected relatives can be informative in highlighting protective or compensatory factors that mitigate familial risks. Such risks (that could be biological processes or cognitive impairments) can be described as “endophenotypes” (Skuse, 2001).
According to Bearden and Freimer (2006), an endophenotype is a “marker” that is associated with the disorder in the population and expressed at a higher rate in unaffected relatives of probands than in the general population. Put another way, it is intermediate between the genotype and the phenotype and, importantly, the impact of such processes can be moderated or compensated for by areas of skill or through interventions. This particular characteristic of an endophenotype deserves mention in relation to comorbidities. When two neurodevelopmental disorders frequently co-occur it is probable that they have endophenotypes in common (Thapar & Rutter, 2015, for a review). Prospective family risk studies can identify putative endophenotypes of dyslexia (or subclinical features) that mark the presence of co-occurring disorders For example, findings of family risk studies can elucidate relationships between difficulties with oral language observed in preschool and later written language disorders, in short, the comorbidity between dyslexia, specific language impairment (Bishop & Snowling, 2004; Pennington & Bishop, 2009, for reviews).
Study Rationale and Hypotheses
Although the discussion above highlights the importance of taking a longitudinal perspective to understanding dyslexia, current knowledge of dyslexia draws mainly on cross-sectional studies involving the comparison of individuals with dyslexia and controls at one specific point in time. Such evidence cannot distinguish adequately between causal and noncausal reasons for associations. Following the pioneering work of Scarborough (1989, 1990), who followed a group of 2-year-old English-speaking children deemed to be at high-risk of dyslexia because of an affected parent, many prospective studies of children at family risk of dyslexia have begun in recent years. At the time of writing, 21 independent studies have been completed, resulting in 95 behavioral publications that are the focus of this review. In addition, our review found 23 descriptive studies using neurophysiological measures that are listed in Supplemental Material Table S1 (in the online supplement file). We used the findings of the behavioral studies to test the hypotheses that follow.
Hypothesis 1: The prevalence of dyslexia in children at family risk. We predicted that dyslexia would be more common in at-risk than control families (Hypothesis 1a) and more prevalent in English because it has an opaque orthography than in other European languages (Hypothesis 1b); in addition, given the large character set, we expected a high prevalence in Chinese. Irrespective of orthography, because dyslexia is a dimensional disorder with no clear boundaries, we predicted that prevalence would depend upon the cut-off used for “diagnosis” (Hypothesis 1c).
Hypothesis 2: The home and literacy environment of children at family risk of dyslexia. Because of the influence of genetic factors and the environments correlated with them, we hypothesized that the home literacy environment would differ between families in which there is a history of reading difficulty from that in families where parents are free of such problems (Hypothesis 2a). We did not specify the ways in which these differences would be manifest but we anticipated that parents with dyslexia would read less for pleasure than controls (Hypothesis 2b). If the home literacy environment influences children’s reading attainment, we predicted that parental literacy skills (a proxy for gene—environment correlation; van Bergen, van der Leij, & de Jong, 2014) would account for independent variance in children’s reading outcomes (Hypothesis 2c).
Hypothesis 3: Endophenotypes of dyslexia. Although endophenotypes can take many forms, we are concerned here with the perceptual and cognitive deficits that are observed among children at family risk of dyslexia. These can be expected to be present in the preschool years before dyslexia is diagnosed (where they can be construed as cognitive risk factors; Hypothesis 3a). Based on the overlap between dyslexia and language impairment (Bishop & Snowling, 2004; Ramus et al., 2012), we expected that delays and difficulties with speech and language development would be common (Hypothesis 3b). More specifically, for alphabetic languages we predicted that three of the skills considered foundational for literacy (letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, and rapid automatized naming [RAN]; Caravolas et al., 2013) would show developmental delay in the preschool period (Hypothesis 3c) and deficits in each would characterize dyslexia in the school years (Hypothesis 3d). Given the known continuity of risk for dyslexia among family members (Pennington & Lefly, 2001), we predicted that unaffected children at family risk of dyslexia would also have poor literacy skills relative to controls but not severe enough to warrant a diagnosis of dyslexia (Hypothesis 3e) and deficits/endophenotypes should be observed in unaffected children but to a milder degree (Hypothesis 3f). Finally, we aimed to evaluate the causal status of basic deficits in speech perception, visual, and auditory processing, attention and motor skills as additional risk factors (Hypothesis 3a).
Hypothesis 4: Predictive relationships between early cognitive abilities and later reading. Reading builds upon spoken language and there are similar heritable influences on both reading comprehension and language comprehension (e.g., Keenan, Betjemann, Wadsworth, DeFries, & Olson, 2006). In this light, we predicted that preschool measures of oral language would predict later reading outcomes, particularly reading comprehension (Hypothesis 4a). Based on previous findings (e.g., Caravolas et al., 2013) we also predicted that, for alphabetic languages, there would be three predictors of decoding skills and hence dyslexia: phonological awareness, symbol knowledge (letters in alphabetic languages) and RAN (Hypothesis 4b).
Hypothesis 5: Interventions for dyslexia. Arguably the ultimate aim of research on dyslexia is to identify effective interventions that will ameliorate its impact on educational attainments. In addition, training studies are important theoretically as tests of causal hypotheses (Snowling & Hulme, 2011). We expected that interventions incorporating training in letter knowledge and phoneme awareness would improve decoding skills in children at family risk of dyslexia (Hypothesis 5a). However, we did not expect such interventions to impact reading comprehension beyond gains in decoding unless they incorporated training in broader oral language skills (e.g., vocabulary training; Hypothesis 5b).
Table 1. Hypotheses for the Review.
Hypotheses guiding the review | Predictions |
---|---|
The prevalence of dyslexia in children at family risk | |
Hypothesis 1a | Dyslexia is more common in at-risk than control families. |
Hypothesis 1b | Dyslexia is more prevalent in English than in other European (regular) languages. |
Hypothesis 1c | The prevalence rates for dyslexia depend upon the cut-off used for “diagnosis.” |
The home and literacy environment of children at family risk of dyslexia | |
Hypothesis 2a | The home literacy environment will differ between families in which there is a history of reading difficulty from that in families where both parents are free of problems. |
Hypothesis 2b | Parents with dyslexia will read less for pleasure than controls. |
Hypothesis 2c | Parental literacy skills will account for independent variance in children’s reading outcomes. |
Endophenotypes of dyslexia | |
Hypothesis 3a | Cognitive and perceptual risk factors that are putative endophenotypes should be present in the preschool years before dyslexia is diagnosed. |
Hypothesis 3b | Children at family risk of dyslexia will show delayed speech/language development. |
Hypothesis 3c | The development of three critical foundations for decoding, viz phonological awareness, symbol knowledge (letters in alphabetic languages) and rapid naming (RAN) will be delayed in children at family risk of dyslexia in preschool. |
Hypothesis 3d | Deficits in phonological awareness, symbol knowledge and RAN will characterize dyslexia in the school years. |
Hypothesis 3e | Unaffected children at family risk of dyslexia will have poorer literacy skills than controls. |
Hypothesis 3f | Endophenotypes of dyslexia will be observed in unaffected children but to a milder degree than in affected children. |
The predictive relationships between early cognitive and later reading skills | |
Hypothesis 4a | Oral language skills will predict literacy outcomes in children at family risk of dyslexia. |
Hypothesis 4b | Three critical foundations for decoding, viz phonological awareness, symbol knowledge (letters in alphabetic languages) and rapid naming (RAN) will be predictors of decoding in children at family risk of dyslexia. |
Interventions for dyslexia | |
Hypothesis 5a | Training in letter knowledge and phoneme awareness will ameliorate decoding difficulties. |
Hypothesis 5b | Training in vocabulary and broader language skills will improve reading comprehension. |
Structure of the Current Review
Table 1 presents a summary of the hypotheses that guided the review. First, to assess Hypotheses 1(a–c) we summarize data from studies examining prevalence of dyslexia in family risk studies; second, to assess Hypotheses 2 (a and b), we use data on the home and literacy environment and to assess Hypothesis 2c we use data examining parental skills as predictors of outcome. To assess evidence for endophenotypes (Hypotheses 3 a–f), we adopt a developmental perspective, presenting evidence from different developmental stages from infancy/toddlerhood to secondary school. Here we will discuss findings of studies that have compared children at family risk of dyslexia with controls (at low risk because they do not have a family history). These studies can tell us about the risk factors associated with familial dyslexia (Hypotheses 3a–d). We will also include findings from retrospective analyses comparing the cognitive profiles of children at family risk who have later been classified as dyslexic or not, and controls. In addition to reinforcing the conclusions above, these studies can assess Hypothesis 3e and elucidate endophenotypes of dyslexia (Hypothesis 3f). Next we will examine the findings of longitudinal studies that have incorporated multiple regression and related statistical techniques to provide additional evidence regarding the predictors of dyslexia (Hypotheses 4a and b) and finally evaluate the evidence for effective interventions (Hypothesis 5a and b).
Method
The review was designed and is reported in line with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (www.prisma-statement.org). PRISMA is a consensus statement developed by an international group of researchers in health care for the conduct and reporting of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. When there is a sufficient number of studies (two or more), we will do a meta-analysis by calculating a mean effect size. If a summary effect is not possible, we will do a systematic review and report effect sizes for individual studies.
Literature Search, Inclusion Criteria, and Coding
Literature search
Details concerning the literature search, criteria for inclusion and flow of studies are shown in Figure 1. The literature search consisted of the following components: Electronic databases (ERIC, Medline, PsychInfo, and all Citation Databases included in ISI web of knowledge from 1980–30 July 2015 with keywords in abstracts at-risk, famil* risk paired with dyslexia, reading disorders, decoding, and decoding problems), citation search on author names, emails to authors and posting at the list server for Scientific Studies of Reading, hand search of journals (Dyslexia, Annuals of Dyslexia, Scientific Studies of Reading), scanning reference lists, and Google Scholar.
Inclusion criteria
To be included a study had to consist of a sample of children with familial risk of dyslexia. Family risk was defined as having a parent and or a sibling (a first-degree relative) with dyslexia. The at-risk status had to be verified by testing or self-report of the relative with reading problems. In addition, the studies had to include a control group of children with no known family risk of dyslexia. Studies vary in the nomenclature they have used. To be consistent, in this review, we use the terms “FR–Dyslexia” to refer to children at family risk (FR) who are identified as having reading problems, “FR–NR” to refer to children at family risk who are identified as typical in their literacy outcome (normal reader) and “control” to refer to children from low-risk groups with no family history, who are considered free of reading difficulties.
In the review we focus on three different study designs: (a) group comparisons, (b) longitudinal prediction studies, and (c) experimental intervention studies. We also include references to neurophysiological studies that are tabulated in Table S1 in the online supplement file. The studies included had to report data so that an effect size could be calculated or significance testing reported for either one of three types of comparisons, based on the different designs we found in the studies in this field: (Comparison Type 1) between children at family risk of dyslexia (FR+) and children without such risk (control). In these studies, reading skills are treated as a continuous variable, and family risk children are not separated into two groups based on whether they have dyslexia or not; (Comparison Type 2) between children at family risk later “diagnosed” with dyslexia (FR–Dyslexia) and control children without family risk (control); and (Comparison Type 3) children at family risk who did not fulfil criteria for dyslexia (FR–NR) and control children without family risk (control).
For the longitudinal prediction studies, in addition to the criteria outlined above, the study must have reported a longitudinal analysis concerning either (a) unique predictors of outcomes treated as continuous variables, or (b) unique predictors of literacy outcome when this is treated as a categorical variable (i.e., presence or absence of reading problem). For a study to be included, the analysis needs to have been conducted so that the predictive patterns pertaining to children at family risk and children without such risk could be compared, or the effect of group membership could be determined. For the experimental intervention studies, some kind of training must have been included with the aim of ameliorating difficulties of reading or literacy in children at family risk.
The neurophysiological studies in Table S1 in the online supplement file used a variety of methodologies and were not amenable to meta-analysis. Most of these studies are of small samples; however, their findings will be used when appropriate to reinforce conclusions.
Coding
Some of the at-risk studies are longitudinal and report data from different stages in development. When coding the studies, it became clear that although there are 95 different publications, many were based on the same study sample (there are 21 independent samples). In Appendices A, B, and C (Column 1 in parentheses after the author name), the sample on which the publication is based is indicated. Special attention had to be taken in the coding to avoid bias related to dependency in the data. To make use of as much information as possible from the different publications, information was coded for different developmental stages: (a) Infants and toddlers (below the age of 3 years), (b) Preschool (below 5.5 years and before formal reading instruction starts), (c) Early Primary school (up to 4th grade), and (d) Late primary school/secondary school (from 5th grade and upward). By doing this, we were able to code information from longitudinal studies twice without merging data from the same study in the same analysis. Therefore, we did not violate the assumption of independence in the data. Nonetheless effect sizes in the different developmental stages will be related because 20 out of 69 publications included in the analyses of group comparisons have data coded from more than one developmental stage.
Furthermore, scrutiny of the studies revealed that several different measures were often reported for the same construct, sometimes in different publications. Table 2 presents the indicators that we selected to represent the higher-order constructs in the review. If there was more than one indicator for a construct from the same developmental stage (e.g., Boston naming test and word definitions for vocabulary knowledge), either in the same or in different publications, the mean of the indicators coded was used in the analysis. An advantage of this procedure is that the mean effect size will be more reliable because it is based on two measures of the same construct. The number of studies and sum of participants reported for the different analyses refers to the number of independent samples and participants that have provided data on a construct and the number of effect sizes contributing to a mean effect size is reported in parentheses. However, if the same test was reported in several different publications on the same sample, the same test was only coded once. In many cases, prevalence of dyslexia is reported for the same sample in several publications. However, such data were only coded once for each independent sample based on one set of reading tests in each study. When it was unclear whether data were based on the same sample or parts of the same sample, authors were contacted by email and asked for clarification. Most authors responded, but in cases where they did not, we have based the overview on information provided in the articles.
Table 2. Indicators for the Constructs Targeted in the Review.
Higher order constructs | Lower order constructs | Indicators coded |
---|---|---|
Home literacy environment | Parental education | Reports of educational background |
Book reading habits and availability | Various questionnaires and self-reports | |
Parental literacy skills | Tests of parental language and reading skills | |
General cognitive abilities and perceptual skills | Nonverbal IQ | Nonverbal problem-solving tasks such as Raven, WISC performance, TONI |
Motor skills | Tests or rating scales of motor skills | |
Auditory processing | Speech perception, tone perception, tone in noise detection | |
Visual processing | Tests of visual perception (e.g. Gardner), visual matching, visual cueing | |
Oral language skills | Articulatory accuracy | Percentage of consonants correct, percentage of intelligible speech |
Vocabulary knowledge | Defining words, naming pictures, parental scales such as CDI | |
Pointing at pictures (such as PPVT) | ||
Grammar | Plural marking, Past tense test, temporal terms, inflection, receptive grammar, expressive syntax | |
Phonological memory | Nonword repetition tests such as CN Rep | |
Verbal short-term memory | Digit span, word span | |
Decoding specific skills | Letter knowledge | Accuracy of letter-naming, naming letter–sound correspondences |
Phoneme awareness | Phoneme manipulation, phoneme detection, phoneme generation, composite tests where phoneme tasks are in majority | |
Rhyme awareness | Rhyme oddity task, rhyme generation tasks, rhyme detection | |
Rapid automatized naming (RAN) | Naming speed for symbolic items (objects, letters, digits or colors) | |
Literacy skills | Word recognition | Word reading accuracy, word reading fluency |
Nonword decoding | Nonword reading accuracy, nonword reading fluency | |
Spelling accuracy | Spelling regular words, spelling irregular words, spelling nonwords | |
Reading comprehension | Cloze tests or open-ended questions about the meaning of a text |
A random sample of 80% of the studies was coded by two independent raters. The interrater correlation (Pearson’s) for main outcomes (means, SD, sample size, and age) was r = .98 and agreement rate = 93%. Any disagreements between raters were resolved by consulting the original article or by discussion.
Procedure and Analysis
The coding of studies and analyses were conducted using the “comprehensive meta-analysis” program (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005). Two different effect sizes were used in the meta-analysis. For group differences between the family risk groups and the children from low-risk groups with no family history, we used Cohen’s d. Cohen’s ds were calculated using Hedges’ corrections for small sample sizes (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). When Cohen’s d is negative, children from high-risk groups with a family history have the lowest score. For judging the size of the effect, for group comparisons Cohen’s tentative guidelines were used. According to Cohen, d = 0.2 is a small effect, 0.4 is moderate and 0.8 is large. However, note that according to Cohen (1968), such guidelines should only be used when no better basis for estimating the effect size is available. In the intervention studies, if Cohen’s d is positive, the group that has received the intervention has the highest gain between pre- and posttest. For the intervention studies, two influential policy organizations (What works clearinghouse [WWC] and Promising Practices Network [PPN]), have set a limit of d = 0.25 for when results of high-quality randomised trials should be taken as having policy implications (see Cooper, 2008). In the intervention studies we adopt these guidelines in preference to Cohen. For estimates of prevalence of children in the family risk samples and the control samples affected with dyslexia, we used percentage affected with dyslexia as the effect size.
Mean effect sizes were estimated by calculating a weighted average of individual effect sizes using a random effects model. A mean effect size was calculated if there were two or more studies. A 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated for each effect size to establish whether it was statistically significantly larger than zero. If confidence intervals cross zero, the result is not significantly different from zero.
To examine the variation in effect sizes between studies, the Tau2 was used (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). As a rule of thumb, if Tau2 exceeds 1, the variation between the studies is large. I2 was also used to determine the degree of true heterogeneity. I2 assesses the percentage of between-study variance that is attributable to true heterogeneity rather than random error. Notably, I2 does not say anything about the size of the variation between the studies in general, that is, I2 can be 100% or 10% but in both cases variation between studies can be small or large. Thus, I2 can only be used to determine the part of the heterogeneity that is due to true variation between studies rather than sampling error.
For moderator variables, studies were separated into subsets based on the categories in the categorical moderator variable, and a Q-test was used to examine whether the effect sizes differed between subsets. When there were fewer than two studies in a category, this analysis was not conducted. To examine the size of difference between subsets of studies, overlap between CIs was also examined. In cases of multiple significance tests of moderators on the same data set, the results are reported both with and without Bonferroni correction.
When we coded articles, it became clear that there were numerous instances of missing data. If data were critical to calculate an effect size, articles with missing data were excluded if authors did not respond to an email request to provide the data (see inclusion criteria in flowchart). In cases where an effect size could be computed on one outcome but data were missing on other outcomes or moderator variables, the study was included in all the analyses for which sufficient data were provided.
Results From the Review
Appendices A, B, and C contains all the necessary information to replicate the results from our meta-analysis. Appendix A shows characteristics of studies comparing children at family risk (not yet diagnosed) and control children. In the results, data based on Appendix A are presented as group comparisons of children at family risk versus controls not at-risk. Appendix B shows characteristics of studies comparing children at family risk diagnosed with dyslexia and controls and the prevalence of dyslexia. In the results, data based on Appendix B are presented as group comparisons of (a) family risk children with dyslexia versus controls not at-risk and (b) family risk children who are normal readers versus controls not at-risk. Appendix C shows characteristics of intervention studies concerning children with family risk of dyslexia; results based on these data are presented in a separate section of the review on intervention studies.
Prevalence of Dyslexia
Prevalence of dyslexia in family risk samples
The first analysis considered the prevalence of dyslexia in families where there was a positive history. Fifteen independent studies had examined prevalence of dyslexia/reading disorders in samples of children at family risk. The studies included 420 children with dyslexia (mean sample size 29.4, SD = 13.9, range 9 to 53). Mean prevalence (expressed as percentage) of dyslexia in the family risk samples in the different studies was 45% (95% CI [39%, 51.2%] I2 = 36.5%, Tau2 = 0.08; see Figure 2), confirming that children with a first-degree relative with reading problems have a high risk of developing such problems themselves1 (Hypothesis 1a).
An analysis of moderator variables showed that, as to be expected, the cut-off point for diagnosing dyslexia had a significant impact on the mean prevalence (Hypothesis 1c). In eight studies where the criterion for diagnosing reading disorder was set above the 10th percentile, the mean prevalence was 53% (95% CI [46.3%, 60.0%], I2 = 13.54%, Tau2 = 0.02) while in six studies where the criterion was set below the 10th percentile, the mean prevalence was 33.7%, (95% CI [26.6%, 41.8%], I2 = 0%, Tau2 = 0). Notably, one study was excluded from this analysis because it did not report a cut-off. Because of distributional characteristics, the impact from the diagnostic cut-off variable had to be separated into two categories rather than be analyzed as a continuous variable; the difference between the two categories was significant, Q(1) = 11.84, p < .001, also after correcting for multiple comparisons, p = .001.
Turning to differences in prevalence according to orthography (excluding Chinese studies), for English 54% (95% CI [41.3%, 66.2%], I2 = 52.85%, Tau2 = 0.17). For studies from more transparent languages (Dutch, Danish, or Finnish), 40% (95% CI [35.0%, 46.4%], I2 = 0%, Tau2 = 0). This difference was approaching significance, Q(1) = 3.62, p = .058. Further, after examining the studies, it was clear that more studies with English samples had lenient cut-off criteria. After controlling for cut-off in the analysis of group differences in orthography, the difference between the groups was not significant (β = −0.21, p = .13; Hypothesis 1b).
Prevalence of dyslexia in control samples
To estimate how much the risk of dyslexia is increased in families with a history of reading difficulty, it is important also to assess the prevalence of dyslexia in control samples without a family history. Eleven studies reported information from the control group; the studies included 540 control children without family risk (mean sample size 49.09, SD = 20.5, range 23 to 93). The mean prevalence of dyslexia in the samples without family risk was 11.6% (95% CI [8.4%, 15.8%] I2 = 35.23%, Tau2 = 0.12). Compared with the 45% prevalence of dyslexia found in the family risk group, this confirms that the children at family risk have a reliably higher likelihood of developing dyslexia/reading disorders than children in the general population without such risk (there is no overlap between CIs; Hypothesis 1a).
In six studies where the criterion for diagnosing reading disorder was set above the 10th percentile, the mean prevalence was 16% (95% CI [11.5%, 21.2%], I2 = 6.97%, Tau2 = 0.02) while in four studies where the criterion was set below the 10th percentile, the mean prevalence was 7.8% (95% CI [4.9%, 12.2%], I2 = 0%, Tau2 = 0). One study did not report the cut-off. The difference between the two categories was significant, Q(1) = 4.09, p = .01, also after correcting for multiple comparisons (p = .02). As for differences between orthography, for the English studies,11.2% (95% CI [0.06%, 19.8%], I2 = 46.41%, Tau2 = 0.21); for studies from more transparent languages, 11.6% (95% CI [7.6%, 17.2%], I2 = 38.44%, Tau2 = 15). This difference was not significant, Q(1) = 0.46, p = .50. Finally, the single study reporting prevalence in a family risk study of Chinese readers estimated this to be 47% but no data were reported for controls.
The Home and Literacy Environment of Children at Family Risk of Dyslexia
A small number of family risk studies have explored the hypothesis that parents with dyslexia offer a different “diet” of books, print, and other literary experiences to parents who have not experienced reading problems.
Parental education
One factor which might influence the home literacy environment is parental educational level. Four studies comparing children at family risk (FR+) with controls report data on the education level of the mothers. In these studies, the difference in mothers’ educational levels was small and not significant, d = 0.13 (95% CI [−0.32, 0.07], Tau2 = 0.00, I2 = 0). Three studies reported data on fathers’ education, and here the difference between the FR+ and control groups was larger, d = −0.63 (95% CI [−1.43, 0.17], Tau2 = 0.45, I = 90.75%). One study (Black, Tanaka, et al., 2012) also reported differences in parental verbal language skills (as measured by WAIS verbal IQ), finding that FR+ mothers had significantly poorer verbal skills than those without such risk d = −0.73, (95% CI [−1.20, −0.17]). As for fathers, the difference was smaller and not significant, d = −0.37 (95% CI [−0.91, 0.18]).
Stronger evidence comes from three studies that compared groups according to children’s literacy outcomes. Mothers of children with a reading disorder (FR–Dyslexia) had moderately lower education levels than mothers of controls, but this difference is not significant, d = −0.32 (95% CI [−2.06, 1.42], Tau2 = 2.18, I = 93.09%). A similar pattern is present when comparing mothers of children in the FR–NR group and controls, d = −0.68 (95% CI [−1.15, 0.20], Tau2 = 0.44, I = 76.44%). Compared with controls, fathers in the FR–Dyslexia group have moderately but not significantly lower levels of education, d = −0.45 (95% CI [−1.96, 1.06], Tau2 = 1.07, I = 89.78%). Fathers in the FR–NR group also have moderately lower educational level than fathers of controls and again the difference is significant, d = −0.47 (95% CI [−0.90, −0.04], Tau2 = 0, I = 0%, k = 2).
Home literacy environment
Torppa et al. (2007) made a comprehensive assessment of the home literacy environment (HLE) of families participating in the Finnish Jyväskylä Study. Parental questionnaires completed when children were 2, 4, 5, and 6 years of age provided measures of shared reading, access to print, the child’s interest in reading, and the modeling of literate behaviours in the home. The group means across age for the HLE composites were similar in children at family risk (FR+) and controls for shared reading across age groups, d = −0.08 (95% CI [−0.23, 0.05], Tau2 = 0, I = 0%); for access to print, d = −0.05, 95% CI [−0.20, 0.11], Tau2 = 0, I = 0%; and for child’s reading interest, d = −0.14, (95% CI [−0.28, 0.01], Tau2 = 0, I = 0%). However, the scores for shared reading when the children were 2 years old and for parental modeling of literate behaviors were more variable in the FR group (Hypothesis 2a). Moreover, there was a significant difference between the FR+ and control groups in the frequency of parental modeling activities in reading books, newspapers, magazines, and so forth, with parents from at-risk families typically engaging in such activities less frequently than parents without a history of dyslexia: for fathers, d = −0.44, (95% CI [−0.73, −0.16]) and for mothers, d = −0.66 (95% CI [−0.95, −0.38]; Hypothesis 2b).
Scarborough (1991b) also reported differences in literacy experiences of children from families at risk of dyslexia but data are not reported to allow calculation of an effect size. A graph presented in the article shows that during the preschool years, children who went on to be dyslexic (FR–Dyslexia) were read to less often by their fathers than children from the family risk group who went on to be normal readers and controls. For mothers, there was less joint reading for children at family risk at 30 months than for controls but not thereafter (Hypothesis 2a). However, mothers also rated how often they observed their children reading to themselves. There were fewer occurrences of solitary book reading by the FR–Dyslexia group at all ages (two to three times per week compared with five to seven times for controls) suggesting that group differences may be child-driven and less frequent exposure to books could be self-imposed by affected children. A follow-up in adolescence of children at family risk of dyslexia by Snowling, Muter, and Carroll (2007) found that there was a trend for those in the FR–Dyslexia group to report themselves as reading less than those in the FR–NR group, but this was not significant (p = .08). There were no group differences in how often the families bought books and gave books for presents (p = .25) but the dyslexia group had less knowledge of book titles and authors (but not magazines) as assessed by a print exposure questionnaire.
Exploring whether parental reading skills predict reading outcomes in children at family risk of dyslexia, (Torppa, Eklund, et al., 2011; N family risk dyads = 31, control dyads = 68) showed that beyond the influence of children’s language and decoding-related skills, parental literacy skills predicted early reading and spelling outcomes (Hypothesis 2c). However, parental skills did not account for variance in reading fluency in third grade once children’s skills at earlier points in time were taken into account.2 A similar conclusion was reached by Aro, Poikkeus et al. (2009) using data from the same study.
Endophenotypes of Dyslexia
Having considered what is known about environmental variables, we turn to consider how being at family risk of dyslexia affects the development of perceptual and cognitive skills and ultimately literacy outcomes.
Characteristics of infants and toddlers at family risk of dyslexia
The first developmental stage investigated has been from birth to age 3, namely infants and toddlers. We will review data pertaining to each of the main constructs in turn to evaluate Hypotheses 3a and 3b.
Table 3 shows results from studies of infants and toddlers. First, note that the evidence is scarce. As can be seen in Table 3, there was a significant difference between family risk children who went on to develop dyslexia and control children in general cognitive abilities. Note that the test of cognitive abilities also included language-related tasks. Furthermore, the family risk children showed poorer articulatory skills than peers with no family risk, and also reliably poorer vocabulary knowledge and grammar. This was not the case for the family risk children who did not develop dyslexia (Hypothesis 3b).
Table 3. Group Comparisons in Studies of Infants and Toddlers at Family Risk of Dyslexia.
Construct | Comparison type | Mean effect size (d) [95% CI] | Number of studies [N family-risk (N control)] | I2 | Tau2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. CI = confidence interval. | |||||
* p < 0.05. | |||||
General cognitive abilities | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.15 [−.61, .31] | 1 [49, (28)] | — | — |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.60 [−1.20, .00]* | 1 [19, (22)] | — | — | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.05 [−.61, .50] | 1 [24, (22)] | — | — | |
Motor skills | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | .10 [−.14, .34] | 2 [137, (124)] | 0 | 0 |
Articulatory accuracy | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.93 [−1.57, −.28]* | 1 [19, (20)] | — | — |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.01 [−1.66,−.37]* | 1 [20, (20)] | — | — | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.08 [−.68, .62] | 1 [19, (20)] | — | — | |
Vocabulary knowledge | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.32 [−.53, −.12]* | 4 [315, (336)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.55 [−1.27, −.24]* | 2 [29, (29)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.55 [−1.24, .14] | 2 [21, (29)] | 0 | 0 | |
Grammar | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −1.0 [−1.52, −.47]* | 1 [35, (27)] | — | — |
A number of studies not amenable to meta-analysis have reported qualitative differences in language development between FR+ children and controls. An early study by Locke et al., (1997) suggested differences in babbling between children at family risk and controls. Also in infancy, Wilsenach and Wijnen (2004) demonstrated a preference for grammatical passages (as opposed to nongrammatical) containing forms of morphosyntactic agreement in Dutch among not-at-risk compared to at-risk infants, and van Alphen, de Bree et al. (2004; N family risk 35, controls 27) found that FR+ children paid reliably less attention to stress patterns in words than children not at-risk. Koster et al., (2005; N family risk = 111, 87 controls) focused on the growth of vocabulary. For common nouns and predicates, FR+ and typically developing (TD) groups followed the same course but there was divergence for the production of verbs and closed class words when the FR+ group appeared to plateau at a vocabulary size between 50 and 100 words. These findings chime with those of Scarborough (1990, 1991a) who reported group differences in lexical diversity, accuracy of phonological production, utterance length, and grammatical complexity in natural language samples. Finally, Lyytinen, Poikkeus, Laasko, Eklund, and Lyytinen (2001; N = 107 FR+ and 93 controls) documented the emergence of language skills in children at family risk of dyslexia and controls. Group differences did not emerge until 24 months when the family risk group used shorter sentences. Whereas late talkers in the control group tended to catch up, those in the family risk group remained delayed up until 3.5 years.
Finally, six small-scale neurophysiological studies (Table S1 in the online supplement file) reported differences in brain response (primarily ERP) to speech stimuli and two studies reported reduced sensitivity to higher word-level semantic information in infants at family risk of dyslexia.
Characteristics of preschool children at family risk of dyslexia
The studies reviewed in this section concern preschool children, aged from 3–5.5 years (dependent on school entry in the country of sample origin). Table 4, Panel A, B, and C shows the results from these studies. The focus is on the known precursors of word decoding as well as more general developmental progress (Hypotheses 3a and b).
Table 4. Group Comparisons in Studies of Preschool Children.
Construct | Comparison type | Mean effect size (d) [95% CI] | Number of studies [N family-risk (N control)] | I2 | Tau2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. CI = confidence interval. | |||||
* p < 0.05. | |||||
Panel A: General cognitive abilities and perceptual skills | |||||
Nonverbal IQ | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.35 [−.55, −.14]* | 7 [315, (336)] | 46.40 | .04 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.21 [−.48, .06] | 6 [201, (276)] | 50.59 | .06 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.06 [−.30, .18] | 6 [227, (242)] | 33 | .03 | |
Auditory processing | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.31 [−.59, −.03]* | 2 [100, (100)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.80 [−1.57, −.04]* | 1 [9, (22)] | — | — | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.21 [−.77, .35] | 1 [26, (22)] | — | — | |
Visual processing | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.11 [−.29, .07] | 2 [217, (2148)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.34 [−.82, .13] | 1 [26, (47)] | — | — | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.08 [−.59, .43] | 1 [21, (47)] | — | — | |
Motor skills | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | .25 [−.03, .53] | 1 [101, (89)] | — | — |
Panel B: Oral language skills | |||||
Articulatory accuracy | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.79 [−.79, −.01]* | 3 [164, (133)] | 61.43 | .07 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.41 [−.94, .11] | 4 [125, (187)] | 46.69 | .22 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.31 [−.69, .08] | 4 [87, (187)] | 33.84 | .05 | |
Vocabulary knowledge | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.65 [−.91, −.38]* | 6 [436, (374)] | 54.61 | .06 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.83 [−1.42, −.24]* | 6 [259, (235)] | 88.38 | .47 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.34 [−.54, −.15]* | 5 [236, (192)] | 0 | 0 | |
Grammar | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.26 [−.66, .15] | 6 [340, (276)] | 81.35 | .19 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.72 [−1.05, −.40]* | 2 [55, (121)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.28 [−.54, −.02]* | 3 [101, (134)] | 0 | 0 | |
Phonological memory | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.57 [−.79, −.35]* | 4 [174, (151)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.11 [−1.39, −.84]* | 5 [98, (180)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.47 [−.72, −.22]* | 5 [101, (180)] | 0 | 0 | |
Verbal short-term memory | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.45 [−.89, −.04]* | 4 [216, (190)] | 74.07 | .13 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.65 [−1.18, −.12]* | 6 [168, (182)] | 80.93 | .35 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.15 [−.40, .11] | 5 [143, (135)] | 10.69 | .01 | |
Panel C: Decoding specific skills | |||||
Letter knowledge | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.47 [−.60, −.34]* | 7 [447, (478)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at -risk | −.94 [−1.42, −.45]* | 7 [206, (337)] | 83.74 | .35 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.32 [−.50, −.13]* | 7 [226, (290)] | 4.41 | .002 | |
Phoneme awareness | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.56 [−.70, −.43]* | 6 [482, (453)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.83 [−1.17, −.48]* | 11 [218, (376)] | 73.74 | .23 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.32 [−.53, −.10]* | 10 [262, (350)] | 39.28 | .05 | |
Rhyme awareness | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.90 [−1.36, −.44]* | 2 [106, (94)] | 54.85 | .06 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.00 [−1.70, −.23]* | 1 [37, (25)] | — | — | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.55 [−1.12, .02] | 1 [19, (25)] | — | — | |
Rapid naming | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.61 [−.80, −.41]* | 6 [381, (385)] | 36.08 | .02 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.03 [−1.37, −.69]* | 5 [119, (206)] | 29.4 | .04 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.47 [−.71, −.22]* | 5 [185, (206)] | 0 | 0 |
General cognitive abilities and perceptual skills
Table 4 Panel A shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies, effect sizes, and persons) and heterogeneity measures for measures of general cognitive abilities, perceptual processes, and motor skills. For nonverbal IQ, it is apparent that groups differ more in the studies comparing FR+ children and controls, than in the studies in which the family risk group with and without dyslexia are separately compared with controls.
There are few studies of auditory or visual processing in this age group. For auditory processing, there is a significant difference in studies that compare FR+ children and controls, and in one longitudinal study, children at family risk who go on to be dyslexic (FR–Dyslexia) demonstrate poorer auditory processing skills than controls but the FR–NR group do not differ significantly (Hypothesis 3a). In addition one neurophysiological study reports reduced sensitivity to rapid auditory processing. Likewise studies of speech perception are inconclusive and data are not amenable to meta-analysis: studies from the Utrecht dyslexia project reported differences between children at family risk of dyslexia and controls in categorical speech perception for consonants (Gerrits & de Bree, 2009), but not for a vowel continuum (van Alphen, et al., 2004). From the same project, de Bree, van Alphen, Fikkert, and Wijnen, (2008) assessed comprehension of metrical stress using pictures of objects with stress patterns which were either strong–weak (′zebra) or weak–strong (ka′do), accompanied by presentations of the object name pronounced with correct or incorrect stress. Overall, FR+ children looked relatively less to the targets when they heard a weak—strong pattern than controls. In addition one neurophysiological study reports reduced sensitivity to phoneme deviance. For neither visual processing nor motor skills are there reliable group differences, but the evidence here is extremely limited with very few studies.
Oral language skills
Table 4 Panel B shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies and persons) and heterogeneity measures for speech and language skills including articulatory accuracy, vocabulary knowledge, grammar, phonological memory, and verbal short-term memory (STM; Hypothesis 3b). For articulatory accuracy, there are few studies of preschool children so the evidence is limited. Across three concurrent studies, FR+ children show reliably poorer articulatory accuracy than controls. However, group differences are not significant when the family risk group is classified according to reading outcome. One of the longitudinal samples overlaps with one of the concurrent studies examining FR+ children. Further, two studies found that, although 3-year-old children at family risk of dyslexia did less well on speech production tasks than controls, their errors obeyed Dutch stress rules (de Bree, Wijnen, & Zonneveld, 2006; Gerrits & de Bree, 2009).
For lexical/vocabulary knowledge, children at family risk have reliably poorer skills than controls. The results show that the FR–Dyslexia group shows a large deficit in vocabulary knowledge in preschool. The FR–NR group also shows a significant deficit, but in size this is only one-quarter of the size compared with the effect size for those who do develop a reading problem. Here, one sample included as a concurrent comparison is also included in comparisons comparing FR–Dyslexia and FR–NR groups with controls. For grammar, children at family risk have reliably poorer grammatical skills than their peers, irrespective of whether they actually go on to develop a reading problem. However, those who develop reading problems have more severe difficulties (Hypothesis 3f).
Turning to measures of verbal/phonological processing, children at family risk have significantly poorer phonological memory than controls for all comparison types (there is one overlapping sample between the different comparisons). FR+ children also have reliably poorer verbal STM than controls; those who go on to have reading problems (FR–Dyslexia) have particularly severe difficulties whereas the FR–NR group does not differ reliably from controls. In this case, there are three overlapping samples between the comparison types and the differences between the family risk and control groups increase when publication bias is taken into account.
Decoding-related skills
Table 4 Panel C shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies and persons) and heterogeneity measures for decoding-related predictors, that is, letter knowledge, phoneme awareness, rhyme awareness and rapid naming (Hypothesis 3c). First, for letter knowledge, it is clear that FR+ children and family risk children who go on to develop a reading problem are slow to acquire letter knowledge. The FR–NR group is also reliably slower to acquire letter knowledge than the controls, but the effect size is only one third compared with the FR+ children. It is noteworthy that effect sizes decline for letter knowledge when publication bias is taken into account; also three samples overlap between the group comparisons.
For phoneme awareness, there is a marked deficit in preschool children at family risk, but this is larger for those who go on to develop a reading problem (FR–Dyslexia). The group difference between FR–NR and control groups is barely significant, and drops to nonsignificant when adjusted for publication bias. Three comparisons involve overlapping samples.
For rhyme awareness the evidence is very limited, but there are reliable differences both between FR+ and control groups, and between FR–Dyslexia and control groups. For rapid naming, all three comparisons suggest reliably poorer skills in family risk children regardless of outcome and controls. However, performance on rapid naming tasks is much poorer in the FR–Dyslexia than the FR–NR groups, as shown by the confidence intervals which show little overlap. Furthermore, the differences between these two comparisons increases when publication bias is taken into account. There are two overlapping samples between the comparisons.
For phoneme awareness it is possible to do a moderator analysis to examine the effect of orthography in studies comparing children at family risk with controls. The results for English studies was d = −0.42, 95% CI [−0.64, −0.20], p = .0001, Q(2) = 1.39, p > .01, Tau2 = 0, I = 0%, k = 3 and for studies from more transparent languages (Danish, Finnish, and Italian) d = −0.60, 95% CI [−0.76, −0.43], p = .0001, Q(3) = 0.52, p > .01, Tau2 = 0, I = 0%, k = 4. This difference was not significant, Q(1) = 1.59, p = .21.
Characteristics of children in early primary school at family risk of dyslexia
The studies reviewed in this section concern primary schoolchildren from school entry to 4th grade (age dependent on school entry in the country of sample origin). Table 5 Panel A, B, C and D shows the results from these studies.
Table 5. Group Comparisons in Studies of Primary School Children.
Construct | Comparison type | Mean effect size (d) [95% CI] | Number of studies [N family-risk (N control)] | I2 | Tau2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. CI = confidence interval. | |||||
* p < 0.05. | |||||
Panel A: General cognitive abilities and perceptual skills | |||||
Nonverbal IQ | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.27 [−.50, −.04]* | 5 [315, (336)] | 35.05 | .02 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.19 [−.49, .11] | 5 [192, (248)] | 55.09 | .07 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.02 [−.29, .25] | 5 [190, (201)] | 38.52 | .04 | |
Auditory processing | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.48 [−.95, −.02]* | 1 [36, (36)] | — | — |
Visual processing | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.42 [−.75, −.09]* | 3 [151, (186)] | 43.61 | .04 |
Motor skills | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | .28 [−.01, .57] | 1 [98, (85)] | — | — |
Panel B: Oral language skills | |||||
Articulatory accuracy | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.21 [−1.07, .63] | 2 [63, (145)] | 80.48 | .31 |
Vocabulary knowledge | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.59 [−1.21, .02] | 5 [155, (268)] | 86.28 | .41 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.46 [−.80, −.11]* | 3 [74, (63)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.15 [−.46, .18] | 3 [105, (63)] | 0 | 0 | |
Grammar | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.31 [−.87, .25] | 2 [81, (164)] | 74.12 | .12 |
Phonological memory | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −1.02 [−1.42, −.61]* | 3 [51, (53)] | 0 | 0 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.83 [−2.07, .41] | 2 [70, (102)] | 92.18 | .74 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.22 [−.65, .22] | 2 [86, (102)] | 44.53 | .04 | |
Verbal short-term memory | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.82 [−1.20, −.45]* | 4 [100, (88)] | 34.66 | .05 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.29 [−1.84, −.73]* | 1 [37, (25)] | — | — | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.27 [−.86, .32] | 1 [19, (25)] | 10.69 | .01 | |
Panel C: Decoding specific skills | |||||
Letter knowledge | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.41 [−1.20, .41] | 3 [47, (93)] | 76.15 | .62 |
Phoneme awareness | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.69 [−1.09, −.29]* | 7 [482, (453)] | 70.12 | .19 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.63 [−1.93, −1.33]* | 3 [115, (116)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.56 [−.87, −.27]* | 3 [158, (116)] | 25.08 | .02 | |
Rhyme awareness | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −1.94 [−2.04, −.95]* | 1 [37, (29)] | — | — |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.13 [−1.48, −.79]* | 2 [63, (34)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.23 [−.75, .28] | 2 [19, (25)] | 0 | 0 | |
Rapid naming | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.73 [−1.18, −.28]* | 5 [119, (162)] | 66.12 | .17 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.78 [−1.03, −.53]* | 3 [111, (168)] | 0 | 0 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.07 [−.27, .13] | 3 [206, (168)] | 0 | 0 | |
Panel D: Literacy skills | |||||
Word recognition | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.88 [−1.20, −.66]* | 9 [440, (378)] | 69.70 | .15 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −2.44 [−2.80, −2.08]* | 9 (15) [246, (354)] | 49.70 | .14 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.29 [−.47, −.10]* | 9 (15) [330, (352)] | 0 | 0 | |
Nonword decoding | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −1.12 [−1.64, −.60]* | 4 [153, (158)] | 76.42 | .21 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.83 [−2.23, −1.44]* | 6 (10) [163, (264)] | 49.59 | .11 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.56 [−1.00, −.12]* | 6 (10) [257, (264)] | 73.99 | .21 | |
Spelling accuracy | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −1.06 [−1.88, −.25]* | 3 [37, (29)] | 90.83 | .45 |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −2.10 [−2.40, −1.81]* | 6 (10) [190, (280)] | 9.93 | .01 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.42 [−.63, −.21]* | 6 (10) [288, (280)] | 0 | 0 | |
Reading comprehension | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.77 [−1.14, −.41]* | 1 [67, (57)] | — | — |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.89 [−2.92, −.86]* | 3 (4)[64, (110)] | 84.50 | .91 | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.40 [−.91, .12] | 3 (4) [50, (110)] | 54.40 | .15 |
General cognitive abilities and perceptual skills
Table 5 Panel A shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies and persons) and heterogeneity measures for skills related to general cognitive, perceptual and motor skills (Hypothesis 3a). For nonverbal IQ, there is a significant difference in nonverbal IQ between FR+ and control groups, with no overlap between the samples in the three comparison types. For auditory processing, visual processing and motor skills, the evidence is very limited. There are, however, reliable differences in auditory and visual processing, but not in motor skills between family risk and control groups. In addition, two neurophysiological studies report deviant auditory processing in children at family risk of dyslexia.
Oral language skills
Table 5 Panel B shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies and persons) and heterogeneity measures for oral language skills. For articulatory accuracy, vocabulary knowledge, and grammar, the evidence suggests that the difficulties observed early in development amongst children at family risk of dyslexia in these areas are mostly resolved in early primary school. An exception seems to be vocabulary knowledge, where the FR–Dyslexia group still have reliably poorer skills than their peers (Hypothesis 3a). For phonological and verbal STM, results indicate that the problems here are more severe and persistent and poor verbal STM is observed in the FR+ as well as the FR–Dyslexia groups (Hypothesis 3a). While the number of studies here are limited, there is no overlap between the samples in the different comparison types.
Decoding-related skills
Table 5 Panel C shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies and persons) and heterogeneity measures for decoding-related predictors. For letter knowledge, the difficulties experienced in preschool seem to have resolved by early primary school. However, problems with phoneme awareness are now more severe both in FR+ children and in the FR–Dyslexia group (Hypothesis 3d). For the FR–NR group, the problems are less severe, but the group difference is still as much as half a SD unit (Hypothesis 3f). This is not the case for rhyme awareness: here both FR+ children and the FR–Dyslexia group show poor performance but the FR–NR group do not differ from controls. Similarly, for rapid naming, the FR–Dyslexia group has persistent and severe difficulties, while those who do not develop a reading problem have no difficulties (FR–NR; Hypothesis 3d). For phoneme awareness one study is based on the same sample for all three comparison types, for rhyme awareness all comparison types are based on the same sample and for the other measures there is no overlap.
For phoneme awareness in studies that compared family risk children with controls it was possible to analyze orthography as a moderator. For English studies, d = −0.96 (95% CI [−1.64, −0.28], p = .03, Tau2 = 0.26, I = 72.82%) and for studies from more transparent languages, d = −0.72 (95% CI [−1.33, −0.10], Tau2 = 0.27, I = 71.72%). This difference was not significant, Q(1) = 0.27, p = .60.
Literacy skills
Table 5 Panel D shows mean effect size with 95% CIs, sample size (studies and persons) and heterogeneity measures for literacy outcomes namely word reading, nonword reading, spelling, and reading comprehension. For literacy outcomes, the FR–Dyslexia group has severe difficulties; their skills are around 2 SD units poorer than controls on all measures, implying a cut-off of around the 2nd percentile. Thus, even though the analysis of prevalence showed that the cut-off limits for reading disorder varied considerably, on average at a sample level, the reading disorders are severe. In addition, the FR–NR group have moderate difficulties in all literacy domains compared with the controls but with performance reliably better than that of the FR–Dyslexia group (Hypothesis 3e) except for reading comprehension where there was no overlap between the confidence intervals. For word decoding there are three overlapping samples between the comparisons, for nonword decoding one overlapping and for spelling three overlapping samples.
For orthography, a moderator analysis for studies examining word reading was possible for all three comparison types. For English studies comparing FR+ and control groups, d = −0.56 (95% CI [−0.98, −0.13], Tau2 = 0, I = 0%, k = 2) and for studies from more transparent languages d = −1.08 (95% CI [−1.65, −0.40], Tau2 = 0.33, I = 80.53%, k = 5). This difference was not significant, Q(1) = 2.04, p = .15. For the comparison of FR–Dyslexia and control groups in English studies, d = −2.68 (95% CI [−3.50, −1.86], Tau2 = 0.47, I = 73.33%, k = 4) and for studies from more transparent languages, d = −2.39 (95% CI [−2.84, −1.94], Tau2 = 0.0, I = 0%, k = 3). This difference was not significant, Q(1) = 0.37, p = .54. For studies comparing the FR–NR group with controls: for English, d = −0.41 (95% CI [−0.70, −13], Tau2 = 0, I = 0%, k = 4), and for studies from more transparent languages d = −0.28 (95% CI [−1.85, 0.07], Tau2 = 0.0, I = 0% k = 3). This difference was not significant, Q(1) = 0.36, p = .55.
Longer-term outcomes: Group comparisons in late primary and secondary school
The evidence for long-term outcomes of children at family risk is limited, and consists of two studies, Snowling, Muter, and Carroll (2007) and Elbro and Petersen (2004; reading comprehension). The results reported in these studies are summarized in Table 6. Table 6 suggests that children at family risk who develop dyslexia show persistent difficulties in a range of language- and literacy-related areas. Those family risk children who do not develop a reading disorder also experience some difficulties but these appear to be restricted to word-level reading and spelling (Hypothesis 3f).
Table 6. Group Comparisons in Studies of Secondary School Children at Family Risk of Dyslexia.
Construct | Comparison type | Mean effect size (d) [95% CI] | Number of studies [N family-risk (N control)] |
---|---|---|---|
Note. CI = confidence interval. | |||
* p < 0.05. | |||
Vocabulary knowledge | Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.75 [−2.49, −1.01]* | 1 [21, (17)] |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.43 [−1.02, .17] | 1 [29, (17)] | |
Phonological memory | Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.16 [−1.83, −.48]* | 1 [21, (17)] |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.21 [−.80, .38] | 1 [29, (17)] | |
Phoneme awareness | Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.36 [−2.06, −.67]* | 1 [21, (17)] |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.26 [−.85, .32] | 1 [29, (17)] | |
Word recognition | Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.94 [−2.71, −1.18]* | 1 [21, (17)] |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.80 [−1.41, −.19]* | 1 [29, (17)] | |
Spelling accuracy | Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −2.75 [−3.63, −1.87]* | 1 [21, (17)] |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.89 [−1.51, −.28]* | 1 [29, (17)] | |
Reading comprehension | Family-risk children vs. controls not at-risk | −.56 [−.99, −.14]* | 1 [47, (41)] |
Family-risk children with dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −1.00 [−1.67, −.30]* | 1 [64, (110)] | |
Family-risk children without dyslexia vs. controls not at-risk | −.10 [−.69, .48] | 1 [50, (110)] |
Longitudinal Prediction Studies
Thus far the review has highlighted some of the precursors of dyslexia. However, since many of these are correlated with each other, their causal status is unclear. Stronger evidence for causal risk factors comes from longitudinal studies that assess the factors that make a unique contribution to literacy outcomes or to dyslexia status as a categorical variable. It was not possible, given the diversity of the published longitudinal prediction studies and the fact that many report data on the same sample, to code a correlation matrix based on mean correlations from the studies and use this in a metaregression. Therefore, we present the findings study by study. We focus here on studies predicting literacy outcomes. We also detected four longitudinal studies (all based on the Jyväskylä sample) predicting vocabulary knowledge (Laakso, Poikkeus, & Lyytinen, 1999; Lyytinen, Ahonen, et al., 2001; Lyytinen et al., 2001; Natale, Aunola, et al., 2008). Because literacy was not an outcome in these studies, they were considered to be beyond the scope of this review.
The first longitudinal study to report analyses of the predictors of literacy outcome for at-risk and control samples separately was by Pennington and Lefly (2001; N family risk = 67, control = 57 studied from kindergarten to summer after second grade). Using stepwise multiple regression analysis they reported that for controls, the pattern of prediction was stable across age, with phonological awareness being the main predictor, accounting for between 18 and 39% of the variance in the literacy variables. In contrast, for the family risk group, the predictive pattern changed over time, with letter knowledge as the most important predictor at the first two time points and phonological awareness at Time 3 (Hypothesis 4b).
Using the same sample, Cardoso-Martins and Pennington (2004; same N as in the previous paragraph) assessed the contribution of phoneme awareness and rapid naming to literacy outcomes. For controls, none of the rapid naming measures contributed significantly to reading or spelling when the differences in children’s IQ and phoneme awareness were taken into account. However, for children at family risk, rapid naming skills were uniquely related to literacy skills (Hypothesis 4b). Together the findings suggest that the acquisition of phonological awareness is delayed in children at family risk of dyslexia and learning to read may be more dependent, at least initially, on letter knowledge.
The largest family risk study to date is the Jyväskylä study. Eight publications predicting literacy skills are based on this sample. Among these, a series of three have examined the relative influence of oral language and decoding-related skills on literacy outcomes (Hypothesis 4a and b). Torppa, Poikkeus et al. (2007; N family risk = 96, controls = 90) examined the development of phonological awareness and early reading, also taking account of the home literacy environment (age 6.5). Despite the FR+ group having poorer language skills, the predictive pattern was very similar for the two groups though, in the FR+ group, the home literacy variables and children’s reading interest had stronger associations with each other and with skill development. A latent variable model using growth curves showed that both for FR+ children and controls, vocabulary and letter knowledge predicted phonological awareness and vice versa, phonological awareness also predicted vocabulary and letter knowledge. The effects of home literacy environment, principally shared reading, were mediated by vocabulary. Taking this further, Torppa, Lyytinen et al. (2010; N FR–Dyslexia = 46, FR–NR = 68, control = 84) used a path model with observed variables to examine the longitudinal predictors of reading. There were strong predictive links to reading (particularly to reading accuracy) from receptive and expressive language (Hypothesis 4a), via letter knowledge, rapid naming, phonological awareness, and morphology (Hypothesis 4b; see also Torppa, Eklund, et al., 2011 above).
In addition one study has examined neurophysiological measures of speech perception as predictors of literacy skills (Pennala, Eklund, et al., 2010; N FR–Dyslexia = 35, FR–NR = 69, control = 80). Beyond STM, phonological memory and rapid naming, phonemic length discrimination ability in Grade 1 explained both spelling and reading skills as late as third grade (R2 reading accuracy = 15%, spelling 12%).
Another important contribution of the Jyväskylä group has been to describe trajectories of reading development and predictors of literacy development at the individual level. Torppa, Poikkeus et al. (2006; N family risk = 96, controls = 90) examined the development of letter knowledge using longitudinal trajectory analysis. Three separate clusters were identified to describe development: delayed, linearly growing, and precocious. The delayed group was dominated by children at family risk of dyslexia. Phonological sensitivity, phonological memory, and rapid naming significantly predicted letter knowledge, and there was a strong relationship between the letter knowledge cluster to which a child belonged and their early reading skills.
Developmental trajectories from birth to school age were also examined by Lyytinen, Erskine et al. (2006; N family risk = 106, controls 93). In this study, mixture modeling revealed four subgroups with different trajectories of development. The largest subgroup, labelled “typical trajectory,” contained mostly children from the control group. A group labelled “unexpected trajectory” contained an equal number at-risk and not-at-risk. Two smaller subgroups showed either a “dysfluent trajectory” or a “declining trajectory.” The troubled developmental pathways were characterized by problems in phonological awareness, rapid naming or letter knowledge. Examining individual risk of dyslexia, Puolakanaho, Ahonen et al. (2007; N FR–Dyslexia = 46, without reading disabilities = 152) used receiver operating curve plots and logistic regression models to investigate the key predictors of outcome from the ages of 3.5–5.5 years. Results showed that the models with family risk, letter knowledge, phonological awareness, and rapid naming provided prediction reliability as high as 0.80 (Hypothesis 4b).
One further publication (Aro, Poikkeus, et al., 2009, N family risk = 101, controls = 89) went beyond children’s cognitive skills to examine the effects of risk accumulation across different domains on cognitive, behavioral, and academic outcomes. For reading fluency, a hierarchical regression analysis showed that family risk of dyslexia (vs. no risk) added significant explanatory value (8%, p = .000), and that neurocognitive risks (measured by a composite of earlier measured language skills, phonological awareness, rapid naming, visuomotor skills, motor skills, and memory) added a large amount of explanatory value (20.7%, p = .000) while parental literacy levels did not add significant explanatory value (1.7%, ns [p value not reported]).
Finally, one publication reports longitudinal predictive analyses for Chinese children at family risk of dyslexia (Wong, McBride-Chang, et al., 2012; N at-risk dyslexia = 57, at-risk without reading disorders = 57, no control data for sample not at-risk reported). Using three logistic equation models, the best-fitting models for predicting individual risk of dyslexia included all print-related variables (rapid naming, character recognition, and letter identification), and gender but not family risk. It should be noted, however, that the at-risk sample consisted of children identified as “at risk” because of poor performance on language tests, and not according to family risk only.
Intervention Studies
The findings from the longitudinal studies are, arguably, most useful for identifying the precursors of dyslexia; however, the only way to establish more conclusively whether a causal relationship exists is through the use of experimental training studies (Bradley & Bryant, 1983). Six intervention studies investigating children at family risk of dyslexia are summarized in Appendix C. Four have used interventions to promote the foundations of decoding (letter knowledge and phonological awareness training; Hypothesis 5a), one used dialogical reading targeting broader language skills (encouraging the child to be the “reader” of a shared book and talk about it), and one used a combination of training of phonological awareness and broader language skills (Hypothesis 5b). There is only one randomized study, the others are quasi-experiments with a control group and baseline measures.
Table 7 show results from intervention studies. The results show effects on letter knowledge, phonological awareness, and word decoding when compared with at risk controls, but no effects when compared with controls not at risk (Hypothesis 5a). If we look at the studies that have included a follow-up measure in first or second grade, the results are disappointing: for word reading, d = −0.22 (95% CI [−0.50, 0.06]; compared with controls at-risk), and for one study of nonword reading, d = −0.10 (95% CI [−0.44, 0.42]; also compared with controls at-risk). In summary, the findings from intervention studies hold promise for promoting phoneme awareness, letter knowledge and decoding at posttest immediately after the intervention, but are not very encouraging in terms of transfer to follow-up tests of reading or language skills. Together they suggest that gains may be short-lived and it is difficult once poor reading has set in, for at-risk readers to catch up with their peers.
Table 7. Intervention Studies.
Construct | Comparison | Mean effect size (d) [95% CI] | Number of studies [N family-risk (N control)] | I2 | Tau2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. CI = confidence interval. | |||||
* p < 0.05. | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge | At risk controls | 0 [−.28, .28] | 2 [93, (100)] | 0 | 0 |
Letter knowledge | At risk controls | .53 [.19, .87]* | 5 [232, (194)] | 62.41% | .09 |
Not at risk controls | .29 [−.31, .89] | 3 [135, (122)] | 80.70% | .22 | |
Phonological awareness | At risk controls | .55 [.26, .85]* | 5 [228, (190)] | 57.08 | .08 |
Not at risk controls | .01 [−.26, .28] | 3 [135, (122)] | 14.45 | .01 | |
Rapid naming | At risk controls | .09 [−.44, .24] | 3 [96, (100)] | 30.84% | .03 |
Not at risk controls | .03 [−.37, .33] | 2 [66, (67)] | 0 | 0 | |
Word recognition | At risk controls | .52 [.55, 1.57]* | 2 [93, (100)] | 90.01 | .54 |
Discussion
Studies of children at family risk of dyslexia have been conducted in several different alphabetic languages and Chinese. The findings are important because they derive from prospective longitudinal studies that have recruited children before they have failed to learn to read and, therefore, are comparatively free of clinical bias. This review presents the first comprehensive analysis of data from such studies and incorporates a meta-analysis. It provides robust evidence concerning the precursors of dyslexia in preschool and the familial risk factors associated with poor literacy attainments. It also elucidates potentially heritable endophenotypes of dyslexia and protective factors that enable some family members to “compensate” to circumvent poor reading, while others succumb to literacy problems.
The findings of our review are novel and surprisingly consistent across languages: there is a heightened risk of dyslexia in families in which a first-degree relative is affected, such that children at high risk are four times more likely to succumb to reading problems than peers from families with no such risk. A universal finding is that children at family risk of dyslexia acquire language more slowly than their peers, and signs of dyslexia are evident from preschool onward. Further it is evident that children at family risk of dyslexia are slow to develop phonological awareness and to learn letters (and symbols) such that, by early primary school, most children at family risk of dyslexia, and more specifically those who develop a reading problem, have difficulties in acquiring the skills that underpin these abilities, such as phoneme awareness.
Summary of the Evidence
In the beginning of the article we laid out five sets of hypotheses. We now turn now to assess the evidence in support of each of the predictions with reference to these in Table 1.
1. The prevalence of dyslexia in children at family risk
Using data from 15 studies we found evidence in support of Hypotheses 1a and c. Thus, the mean prevalence of dyslexia in children at family risk is 45%, with estimates ranging from 29%, for a Dutch study to 66% for an English study. Consistent with Hypothesis 1a, these estimates are much higher than those reported for control samples. Contrary to Hypothesis 1b, the effect of orthography was only marginal and the cut-off criterion for dyslexia was the only robust factor determining differences between studies. Indeed, consistent with Hypothesis 1c, the prevalence was lower for studies that used stricter (more conservative) criteria for classifying poor readers. Notwithstanding this, it was noteworthy that the average group deficit leading to a diagnosis of dyslexia was large across studies, suggesting the bottom 2% of readers were classified as dyslexic.
2. Differences in the home and literacy environment of children at family risk of dyslexia and controls
Data on the home literacy environment of children at family risk of dyslexia are scarce. While it should be noted that many studies have deliberately recruited families to be matched for socioeconomic circumstances, we found evidence that provided some support for Hypotheses 2a, b, and c. Consistent with Hypothesis 2a, children at family risk appear to be read to less often than children in families in which neither parent is dyslexic (but it is important to bear in mind that this difference could be child-driven). Also as predicted by Hypothesis 2b, there was a trend for parents with dyslexia, perhaps unsurprisingly, to have lower educational levels and to read to themselves less frequently than parents of controls but in most cases differences fail to reach significance and are in need of replication. Such findings might explain why, consistent with Hypothesis 2c, parental reading status (itself possibly a proxy for gene—environment interaction) accounts for variance in the prediction of reading over and above what is predicted by cognitive variables
3. Endophenotypes of dyslexia
Here we found evidence in support of all of our hypotheses. Consistent with Hypothesis 3a, there are very early developmental differences between family risk children who go on to be dyslexic and controls in general cognitive development. The limited evidence from preschool children suggests those at family risk of dyslexia have poor performance on auditory but not on visual processing tasks (though evidence for a direct causal influence of such skills on literacy outcomes is sparse).
As predicted by Hypothesis 3b, from infancy through toddlerhood, children at family risk of dyslexia who go on to develop dyslexia have reliably slower speech and language development as indicated by poorer performance than controls in tests of articulation, lexical/ vocabulary knowledge, and grammar. Children at family risk who do not go on to develop dyslexia show poorer performance on all measures than controls, but the size of the effects do not reach significance. In preschool, children at family risk of dyslexia are slow to develop language skills, with nonword repetition (phonological memory) skills being particularly poor. By school age however, apart from tasks related to phonological and verbal STM, many of the oral language problems evident in preschool seem to be resolved or are much milder by early primary school though children classified as dyslexic have significantly poorer vocabulary than controls (Hypothesis 3b).
During the preschool years, it is clear that children at family risk of dyslexia face challenges in acquiring letter knowledge, phoneme awareness and rapid naming skills, consistent with Hypothesis 3c. For rapid naming, nonword repetition and rhyme awareness family risk children who later develop a reading problem do particularly poorly but family risk children who go on to be normal readers also experience difficulties relative to controls, as they do in vocabulary, grammar, and letter knowledge (consistent with Hypothesis 3f). The two studies reporting findings for Chinese found deficits in knowledge of Chinese characters, phonological awareness of tones (paralleling letter knowledge and phoneme awareness in alphabetic languages), and in RAN.
In early primary school, family risk children in general (FR+), and more specifically those who develop a reading problem (FR–Dyslexia) have difficulties with decoding-related skills (Hypothesis 3d). Rhyme awareness and rapid naming show a different pattern than for the other predictors: children who do not develop a reading problem perform in the normal range in RAN tasks and in rhyme awareness. Consistent with Hypothesis 3e, family risk children who do not fulfil criteria for dyslexia (FR–NR) demonstrate reliably poorer performance than their peers in word decoding, nonword decoding and spelling underlining the fact that dyslexia is a continuous trait (e.g., Pennington, 2006). However, their deficits do not extend to reading comprehension (perhaps because this group has stronger vocabulary and language skills). In line with this view, children at family risk who do not develop reading problems appear to overcome delays in the development of vocabulary, grammar, and phonological skills observed in the preschool years by the time of formal schooling.
4. Predictive relationships between early cognitive and later reading skills
The studies reviewed did not test our hypotheses directly but they do provide evidence largely consistent with both Hypotheses 4a and b. Results show that phonological awareness, letter knowledge and rapid naming are strong predictors of literacy skills in children at family risk of dyslexia, as they are in children without such risk. However, letter knowledge may be a predictor for a longer period of time in children at risk than in typical readers (where it reaches ceiling earlier) and hence rapid naming might be more important as a unique predictor in at-risk children. There is also evidence that parental reading status and shared book reading have an impact on children’s vocabulary and letter knowledge, and indirectly on literacy skills (Hypothesis 4b). Although there were not a sufficient number of studies to conduct a systematic analysis, a similar set of predictors (i.e., letter/symbol knowledge, phonological awareness, and rapid naming) stand out as important in English, Finnish, and Chinese children.
5. Intervention studies
Studies that have evaluated interventions for dyslexia are generally poor in quality and evidence is, therefore limited. There is some evidence consistent with Hypothesis 5a that training in decoding related skills has positive effects but only when treated and untreated at-risk controls are compared. Regardless of the intervention, the size of the effects is small and the effects are not long-lasting. The evidence available neither confirms nor refutes Hypothesis 5b.
Methodological Issues
This review has identified a number of methodological issues that limit the conclusions that can be drawn from studies of children at family risk of dyslexia. We used the GRADE system (Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation) to critically appraise the evidence that we have discussed. GRADE is an approach that rates the quality of evidence and is used widely by, for instance, Cochrane and the World Health Organization (Guyatt, Oxman, et al., 2008, 2011). Quality reflects confidence in the results based on the study limitations and risk of bias; there are four categories—high, moderate, low, and very low. Observational studies and intervention studies are rated based on the limitation of the study, inconsistency of results, indirectness of evidence, imprecision, and reporting bias. Here we will discuss the study quality of the observational studies under four criteria: failure to develop and apply appropriate eligibility criteria, flawed measurement, failure to control for confounding variables, and incomplete follow-up.
1. Failure to develop and apply appropriate eligibility criteria
We found considerable variation across studies of children at family risk of dyslexia in how families are recruited: while most studies begin by recruiting children who have at least one parent with dyslexia, some have recruited younger siblings of children with dyslexia, and studies vary in how dyslexia is established in the index relative. Such variation may affect the identification of endophenotypes and affect conclusions regarding familial risk factors. For example, in many studies dyslexia status is based on self-report rather than confirmed with standardized tests and typically no screening is done for other conditions which may affect parents or other relatives, such as attention disorders (Snowling, Dawes, Nash, & Hulme, 2012). Further, parents who volunteer to take part in a study of children at family risk of dyslexia are already aware of the condition and, if diagnosed themselves, they will be highly motivated to ensure their children get the best of opportunities (see Leavett, Nash, & Snowling, 2014). In this light, the poor response of at-risk children to intervention may to some extent reflect that the home literacy environment of volunteers for such trials is already rich in activities to promote prereading skills.
2. Flawed measurement
Not all of the studies which report group differences between family risk and no-risk groups have published data on dyslexia outcomes and, therefore, it is difficult to use these studies to clarify the precursors of dyslexia. Among those that have, sample sizes are often small. Furthermore, outcomes have not been defined consistently across studies with some studies assessing reading accuracy, speed, or fluency, others both accuracy and comprehension, and many silent with regard to IQ. Also a common limitation is that sensitivity and specificity data that could potentially add valuable information about the prospects of identifying dyslexia at an early age are not reported for the tests. More generally, measures, particularly of language, are not pure and often tap processes such as executive skills that are not controlled. Further, measurement error is rarely dealt with in the studies, and only a minority report α reliability, other types of measurement reliability or latent variables. The statistical treatment of data has also been diverse. While some studies use sophisticated multivariate techniques, most studies are relatively small given the parameters in the models and there has been a strong tendency to pool across high- and low-risk samples to examine the predictors of dyslexia. As a result the conclusions, particularly from predictive longitudinal and neurophysiological studies are in need of replication and should go beyond the investigation of predictors to examine the moderators or mediators of reading outcomes.
3. Failure to control for confounding variables
A salutary finding of family risk studies is the lack of screening for comorbid disorders; undoubtedly this limits the conclusions that can be drawn. One possibility, which the extant studies are not set up to address, is that low general cognitive ability, a measure of basic processing capacity that draws on perceptual—motor as well as cognitive skills, is a marker of comorbid conditions. Notably, if samples of children at family risk of dyslexia contain some children with primary impairments of speech and language, this could explain why, at the group level, they show moderate to severe difficulties in the linguistic domain. Relevant to this issue, Nash, Hulme, Gooch, and Snowling (2013) reported that approximately one-third of their sample of children at family risk of dyslexia fulfilled diagnostic criteria for SLI. Further, two cross-sectional studies of children at family risk of dyslexia (FR+) provide some relevant data pertinent to the issue of uncontrolled speech problems (Carroll & Snowling, 2004; Carroll & Myers, 2010). In one of the Chinese studies reviewed, McBride-Chang et al. (2008) reported that 5-year olds with language-delay performed significantly worse than controls across all tasks known to be concurrent predictors of reading in Chinese (namely, syllable deletion, tone detection, RAN digits, morphological awareness, and a task requiring mapping of characters to sounds, tapping visuospatial processing). In contrast, children at family risk of dyslexia were only poorer in Chinese word recognition, tone detection and morphological awareness. In short, there is a need for research that tracks the growth of reading skills in children with different patterns of language strength and weakness as well as co-occurring risk factors (e.g., attention problems, McGrath et al., 2011) to identify pathways to dyslexia in children with a broad range of language learning impairments. Finally, another possible confounder that is rarely reported is gender. Although gender is sometimes seen as a risk/protective factor, very few studies report its effect on literacy outcomes.
4. Incomplete follow-up
In many studies of children at family risk of dyslexia, especially those that are longer term, attrition rates are high, and there is little information about how the missing data have been dealt with.
The GRADE system lists five different study limitations that can afflict randomized controlled trials. However, only one intervention study with children at family risk was randomized. This is a major shortcoming of the extant research. In addition, although not discussed in the GRADE system but important to note, is the duplicate publishing of articles reporting data based on the same sample. When there are several publications based on the same sample it is critical to describe clearly the sample size and how it relates to previously documented research drawing on the same sample so that bias can be assessed.
In summary, the large and growing body of research on children at family risk of dyslexia has strengthened evidence for causal relationships between a range of oral language deficits and later literacy skills in this population. However, few studies have sampled from across the range of social classes, many have not matched groups for socioeconomic status and few have controlled for co-occurring disorders. An outstanding question is whether the effects of language are direct or fully mediated by skills which are the foundations of decoding and there is still a pressing need for higher quality training (experimental) studies.
Conclusions and Educational Implications
Although there is debate as to whether dyslexia should be considered a diagnostic category, there is strong evidence from the studies reviewed here, that it is a developmental disorder, signs of which are apparent in the preschool years. This is the first rigorous review of the findings from the 21 studies that have followed children at family risk of dyslexia from preschool through the early school years, published in over 100 articles. Therefore, it provides a comprehensive analysis of the risks associated with familial dyslexia that predate the onset of literacy and elucidates likely causal factors. A novel finding is that the risks associated with dyslexia appear to be universal across languages. Because the focus was on perceptual and cognitive risk factors, the findings highlight potential targets for interventions to prevent dyslexia.
The findings of the review confirm that the likelihood of reading difficulties is significantly increased in children at family risk, with offspring in such families varying along a continuum of severity, consistent with the notion of a dimensional disorder. As for all complex disorders, an interaction of genetic and environmental risk and protective factors determine where the skills of an individual fall on this continuum. Although data are limited, studies of children at family risk of dyslexia also remind us of the importance of gene—environment correlations, captured for example, by differences, albeit subtle, in the home and literacy environment experienced by children of parents with dyslexia.
Turning to cognitive risk factors, studies of children at family risk confirm that a phonological deficit is a primary risk factor for dyslexia throughout development, consistent with findings from a large body of research on school-age children with dyslexia and universally across languages. In the preschool years this appears best captured by a deficit in phonological memory (nonword repetition) and in the school years by a deficit in phoneme awareness (that becomes more marked (relative to controls) in those with reading problems). However, it is clear that a phonological deficit alone is not sufficient to account for dyslexia (Pennington, 2006). Two findings are of particular importance. First, children at family risk who go on to be classified as “not dyslexic” share some of the same impairments as their affected relatives. Principally, they experience phonological difficulties, consistent with the notion of a “phonological endophenotype” of dyslexia (Moll, Loff, & Snowling, 2013). However, in preschool they also experience delayed language development, poorer letter knowledge and RAN deficits, relative to controls. Second, children who go on in the school years to be identified as dyslexic show deficits not only in phonological skills but also in broader language skills (grammar and vocabulary are typically affected in preschool and vocabulary knowledge during the school years). Additional risks are conferred by lower nonverbal ability, weaknesses in auditory processing and limitations of verbal STM. Together, these findings add to a growing body of evidence that a phonological deficit is one of a number of risk factors for dyslexia that accumulate toward a threshold that characterizes the disorder (Pennington et al., 2012).
In line with this, there is suggestive evidence that there is more than one developmental trajectory leading to dyslexia (e.g., Lyytinen et al., 2006). The findings of the review show that, although children at family risk of dyslexia share phonological deficits, not all become classified as poor readers. The divergence in the pattern of literacy development observed here is reminiscent of the pattern reported by Bishop and Adams (1990) for children with a preschool history of language difficulties who do and do not resolve their language impairments. Subgroup differences may turn on the severity of the phonological deficit—such that those with more severe deficits are more likely to be poor readers and have enduring vocabulary impairments. On the other hand, phonological deficits may have different origins in different subgroups; for example, as a consequence of poor verbal STM in children at family risk who do not develop reading problems or as a marker of language impairment and associated auditory processing deficits in children who go on to be dyslexic as a facet of language learning impairment (cf. Conti-Ramsden, Botting, & Faragher, 2001).
There are apparently also protective factors that prevent some children at family risk from developing significant reading difficulties. Such children tend to have stronger language skills (principally better vocabulary) and they perform within the normal range on rapid naming tasks. Good language skills may have both direct and indirect effects on reading development. According to Storch and Whitehurst (2001), the influence of language is indirect because children with better language have better phonological skills. Alternatively, Nation and Snowling (1998) proposed that children with decoding difficulties who have good language, particularly vocabulary, can make use of context to bootstrap their deficient skills. In this view, the influence of language is direct. Turning to RAN, again there are a number of possible interpretations. First, if RAN taps a brain mechanism or network involved in the cross-modal integration of visual (symbolic) and verbal codes as some have supposed (e.g., Lervåg & Hulme, 2009; Price & McCrory, 2005), then children who do well on such tasks will be better able to establish the mappings between orthography and phonology that are fundamental to reading. In this view, the protracted period of time required by children with dyslexia to learn letters (or other written symbols) is to be expected. Second, children with better RAN performance have faster speed of processing, indicative of greater resources that may, indirectly, help them to compensate. Indeed, RAN taps a variety of executive processes including sustained attention and inhibition (e.g., Clarke, Hulme, & Snowling, 2005) and children who are free of executive impairments are likely to learn to read better (e.g., Kegel & Bus, 2013).
Because risk and protective factors interact, preschool screening for dyslexia is not straightforward. Using data from the Jyväskylä study, Puolakanaho et al., (2007) showed that being at family risk of dyslexia increases the probability of reading disability. However, if letter-naming skills develop early, this decreases it dramatically. Similarly, for a child with poor letter-naming skills at 4.5 and 5.5 years, the probability of dyslexia is lower if he or she has good phonological awareness or efficient RAN skills. These findings represent a nuanced view of the etiology of dyslexia. First, it is in line with other sources of evidence showing that dyslexia is associated with multiple risks (Pennington et al., 2012; Snowling, 2008). Second, it highlights that different skills interact in the development of literacy, and therefore, dyslexia.
An important dilemma for practitioners is how to proceed in the knowledge that dyslexia is not a clear-cut diagnostic category but a dimensional disorder. This being the case, criteria for dyslexia need to be agreed externally and can be expected to differ in different contexts and in different developmental phases. This review shows that even individuals who do not reach criteria for dyslexia experience significant literacy problems relative to their peers; in situations where these difficulties are disabling, these individuals will need special arrangements. In terms of intervention, although there has been considerable progress in recent years in our understanding of methods for teaching reading (e.g., National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000), effective interventions for those at family risk of dyslexia are awaited.
Supplementary Material
Appendix A. Characteristics of Studies Comparing Children With Family Risk (Not yet Diagnosed) and Control Children Without Family Risk (Age, Sample Size, Recruitment of Relatives With Dyslexia, Socioeconomic Status, Comparison Type, Constructs Coded, Effect Size for the Difference Between Children With a Family Risk of Dyslexia and Control Children Without Such Risk, and Prevalence of Dyslexia in the Studies Included in the Meta-Analyses)
Study name | Age dyslexia risk (control) | Sample size dyslexia risk (control) | Outcome construct (indicator) | Effect size (d) | Recruitment scheme | Socio economic background |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. BAS = British Ability Scales; Bayley MDI = Bayley Mental Developmental Index; BNT = Boston Naming Test; BPVS = British Picture Vocabulary Scale; CELF = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; CN Rep = Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition; Comp = comprehension; DAS = Differential Ability Scales; CPM = Colored Progressive Matrices; DEAP = Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology; GORT = Gray Oral Reading Tests; K-BIT = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; m-ABC = Movement Assessment Battery for Children; M CDI = MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories; NARA = Neale Analysis of Reading Ability; NEPSY = Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PA = phonological awareness; PCC = percentage consonants correct; Phon Aw = phonological awareness; Phon memory = phonological memory; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; RAN = rapid naming; SON-R = Snijders-Oomen nonverbal intelligence tests; vSTM = verbal short-term memory; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WJ = Woodcock Johnson; WOLD = Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions; WORD = Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions; Word ID = word identification; WRMT= Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests; YARC = York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension. | ||||||
Aro et al. 2012 (3) | 5 (5) | 107 (91) | Vocabulary knowledge (expressive vocabulary BNT) | −.40 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
Vocabulary knowledge (M CDI voc production) | ||||||
2 years 6 months (2 years 6 months) | 107 (92) | Grammar (morphological skills) | −.26 | |||
5 (5) | IQ (WPPSI performance) | −.46 | ||||
8 (8) | Social skills (basic social skills) | −.07 | ||||
5 (5) | Visual skills (visual matching and visual motor precision | .04 | ||||
8 (8) | from NEPSY) | −.30 | ||||
Word reading (reading fluency of text containing 25 sentences) | −.59 | |||||
Aro et al. 2009 (3) | 8 (8) | 101 (89) | IQ (WISC IQ) | −.25 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
Language comprehension | −.44 | |||||
(general language comprehension tests) | ||||||
Maternal education | −.04 | |||||
Motor (m-ABC) | .25 | |||||
3.5 (3.5) | Paternal education | −.09 | ||||
Phon Aw (general phonological skills) | −.64 | |||||
vSTM (general memory skills) | −.39 | |||||
Black et al. 2012 (10) | 5.52 (5.66) | 24 (27) | IQ (WJ brief IQ) | −.87 | Parental report of reading disability | No differences in parental educational level |
Maternal education | −.16 | |||||
Maternal skills (verbal IQ WAIS) | −.73 | |||||
Paternal education | −.46 | |||||
Paternal skills (verbal IQ WAIS) | −.37 | |||||
Phon memory CTOPP phon memory (composite vSTM and nonrep) | −.77 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (WJ verbal comprehension) | −1.46 | |||||
Visual skills (WJ visual matching) | −.23 | |||||
Word reading (WJ reading mastery test) | −.69 | |||||
Boets, Wouters, van Wieringen, & Ghesquière, 2006a (1) | 5 (5) | 31 (31) | Auditory processing | −.35 | Self-report form | Lower education level in the at-risk parents |
(tone in noise detection) | ||||||
Letter knowledge | −.38 | |||||
Phon Aw (composite PA) | −.65 | |||||
Phon memory (nonword repetition) | −.55 | |||||
RAN (color and picture naming) | −.62 | |||||
vSTM (digit span) | .06 | |||||
Boets, Wouters, van Wieringen, & Ghesquière, 2006b (1) | 5 (5) | 31 (31) | Visual processing (visual motion detection) | .19 | Self-report form | Lower education level in the at-risk parents |
Cardoso-Martins & Pennington, 2004 (6) | 5.39 (5.3) | 67 (57) | Letter knowledge (letter name) | −.52 | — | At-risk group had similar SES |
Nonword reading (WJ word attack) | −.69 | |||||
Phon Aw (Lindamood) | −.26 | |||||
RAN (mean RAN colors, letters, numbers and objects) | −.59 | |||||
Reading comprehension (GORT) | −.78 | |||||
Spelling (WJ spelling) | −.96 | |||||
Carroll & Myers, 2010 | Reception year or year 1 | 46 (128) | Articulatory accuracy (DEAP) | .17 | Recruited because a parent or sibling had dyslexia | — |
Vocabulary knowledge (expressive vocabulary CELF) | −.02 | |||||
Grammar (sentence structure CELF) | −.05 | |||||
Carroll & Snowling, 2004 | 62.71 (62.53) | 17 (17) | Articulatory accuracy (adapted task based on Cookie Monster from Sesame Street that mispronounce words) | −.70 | Had a parent or sibling with dyslexia | — |
Letter knowledge (knowledge of letters and sounds) | −.02 | |||||
Phon Aw (phoneme matching) | −.37 | |||||
Phon memory (12 two and three syllable nonwords) | −1.30 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (BPVS) | −.22 | |||||
Word reading (word recognition early reading) | −.36 | |||||
de Bree & Kerkhoff, 2010 (13) | 5 years 1 month (5 years 1 month) | 57 (26) | Grammar (plural marking) | −.31 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental educational level |
IQ (Snijders et al IQ test for young children) | −.37 | |||||
de Bree et al., 2006 (13) | 3 years 3 months (3 years 1 month) | 49 (28) | IQ (Snijders et al IQ test for young children) | −.15 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental educational level |
Elbro & Petersen, 2004 (4) | 6 years 2 months (6 years 4 months) | 47 (41) | Articulatory accuracy (the poor parrot paradigme-pronounciation correction) | −.40 | Self-report confirmed with testing | No sing differences in parental education |
IQ (Raven) | −.12 | |||||
Letter knowledge (name 29 letters in Danish) | −.70 | |||||
Maternal education | −.37 | |||||
Nonword reading (nonword reading accuracy) | −.74 | |||||
Paternal education | −1.37 | |||||
6 years 2 months (6 years 4 months) | Phon Aw (phoneme identification) | −.64 | ||||
RAN (30 pictured objects presented three at the time [number correct per minute]) | −.47 | |||||
7th grade | Reading comprehension | −.56 | ||||
6 years 2 months (6 years 4 months) | Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.73 | ||||
vSTM (digit span WISC) | −.37 | |||||
2nd grade | Word reading (word reading accuracy) | −.74 | ||||
Eleveld, 2005 (2) | 4 years 10 months (4 years 10 months) | 117 (52) | Letter knowledge (not specified) | −.41 | Self-report confirmed by screening test | The groups were not significantly different at any of the background variables |
Phon Aw (composite phoneme awareness tasks) | −.63 | |||||
RAN (composite colors, objects digits) | −.58 | |||||
Visual skills (visual matching of number letters and figures) | −.19 | |||||
1st grade | 67 (25) | Word reading (3 min reading test) | −.28 | |||
Facoetti, Corradi, Ruffino, Gori, & Zorzi, 2010 | 5.85 (5.67) | 20 (67) | Vocabulary knowledge (similarities WISC) | −.07 | Assigned based on the adult dyslexia check list | — |
IQ (block design WISC) | .06 | |||||
Letter knowledge (identification of letters) | .05 | |||||
Phon Aw (segmenting syllables by parsing a word into its constituent syllables) | −.05 | |||||
RAN (RAN colors) | −1.17 | |||||
Visual skills (visual cueing task) | −.83 | |||||
Gerrits & de Bree, 2009 (13) | 3 years 10 months (3 years 6 months) | 34 (23) | Articulatory accuracy (percentage of consonants correct) | −.84 | Confirmed by testing | — |
Gibbs, 2005 (5) | 6 years 1 month (6 years 1 month) | 37 (29) | Vocabulary knowledge (vocabulary WISC III) | −1.08 | Recruited through British dyslexia association | — |
Phon Aw (phoneme deletion) | −1.54 | |||||
3 years 9 months (3 years 9 months) | Vocabulary knowledge (BPVS) | −1.24 | ||||
Rhyme awareness (rhyme awareness) | −1.18 | |||||
6 years 1 month (6 years 1 month) | Rhyme awareness (rhyme oddity) | −1.49 | ||||
3 years 9 months (3 years 9 months) | vSTM (digit span) | −1.16 | ||||
6 years 1 month (6 years 1 month) | vSTM (digit span) | −1.31 | ||||
Hindson et al., 2005 (15) | 54.6 (55.5) | 69 (65) | Auditory processing (speech discrimination) | −.29 | Recruited through newspaper ads | More unemployed and single parents in at-risk sample than typical sample |
Grammar (temporal terms) | .51 | |||||
IQ (block design) | −.71 | |||||
Letter knowledge (multiple choice task were the child was asked to point to one out of four letters that was said by the tester) | −.64 | |||||
101 (68) | Phon Aw (phoneme identity task where the child should indicate if two words start at the same sound as a target word) | −.51 | ||||
55 (55.9) | 49 (41) | Phon memory (CN Rep) | −.60 | |||
54.6 (55.5) | 69 (65) | Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.79 | |||
Rhyme awareness (recognition of rhymes) | −.71 | |||||
Visual skills (visual matching) | −.09 | |||||
Hosseini et al., 2013 (10) | 5.65 (5.59) | 22 (20) | Phon Aw (CTOPP PA) | −.91 | Parental report of reading disability | No differences in parental educational level |
RAN (RAN digits) | −.27 | |||||
vSTM (CTOPP phon memory) | −.56 | |||||
Kerkhoff, deBree, de Klerk, & Wijnen, 2013 (13) | 1 years 6 months (1 years 6 months) | 30 (31) | Vocabulary knowledge (M CDI expressive) | −.16 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental educational level |
Maternal education | −.05 | |||||
Motor (age first walking) | .29 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (M CDI receptive) | −.66 | |||||
Kevan & Pammer, 2008 | 67.5 (66.52) | 20 (42) | IQ (brief intellectual ability measure Woodcock Johnson III) | −.38 | Responded to media campaigns | — |
Letter knowledge (letter word identification WJ III) | .00 | |||||
Koster et al., 2005 (2) | 17 months | 111 (87) | Vocabulary knowledge (number of words produced) | −.30 | Volunteers that responded to publicity campaigns | Significantly lower parental education level in the at-risk group |
Laakso et al., 1999 (3) | 18 months | 39 (89) | Vocabulary knowledge (Reynell language production) | −.02 | Confirmed with testing | Significantly lower parental education level in the at-risk group |
18 months | 111 | Vocabulary knowledge (Reynell) | −.02 | |||
Lovio, Naatanen, & Kujala, 2010 (3) | 6.7 (6.5) | 10 (9) | Letter knowledge (letter knowledge not specified) | −1.55 | Confirmed with testing | — |
Phon Aw (NEPSY phonological processing) | −1.28 | |||||
Phon memory (nonrep from NEPSY) | −1.22 | |||||
RAN (RAN colors, numbers and letters) | −1.83 | |||||
vSTM (digit span WISC III) | −.71 | |||||
Word reading (reading syllables and nonwords) | −3.25 | |||||
Lyytinen, Ahonen, et al., 2004 (3) | 2 years | 107 (93) | IQ (Bayley MDI) | −.11 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
5 years | 107 (93) | Language comprehension (verbal IQ) | −.44 | |||
3.5 (3.5) | 107 (93) | Sentence rep (NEPSY sentence repetition) | −.13 | |||
Lyytinen, Aro, et al., 2004 (3) | 5 (5) | 107 (93) | Sentence rep (sentence repetition) | −.17 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
Lyytinen, Eklund, & Lyytinen, 2005 (3) | 3.5 (3.5) | 107 (90) | Vocabulary knowledge (Reynell receptive) | −.20 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
18 months | 49 (49) | Vocabulary knowledge (Reynell language skills) | −.02 | |||
Lyytinen, Eklund, Erskine, et al., 2004 (3) | 2 years | 107 (93) | Motor (Bayley motor index) | .04 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
Lyytinen, Poikkeus, et al., 2001 (3) | 3.5 (3.5) | 106 (94) | Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.21 | Confirmed with testing | No differences in parental education level between the groups |
McBride-Chang et al., 2008 (9) | 61.06 (61.28) | 36 (36) | Auditory processing (tone detection) | −.48 | 72 children referred to clinical assessment, out of these 36 had a family risk | — |
Grammar (Children were required to combine familiar morphones in new ways to create new structurally legal multi-morphemic nonwords) | −.63 | |||||
Phon Aw (syllable deletion) | −.45 | |||||
RAN (digits) | −.37 | |||||
Word reading (Chinese word recognition) | −.96 | |||||
Nash et al., 2013 (14) | 3 years 7 months | 83 (69) | Articulatory accuracy (PCC) | −.12 | Confirmed with testing | Typically developing significantly higher SES than at-risk sample |
Vocabulary knowledge (CELF expressive vocabulary) | −.37 | |||||
Grammar (past tense test) | −.65 | |||||
IQ (two WISC III performance subtests) | −.53 | |||||
Phon memory (PS Rep) | −.67 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (CELF basic concepts) | −.50 | |||||
Noordenbos, Segers, Seniclaes, Mitterer, & Verhoeven, 2012 (2) | 6.93 (6.94) | 31 (30) | Nonword reading (3 min nonword reading test) | −1.28 | Confirmed with testing | — |
Phon Aw (phoneme segmentation and phoneme deletion) | −.49 | |||||
RAN (RAN letters) | −.52 | |||||
vSTM (repeating words and sentences) | −.62 | |||||
Word reading (3 min reading test) | −1.20 | |||||
Puolakanaho, Poikkeus, Ahonen, Tolvanen, & Lyytinen, 2004 (3) | 3 years 5 months | 98 (91) | Phon Aw (PA mean; word, syllable level, phonological units) | −.52 | Confirmed with testing | — |
Raschle, Chang, & Gaab, 2011 (11) | 5 years 11 months (5 years 7 months) | 10 (10) | Vocabulary knowledge (CELF vocabulary knowledge) | −.70 | Confirmed with testing | No significant SES difference |
Grammar (CELF language structure) | −.31 | |||||
Parental education | −.05 | |||||
Raschle, Stering, Meissner, & Gaab, 2013 (11) | 5 years 9 months (5 years 6 months) | 14 (14) | Grammar (VAT inflection) | −.37 | Confirmed with testing | No significant SES difference |
IQ (K-BIT) | −.10 | |||||
Phon Aw (deletion) | −.63 | |||||
Phon memory (nonrep) | −.05 | |||||
RAN (objects) | −1.75 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (CELF) | −.28 | |||||
Richardson, Kulju, et al., 2009 (3) | 24.00 (24) | 19 (20) | Articulatory accuracy (amount of unintelligible speech) | .93 | — | — |
30.00 (30) | 19 (20) | Vocabulary knowledge (mean length of utterance) | −.41 | |||
Torkildsen, Syversen, et al., 2007 | 24 (24) | 9 (17) | Vocabulary knowledge (M CDI expressive) | −.93 | Recruited through newspaper ads, diagnosed by municipal authorities in school | — |
Torppa, Georgiou, et al., 2012 (3) | 6 years 5 months | 105 (90) | Phon Aw (composite) | −.40 | Confirmed with testing | No significant difference in education levels |
RAN (composite objects, colors, numbers) | −.50 | |||||
7 (7) | 105 (90) | Spelling (spelling nonwords) | −.31 | |||
Word reading (text reading accuracy) | −.39 | |||||
Torppa et al., 2007 (3) | 5.5 (5.5) | 96 (90) | Letter knowledge (letter knowledge) | −.47 | Confirmed with testing | — |
Phon Aw (phonological awareness composite) | −.48 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (receptive vocabulary) | −.36 | |||||
van Alphen, de Bree, et al., 2004 (13) | 319.17 months | 35 (27) | Grammar (plurals) | −1.00 | Confirmed with testing | — |
Vandermosten, Boets, et al., 2011 (1) | 11.6 (11.7) | 13 (25) | IQ (Raven) | −.32 | — | — |
Nonword reading | −2.16 | |||||
Spelling | −2.19 | |||||
Word reading | −2.33 | |||||
Viholainen, Ahonen, et al., 2006 (3) | 3 years 6 months (3 years 6 months) | 13 (25) | Vocabulary knowledge (Boston naming test) | −.43 | Confirmed with testing | — |
3 years 6 months (3 years 6 months) | 75 (79) | Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.20 | |||
Viholainen, Aro, et al., 2011 (3) | 8 years 6 months | 98 (85) | IQ (WISC full scale) | −.20 | Confirmed with testing | — |
6 years 6 months | 98 (85) | Motor (m-ABC) | .28 | |||
9 (9) | 98 (85) | Word reading (word list reading) | −.52 |
Appendix B. Characteristics of Studies Comparing Children With Family Risk Diagnosed With Dyslexia and Control Children Without Family Risk (Age, Sample Size, Recruitment of Relatives With Dyslexia, Socioeconomic Status, Comparison Type, Constructs Coded, Effect Size for the Difference Between Children With a Family Risk of Dyslexia and Control Children Without Such Risk, and Prevalence of Dyslexia in the Studies Included in the Meta-Analyses)
Study name | Age when effect size was coded family-risk (control) | Sample size Group 1 (Group 2) | Construct (outcome test) | Effect size (d) | Prevalence of dyslexia (in family-risk sample/in control sample without family risk) | Recruitment of relatives with dyslexia | Socioeconomic status |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. BAS = British Ability Scales; Bayley MDI = Bayley Mental Developmental Index; BNT = Boston Naming Test; BPVS = British Picture Vocabulary Scale; CELF = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; CN Rep = Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition; Comp = comprehension; DAS = Differential Ability Scales; CPM = Colored Progressive Matrices; DEAP = Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology; GORT = Gray Oral Reading Tests; K-BIT = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; m-ABC = Movement Assessment Battery for Children; M CDI = MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories; NARA = Neale Analysis of Reading Ability; NEPSY = Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PA = phonological awareness; PCC = percentage consonants correct; Phon Aw = phonological awareness; Phon memory = phonological memory; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; RAN = rapid naming; SON-R = Snijders-Oomen nonverbal intelligence tests; vSTM = verbal short-term memory; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WJ = Woodcock Johnson; WOLD = Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions; WORD = Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions; Word ID = word identification; WRMT = Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests; YARC = York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension. | |||||||
Blomert & Willems, 2010 | Kgarten | 53 (47) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 44%/.09% | Parental questionnaire, sibling with a diagnosis of dyslexia | — | |
Articulatory accuracy (pronounce a word correctly after parts have been deleted) | .09 | ||||||
IQ (general intelligence test not specified) Verbal short term memory (word span) | |||||||
Receptive vocabulary (test not specified) | .19 | ||||||
Letter knowledge (correctly name or sound out visual presented letters) | .18 | ||||||
Phon Aw (blend phonemes or phoneme strings) | .37 | ||||||
.11 | |||||||
.10 | |||||||
Boets, De Smedt, et al., 2010 (1) | Dyslexia versus controls: | — | Maternal and paternal education are measured | ||||
5.33 (5.33) | 16 (26) | IQ (Raven) | −.32 | ||||
Phon Aw (composite PA) | −1.37 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −1.23 | ||||||
RAN (colors and objects) | −.80 | ||||||
vSTM (digit span) | −.40 | ||||||
1st grade | Vocabulary knowledge (WISC vocabulary) | −.49 | |||||
Nonword reading (nonword reading fluency) | −1.56 | ||||||
Paternal education | .31 | ||||||
Maternal education | .00 | ||||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | 44% | — | Maternal and paternal education are measured | ||||
5.33 (5.33) | 20 (26) | IQ (Raven) | −.32 | ||||
Phon Aw (composite PA) | −.52 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −.65 | ||||||
RAN (colors and objects) | −.32 | ||||||
vSTM (digit span) | .25 | ||||||
1st grade | Vocabulary knowledge (WISC vocabulary) | −.40 | |||||
Nonword reading (nonword reading fluency) | −.27 | ||||||
Paternal education | −.28 | ||||||
Maternal education | .00 | ||||||
Boets, Vandermosten, et al., 2011 (1) | 5:6 (5:6) | 16 (26) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 44% | — | Groups matched on parental education level | |
IQ (WISC III block design) | .00 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (WISC III vocabulary) | −.40 | ||||||
Spelling (spelling test Dudal, 1997) | −1.92 | ||||||
6:10 (6:10) | Word reading (word reading test) | −2.53 | |||||
20 (26) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Matched | |||||
5:6 (5:6) | IQ (WISC III block design) | .00 | |||||
Language comprehension (WISC III vocabulary) | .00 | ||||||
6:10 (6:10) | Spelling (spelling test Dudal, 1997) | −.59 | |||||
Word reading (word reading test) | −.43 | ||||||
Boets, Wouters, van Wieringen, et al., 2007 (1) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 29%/.09% | At least one first-degree relative diagnosed with dyslexia | Significant differences in maternal or paternal level between the at-risk group and the control group | |||
63 (64) | 9 (28) | Rhyme awareness (rhyme identity) | −1.00 | ||||
Auditory processing (speech perception) | −.80 | ||||||
IQ (Raven CPM) | −.23 | ||||||
Phon Aw (composite phoneme and rhyme identity tasks) | −1.29 | ||||||
Phon memory (Dutch adaption of CN Rep) | −.85 | ||||||
RAN (rapid naming of colors and objects) | −.95 | ||||||
64 (64) | 22 (28) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | — | At least one first-degree relative diagnosed with dyslexia | |||
Rhyme awareness (rhyme identity) | −.21 | ||||||
Auditory processing (speech perception) | −.55 | ||||||
IQ (Raven CPM) | −.33 | ||||||
Phon Aw (composite phoneme and rhyme identity tasks) | −.65 | ||||||
Phon memory (Dutch adaption of CN Rep) | −.67 | ||||||
RAN (rapid naming of colors and objects) | −.50 | ||||||
Carroll, Mundy, & Cunningham, 2014 | 67.67 (64.12) | 78 (17) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 44.7%/17% | At least one first-degree relative diagnosed with dyslexia after a professional assessment | ||
Articulatory accuracy (judging pronunciation of words) | −1.09 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme matching) | −.83 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −1.27 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (CELF) | −1.34 | ||||||
Letter knowledge | −.51 | ||||||
Word reading (YARC) | −2.81 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (YARC) | −1.11 | ||||||
78 (21) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | ||||||
Articulatory accuracy (judging pronunciation of words) | −.34 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme matching) | −.11 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −.07 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (CELF) | −.37 | ||||||
Letter knowledge | −.00 | ||||||
Word reading (YARC) | −.51 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (YARC) | −.42 | ||||||
de Bree, Wijnen, & Gerrits, 2010 (13) | Dyslexia versus controls: | −1.16 | 50%/22% | Standardized tests of parents | — | ||
4:4 (4:5) | 19 (23) | Phon memory (Dutch version of Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998) | |||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | — | Standardized tests of parents | — | ||||
4:4 (4:5) | 19 (23) | Phon memory (Dutch version of Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998) | −.66 | ||||
Elbro & Petersen, 2004 (4) | 7th grade | 47 (41) | Only prevalence included in the meta-analysis since test data were reported for at-risk children versus controls | — | 55%/20% | Self-report and test | Fathers in the not-at-risk group had significantly higher education than fathers in the at-risk group |
Elbro, Borstrøm, & Petersen, 1998 (4) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 37%/11% | Self-report and test | Fathers in the not-at-risk group had significantly higher education than fathers in the at-risk group | |||
2nd grade | 18 (42) | Articulatory accuracy (articulatory skills) grammar (inflection formation) IQ (Raven) letter knowledge | −.59 | ||||
Phon Aw (phoneme discrimination) RAN (picture naming) vocabulary knowledge (language PPVT) | −.59 | ||||||
vSTM (WISC-R) | −.26 | ||||||
−1.17 | |||||||
−.89 | |||||||
−.48 | |||||||
−.90 | |||||||
−.81 | |||||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Self-report and test | Fathers in the not-at-risk group had significantly higher education than fathers in the at-risk group | |||||
2nd grade | 31 (42) | Articulatory accuracy (articulatory skills) | −.59 | ||||
Grammar (inflection formation) | −.27 | ||||||
IQ (Raven) | −.26 | ||||||
Letter knowledge | −.63 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme discrimination) | −.47 | ||||||
RAN (picture naming) | −.51 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (language PPVT) | −.37 | ||||||
vSTM (WISC-R) | −.20 | ||||||
Gallagher, Frith, & Snowling, 2000 (5) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 66%/16% | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | |||
6 years (6 years) | 63 (34) | Rhyme awareness (rhyme knowledge) vSTM (digit span forward) | −1.00 | ||||
45 months | −1.35 | ||||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | 66%/16% | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | ||||
6 years (6 years) | Rhyme awareness (rhyme knowledge) vSTM (digit span forward) | −.22 | |||||
45 months | −.35 | ||||||
Ho, Leung, & Cheung, 2011 | 36 (25) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 47% | Confirmed with standardized testing | At-risk parents had significantly lower education and income than the controls | ||
5:9 (6:18) | IQ (Raven CPM) | .55 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (adapted word definitions) | −.67 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (picture vocabulary) | −1.17 | ||||||
Phon Aw (syllable deletion) | −1.36 | ||||||
RAN (rapid naming of colors and objects) | −.58 | ||||||
Spelling (Chinese spelling) | −2.40 | ||||||
Word reading (Chinese word reading) | −1.82 | ||||||
40 (25) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | At-risk parents had significantly lower education and income than the controls | ||||
6:6 (6:18) | IQ (Raven CPM) | −.17 | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (adapted word definitions) | −.14 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (picture vocabulary) | −.29 | ||||||
Phon Aw (syllable deletion) | −.23 | ||||||
RAN (rapid naming of colors and objects) | −.05 | ||||||
Spelling (Chinese spelling) | −.11 | ||||||
Word reading (Chinese word reading) | .15 | ||||||
Leppänen, Hämäläinen, et al., 2010 (3) | Dyslexia versus controls: | — | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | |||
3.5 (3.5) | 8 (25) | Phon Aw (composite PA) | −.85 | ||||
5.0 (5.0) | vSTM (composite vSTM) | −.86 | |||||
5.5 (5.5) | RAN (composite RAN) | −1.61 | |||||
9.0 (9.0) | Word reading (composite word reading accuracy and fluency) | −2.78 | |||||
Spelling (composite) | −1.94 | ||||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | — | Confirmed with standardized testing | |||||
3.5 (3.5) | 14 (25) | Phon Aw (composite) | −.83 | ||||
5.0 (5.0) | vSTM (composite) | −.39 | |||||
5.5 (5.5) | RAN (composite) | −.15 | |||||
9.0 (9.0) | Spelling (composite) | −.02 | |||||
Word reading (composite word reading accuracy and fluency) | −.05 | ||||||
McBride-Change et al., 2011 (9) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Merged at-risk sample, both family-risk and delayed language development | — | ||||
Kgarten | 26 (47) | IQ (not specified) | .23 | ||||
Phon Aw (syllable deletion) | −.54 | ||||||
RAN (digits) | −1.13 | ||||||
Visual skills (Gardener test of visual perception) | −.34 | ||||||
7:3 | Word reading (Chinese character reading) | −2.50 | |||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Merged at-risk sample, both family-risk and delayed language development | — | |||||
Kgarten | 21 (47) | IQ (not specified) | .03 | ||||
Phon Aw (syllable deletion) | −.41 | ||||||
RAN (digits) | −.56 | ||||||
Visual skills (Gardener test of visual perception) | −.08 | ||||||
7:3 | Word reading (Chinese character reading) | −.33 | |||||
Pennala, Eklund, et al., 2010 (3) | 3rd grade | 33 (77) | Dyslexia versus controls: | — | |||
Phon memory (NEPSY nonrep) | −.21 | Confirmed with standardized testing | |||||
RAN (naming speed composite) | −.70 | ||||||
Spelling (word spelling) | −1.85 | ||||||
Word reading (word reading accuracy) | −2.39 | ||||||
3rd grade | 68 (80) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | |||
Phon memory (nonrep NEPSY) | −.05 | ||||||
RAN (nonrep composite) | −.00 | ||||||
Spelling (word spelling) | −.37 | ||||||
Word reading (word reading accuracy) | −.28 | ||||||
Pennington & Lefly, 2001 (6) | Dyslexia versus controls | 34%/.06% | Reading history questionnaire that in cases of doubt were supplemented by testing | Matched on SES | |||
5:4 (5:3) | 22 (54) | Letter knowledge (letter name knowledge) | −1.32 | ||||
5:4 (5:3) | 42 (54) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Reading history questionnaire that in cases of doubt were supplemented by testing | Matched on SES | |||
Letter knowledge (letter names) | −.08 | ||||||
End of second grade | 22 (54) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Reading history questionnaire that in cases of doubt were supplemented by testing | Matched on SES | |||
IQ (WISC vocabulary and block design subtests) | −.31 | ||||||
Nonword reading (WJ word attack) | −1.29 | ||||||
Spelling (WRAT spelling) | −1.89 | ||||||
Word reading (WJ letter word identification) | −1.75 | ||||||
End of second grade | 42 (54) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Reading history questionnaire that in cases of doubt were supplemented by testing | Matched on SES | |||
IQ (WISC vocabulary and block design subtests) | −.22 | ||||||
Nonword reading (WJ word attack) | −.39 | ||||||
Spelling (WRAT spelling) | −.69 | ||||||
Word reading (WJ letter word identification) | −.50 | ||||||
Plakas, van Zuijen, et al., 2013 (2) | 3:4 (3:4) | 10 (13) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | ||
IQ (SON-R) | −.60 | ||||||
Language comprehension (Reynell) | −.74 | ||||||
Phon Aw (deletion) | −2.37 | ||||||
vSTM (word span) | −.80 | ||||||
3:4 (3:4) | 15 (13) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | |||
IQ (SON-R) | −.22 | ||||||
Language comprehension (Reynell) | .26 | ||||||
Phon Aw (deletion) | −.52 | ||||||
vSTM (word span) | .26 | ||||||
Plakas, van Zuijen, et al., 2013 (2) | 41 months | 15 (13) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | ||
Grammar (expressive syntax) | .17 | ||||||
Regtvoort, van Leeuwen, et al., 2006 (2) | 5:6 (5) | 11 (12) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | ||
Nonword reading (1-min nonword reading test) | −.99 | ||||||
Spelling (spelling test) | −1.84 | ||||||
Word reading (1-min reading test) | −2.72 | ||||||
5:6 (5) | 15 (12) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | — | |||
Nonword reading (1-min nonword reading test) | −.72 | ||||||
Spelling (spelling test) | −.29 | ||||||
Word reading (1 min reading test) | −.16 | ||||||
Scarborough, 1990 (7) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Matched on SES | ||||
30 months | 20 (20) | Articulatory accuracy (consonant errors) | −1.01 | ||||
Vocabulary knowledge (BNT) | −.50 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.86 | ||||||
2nd grade | IQ (WISC) | −.42 | |||||
Word reading (WJ reading cluster) | −2.99 | ||||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Matched on SES | |||||
30 months | 12 (20) | Articulatory accuracy (consonant errors) | −.08 | ||||
Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.20 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (BNT) | −.32 | ||||||
2nd grade | IQ (WISC) | .28 | |||||
Word reading (WJ reading cluster) | −.03 | ||||||
Scarborough, 1991a (7) | 2nd grade | 35 (44) | Only prevalence coded since means and SDs on single tests were not reported. | 65%/.05% | Confirmed with standardized testing | Matched on SES | |
Smith, 2009 (8) | 8:9 (8:9) | 10 (7) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 58% | Confirmed with standardized testing | Mothers in high-risk sample had significantly lower education level than mothers in control sample | |
Maternal education | −1.20 | ||||||
Nonword reading (WJ word attack) | −.78 | ||||||
Word reading (WRMT-R word ID) | −2.56 | ||||||
Reading comprehension | −2.30 | ||||||
8:9 (8:9) | 10 (7) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | −1.83 | Confirmed with standardized testing | Mothers in high-risk sample had significantly lower education level than mothers in control sample | ||
Maternal education | .75 | ||||||
Nonword reading (WJ word attack) | .30 | ||||||
Word reading (WRMT-R word ID) | .36 | ||||||
Reading comprehension | |||||||
Smith, Roberts, Locke, & Tozer, 2010 (8) | 8–19 months | 6 (5) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Mothers in high-risk sample had significantly lower education level | ||
Articulatory accuracy (syllable index score) | −.71 | ||||||
Language comprehension (general conceptual ability) | −.88 | ||||||
Smith, Smith, et al., 2008 (8) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Mothers in high-risk sample had significantly lower education level | ||||
Age 2–3 | 9 (9) | Vocabulary knowledge (DAS expressive vocabulary) | −.71 | ||||
IQ (Bayley comprehension) | −.60 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (DAS language comprehension) | −.95 | ||||||
vSTM (DAS digit recall) | −.79 | ||||||
Nonword reading (WRMT word attack) | −2.96 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (GORT | −1.17 | ||||||
Grade school | Word reading (WRMT word ID) | −5.46 | |||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Mothers in high-risk sample had significantly lower education level | |||||
Age 2–3 | 9 (9) | Vocabulary knowledge (DAS expressive vocabulary) | −.88 | ||||
IQ (Bayley comprehension) | .19 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (DAS language comprehension) | −1.08 | ||||||
vSTM (DAS digit recall) | −.58 | ||||||
Grade school | Nonword reading (WRMT word attack) | −.42 | |||||
Reading comprehension (GORT | −.19 | ||||||
Word reading (WRMT word ID) | −.69 | ||||||
Snowling et al., 2003 (5) | 3:9 (3:9) | 37 (25) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 66%/16% | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | |
Articulatory accuracy (Edinburgh articulation test) | −.14 | ||||||
Rhyme awareness (nursery rhymes) | −1.20 | ||||||
IQ (Draw A Man [Harris, 1963]) | −.45 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (BPVS) | −1.40 | ||||||
Letter knowledge (identify 26 letters by name and sound) | −1.06 | ||||||
Phon Aw (nursery rhyme knowledge) | −1.21 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −.99 | ||||||
3:9 (3:9) | 19 (25) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | |||
Articulatory accuracy (Edinburgh articulation test) | .20 | ||||||
Rhyme awareness (nursery rhymes) | −.54 | ||||||
IQ (Draw a man (Harris, 1963) | .62 | ||||||
Language comprehension (BPVS) | −.40 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (BPVS) | −.40 | ||||||
Letter knowledge (identify 26 letters by name and sound) | −.48 | ||||||
Phon Aw (nursery rhyme knowledge) | −.54 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −.53 | ||||||
8 years | 37 (25) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | |||
IQ (composite BAS Matrices and BAS Recall of Designs) | −.65 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (WOLD listening comprehension) | −1.66 | ||||||
Nonword reading (Graded Nonword Reading Test [Snowling et al. 1996]) | −2.36 | ||||||
Phon Aw (Phoneme deletion) | −1.58 | ||||||
Phon memory (nonword repetition three and four syllable) | −1.48 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (WORD reading comprehension) | −3.04 | ||||||
Spelling (WORD spelling) | −2.80 | ||||||
Word reading (WORD reading) | −2.79 | ||||||
Rhyme awareness (rhyme oddity) | −1.30 | ||||||
8 years | 19 (25) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | |||
IQ (composite BAS Matrices and BAS Recall of Designs) | −.29 | ||||||
Vocabulary knowledge (WOLD listening comprehension) | −.47 | ||||||
Nonword reading (Graded Nonword Reading Test (Snowling et al. 1996)) | −1.29 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme deletion) | −.80 | ||||||
Phon memory (nonword repetition three and four syllable) | −.52 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (WORD reading comprehension) | −1.02 | ||||||
Spelling (WORD spelling) | −.25 | ||||||
Word reading (WORD reading) | −.60 | ||||||
Rhyme awareness (rhyme oddity) | −.24 | ||||||
Snowling, Muter, & Carroll, 2007 (5) | 12–13 | 21 (17) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | ||
Vocabulary knowledge (WASI vocabulary) | −1.75 | ||||||
Nonword reading (from Griffiths & Snowling, 2001) | −1.82 | ||||||
Phon Aw (Phoneme deletion) | −1.36 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −1.16 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (NARA) | −1.05 | ||||||
Spelling (WORD spelling) | −2.75 | ||||||
Word reading (exception word reading) | −1.93 | ||||||
12–13 | 29 (17) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | −.43 | Confirmed with standardized testing | Middle-class volunteer sample | ||
Vocabulary knowledge (WASI vocabulary) Nonword reading (from Griffiths & Snowling, 2001) | −.37 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme deletion) | −.26 | ||||||
Phon memory (CN Rep) | −.21 | ||||||
Reading comprehension (NARA) | −.10 | ||||||
Spelling (WORD spelling) | −.89 | ||||||
Word reading (exception word reading) | −.80 | ||||||
Torppa, Lyytinen, Erskine, et al., 2010 (3) | 5 years | 37 (84) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with testing | Mothers of the children in the dyslexia affected group had on average a lower education level than children in the two other groups | ||
Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.65 | ||||||
Letter knowledge (nonstandardized task) | −1.35 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phonological sensitivity [NEPSY]) | −.76 | ||||||
RAN (Denckla & Rudel, 1976) | −1.06 | ||||||
Grammar (inflectional morphology adverb and past tense) | −.79 | ||||||
5 years | 68 (84) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | |||||
Vocabulary knowledge (PPVT) | −.32 | ||||||
Letter knowledge (nonstandardized task) | −.33 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phonological sensitivity [NEPSY]) | −.28 | ||||||
RAN (Denckla & Rudel, 1976) | −.25 | ||||||
Grammar (inflectional morphology adverb and past tense) | −.37 | ||||||
Second grade | 37 (84) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 35%/10% | Confirmed with testing | Mothers of the children in the dyslexia affected group had on average a lower education level than children in the two other groups | ||
Nonword reading (nonword reading) | −1.65 | ||||||
Spelling (spelling words and nonwords) | −1.71 | ||||||
Word reading (word reading accuracy in meaningful text) | −1.38 | ||||||
Second grade | 68 (84) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with testing | Mothers of the children in the dyslexia affected group had on average a lower education level than children in the two other groups | |||
Nonword reading (nonword reading) | −.17 | ||||||
Spelling (spelling words and nonwords) | −.16 | ||||||
Word reading (word reading accuracy in meaningful text) | −.04 | ||||||
van Bergen, de Jong, Plakas, et al., 2012 (2) | 48 (48) | 42 (66) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 30%/.03% | Confirmed with testing | Children in at-risk group had parents (both mothers and fathers) with significantly lower education than in the control sample | |
IQ (SON-R nonverbal IQ test) | −.53 | ||||||
Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | |||||||
IQ (SON-R nonverbal IQ test) | −.03 | ||||||
98 (96) | 42 (66) | Dyslexia versus controls: | Confirmed with testing | Children in at-risk group had parents (both mothers and fathers) with significantly lower education than in the control sample | |||
Nonword reading (nonword reading accuracy [de Jong & Wolters, 2002]) | −2.06 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme deletion) | −1.84 | ||||||
RAN (digits and colors) | −.98 | ||||||
Spelling (write down words presented in a sentence) | −1.74 | ||||||
Word reading (word reading accuracy test) | −2.42 | ||||||
98 (96) | 99 (66) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with testing | Children in at-risk group had parents (both mothers and fathers) with significantly lower education than in the control sample | |||
Nonword reading (nonword reading accuracy [de Jong & Wolters, 2002]) | −.56 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme deletion) | −.66 | ||||||
RAN (digits and colors) | −.15 | ||||||
Spelling (write down words presented in a sentence) | −.37 | ||||||
Word reading (word reading accuracy test) | −.64 | ||||||
van Bergen, de Jong, Regtvoort et al. 2011 (2) | 70.4 (70) | 22 (12) | Dyslexia versus controls: | 32% | Confirmed with testing | Children in at-risk group had parents (both mothers and fathers) with significantly lower education than in the control sample | |
Letter knowledge (receptive letter knowledge task) | −1.42 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme blending and segmentation) | −.10 | ||||||
RAN (colors and object) | −1.39 | ||||||
Word reading (3 min reading test) | −3.13 | ||||||
70.6 (70) | 45 (12) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | Confirmed with testing | Children in at-risk group had parents (both mothers and fathers) with significantly lower education than in the control sample | |||
Letter knowledge (receptive letter knowledge task) | −.54 | ||||||
Phon Aw (phoneme blending and segmentation) | .02 | ||||||
RAN (colors and object) | −.64 | ||||||
Word reading (3 min reading test) | −2.16 | ||||||
Dyslexia versus controls: | |||||||
Grade 1 | 22 (12) | IQ (Raven) | −.37 | ||||
Language comprehension (similar to PPVT) | −.11 | ||||||
Grade 5 | 22 (12) | Nonword reading (2 min pseudoword test) | −4.60 | ||||
Grade 1 | 45 (12) | Unaffected family-risk versus controls: | |||||
IQ (Raven) | −.22 | ||||||
Grade 5 | 45 (12) | Language comprehension (similar to PPVT) | −.09 | ||||
Nonword reading (2 min pseudoword test) | −2.92 |
Appendix C. Characteristics of Intervention Studies Concerning Children with Family Risk of Dyslexia
Study name | Comparison | Time point | Sample size T (C) | Outcome | Effect size (d) | Age T (C) | Design | Intervention type/Intervention duration |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Note. Many of the publications included in the review are based on data from the same sample or parts of the same sample. In a parenthesis in the first column of the tables, after the author names, a number in a parenthesis indicates which sample or part of a sample the data are based on. The samples are labelled as follows: Boets et al. = 1; Dutch sample of dyslexia = 2; Jyväskylä sample = 3; Danish sample = 4; Snowling et al. = 5; Pennington et al. = 6; Scarborough sample = 7; Smith et al. = 8; McBride-Chang et al. = 9; Hoeft et al. = 10; Gaab et al. = 11; de Bree et al. = 13; ELDEL sample = 14; Hindson et al. = 15. BAS = British Ability Scales; Bayley MDI = Bayley Mental Developmental Index; BNT = Boston Naming Test; BPVS = British Picture Vocabulary Scale; CELF = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals; CN Rep = Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition; Comp = comprehension; DAS = Differential Ability Scales; CPM = Colored Progressive Matrices; DEAP = Diagnostic Evaluation of Articulation and Phonology; GORT = Gray Oral Reading Tests; K-BIT = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; m-ABC = Movement Assessment Battery for Children; M CDI = MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories; NARA = Neale Analysis of Reading Ability; NEPSY = Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; PA = phonological awareness; PCC = percentage consonants correct; Phon Aw = phonological awareness; Phon memory = phonological memory; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; RAN = rapid naming; SON-R = Snijders-Oomen nonverbal intelligence tests; vSTM = verbal short-term memory; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WJ = Woodcock Johnson; WOLD = Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions; WORD = Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions; Word ID = word identification; WRMT= Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests; YARC = York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension. | ||||||||
Duff et al., (2015) (14) | Trained at risk vs. controls at risk. | Posttest | 67 (77) | Language comprehension | .03 | 6 years (6 years) | Randomized waiting list design | Reading and language intervention (RALI), phoneme awareness/decoding, and language comprehension skills |
Letter knowledge | One group 9 weeks of intervention, the other group 18 weeks of intervention alternating between 20 and 30 min group and individual daily sessions. Implemented by teaching assistants. | |||||||
Nonword reading | .24 | |||||||
Phon Aw | .17 | |||||||
Spelling | .40 | |||||||
Word reading | −.21 | |||||||
.00 | ||||||||
Elbro & Petersen, 2004 (4) | Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Posttest | 35 (47) | Letter knowledge | .99 | 6:2(6:2) | Nonrandomized pre/posttest control group design | Training program written for the study, inspired by the Lundberg and the Lindamood programs. Focused on phoneme-awareness training and letter knowledge. Intervention was 30 min a day for 17 weeks, delivered by kindergarten teachers. |
Phon Aw | 1.14 | |||||||
RAN | −.22 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls not at risk | Posttest | 35 (41) | Letter knowledge | .39 | ||||
Phon Aw | .24 | |||||||
RAN | .06 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Follow-up gain 2nd–3rd grade | 35 (47) | Nonword reading | −.01 | ||||
Word reading | −.18 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls not at risk | Follow-up gain 2nd–3rd grade | 35 (41) | Nonword reading | −.04 | ||||
Word reading | −.17 | |||||||
Fielding-Barnsley, & Purdie, 2003 | Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Posttest | 26 (23) | Language comprehension | −.08 | 70.2 (70.5) | Nonrandomized pre/posttest control group design | Dialogic reading intervention where parents were asked to read to their children using the principles of dialogic reading. Five times during the 8-week intervention. |
Phon Aw | ||||||||
Spelling | .23 | |||||||
Word reading | 1.26 | 1st grade | ||||||
1.09 | ||||||||
Hindson et al., 2005 (15) | Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Posttest | 69 (17) | Letter knowledge | .12 | 54.6 (55.5) | Nonrandomized pre/posttest control group design | The sound foundations package based on training the children on a limited set of phonemes. The intervention was delivered by one of two teachers from research group either at home or in school. Each session 30 min, 11–17 sessions within a period of 3 months |
Ph awareness | .88 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls not at risk | Posttest | 69 (65) | Letter knowledge | −.22 | ||||
Phon Aw | −.18 | |||||||
Regtvoort & van der Leij, 2007 (2) | Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Posttest | 31 (26) | Letter knowledge | .87 | 5:7 (5:7) | Nonrandomized pre/posttest control group design | Word building (Beck 1989). Focuses on reading instruction, letter–sound correspondence, and phonemic awareness. Children were trained by their parents, 10 min a day, 5 days a week for 14 weeks. |
Phon Aw | .48 | |||||||
RAN | −.32 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls not at risk | Posttest | 31 (16) | Letter knowledge | .84 | ||||
Phon Aw | .13 | |||||||
RAN | −.19 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Follow-up gain middle 1st grade to end 1st | 31 (26) | RAN | −.09 | ||||
Word reading | −.16 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls not at risk | Follow-up gain middle 1st grade to end 1st | 31 (16) | RAN | −.14 | ||||
Word reading | −.15 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Follow-up gain end of 1st grade to end of 2nd | 31 (26) | Nonword reading | .46 | ||||
RAN | .20 | |||||||
Spelling | −.17 | |||||||
Word reading | −.10 | |||||||
Trained at risk vs. controls not at risk | Follow-up gain end of 1st grade to end of 2nd | 31 (16) | Nonword reading | .26 | ||||
RAN | .75 | |||||||
Spelling | −.58 | |||||||
Word reading | .01 | |||||||
van Otterloo & van der Leij, 2009 (2) | Trained at risk vs. controls at risk | Posttest | 30 (27) | Letter knowledge | .50 | 69.87 (71.13) | Posttest-only control group design | Sounding sounds and Jolly letters–a home-based prereading program with parents as tutors, 10 min a day, 5 days a week for 14 weeks. |
Phon Aw | .19 | |||||||
RAN | .27 | |||||||
Follow-up mid 1st grade | 30 (27) | Phon Aw | .09 | Middle of first grade | ||||
Word reading | −.34 | |||||||
Follow-up end 1st grade | Phon Aw | −.11 | End of first grade | |||||
Word reading | −.41 |
Footnotes
Scarborough (1989) and Scarborough (1991b) report slightly different prevalence estimates from the same sample. Here the 1991 article is included.
A small amount of variance (7%) in reading accuracy in third grade was attributable to parents’ spelling skills.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis
References marked (1) at the end are included in meta-analysis of prevalence
References marked (2) at the end are included in meta-analysis of studies comparing children at risk classified as dyslexic and children at risk who are not considered dyslexic versus normal reading controls
References marked (3) at the end are included in the meta-analysis of studies comparing samples at risk versus controls
References marked (4) at the end are cited in the review of longitudinal prediction studies
References marked (5) at the end are included in the meta-analysis of intervention studies
References marked (6) at the end are excluded from the review because they are reviews, contain sibling or parental data only, all data are reported elsewhere in a study already included, or they consist of an impure at-risk group (mainly children with no family risk)
References marked (7) at the end are included in the review of neurophysiological data
- Adlard A., & Hazan V. (1998). Speech perception in children with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 51, 153–177. 10.1080/713755750 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahissar M., Lubin Y., Putter-Katz H., & Banai K. (2006). Dyslexia and the failure to form a perceptual anchor. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1558–1564. 10.1038/nn1800 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual 5th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; Retrieved from www.dsm5.org [Google Scholar]
- *Aro T., Eklund K., Nurmi J. E., & Poikkeus A. M. (2012). Early language and behavioral regulation skills as predictors of social outcomes. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 55, 395–408. 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0245) (3, 4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Aro T., Poikkeus A. M., Eklund K., Tolvanen A., Laakso M. L., Viholainen H., et al. Ahonen T. (2009). Effects of multidomain risk accumulation on cognitive, academic, and behavioural outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 883–898. 10.1080/15374410903258942 (3, 4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bearden C. E., & Freimer N. B. (2006). Endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders: Ready for primetime? Trends in Genetics, 22, 306–313. 10.1016/j.tig.2006.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V. M. (2006). Developmental cognitive genetics: How psychology can inform genetics and vice versa. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 59, 1153–1168. 10.1080/17470210500489372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V. M. (2007). Using mismatch negativity to study central auditory processing in developmental language and literacy impairments: Where are we, and where should we be going? Psychological Bulletin, 133, 651–672. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V. M., & Adams C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00844.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop D. V. M., & Snowling M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Black J. M., Tanaka H., Stanley L., Nagamine M., Zakerani N., Thurston A., et al. Hoeft F. (2012). Maternal history of reading difficulty is associated with reduced language-related gray matter in beginning readers. NeuroImage, 59, 3021–3032. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.024 (2, 3, 7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blomert L. (2005). Dyslexie in Nederland, theorie, praktijk en beleid [Dyslexia in the Netherlands: Theory, Practice and Policy]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Nieuwezijds. [Google Scholar]
- *Blomert L., & Willems G. (2010). Is there a causal link from a phonological awareness deficit to reading failure in children at familial risk for dyslexia? Dyslexia, 16, 300–317. 10.1002/dys.405 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boada R., & Pennington B. F. (2006). Deficient implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 153–193. 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boden C., & Giaschi D. (2007). M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 346–366. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Boets B., De Smedt B., Cleuren L., Vandewalle E., Wouters J., & Ghesquière P. (2010). Towards a further characterization of phonological and literacy problems in Dutch-speaking children with dyslexia. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 5–31. 10.1348/026151010X485223 (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boets B., Ghesquière P., van Wieringen A., & Wouters J. (2007). Speech perception in preschoolers at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with low-level auditory processing and phonological ability. Brain and Language, 101, 19–30. 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.009 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boets B., Vandermosten M., Cornelissen P., Wouters J., & Ghesquière P. (2011). Coherent motion sensitivity and reading development in the transition from prereading to reading stage. Child Development, 82, 854–869. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01527.x (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Boets B., Vandermosten M., Poelmans H., Luts H., Wouters J., & Ghesquière P. (2011). Preschool impairments in auditory processing and speech perception uniquely predict future reading problems. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 560–570. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.020 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Boets B., Wouters J., van Wieringen A., & Ghesquière P. (2006a). Auditory temporal information processing in preschool children at family risk for dyslexia: Relations with phonological abilities and developing literacy skills. Brain and Language, 97, 64–79. 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.07.026 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Boets B., Wouters J., van Wieringen A., & Ghesquière P. (2006b). Coherent motion detection in preschoolers at family risk for dyslexia. Vision Research, 46, 527–535. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.009 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Boets B., Wouters J., van Wieringen A., & Ghesquière P. (2007). Auditory processing, speech perception and phonological ability in pre-school children at high risk for dyslexia: A longitudinal study of the auditory temporal processing theory. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1608–1620. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.009 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M., Hedges L., Higgins J., & Rothstein H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat. [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., & Rothstein H. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. London: Wiley; 10.1002/9780470743386 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley L., & Bryant P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read—Causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421. 10.1038/301419a0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bus A. G., van Ijzendoorn M. H., & Pellegrini A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1–21. 10.3102/00346543065001001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Hove: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B., Fielding-Barnsley R., & Ashley L. (2000). Effects of preschool phoneme identity after six years: Outcome level distinguished from rate of response. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 659–667. 10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.659 (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas M., Lervåg A., Defior S., Seidlová Málková G., & Hulme C. (2013). Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science, 24, 1398–1407. 10.1177/0956797612473122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas M., Lervåg A., Mousikou P., Efrim C., Litavsky M., Onochie-Quintanilla E., et al. Hulme C. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678–686. 10.1177/0956797611434536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Cardoso-Martins C., & Pennington B. F. (2004). The relationship between phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: The role of developmental period and reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 27–52. 10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_3 (3, 4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll J. M., & Iles J. E. (2006). An assessment of anxiety levels in dyslexic students in higher education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 651–662. 10.1348/000709905X66233 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll J. M., Mundy I. R., & Cunningham A. J. (2014). The roles of family history of dyslexia, language, speech production and phonological processing in predicting literacy progress. Developmental Science, 17, 727–742. 10.1111/desc.12153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Carroll J. M., & Myers J. M. (2010). Speech and language difficulties in children with and without a family history of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 247–265. 10.1080/10888430903150634 (3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Carroll J. M., & Snowling M. J. (2004). Language and phonological skills in children at high risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45, 631–640. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00252.x (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castles A., & Coltheart M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77–111. 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00164-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castro-Caldas A., Petersson K. M., Reis A., Stone-Elander S., & Ingvar M. (1998). The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 121, 1053–1063. 10.1093/brain/121.6.1053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chazan-Cohen R., Raikes H., Brooks-Gunn J., Ayoub C., Pan B. A., Kisker E. E., et al. Fuligni A. S. (2009). Low-income children’s school readiness: Parent contributions over the first five years. Early Education and Development, 20, 958–977. 10.1080/10409280903362402 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke P., Hulme C., & Snowling M. J. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: A timing analysis. Journal of Reading Research, 28, 73–86. 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2005.00255.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. (1968). Multiple regression as a general data-analytic system. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 426–443. 10.1037/h0026714 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Consortium S. L. I. (2004). Highly significant linkage to SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by Specific Language Impairment (SLI). American Journal of Human Genetics, 94, 1225–1238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conti-Ramsden G., Botting N., & Faragher B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741–748. 10.1111/1469-7610.00770 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper H. (2008). The search for meaningful ways to express the effects of interventions. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 181–186. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00063.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper H. M., Hedges L. V., & Valentine J. (Eds.). (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- *de Bree E., & Kerkhoff A. (2010). Bempen or Bemben: Differences between children at- risk of dyslexia and children with SLI on a morpho-phonological task. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 85–109; 10.1080/10888430903242050 (3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de Bree E., van Alphen P., Fikkert P., & Wijnen F. (2008). Metrical stress in comprehension and production of Dutch children at-risk of dyslexia In Chan H., Jacob H., & Kapia E. (Eds.), BUCLD 32: Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference on language development (pp. 60–71). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
- *de Bree E., Wijnen F., & Gerrits E. (2010). Non-word repetition and literacy in Dutch children at-risk of dyslexia and children with SLI: Results of the follow-up study. Dyslexia, 16, 36–44. 10.1002/dys.395 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *de Bree E., Wijnen F., & Zonneveld W. (2006). Word stress production in three-year-old children at risk of dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 304–331. 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00310.x (3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- de Jong P. F., & Wolters G. (2002). Fonemisch bewustzijn, benoemsnelheid en leren lezen [Phonemic awareness, rapid naming, and learning to read]. Pedagogische Studiën [Pedagogical Studies], 79, 53–63. [Google Scholar]
- Denckla M. B., & Rudel R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479. 10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dollagahan C., & Campbell T. F. (1998). Nonword Repetition and Child Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1136–1146. 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Duff F., Hulme C., Grainger K., Hardwick S. J., Miles J. N., & Snowling M. J. (2015). Reading and language intervention for children at risk of dyslexia: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 1234–1243. 10.1111/jcpp.12257 (5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudal P. (1997). Leerlingvolgsysteem VCLB (CSBO). Spelling: Toetsen 1-2-3. Basisboek en kopieerbundel [Student trajectory system. Spelling: Grade 1-2-3. Manual]. Leuven: Garant. [Google Scholar]
- *Elbro C., Borstrom I., & Petersen D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36–60. 10.1598/RRQ.33.1.3 (1, 2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Elbro C., & Petersen D. K. (2004). Long-term effects of phoneme awareness and letter sound training: An intervention study with children at risk for dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 660–670. 10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.660 (1, 3, 5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Eleveld M. (2005). At risk for dyslexia: The role of phonological abilities, letter knowledge, and speed of serial naming in early intervention and diagnosis. Antwerpen: Garant; (3) [Google Scholar]
- *Facoetti A., Corradi N., Ruffino M., Gori S., & Zorzi M. (2010). Visual spatial attention and speech segmentation are both impaired in preschoolers at familial risk for developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 16, 226–239. 10.1002/dys.413 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Facoetti A., Paganoni P., Turatto M., Marzola V., & Mascetti G. G. (2000). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36, 109–123. 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70840-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Fielding-Barnsley R., & Purdie N. (2003). Early intervention in the home for children at risk of reading failure. Support for Learning: British Journal of Learning Support, 18, 77–82. 10.1111/1467-9604.00284 (5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fischbach A., Schuchardt K., Brandenburg J., Klesczewski J., Balke-Melcher C., Schmidt C., et al. Hasselhorn M. (2013). Prävalenz von Lernschwächen und Lernstörungen: Zur Bedeutung der Diagnosekriterien [Prevalence of poor learners and children with learning disorders: Investigating the role of diagnostic criteria]. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 65–76. 10.1024/2235-0977/a000035 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friend A., DeFries J. C., & Olson R. K. (2008). Parental education moderates genetic influences on reading disability. Psychological Science, 19, 1124–1130. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02213.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frijters J. C., Barron R. W., & Brunello M. (2000). Direct and mediated influences of home literacy and literacy interest on prereaders’ oral vocabularly and early written language skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 466–477. 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.466 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Gallagher A., Frith U., & Snowling M. J. (2000). Precursors of literacy-delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 41, 203–213. 10.1111/1469-7610.00601 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Gerrits E., & de Bree E. (2009). Early language development of children at familial risk of dyslexia: Speech perception and production. Journal of Communication Disorders, 42, 180–194. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.10.004 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Gibbs S. (2005). The interaction of vocabulary and short-term memory in predicting phonological awareness: A comparison of dyslexic and non-dyslexic children. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs, 5, 62–67. 10.1111/J.1471-3802.2005.00043.x (3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gilger J. W., Hanebuth E., Smith S. D., & Pennington B. F. (1996). Differential risk for developmental reading disorders in the offspring of compensated versus noncompensated parents. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 407–417. 10.1007/BF00404002 (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gilliver M., & Byrne B. (2009). What’s in a name? Preschoolers’ noun learning performance in relation to their risk for reading disability. Reading and Writing, 22, 637–659. 10.1007/s11145-008-9155-5 (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gooch D. C., Hulme C., Nash H. M., & Snowling M. J. (2014). Comorbidities in preschool children at risk of dyslexia: The role of language ability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 237–246. 10.1111/jcpp.12139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 3–10. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths Y. M., & Snowling M. J. (2001). Auditory word identification and phonological skills in dyslexic and average readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 419–439. 10.1017/S0142716401003071 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Guttorm T. K., Leppanen P. H., Hamalainen J. A., Eklund K. M., & Lyytinen H. (2010). Newborn event-related potentials predict poorer pre-reading skills in children at risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 391–401. 10.1177/0022219409345005 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Guttorm T. K., Leppänen P. H. T., Richardson U., & Lyytinen H. (2001). Event-related potentials and consonant differentiation in newborns with familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 534–544. 10.1177/002221940103400606 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Guttorm T. K., Leppänen P. H. T., Tolvanen A., & Lyytinen H. (2003). Event-related potentials in newborns with and without familial risk for dyslexia: Principal component analysis reveals differences between the groups. Journal of Neural Transmission, 110, 1059–1074. 10.1007/s00702-003-0014-x (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guyatt G. H., Oxman A. D., Vist G., Kunz R., Brozek J., Alonso-Coello P., et al. Schünemann H. J. (2011). GRADE guidelines: 4. Rating the quality of evidence—Study limitations (risk of bias). Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 64, 407–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guyatt G. H., Oxman A. D., Vist G. E., Kunz R., Falck-Ytter Y., Alonso-Coello P., et al. the GRADE Working Group (2008). GRADE: An emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ, 336, 924–926. 10.1136/bmj.39489.470347.AD [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hämäläinen J. A., Salminen H. K., & Leppänen P. H. T. (2013). Basic auditory processing deficits in dyslexia: Systematic review of the behavioral and event-related potential/ field evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46, 413–427. 10.1177/0022219411436213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley R. (2005). Learning to read in Chinese In Snowling M. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading. Oxford: Blackwell; 10.1002/9780470757642.ch17 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Harris D. B. (1963). Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Hedges L. V., & Olkin I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Helenius P., Salmelin R., Service E., Connolly J. F., Leinonen S., & Lyytinen H. (2002). Cortical activation during spoken-word segmentation in nonreading-impaired and dyslexic adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2936–2944. Retrieved from http://www.jneurosci.org/content/22/7/2936.short(6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helland T., Tjus T., Hovden M., Ofte S. H., & Heimann M. (2011). Effects of a bottom-up and a top-down intervention principle in emergent literacy in children at risk of developmental dyslexia. A longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 105–122. 10.1177/0022219410391188 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Hindson B., Byrne B., Fielding-Barnsley R., Newman C., Hine D. W., & Shankweiler D. (2005). Assessment and early instruction of pre-school children at risk for reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 687–704. 10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.687 (3, 5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ho C. S-H., Chan D. W. O., Lee S. H., Tsang S-M., & Luan V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75. 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00163-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho C. S-H., Chan D. W. O., Tsang S. M., & Lee S.-H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Ho S. C., Leung M., & Cheung H. (2011). Early difficulties of Chinese preschoolers at familial risk for dyslexia: Deficits in oral language, phonological processing skills, and print-related skills. Dyslexia, 17, 143–164. 10.1002/dys.429 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Hosseini S. M. H., Black J. M., Soriano T., Bugescu N., Martinez R., Raman M. M., et al. Hoeft F. (2013). Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks of children at family risk of reading difficulties. NeuroImage, 71, 264–274. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.013 (3, 7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hulme C., Caravolas M., Málková G., & Brigstocke S. (2005). Phoneme isolation ability is not simply a consequence of letter-sound knowledge. Cognition, 97, B1–B11. 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hulme C., & Snowling M. J. (1992). Deficits in output phonology: An explanation of reading failure? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 47–72. 10.1080/02643299208252052 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hulme C., & Snowling M. J. (2009). Developmental disorders of language, learning and cognition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Keenan J. M., Betjemann R. S., Wadsworth S. J., DeFries J. C., & Olson R. K. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on reading and listening comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 75–91. 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00293.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kegel C. A. T., & Bus A. G. (2013). Links between DRD4, executive attention, and alphabetic skills in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 305–312. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02604.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Kerkhoff A. O., de Bree E., de Klerk M., & Wijnen F. N. K. (2013). Non-adjacent dependency learning in infants at familial risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Language, 40, 11–28. 10.1017/S0305000912000098 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Kevan A., & Pammer K. (2008). Visual processing deficits in preliterate children at familial risk for dyslexia. Vision Research, 48, 2835–2839. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.09.022 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kevan A., & Pammer K. (2009). Predicting early reading skills from pre-reading measures of dorsal stream functioning. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3174–3181. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.016 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Koster C., Been P. H., Krikhaar E. M., Zwarts F., Diepstra H. D., & Leeuwen T. H. V. (2005). Differences at 17 months: Productive language patterns in infants at familial risk for dyslexia and typically developing infants. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, 426–438. 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/029) (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Laakso M-L., Poikkeus A. M., & Lyytinen P. (1999). Early intentional communication as a predictor of language development in young toddlers. First Language, 19, 207–231. 10.1177/014272379901905604 (3, 4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leavett R., Nash H. M., & Snowling M. J. (2014). Am I dyslexic? Parental self-report of literacy difficulties. Dyslexia, 20, 297–304. 10.1002/dys.1481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Leppänen P. H., Hämäläinen J. A., Guttorm T. K., Eklund K. M., Salminen H., Tanskanen A., et al. Lyytinen H. (2011). Infant brain responses associated with reading-related skills before school and at school age. Clinical Neurophysiology, 42, 35–41. 10.1016/j.neucli.2011.08.005 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Leppänen P. H., Hämäläinen J. A., Salminen H. K., Eklund K. M., Guttorm T. K., Lohvansuu K., et al. Lyttinen H. (2010). Newborn brain event-related potentials revealing atypical processing of sound frequency and the subsequent association with later literacy skills in children with familial dyslexia. Cortex, 46, 1362–1376. 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.003 (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Leppänen P. H. T., Pihko E., Eklund K. M., & Lyytinen H. (1999). Cortical responses of infants with and without a genetic risk for dyslexia: II. Group effects. NeuroReport, 10, 969–973. 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00014 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lervåg A., Bråten I., & Hulme C. (2009). The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: A Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 45, 764–781. 10.1037/a0014132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lervåg A., & Hulme C. (2009). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a basic constraint on the development of reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20, 1040–1048. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02405.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locke J. L., Hodgson J., Macaruso P., Roberts J., Lambrecht-Smith S., & Guttentag C. (1997). The development of developmental dyslexia In Hulme C. & Snowling M. (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention (pp. 72–96). London: Whurr. [Google Scholar]
- *Lovio R., Näätänen R., & Kujala T. (2010). Abnormal pattern of cortical speech feature discrimination in six-year-old children at-risk for dyslexia. Brain Research, 1335, 53–62. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.097 (3, 7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen H., Ahonen T., Eklund K., Guttorm T., Kulju P., Laakso M.-L., et al. Viholainen H. (2004). Early development of children at familial risk for dyslexia—Follow-up from birth to school age. Dyslexia, 10, 146–178. 10.1002/dys.274 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen H., Ahonen T., Eklund K., Guttorm T. K., Laakso M.-L., Leinonen S., et al. Viholainen H. (2001). Developmental pathways of children with and without familial risk for dyslexia during the first years of life. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 535–554. 10.1207/S15326942DN2002_5 (4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen H., Aro M., Eklund K., Erskine J., Guttorm T. K., Laakso M.-L., et al. Torppa M. (2004). The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 184–220. 10.1007/s11881-004-0010-3 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen H., Eklund K., Erskine J., Guttorm T. K., Laakso M.-L., Leppänen P., et al. Torppa M. (2004). Development of children at familial risk for dyslexia before school age. Enfance, 3, 3–9. 10.3917/enf.563.0289 (3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lyytinen H., Erskine J., Kujala J., Ojanen E., & Richardson U. (2009). In search of a science-based application: A learning tool for reading acquisition. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 668–675. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00791.x (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen H., Erskine J. M., Tolvanen A., Torppa M., Poikkeus A.-M., & Lyytinen P. (2006). Trajectories of reading development; a follow-up from birth to school age of children with and without risk for dyslexia. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 514–546. 10.1353/mpq.2006.0031 (4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lyytinen H., Guttorm T. K., Huttunen T., Hämäläinen J., Leppänen P. H. T., & Vesterinen M. (2005). Psychophysiology of developmental dyslexia: A review of findings including studies of children at risk for dyslexia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 167–195. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2004.11.001 (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lyytinen H., Ronimus M., Alanko A., Poikkeus A., & Taanila M. (2007). Early identification of dyslexia and the use of computer game-based practice to support reading acquisition. Nordic Psychology, 59, 109–126. 10.1027/1901-2276.59.2.109 (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen P., Eklund K., & Lyytinen H. (2003). The play and language behavior of mothers with and without dyslexia and its association to their toddlers’ language development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 74–86. 10.1177/00222194030360010901 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen P., Eklund K., & Lyytinen H. (2005). Language development and literacy skills in late-talking toddlers with and without familiar risk for dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 166–192. 10.1007/s11881-005-0010-y (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Lyytinen P., Poikkeus A. M., Laakso M-L., Eklund K., & Lyytinen H. (2001). Language development and symbolic play in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 44, 873–885. 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/070) (3, 4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maurer U., Bucher K., Brem S., & Brandeis D. (2003). Altered responses to tone and phoneme mismatch in kindergartners at familial dyslexia risk. NeuroReport, 14, 2245–2250. 10.1097/00001756-200312020-00022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Maurer U., Bucher K., Brem S., & Brandeis D. (2003). Development of the automatic mismatch response: From frontal positivity in kindergarten children to the mismatch negativity. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 808–817. 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00032-4 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McArthur G. M., Hogben J. H., Edwards V. T., Heath S. M., & Mengler E. D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 869–874. 10.1111/1469-7610.00674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *McBride-Chang C., Lam F., Lam C., Chan B., Fong C. Y.-C., Wong T. T.-Y., & Wong S. W.-L. (2011). Early predictors of dyslexia in Chinese children: Familial history of dyslexia, language delay, and cognitive profiles. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 204–211. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02299.x (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *McBride-Chang C., Lam F., Lam C., Doo S., Wong S. W. L., & Chow Y. Y. Y. (2008). Word recognition and cognitive profiles of Chinese pre-school children at risk for dyslexia through language delay or familial history of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 211–218. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01837.x (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrath L. M., Pennington B. F., Shanahan M. A., Santerre-Lemmon L. E., Barnard H. D., Willcutt E. G., et al. Olson R. K. (2011). A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Searching for shared cognitive deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 547–557. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02346.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melby-Lervåg M., & Lervåg A. (2012). Oral language skills moderate nonword repetition skills in children with dyslexia: A meta-analysis of the role of nonword repetition skills in dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 1–34. 10.1080/10888438.2010.537715 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Melby-Lervåg M., Lervåg A., Lyster S. A. H., Klem M., Hagtvet B., & Hulme C. (2012). Nonword-repetition ability does not appear to be a causal influence on children’s vocabulary development. Psychological Science, 23, 1092–1098. 10.1177/0956797612443833 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melby-Lervåg M., Lyster S. A. H., & Hulme C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 322–352. 10.1037/a0026744 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moll K., Loff A., & Snowling M. J. (2013). Cognitive endophenotypes of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 385–397. 10.1080/10888438.2012.736439 (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morais J., Cary L., Alegria J., & Bertelson P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323–331. 10.1016/0010-0277(79)90020-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morais J., & Kolinsky R. (2005). Literacy and cognitive change In Snowling M. J. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 188–203). Oxford: Blackwell; 10.1002/9780470757642.ch11 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morton J., & Frith U. (1995). Causal modelling: A structural approach to developmental psychopathology In Cicchetti D. & Cohen D. J. (Eds.), Manual of developmental psychopathology (pp. 357–390). New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Muter V., Hulme C., Snowling M. J., & Stevenson J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665–681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nag S., Caravolas M., & Snowling M. J. (2011). Beyond alphabetic processes: Literacy and its acquisition in the alphasyllabic languages. Reading and Writing, 24, 615–622. 10.1007/s11145-010-9259-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nag S., & Snowling M. J. (2011). Cognitive profiles of poor readers of Kannada. Reading and Writing, 24, 657–676. 10.1007/s11145-010-9258-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Nash H. M., Hulme C., Gooch D., & Snowling M. J. (2013). Preschool language profiles of children at family risk of dyslexia: Continuities with SLI. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54, 958–968. 10.1111/jcpp.12091 (3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Natale K., Aunola K., Nurmi J., Poikkeus A., Lyytinen P., & Lyytinen H. (2008). Mothers’ causal attributions concerning the reading achievement of their children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 274–285. 10.1177/0022219408316094 (4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nathan L., Stackhouse J., Goulandris N., & Snowling M. J. (2004). The development of early literacy skills among children with speech difficulties: A test of the “critical age hypothesis.” Journal of Speech, Hearing, and Language Research, 47, 377–391. 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nation K., & Hulme C. (2011). Learning to read changes children’s phonological skills: Evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study of reading and nonword repetition. Developmental Science, 14, 649–659. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01008.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nation K., & Snowling M. J. (1998). Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69, 996–1011. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06157.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000). Report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. (NIH Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office. [Google Scholar]
- Newbury D. F., Monaco A. P., & Paracchini S. (2014). Reading and language disorders: The importance of both quantity and quality. [Basel]. Genes, 5, 285–309. 10.3390/genes5020285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41, 428–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolson R. I., & Fawcett A. J. (1990). Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 35, 159–182. 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90013-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolson R. I., Fawcett A. J., & Dean P. (1995). Time estimation deficits in developmental dyslexia: Evidence of cerebellar involvement. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 259, 43–47. 10.1098/rspb.1995.0007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nittrouer S. (1999). Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 925–942. 10.1044/jslhr.4204.925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noordenbos M. W., Segers E., Mitterer H., Serniclaes W., & Verhoeven L. (2013). Deviant neural processing of phonotactic probabilities in adults with dyslexia. NeuroReport, 24, 746–750. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328364b67c (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Noordenbos M. W., Segers E., Serniclaes W., Mitterer H., & Verhoeven L. (2012). Neural evidence of allophonic perception in children at risk for dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 50, 2010–2017. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.026 (3, 7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson R. K. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on phonological abilities and reading achievement In Brady S., Braze D., & Fowler C. (Eds.), Explaining individual differences in reading: Theory and evidence (pp. 197–216). New York, NY: Psychology Press/Taylor-Francis. [Google Scholar]
- Paracchini S., Scerri T., & Monaco A. P. (2007). The genetic lexicon of dyslexia. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 8, 57–79. 10.1146/annurev.genom.8.080706.092312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Pennala R., Eklund K., Hämäläinen J., Richardson U., Martin M., Leiwo M., et al. Lyytinen H. (2010). Perception of phonemic length and its relation to reading and spelling skills in children with family risk for dyslexia in the first three grades of school. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 53, 710–724. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0133) (2, 4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington B. F. (2002). The development of psychopathology: Nature and nurture. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pennington B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385–413. 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington B. F., & Bishop D. V. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283–306. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Pennington B. F., & Lefly D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816–833. 10.1111/1467-8624.00317 (1, 2, 4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington B. F., & Olson R. K. (2005). Genetics of Dyslexia In Snowling M. J. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 453–472). Oxford: Blackwell; 10.1002/9780470757642.ch24 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington B. F., Santerre-Lemmon L., Rosenberg J., MacDonald B., Boada R., Friend A., et al. Olson R. K. (2012). Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 212–224. 10.1037/a0025823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perfetti C., Beck I., Bell L., & Hughes C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283–319. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips B. M., & Lonigan C. J. (2005). Social correlates of emergent literacy In Snowling M. J. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 173–187). Oxford: Blackwell; 10.1002/9780470757642.ch10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Pihko E., Leppänen P. H. T., Eklund K. M., Cheour M., Guttorm T. K., & Lyytinen H. (1999). Cortical responses of infants with and without a genetic risk for dyslexia: I. Age effects. NeuroReport, 10, 901–905. 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00002 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Plakas A., van Zuijen T., van Leeuwen T., Thomson J. M., & van der Leij A. (2013). Impaired non-speech auditory processing at a pre-reading age is a risk-factor for dyslexia but not a predictor: An ERP study. Cortex, 49, 1034–1045. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.013 (2, 7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin R., & Kovas Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 592–617. 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price C. J., & McCrory E. (2005). Functional brain imaging studies of skilled reading and developmental dyslexia In Snowling M. J. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 473–496). Oxford: Blackwell; 10.1002/9780470757642.ch25 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Puolakanaho A., Ahonen T., Aro M., Eklund K., Leppänen P. H. T., Poikkeus A.-M., et al. Lyytinen H. (2007). Very early phonological and language skills: Estimating individual risk of reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 923–931. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01763.x (4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Puolakanaho A., Poikkeus A. M., Ahonen T., Tolvanen A., & Lyytinen H. (2004). Emerging phonological awareness differentiates children with and without familial risk for dyslexia after controlling for general language skills. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 221–243. 10.1007/s11881-004-0011-2 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rack J., Snowling M. J., & Olson R. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 28–53. 10.2307/747832 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212–218. 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia: A reinterpretation of the data. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 720–726. 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F., & Ahissar M. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: The difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29, 104–122. 10.1080/02643294.2012.677420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F., Marshall C. R., Rosen S., & van der Lely H. K. J. (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 136, 630–645. 10.1093/brain/aws356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F., Rosen S., Dakin S. C., Day B. L., Castellote J. M., White S., & Frith U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 126, 841–865. 10.1093/brain/awg076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramus F., & Szenkovits G. (2008). What phonological deficit? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129–141. 10.1080/17470210701508822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Raschle N. M., Chang M., & Gaab N. (2011). Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. Neuroimage, 57, 742–749. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.055 (3, 7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Raschle N. M., Stering P. L., Meissner S. N., & Gaab N. (2013). Altered neuronal response during rapid auditory processing and its relation to phonological processing in prereading children at familial risk for dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 2489–2501. 10.1093/cercor/bht104 (3, 7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Raschle N. M., Zuk J., & Gaab N. (2012). Functional characteristics of developmental dyslexia in left-hemispheric posterior brain regions predate reading onset. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America [Published ahead of print]. 10.1073/pnas.1107721109 (7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Read C., Zhang Y. F., Nie H. Y., & Ding B. Q. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 31–44. 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90003-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Regtvoort A. G. F. M., & van der Leij A. (2007). Early intervention with children of dyslexic parents: Effects of computer-based reading instruction at home on literacy acquisition. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 35–53. 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.01.005 (5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Regtvoort A., Zijlstra H., & van der Leij A. (2013). The effectiveness of a 2-year supplementary tutor-assisted computerized intervention on the reading development of beginning readers at risk for reading difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Dyslexia, 19, 256–280. 10.1002/dys.1465 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Regtvoort A. G. F. M., van Leeuwen T. H., Stoel R. D., van der Leij A. (2006). Efficiency of visual information processing in children at-risk for dyslexia: Habituation of single-trial ERPs. Brain Language, 98, 319–331. 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.06.006 (2, 7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Richardson U., Kulju P., Nieminen L., & Torvelainen P. (2009). Early signs of dyslexia from the speech and language processing of children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 366–380. 10.1080/17549500903147545 (3, 5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Richardson U., Leppänen P., Leiwo W., & Lyytinen H. (2003). Speech perception differs in infants at familial risk for dyslexia as early as six months of age. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 385–397. 10.1207/S15326942DN2303_5 (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rochelle K. S. H., & Talcott J. B. (2006). Impaired balance in developmental dyslexia? A meta-analysis of the contending evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1159–1166. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01641.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose J. (2009). Identifying and teaching children and young people with dyslexia and literacy difficulties. DCSF Publications, DCSF-00659–2009.
- Rutter M., Caspi A., Fergusson D., Horwood L. J., Goodman R., Maughan B., et al. Carroll J. (2004). Sex differences in developmental reading disability: New findings from 4 epidemiological studies. Journal of the American Medical Association, 291, 2007–2012. 10.1001/jama.291.16.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M., & Maughan B. (2005). Dyslexia: 1965–2005. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 389–402. 10.1017/S1352465805002316 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sanderson S., Tatt I. D., & Higgins J. P. (2007). Tools for assessing quality and susceptibility to bias in observational studies in epidemiology: A systematic review and annotated bibliography. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 666–676. 10.1093/ije/dym018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saygin Z. M., Norton E. S., Osher D. E., Beach S. D., Cyr A. B., Ozernov-Palchik O., et al. Gabrieli J. D. (2013). Tracking the roots of reading ability: White matter volume and integrity correlate with phonological awareness in prereading and early-reading kindergarten children. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 13251–13258. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4383-12.2013 (6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough H. S. (1989). Prediction of reading disability from familial and individual differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 101–108. 10.1037/0022-0663.81.1.101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743. 10.2307/1130834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Scarborough H. S. (1991a). Early syntactic development of dyslexic children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 207–220. 10.1007/BF02648087 (1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Scarborough H. S. (1991b). Antecedents to reading disability: Preschool language development and literacy experiences of children from dyslexic families. Reading and Writing, 3, 219–233. 10.1007/978-94-011-2450-8_3 (2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Seidenberg M. S. (2011). Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions In McCardle P., Miller B., Lee J. R., & Tzeng O. J. L. (Eds.), Dyslexia across languages: Orthography and the brain–gene–behavior link (pp. 146–168). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Senechal M., & LeFevre J. A. (2001). Storybook reading and parent teaching: Links to language and literacy development. New Directions in Child & Adolescent Development, 92, 39–52; discussion 91–38 10.1002/cd.14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serniclaes W., Van Heghe S., Mousty P., Carré R., & Sprenger-Charolles L. (2004). Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 336–361. 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seymour P. H. K. (2005). Early reading development in European orthographies In Snowling M. J. & Hulme C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 296–315). Oxford: Blackwell; 10.1002/9780470757642.ch16 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Share D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615. 10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz B. A., Shaywitz S. E., Pugh K. R., Mencl W. E., Fulbright R. K., Skudlarski P., et al. Gore J. C. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101–110. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01365-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shu H., & Anderson R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 78–89. 10.1598/RRQ.32.1.5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Skottun B. C. (2000). The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: The evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 40, 111–127. 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00170-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skuse D. H. (2001). Endophenotypes and child psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 395–396. 10.1192/bjp.178.5.395 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. B., Locke J. L., & Farkas L. (2008). Precursors of dyslexia in early conversational turn exchange. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 5–14. 10.1097/01.adt.0000311412.68180.da (6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Smith A., Smith S., Locke J. L., & Bennett J. A. (2008). Longitudinal study of speech timing in young children later found to have reading disability. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1300–1314. 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/06-0193) (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Smith S. L. (2009). Early phonological and lexical markers of reading disabilities. Reading and Writing, 22, 25–40. 10.1007/s11145-007-9101-y (1, 2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S. D., Pennington B. F., Boada R., & Shriberg L. D., (2005). Linkage of speech sound disorder to reading disability loci. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1057–1066. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01534.x (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Smith S. L., Roberts J. A., Locke J. L., & Tozer R. (2010). An exploratory study of the development of early syllable structure in reading-impaired children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 294–307. 10.1177/0022219410369094 (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M. J. (2008). Specific disorders and broader phenotypes: The case of dyslexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 61, 142–156. 10.1080/17470210701508830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M. J. (2012). Changing concepts of dyslexia: Nature, treatment and comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, e1–e3. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02197.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M., Bishop D. V. M., & Stothard S. E. (2000). Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 587–600. 10.1111/1469-7610.00651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M., Dawes P., Nash H., & Hulme C. (2012). Validity of a protocol for adult self-report of dyslexia and related difficulties. Dyslexia, 18, 1–15. 10.1002/dys.1432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Snowling M. J., Gallagher A., & Frith U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358–373. 10.1111/1467-8624.7402003 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M. J., & Hulme C. (2011). Evidence-based interventions for reading and language difficulties: Creating a virtuous circle. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 1–23. 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2010.02014.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Snowling M. J., Muter V., & Carroll J. M. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: A follow-up in adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 609–618. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01725.x (2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowling M. J., Stothard S. E., & McLean J. (1996). Graded nonword reading test. Oxford: Pearson Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Specht K., Hugdahl K., Ofte S., Nygård M., Bjørnerud A., Plante E., & Helland T. (2009). Brain activation on pre-reading tasks reveals at-risk status for dyslexia in 6-year old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 79–91. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00688.x (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storch S. A., & Whitehurst G. J. (2001). The role of family and home in the literacy development of children from low-income backgrounds. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 92, 53–71. 10.1002/cd.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stothard S. E., Snowling M. J., Bishop D. V. M., Chipchase B. B., & Kaplan C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418. 10.1044/jslhr.4102.407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swan D., & Goswami U. (1997). Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 18–41. 10.1006/jecp.1997.2375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallal P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182–198. 10.1016/0093-934X(80)90139-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A., & Rutter M. (2015). Neurodevelopmental disorders In Thapar A., S Pine D., Leckman J. F., Scott S., Snowling M. J., & Taylor E. (Eds.), Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry sixth edition (pp. 31–40). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; 10.1002/9781118381953.ch3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Torgesen J., Wagner R., & Rashotte C. (1997). Test of word reading efficiency (TOWRE). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. [Google Scholar]
- *Torkildsen J. V. K., Syversen G., Simonsen H. G., Moen I., & Lindgren M. (2007). Brain responses to lexical-semantic priming in children at-risk for dyslexia. Brain and Language, 102, 243–261. 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.11.010 (3, 7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Torppa M., Eklund K., van Bergen E., & Lyytinen H. (2011). Parental literacy predicts children’s literacy: A longitudinal family-risk study. Dyslexia, 17, 339–355. 10.1002/dys.437 (4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Torppa M., Georgiou G., Salmi P., Eklund K., & Lyytinen H. (2012). Examining the double-deficit hypothesis in an orthographically consistent language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 287–315. 10.1080/10888438.2011.554470 (3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Torppa M., Lyytinen P., Erskine J., Eklund K., & Lyytinen H. (2010). Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 308–321. 10.1177/0022219410369096 (1, 2, 4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Torppa M., Poikkeus A.-M., Laakso M.-L., Tolvanen A., Leskinen E., Leppänen P. H. T., et al. Lyytinen H. (2007). Modeling the early paths of phonological awareness and factors supporting its development in children with and without familial risk of dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 73–103. 10.1080/10888430709336554 (2, 3, 4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *Torppa M., Poikkeus A.-M., Laasko M.-L., Eklund K., & Lyytinen H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to Grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1128–1142. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1128 (4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valdois S., Bosse M.-L., & Tainturier M.-J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339–363. 10.1002/dys.284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Alphen P., de Bree E., Fikkert P., & Wijnen F. (2007). The role of metrical stress in comprehension and production in Dutch children at-risk of dyslexia. Proceedings of Interspeech, 2007 (pp. 2313–2316). Adelaide: Causal Productions. (2). [Google Scholar]
- *van Alphen P., de Bree E., Gerrits E., de Jong J., Wilsenach C., & Wijnen F. (2004). Early language development in children at risk of dyslexia. Dyslexia, 10, 265–288. 10.1002/dys.272 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Bergen E., de Jong P. F., Plakas A., Maassen B., & van der Leij A. (2012). Child and parental literacy levels within families with a history of dyslexia. Journal Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 53, 28–36. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02418.x (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Bergen E., de Jong P. F., Regtvoort A., Oort F., van Otterloo S., & van der Leij A. (2011). Dutch children at family risk of dyslexia: Precursors, reading development, and parental effects. Dyslexia, 17, 2–18. 10.1002/dys.423 (1, 2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Bergen E., van der Leij A., & de Jong P. F. (2014). The intergenerational multiple deficit model and the case of dyslexia [Advance online publication]. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 346 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Leij A. (2013). Dyslexia and early intervention: What did we learn from the Dutch Dyslexia Programme? Dyslexia, 19, 241–255. 10.1002/dys.1466 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Leij A., de Jong P. F., & Rijswijk-Prins H. (2001). Characteristics of dyslexia in a Dutch family. Dyslexia, 7, 105–124. 10.1002/dys.197 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Vandermosten M., Boets B., Luts H., Poelmans H., Wouters J., & Ghesquière P. (2011). Impairments in speech and nonspeech sound categorization in children with dyslexia are driven by temporal processing difficulties. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 593–603. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.015 (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Herten M., Pasman J., van Leeuwen T. H., Been P. H., van der Leij A., Zwarts F., & Maassen B. (2008). Differences in AERP responses and atypical hemispheric specialization in 17-month-old children at risk of dyslexia. Brain Research, 27, 100–105. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.060 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Leeuwen T., Been P., van Herten M., Zwarts F., Maassen B., & van der Leij A., (2007). Cortical categorisation failure in 2-month old infants at risk of dyslexia. Neuroreport, 8, 857–861. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280c1e2bf (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Leeuwen T., Been P., van Herten M., Zwarts F., Maassen B., & van der Leij A., (2008). Two-month-old infants at risk for dyslexia do not discriminate /bAk/ from /dAk/: A brain-mapping study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 333–348. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.07.004 (7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- *van Otterloo S. G., & van der Leij A. (2009). Dutch home-based pre-reading intervention with children at familial risk of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 169–195. 10.1007/s11881-009-0030-0 (5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Otterloo S. G., van der Leij A., & Henrichs L. F. (2009). Early home-based intervention in the Netherlands for children at familial risk of dyslexia. Dyslexia: The Journal of the British Dyslexia Association, 15, 187–217. 10.1002/dys.376 (5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Otterloo S. G., van der Leij A., Veldkamp E. (2006). Treatment integrity in a home-based pre-reading intervention programme. Dyslexia, 12, 155–176. 10.1002/dys.311 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *van Zuijen T. L., Plakas A., Maassen B. A. M., Maurits N. M., & van der Leij A. (2013). Infant ERPs separate children at risk of dyslexia who become good readers from those who become poor readers. Developmental Science, 16, 554–563. 10.1111/desc.12049 (7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vellutino F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vellutino F. R., Fletcher J. M., Snowling M. J., & Scanlon D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40. 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vicari S., Marotta L., Menghini D., Molinari M., & Petrosini L. (2003). Implicit learning deficit in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 41, 108–114. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00082-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidyasagar T. R., & Pammer K. (2010). Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 57–63. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Viholainen H., Ahonen T., Lyytinen P., Cantell M., Tolvanen A., & Lyytinen H. (2006). Early motor development and later language and reading skills in children at risk for familial dyslexia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48, 367–373. 10.1017/S001216220600079X (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Viholainen H., Aro M., Ahonen T., Crawford S., Cantell M., & Kooistra L. (2011). Are balance problems connected to reading speed or the familial risk for dyslexia? Developmental Medicine Child Neurology, 53, 350–353. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03856.x (3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White S., Milne E., Rosen S., Hansen P., Swettenham J., Frith U., & Ramus F. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: A multiple case study of dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 9, 237–255. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00483.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willcutt E. G., & Pennington B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Differences by gender and subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 179–191. 10.1177/002221940003300206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilsenach C., & Wijnen F. (2004). Perceptual sensitivity to morphosyntactic agreement in language learners: Evidence from Dutch children at risk for developing dyslexia In Brugos A., Micciulla L. & Smith C. E. (Eds.), BUCLD 28 proceedings (pp. 645–656). Somerville: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff P. H., & Melngailis L. (1994). Family patterns of developmental dyslexia: Clinical findings. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatry Genetics) 54, 122–131. 10.1002/ajmg.1320540207 (6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *Wong S. W. L., McBride-Chang C., Lam C., Chan B., Lam F. W. F., & Doo S. (2012). The joint effects of risk status, gender, early literacy and cognitive skills on the presence of dyslexia among a group of high-risk Chinese children. Dyslexia, 18, 40–57. 10.1002/dys.1434 (4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler J. C., Bertrand D., Tóth D., Csépe V., Reis A., Faísca L., et al. Blomert L. (2010a). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551–559. 10.1177/0956797610363406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler J. C., & Goswami U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29. 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler J. C., Pech-Georgel C., Dufau S., & Grainger J. (2010b). Rapid processing of letters, digits and symbols: What purely visual-attentional deficit in developmental dyslexia? Developmental Science, 13, F8–F14. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00983.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler J. C., Pech-Georgel C., George F., & Lorenzi C. (2009). Speech-perception-in-noise deficits in dyslexia. Developmental Science, 12, 732–745. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00817.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.