Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Apr 8.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2015 Feb 27;28(1):1–14. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414001485

Early-adult correlates of maltreatment in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Increased risk for internalizing symptoms and suicidality

MAYA D GUENDELMAN a, ELIZABETH B OWENS a, CHARDEE GALÁN b, ARIANNA GARD c, STEPHEN P HINSHAW a
PMCID: PMC4826281  NIHMSID: NIHMS772502  PMID: 25723055

Abstract

We examined whether maltreatment experienced in childhood and/or adolescence prospectively predicts young adult functioning in a diverse and well-characterized sample of females with childhood-diagnosed attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (N = 140). Participants were part of a longitudinal study and carefully evaluated in childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood (Mage = 9.6, 14.3, and 19.7 years, respectively), with high retention rates across time. A thorough review of multisource data reliably established maltreatment status for each participant (Mκ = 0.78). Thirty-two (22.9%) participants experienced at least one maltreatment type (physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect). Criterion variables included a broad array of young adult measures of functioning gleaned from multiple-source, multiple-informant instruments. With stringent statistical control of demographic, prenatal, and family status characteristics as well as baseline levels of the criterion variable in question, maltreated participants were significantly more impaired than nonmaltreated participants with respect to self-harm (suicide attempts), internalizing symptomatology (anxiety and depression), eating disorder symptomatology, and well-being (lower overall self-worth). Effect sizes were medium. Comprising the first longitudinal evidence linking maltreatment with key young adult life impairments among a carefully diagnosed and followed sample of females with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, these findings underscore the clinical importance of trauma experiences within this population.


Despite voluminous research with males, relatively little is known about factors associated with the long-term developmental outcomes of females with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Although early research viewed ADHD as a time-limited, male disorder, there is now wide consensus that ADHD is a chronic condition, prevalent among girls and strongly predictive of subsequent psychiatric disability and functional impairment in both sexes (Hinshaw et al., 2012; Nigg, 2013). For instance, Biederman et al. (2010) prospectively followed a predominantly Caucasian sample of girls with and without ADHD and found that by young adulthood (Mage = 22 years), the girls with ADHD were at a comparatively high risk for antisocial, addictive, mood, anxiety, and eating disorders (see also Biederman, Petty, O'Connor, Hyder, & Faraone, 2012). In another sample, by 20 years of age girls with ADHD experienced more interpersonal difficulties and depressive symptoms than did a comparison group, but differences did not emerge in job performance, substance use, or self-reported ADHD symptomatology (Babinski et al., 2011). Hinshaw et al. (2012) followed carefully assessed girls with and without ADHD into young adulthood and found serious negative outcomes for the ADHD sample, (e.g., psychiatric comorbidity, global and academic impairment, high service utilization, and high risk for self-harm).

Within a developmental psychopathology framework, ADHD is believed to be a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental condition, the trajectory of which is influenced by multiple biological and environmental factors (Galera et al., 2011). ADHD is familial and highly heritable in children (Nikolas & Burt, 2010), yet the heritability in adults is less clear (Franke et al., 2012). There is growing awareness that certain environmental risk factors are likely to influence the course and alter the outcomes of ADHD over the life span (Biederman, Faraone, & Monuteaux, 2002; Overmeyer, Taylor, Blanz, & Schmidt, 1999; Richards, 2012). As one salient example, converging cross-sectional and epidemiological evidence indicates an important association between ADHD and childhood maltreatment, as reviewed next.

Maltreatment is a potent psychosocial risk factor for a wide array of adverse mental and physical health outcomes (Anda et al., 2006; Briere & Jordan, 2009; Cicchetti & Toth, 2005; Dube et al., 2001, 2003; Edwards, Holden, Felitti, & Anda, 2003; McCrory, De Brito, & Viding, 2010). Lifetime prevalence rates of childhood maltreatment, such as sexual or physical abuse and neglect, range from 10% to 20% and are somewhat higher in girls than in boys (CDC, 2012a). Furthermore, the consequences of early maltreatment on psychological and behavioral functioning may be stronger for girls than for boys (e.g., Lansford et al., 2002). Moreover, in a large, nationally representative sample of adults, Sugaya et al. (2012) found that physical abuse in childhood was associated with elevated odds of multiple adult psychiatric disorders, with ADHD showing the strongest association (see also Beers & De Bellis, 2002; Gunnar et al., 2012; Ouyang, Fang, Mercy, Perou, & Grosse, 2008). Evidence also indicates that children with ADHD are more likely to have experienced maltreatment compared to those without ADHD (Carroll et al., 2012; Ford et al., 2000), suggesting both parent–child and child–parent reciprocal causal pathways.

In an earlier cross-sectional study, utilizing childhood data from the present sample, our research group found that school-aged girls with ADHD were three times more likely to have experienced maltreatment than were matched comparison girls without ADHD (14.3% vs. 4.5%, Mage = 9 years; Briscoe-Smith & Hinshaw, 2006). Furthermore, the abused subgroup of girls with ADHD displayed higher rates of externalizing behaviors and peer rejection than the nonabused subgroup, but no differences in internalizing problems or cognitive deficits emerged.

Research on the long-term consequences of co-occurring maltreatment in individuals with ADHD, especially females, is scarce. De Sanctis, Nomura, Newcorn, and Halperin (2012) retrospectively assessed maltreatment status in a sample of 88 young adults (Mage = 18.4 years) diagnosed with ADHD in childhood, finding that moderate-to-severe childhood maltreatment was associated with increased risk of young adult criminal outcomes such as arrest and recidivism. Outcomes were limited to criminality, however, and the sample was predominantly male (88%; see also De Sanctis et al., 2008). Although not focused on maltreatment per se, Biederman, Petty, Spencer, et al. (2012) found that a diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder occurred more frequently in a mixed-sex ADHD sample than in a comparison sample and was associated with a range of negative outcomes. Still, much remains to be learned about the long-term impact of childhood maltreatment on and functional outcomes among individuals with ADHD, particularly females.

Herein, we examine whether females’ exposure to maltreatment in childhood or adolescence, defined as physical abuse, sexual abuse, and/or neglect, predicts subsequent impairments in early adulthood. Drawing on a large and well-characterized sample of females with ADHD, we focus on an array of criterion domains derived from the prospective investigation of Hinshaw et al. (2012), including externalizing symptoms, internalizing symptoms, substance use, eating disorder symptoms, academic achievement, subjective well-being, self-harm, and service utilization. Our hypotheses are based, first, on the general finding that youth maltreatment is characterized by multifinal outcomes (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005). The principle of multifinality contends that a given vulnerability or exposure can result in divergent patterns of behavioral outcomes, depending on unfolding processes in the person and context over time (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). Although not all individuals exposed to maltreatment develop impairments, there is a strong and graded association between childhood maltreatment and a number of important outcome domains, including poorer educational outcomes (Slade & Wissow, 2007) and lower IQ (Tomoda et al., 2009); disturbances in self-esteem and interpersonal relationships (Briere & Jordan, 2009); externalizing behaviors and lower global assessments of functioning (Silverman, Reinherz, & Giaconia, 1996); eating disturbances (Bardone-Cone et al., 2008); and high rates of mood and anxiety disorders, substance use, disruptive behavior disorders, and psychosis (Anda et al., 2006; Green et al., 2010; Keyes et al., 2012; Sugaya et al., 2012). Thus, our initial prediction is that the sample with childhood ADHD will display higher rates of maltreatment than will the comparison sample.

Second, within the ADHD group, we hypothesize that the maltreated subsample will display wide-ranging impairments compared to the nonmaltreated subsample. Our analyses within an ADHD sample, already known to have high risk for functional impairment (e.g., Hinshaw, 2002; Hinshaw et al., 2012), provide a stringent test of the specific incremental risk incurred by maltreatment.

Third, we predict that relative to the nonmaltreated group, the maltreated subsample will display particularly worse young adult functioning in the domains of internalizing symptomatology and self-harm (see, in particular, Dube et al., 2001; Silverman et al., 1996; Sugaya et al., 2012). This hypothesis draws from work by Keyes et al. (2012), who suggest that for females, the long-term correlates of maltreatment are mediated through females’ tendencies to internalize, placing them at high risk for mood and anxiety disorders. We control for key childhood characteristics (demographic, family status, and prenatal) in our analyses, along with childhood measures of the criterion domain of interest, in order to increase confidence in the potential specificity of the predictive validity of maltreatment per se. Although the present sample size does not afford examination of specific maltreatment type (e.g., physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect), these types of maltreatment frequently co-occur and appear to exert rather nonspecific patterns of risk (e.g., Anda et al., 2006; Dong et al., 2004; Keyes et al., 2012; but see also Lansford et al., 2002).

Because of limited statistical power in the comparison (i.e., non-ADHD) sample and because of our core interest in examining the long-term correlates of maltreatment within an already at-risk ADHD sample, we restrict analyses of the comparison group to (a) a contrast with the ADHD group in terms of overall rates of maltreatment (see initial hypothesis), and (b) an examination of whether clinical status (ADHD vs. comparison) moderates the risk for any of the long-term criterion variables of interest.

Method

Overview of procedure

Data were drawn from the Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study (BGALS), an ongoing prospective longitudinal study of ADHD symptomatology and psychosocial functioning in females with childhood diagnoses of ADHD and age-matched comparison girls. Between 1997 and 1999, San Francisco Bay Area girls aged 6–12 years were recruited from local physician offices, mental health centers, school districts, and through direct advertisements to participate in research summer programs. The programs included recreational activities and were not designed as therapeutic interventions, given the priority of collecting ecologically valid data. Overall exclusionary criteria included IQ less than 70; overt neurological damage, psychosis, or pervasive developmental disorder; and medical conditions precluding participation in the summer program.

Following a rigorous screening and diagnostic assessment procedure, 140 girls with ADHD (93 with combined type, 47 with inattentive type) and 88 comparison girls were enrolled (Hinshaw, 2002). ADHD and comparison girls were mixed and grouped by age at the camps, which included classroom, art, drama, and outdoor activities. In addition, validated multiple-method, multiple-informant measures were used to collect information about participants’ psychological, cognitive, social–emotional, and behavioral functioning. Participants previously taking stimulant medication were assessed while unmedicated. Baseline information collected immediately before and during the camps comprises the baseline (Wave 1) database.

We invited all participants for prospective follow-up 5 years (Wave 2; Hinshaw, Owens, Sami, & Fargeon, 2006) and 10 years after the summer programs (Wave 3; Hinshaw et al., 2012). At both Waves 2 and 3, participants and parents came to the University of California, Berkeley, campus for two half-day assessment sessions. When necessary (e.g., if a participant had moved to another state), we performed telephone interviews or home visits. Detailed study methodology has been previously reported (Hinshaw, 2002; Hinshaw et al., 2006, 2012). Assessors were highly trained postbaccalaureate research assistants or graduate students in clinical psychology; they were not informed of the participants’ diagnostic status. In addition, the use of objective measures (i.e., academic testing and computerized structured interviews) in addition to subjective measures (i.e., self-report questionnaires and interview questions) helped to mitigate bias. All procedures were approved by the University of California, Berkeley, Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects.

Participants

Participants in this study were the 140 young women originally diagnosed with ADHD in childhood. Table 1 displays demographic and background characteristics of the sample at Wave 1. Mean age was 9.6 years (SD = 1.7). The sample was ethnically diverse, with 56% Caucasian, 28% African American, 11% Latina, 4% Asian American, and 1% Native American. Total annual household income ranged from less than $10,000 to more than $75,000 (M = $50,000–$60,000). Fifteen percent of households collected some form of public assistance (e.g., Temporary Assistance for Needy Families, Supplemental Security Income, or food stamps). Maternal educational attainment ranged from less than a high school diploma (1%) to advanced degrees (26%), with 74% of mothers having at least a college degree. This level of income and education is reflective of populations in the San Francisco Bay area. Nearly two-thirds of participants (66%) came from households headed by two parents; 22% had either been adopted or were in the foster care system. With respect to their diagnostic status, 47 participants (34%) had ADHD–inattentive type whereas 93 (66%) had ADHD–combined type. The comparison sample was statistically equal for all demographic variables, except Asian American ethnic status (16%) and adopted/foster status (5%).

Table 1.

Childhood maltreatment frequency

Wave 1 (n = 140)
Wave 2 (n = 128)
Waves 1 and 2 Combined (N = 140)
Maltreatment Type n % n % n %
Physical abuse 11 7.9 8 5.7 18 12.9
Sexual abuse 10 7.1 10 7.1 16 11.4
Neglect 8 5.7 2 1.4 9 6.4
Any 21 15 16 11.4 32 22.9

Note: “Any” maltreatment refers to the total number of participants experiencing one or more maltreatment type at a given wave (ns do not add up because some participants experienced more than one maltreatment type). The ns in the Waves 1 and 2 Combined column represent unique participants experiencing a given maltreatment type or “Any” maltreatment.

Retention rates were high at both subsequent assessment time points (Hinshaw et al., 2006, 2012): 91% (128) of the initial 140 participants with ADHD completed follow-up assessments at Wave 2, when the age range was 11–18 years (Mage = 14.3 years), and 93% (130) at Wave 3, when the age range was 17–23 years (Mage = 19.7); age and retention rates were comparable for the comparison group. To evaluate the representativeness of the retained sample, we contrasted 15 Wave 1 demographic and symptom measures for the 10 participants lost to the Wave 3 follow-up versus those retained. The nonretained group had a significantly lower gross household income, t (135) = 2.55, p = .01, d = 0.86, and marginally lower Weschler Intelligence Scales for Children—Third Edition full-scale IQ scores, t (137) = 1.74, p = .08, d = 0.57. There were no baseline differences as a function of maltreatment status, suggesting that the retained sample was generally representative of the overall ADHD sample. Only two comparison girls were not followed up at Wave 3.

Measures

Predictor measure: Maltreatment

The predictor variable was determined via thorough chart review (see Assessing Childhood Maltreatment Section) using the Background Information Questionnaire (BIQ), Family Information Profile (FIP), Hot Sheet, and Report from the University of California, Berkeley, Summer Program. When available, other relevant psychological and/or medical reports and Child Protective Services reports were also included.

The BIQ, a parent-reported measure, was devised for the BGALS study. Administered at Wave 1 (Hinshaw, 2002), it includes basic demographic information about the participant and her family, and circumstances surrounding the participant's gestation and delivery, health, educational background, and family psychiatric history. It contains information about maltreatment incidents that contributed to out-of-home placement. The covariates of age, socioeconomic status, prenatal risk, and adopted/foster care status were obtained from this measure (see Covariates Section).

The FIP was administered to the caregiver (Wave 2) or caregiver or participant (Wave 3). The FIP is a yearly chart of changes in the participant's living circumstances, family characteristics, school situation, cognitive/educational functioning, as well as physical and mental health, since the previous assessment. Although the measure does not specifically query maltreatment, it provides space for written descriptions of life changes, which may involve maltreatment (e.g., why a participant has moved into foster care).

The Hot Sheet, a clinician-reported measure initially devised for the BGALS study by Briscoe-Smith and Hinshaw (2006) and included at all three waves, highlights whether information accumulating in a participant's file documents “known” or “possible” abuse, dates of abuse and age of the participant at the time of abuse, relationship of the abuser to the participant, and a description of the abuse. Thus, it consolidates information from other measures toward the end of ascertaining maltreatment. Only instances categorized as “known” were counted as maltreatment.

In addition, an individualized Report from the University of California, Berkeley, Summer Program was available for each participant. Written by staff and licensed psychologists, the report describes the participant's background history, cognitive functioning, and ratings of her behavior at camp. The reports, available at Wave 1 only, could include qualitative information about maltreatment that may not have been captured by other questionnaires.

Criterion measures

Each of the following instruments has extensive reliability and validity data, which we report only selectively because of space limitations.

Externalizing symptoms and internalizing symptoms

These two domains were assessed using the computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children—Fourth Edition (DISC-IV Young Adult version; Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, 2000), as well as the Adult Behavior Checklist (ABCL) and the Adult Self-Report (ASR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003) at Wave 3. The DISC-IV is a well-validated, highly structured diagnostic interview, designed to assess psychiatric disorders occurring in youth using DSM-IV qualifications (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Parallel versions exist for parents and young adults. The instrument was administered by trained research staff to parents at Wave 1 and to participants at Wave 3. Wave 1 administration yielded the key covariates for externalizing and internalizing behavior (see Covariates Section). Wave 3 administration contributed externalizing (oppositional defiant disorder [ODD] and conduct disorder [CD]) and internalizing (depression/dysthymic disorder and anxiety disorder) criterion measures.

The ABCL and the ASR are widely used parallel forms designed to facilitate comparisons between self-reported functioning (ASR, completed by the participants) and reports of functioning by informants (ABCL, completed by caregivers). Both the ABCL and the ASR have good to excellent reliability and validity (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2003). Each constituent item is rated on a 0 to 2 metric; we used internalizing and externalizing T scores in our analyses.

Within the externalizing symptoms domain, we also included self-reported delinquency (Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, 1985), an instrument in which participants describe their delinquent activities, including property damage, theft, assault, and substance use. We utilized a previously developed score (Hinshaw et al., 2012) that reflects the variety of antisocial acts in which the participant had engaged in the previous 6 months.

Within the internalizing symptoms domain, we also included the Beck Depression Inventory—II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, Ball, & Ranieri, 1996), a commonly utilized and extensively validated 21-question multiple-choice self-report inventory measuring depression severity in adults. It has excellent psychometric properties.

Substance use

This domain was assessed using the Substance Use Questionnaire (Molina & Pelham, 2003), a structured interview that includes both quantity and frequency items for lifetime and current use of licit and illicit substances, as well as inappropriate or nonprescribed use of medications. It is modeled after similar substance use measures in longitudinal and national survey studies of alcohol and other drug use (Jessor, Donovan, & Costa, 1989; National Household Survey on Drug Abuse, 1992). Kappas for 2-week test–retest reliability for “ever trying” one of five substances averaged 0.84. We created a severity score based on variety and frequency of substance usage in the past year; this score is moderately correlated with DISC-IV substance use/dependence symptom levels (r = .45 to .53; Hinshaw et al., 2012).

Eating disorder symptoms

This domain was assessed using the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI-2; Garner, 1991) and the Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26; Garner, Olmstead, Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982). These are well-validated, self-report measures of symptoms and concerns characteristic of eating disorders. For the EDI-2, we analyzed the drive for thinness, bulimia, and body dissatisfaction scales, which are the most strongly correlated with eating-related pathology (Hurley, Palmer, & Stretch, 1990). The EDI-2 scales have a mean internal consistency of 0.87; test–retest reliabilities range from 0.77 to 0.97. For the EAT-26 we analyzed the total score. The EAT has good validity, differentiating individuals with and without anorexia nervosa (Garner & Garfinkel, 1979).

Academic achievement and service utilization

The academic achievement domain was assessed using the word reading and math reasoning subtests of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, Second Version (WIAT-II; Wechsler, 2001), a psychometrically sound, widely used standardized test of academic achievement. Established test–retest reliabilities for the reading and math scores on the WIAT-II range from 0.93 to 0.95 (Wechsler, 2001). WIAT-II testing was conducted on a separate day of assessment in which the participant was not on any stimulant medication. In addition, we quantified each participant's years of education received from the FIP. The FIP also yielded information regarding service utilization: we counted (1 vs. 0) the use of special education services at school (e.g., restricted placement, classroom aides, pull-out services, and onsite mental health services). For non-school services we counted (1 vs. 0) participation in psychotherapy in the community and psychiatric hospitalizations.

Well-being

This domain was assessed using the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter, 1988), a self-report measure on which adolescents/young adults rate their perceived competence in several domains. We analyzed the subscales. Internal consistencies of these subscales ranged from 0.75 to 0.84, with test–retest reliabilities ranging from 0.69 to 0.80 (Harter, 1982) for social acceptance, scholastic competence, and global self-worth.

Self-harm

This domain was assessed using the Barkley Suicide Questionnaire (Barkley, 2006) and the Self-Injury Questionnaire. The Barkley Suicide Questionnaire is a three-item scale that asks whether the respondent has ever (a) considered suicide, (b) attempted suicide, or (c) been hospitalized for an attempt. If the respondent answers “yes” to any question, it is followed up with a frequency question. We analyzed the dichotomous suicide attempts item. In addition, there was one instance in which a suicide attempt was detailed on the FIP, but not on the Barkley Suicide Questionnaire; we added this individual to the count of attempted suicide.

We also assessed variety and frequency of nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) using a modification of Claes, Vandereycken, and Vertommen's (2001) Self-Injury Questionnaire, which has sound psychometric properties when used within eating-disordered samples. Participants were asked whether they had engaged in certain deliberately self-injurious acts (e.g., scratched or cut their skin with objects, burned themselves, hit themselves hard, or pulled hair out) and if so, how often (1 = only once, 6 = a couple of times per day). We created a dichotomous self-injury variable indicating whether or not a participant had ever intentionally injured herself.

Assessing childhood maltreatment

A master coder along with three undergraduate and postbaccalaureate raters thoroughly reviewed the participants’ charts for indications of physical abuse (e.g., a participant's babysitter frequently hit and dragged her or a mother's boyfriend physically assaulted the participant), sexual abuse (e.g., a participant was molested by her father or a participant was raped by a stranger at gunpoint), and neglect (e.g., biological parents failed to provide participant with adequate nutrition). The charts did not indicate the ADHD status of the participant. Only the master coder was aware of study hypotheses. The charts contained a standard set of measures, selected for their potential to yield trauma-relevant data and representing participant, caregiver, and assessor information.

To develop a coding scheme, the team conducted a comprehensive literature review to familiarize themselves with prevailing definitions of maltreatment. There was broad reliance on definitions of child maltreatment (<age 18) outlined by the Centers for Disease Control and the National Center for Injury Prevention (Leeb, Paulozzi, Melanson, Simon, & Arias, 2008). An event qualified as physical abuse if it involved “the intentional use of physical force against a child that results in, or has the potential to result in, physical injury” (Leeb et al., 2008), and the perpetrator was an adult (>age 18), caregiver, or romantic partner. Sexual abuse was coded if there was any completed or attempted sexual act or sexual contact with a child by a caregiver, peer, stranger, or acquaintance. Because of the age of the sample at Waves 1 and 2 and because of the absence of sufficient background information, we did not include sexual harassment (e.g., creating a hostile environment because of comments or attention of a sexual nature) or noncontact sexual abuse (e.g., intentional exposure of a child to pornography). Finally, neglect was defined as a clear failure by a caregiver to meet a child's basic physical, medical/dental, and/or educational needs. A single event could be coded for multiple forms of maltreatment. Because we lacked consistent data across participants regarding emotional neglect, verbal or psychological abuse, or exposure to violent environments (e.g., domestic or community violence), we excluded these potential categories. Simply having been in foster care or adopted, having lived in a chaotic home, or having been exposed to alcohol or drugs in utero did not automatically qualify as maltreatment, nor did participant behavior that was only suggestive of maltreatment (e.g., aggression, sexual precocity, or posttraumatic stress). In short, our aim was to avoid overascription of maltreatment in the absence of clear evidence.

We devised a 3-point scale to code for the presence of documented physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect (0 = not present, 1 = possibly present, or 2 = definitely present). The 1 category was reserved for instances in which raters were unsure of whether the incident met strict criteria, or there were conflicting statements regarding maltreatment from different reporters, or a reporter noted that there was possible abuse without providing further evidence.

After initial rater calibration on a set of 10 charts, each rater coded approximately one-third of the charts. The master rater coded all charts. Thus, each chart was double-coded, once by the master rater and once by another rater. Raters assigned a code for each chart, with respect to each type of maltreatment, at both Waves 1 and 2. Wave 3 measures were included in the review solely for the purpose of capturing maltreatment that may have occurred prior to Waves 1 or 2, but that had not actually been disclosed or documented until Wave 3. Instances of maltreatment were coded for the wave closest to the time at which the maltreatment occurred.

For reliability purposes, 1 codes were counted as 0 scores. Across Waves 1 and 2, the mean κ for presence of any documented childhood maltreatment was 0.78 (range = 0.64–0.89), signaling good to excellent reliability (Bakeman & Gottman, 1997; Fleiss, 1981).

In order to create a dichotomously coded maltreatment variable (ever/never maltreated) to serve as the independent variable for the study, all cases unanimously coded as 2 were automatically considered as positively maltreated. Coding disagreements involving codes of 1 and 2 (comprising 5 cases out of the 32 final ADHD participants designated as “ever maltreated”) were resolved by obtaining a third rater and consensus discussion to break the tie. Cases in which raters assigned only 1s and/or 0s were treated as “never maltreated” for the purposes of the independent variable.

Covariates

To ascertain whether young adult symptoms and impairments were associated specifically with the participants’ maltreatment status rather than with potentially confounding factors, we used four key measures obtained at Wave 1 from the BIQ as covariates in all adjusted analyses: age, given the 6- to 7-year age span across the sample; socioeconomic status, a composite variable derived from demographic data (family's self-reported income and maternal education); prenatal risk; and adopted or in foster care versus raised by a parent, stepparent, or other relative (coded 0 or 1). The prenatal risk composite was created from three variables indexing the participants’ prenatal and perinatal experience: biological mother's use of tobacco and illicit drugs while pregnant, and low birth weight (<2500 g). This covariate ranged from 0 to 3 based on the presence or absence of each of the three constituent items.

We also controlled for conceptually linked Wave 1 variables where applicable. Full-scale IQ from the Weschler Intelligence Scales for Children—Third Edition (Wechsler, 1991) was utilized as a covariate for analyses of criterion measures within the academic achievement domain. For criterion measures within the externalizing domain, we controlled for comorbidities from the Wave 1 DISC-IV, coded as 1 versus 0 for the presence versus absence of ODD or CD. For criterion measures within the domains of internalizing, well-being, and self-harm, we controlled for comorbidities from the Wave 1 DISC-IV, coded as 1 versus 0 for the presence versus absence of depression/dysthymic disorder or anxiety disorder (the latter had to include the presence of one or more conditions beyond specific phobias). The goal was to ascertain the specific predictive validity of maltreatment, unconfounded by early functioning in the criterion domain of interest.

Data analytic plan

First, we conducted a chi-square test to ascertain whether there were higher rates of maltreatment in the group of participants with childhood ADHD than in the comparison group. Second, to examine whether childhood maltreatment status was associated with each of the eight criterion domains and specific measures included in each domain, we conducted univariate analyses of variance (for continuous criterion variables) and chi-square tests (for categorical criterion variables). (See Table 2 for a complete listing of the eight criterion domains and specific measures included within each.) Effect sizes were calculated using the Cohen d for continuous criterion variables and odds ratios for categorical criterion variables. Within each criterion domain yielding at least one significant p value, we used the Benjamini–Hochberg (BH) procedure for multiple testing correction (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). The BH method adjusts for multiple comparisons by controlling false discovery rate; it has been shown to be a preferred method of protecting against Type I error (What Works What Works Clearinghouse, 2008; Williams, Jones, & Tukey, 1999).

Table 2.

Young adult functioning across domains by childhood maltreatment status

Maltreated
Nonmaltreated
Criterion Domain (Measures) n M (SD) n M (SD) pa Effect Sizeb Adjusted pc
Externalizing symptoms
    DISC-IV CD/ODD (%) 30 51.6 96 40.6 ns 1.25 (0.83, 1.88) ns
    ACBL externalizing 25 63.80 (9.79) 80 59.40 (10.62) .07 0.35 ns
    ASR externalizing 29 60.97 (15.49) 94 56.24 (11.29) .08 0.45 .02
    SRD total 30 1.87 (2.00) 95 1.72 (2.01) ns 0.08 ns
Internalizing symptoms
    DISC-IV dep/dys (%) 30 23.3 96 19.8 ns 1.11 (0.68, 1.82) ns
    DISC-IV anxiety (%) 31 51.6 97 26.8 .01* 1.71 (1.12, 2.59) .02*
    ACBL internalizing 25 62.68 (10.23) 80 56.39 (12.49) .02* 0.51 .06
    ASR internalizing 29 59.38 (13.47) 94 53.66 (12.67) .04* 0.44 .01*
    BDI total 30 15.37 (13.16) 93 9.79 (8.81) <.01* 0.54 <.01*
Substance use
    SUQ severity 29 0.21 (0.99) 95 0.02 (0.91) ns 0.37 ns
Eating disorder symptoms
    EAT total 28 58.01 (19.45) 92 51.74 (16.28) .09 0.37 .01*
    EDI bulimia 29 14.46 (6.91) 90 12.64 (5.11) ns 0.32 .01*
    EDI drive for thinness 29 18.61 (8.89) 90 17.26 (7.77) ns 0.17 .08
    EDI body dissatisfaction 29 29.55 (11.93) 90 27.91 (10.65) ns 0.15 .09
Academic achievement
    WIAT math 31 89.13 (15.68) 97 91.90 (16.24) ns 0.17 ns
    WIAT reading 31 96.90 (12.66) 97 97.35 (15.75) ns 0.03 ns
    Years of education 30 12.23 (1.76) 97 12.56 (1.15) ns 0.25 ns
Well-being
    Harter self-worth 25 2.76 (0.70) 90 3.07 (0.73) .06 0.42 .01*
    Harter social 25 2.90 (0.65) 90 3.14 (0.62) .09 0.39 .08
    Harter scholastic 25 2.64 (0.55) 90 2.69 (0.67) ns 0.07 ns
Service utilization
    Any school services (%) 29 75.9 97 61.9 ns 1.39 (0.87, 2.23) ns
    Any mental health tx (%) 32 87.5 97 68.0 .02 1.81 (1.03, 3.19) <.05
    Any stimulant (%) 31 54.8 96 53.1 ns 1.04 (0.69, 1.56) ns
    Any other med (%) 31 35.5 96 25.0 ns 1.29 (0.83, 1.98) ns
Self-harm
    Suicide attempts (%) 30 33.3 94 12.8 .01* 1.85 (1.14, 3.00) .02*
    Self-injury (%) 30 50.0 89 41.6 ns 1.19 (0.78, 1.80) ns

Note: Y, Young Adult Self-Report; P, parent's report on young adult; DISC-IV, Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Fourth Edition; ACBL, Adult Behavior Checklist; ASR, Adult Self-Report; SRD, Self-Report of Delinquency; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory—II; SUQ, Substance Use Questionnaire; EAT, Eating Attitudes Test; EDI, Eating Disorder Inventory—2; WIAT, Wechsler Individual Achievement Test—II; SIQ, Self-Injury Questionnaire; Mental health tx, mental health treatment; Any other med, nonstimulant psychotropic medication; DISC-IV dep/dys, DISC-IV major depressive episode/dysthymia diagnosis.

a

Significance: one-way ANOVA for continuous variables; Pearson chi-square statistic for categorical variables.

b

Effect sizes: Cohens d for continuous variables and odds ratio (95% confidence interval) for dichotomous variables, computed using unadjusted means.

c

Covariates: age, socioeconomic status, prenatal risk, adopted, or in foster care. For all academic achievement measures, Wave 1 full scale IQ is also included as a covariate. For all externalizing symptoms measures, Wave 1 ODD or CD is also included as a covariate. For all internalizing, well-being, and self-harm measures, Wave 1 depression/dysthymic disorder or anxiety disorder is also included as a covariate.

*

Significant at p < .05 after within-domain Benjamini-Hochberg false discovery rate correction.

To determine whether childhood maltreatment contributed to later functioning independent of confounding risk factors, we performed (a) analyses of covariance for each continuous dependent measure and (b) binary logistic (Wald) regressions for each categorical dependent measure. Four covariates (age, socioeconomic status, prenatal risk, and adopted/foster care) were utilized in all of the adjusted analyses. As noted, we also controlled for full-scale IQ for academic achievement; ODD or CD for externalizing symptoms; and depression/dysthymic disorder or anxiety disorder for internalizing symptoms, well-being, and self-harm. (When we conducted alternate covariance analyses using continuous measures of externalizing or internalizing behaviors, instead of the categorical ODD/CD or depression/dysthymic/anxiety disorder variables, results were almost identical in significance and effect size.)

Finally, in order to ascertain whether associations between maltreatment and criterion variables were different across ADHD and comparison groups, we repeated our analyses with all participants, using diagnostic group as both a predictor variable and a moderator (i.e., adding the interaction term between diagnostic group and maltreatment last).

All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS for Mac OS, Version 20 (IBM Corp., 2011).

Results

Descriptive statistics and Wave 1 characteristics

At Wave 2, 11 participants were missing data on their maltreatment status; we included them using only their Wave 1 maltreatment exposure. Out of the 140 probands with ADHD, 32 (22.9%) were positive for childhood maltreatment of any type (physical abuse: 12.9%; sexual abuse: 11.4%; or neglect: 6.4%; Table 1). Of these 32, 21 (65.6%) had experienced maltreatment by Wave 1, with 11 (34.4%) additional participants experiencing maltreatment between Wave 1 and Wave 2. The majority (71.9%) of those who experienced maltreatment had a single type of maltreatment, whereas 9 (28.1%) had experienced a combination of two or all three types of maltreatment.

Within the comparison group, 10 participants (11.4%) had experienced childhood maltreatment of any type. Results from the chi-square test revealed that participants with ADHD were more likely to have experienced maltreatment relative to comparisons, χ2 (1, N = 228) = 4.75, p = .02.

Within the ADHD group, compared to nonmaltreated participants, maltreated participants were significantly more likely at Wave 1 to have a lower total annual family income and were more likely to have been adopted or placed in foster care, born at a low birth weight, and exposed to tobacco and other substances during gestation (Table 3). Maltreated participants were also significantly more likely to have a higher mother-reported Child Behavior Checklist attention problems T score and a higher count of mother-reported hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, as well as a DISC-IV diagnosis of ODD or CD. Yet no significant differences emerged with respect to depression/dysthymia or anxiety disorder (Table 3).

Table 3.

Wave 1 characteristics of overall ADHD sample plus comparisons between maltreated and nonmaltreated subgroups

ADHD Sample (n = 140)
Maltreated (n = 32)
Nonmaltreated (n = 108)
Variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) pa
Demographic
    Age (months) 115.6 (20.2) 116.1 (21.9) 115.5 (19.8) ns
    Total annual family incomeb 6.2 (2.7) 5.3 (2.7) 6.4 (2.7) .04*
    Maternal educationc 4.7 (1.0) 4.8 (1.0) 4.7 (1.0) ns
    Caucasian (%) 56.4 50.0 58.3 ns
    Public assistance (%) 15.0 25.0 12.0 .09
    Two-parent household (%) 65.7 59.4 67.6 ns
    Adopted or in foster care (%) 22.1 34.4 18.5 .04*
Prenatal and cognitive characteristics
    Low birth weight (<2500 g) (%) 9.3 18.8 5.3 .02*
    Prenatally exposed to drugs (%) 7.9 21.9 3.7 <.01*
    Prenatally exposed to tobacco (%) 19.3 31.3 15.7 .01*
    WISC-III full scale IQ 99.7 (13.6) 97.6 (13.4) 100.2 (13.6) ns
ADHD-related symptoms
    Mom CBCL attention problem T score 74.5 (8.8) 77.3 (10.6) 73.7 (8.0) .04*
    Mom SNAP-IV inattention (0-9) 7.6 (1.9) 8.1 (1.4) 7.4 (2.0) .09
    Mom SNAP-IV HI (0-9) 5.5 (2.9) 6.5 (3.0) 5.2 (2.8) .02*
    Teacher SNAP-IV inattention (0-9) 6.0 (2.8) 5.9 (3.4) 6.0 (2.7) ns
    Teacher SNAP-IV HI (0-9) 3.5 (3.1) 4.2 (3.3) 3.3 (3.0) ns
Comorbidities
    DISC-IV ODD (%) 61.4 78.1 56.5 .04*
    DISC-IV CD (%) 20.7 37.5 15.7 .01*
    DISC-IV anxiety disorder (%) 22.9 25.0 22.2 ns
    DISC-IV depression/dysthymia (%) 7.1 12.5 5.6 ns

Note: WISC-III, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; SNAP-IV, Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham rating scale (see Swanson, 1992); HI, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity; DISC-IV, Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Fourth Edition.

a

Maltreated group versus nonmaltreated group. Significance: one-way ANOVA for continuous variables; Pearson chi-square statistic for categorical variables.

b

For total annual family income, 1 ≤ $10,000; 9 ≥ $75,000.

c

For maternal education, 1 = less than 8th grade; 6 = advanced or professional degree.

*

Significant at p < .05.

Young adult criterion measures

The unadjusted analyses indicate that of the eight criterion domains, significant differences between maltreated and nonmaltreated participants emerged in two (internalizing symptoms and self-harm), after applying the BH correction for multiple comparisons. Specifically, within the internalizing symptoms domain, all measures except DISC-IV depression/dysthymia yielded significant findings, with medium effect sizes. For DISC-IV anxiety disorder, maltreated participants had a rate of 52%, nearly double that of their nonmaltreated counterparts (27%), χ2 (1, N = 128) = 6.56, odds ratio (OR) = 1.71, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.12, 2.59; p = .01. For the ACBL internalizing scale (reported by parent), the maltreated group had higher scores than the nonmaltreated group, F (1, 103) = 5.24, p = .02, d = 0.51; in parallel, for ASR internalizing (reported by participant), maltreated participants scored higher than those without, F (1, 121) = 4.38, p = .04, d = 0.44. Maltreated participants also had higher BDI-II total scores than did nonmaltreated participants, F (1, 121) = 7.02, p < .01, d = 0.54.

Within the self-harm domain, differences emerged on suicide attempts, with 33% of maltreated participants endorsing having attempted suicide, compared to 13% of nonmaltreated participants, χ2 (1, N = 124) = 6.59, OR = 1.85, 95% CI = 1.14, 3.00; p = .01. Participants with and without maltreatment did not differ significantly on the self-injury measure.

Regarding service utilization, participants with maltreatment were more likely to endorse utilization of mental health services compared to nonmaltreated participants (88% vs. 68%), χ2 (1, N = 129) = 4.61, OR = 1.81, 95% CI = 1.03, 3.19; p = .02. However, this finding did not attain significance after applying the BH correction.

Covariates

Analyses of covariance and logistic regression analyses with covariate adjustment revealed significant linkages to maltreatment status in four of the eight young adult domains (internalizing symptoms, self-harm, self-worth, and eating disorder symptoms) after applying the BH correction (Table 2). In particular, within the internalizing symptoms domain, maltreated participants reported significantly higher BDI-II total symptoms, F (1, 114) = 7.81, p < .01, as well as higher ASR internalizing, F (1, 113) = 6.71, p = .01. They were also more likely to receive a DISC-IV anxiety disorder diagnosis (Wald = 5.71, p = .02). Within the self-harm domain, maltreated participants reported greater suicide attempts (Wald = 5.55, p = .02), but did not differ significantly from the nonmaltreated participants on the self-injury measure.

Two additional domains emerged as significant with covariate adjustment. Within the eating disorder symptoms domain, both the EAT total score and the EDI bulimia subscale were higher for maltreated compared to nonmaltreated participants, F (1, 112) = 6.82, p = .01, and F (1, 111) = 6.81, p = .01, respectively. Within the well-being domain, the maltreated participants had lower scores than the nonmaltreated participants on self-worth, F (1, 106) = 6.86, p = .01.

With the inclusion of covariates and the application of the BH correction, the differences between maltreated and nonmaltreated participants in the domains of externalizing symptoms, substance use, academic achievement, and service utilization were not significant.

Moderation analyses

Linear regression analyses revealed a significant interaction between maltreatment and group status for one criterion variable, such that following maltreatment, participants with ADHD were more likely than comparisons to receive a DISC-IV diagnosis of anxiety disorder at Wave 3 (F change = 5.22, p = .02, R2 change = .02), even with covariate adjustment. For the remaining 25 criterion variables, the interaction term was not significant. These findings indicate that, overall, long-term negative consequences of maltreatment were parallel in the ADHD and comparison groups.

Discussion

Our objective was to examine the long-term correlates of child and adolescent maltreatment with respect to young adult functioning in a diverse and well-characterized sample of females with ADHD. Our findings revealed that nearly 23% of the sample had experienced at least one documented type of maltreatment (physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect) by the adolescent (Wave 2) follow-up assessment. This rate was double that of our comparison group and is higher than national estimates (10%–20%), consistent with past research indicating that youth with ADHD are at increased risk for maltreatment (Briscoe-Smith & Hinshaw, 2006; Carroll et al., 2012; Ford et al., 2000). According to our findings, four of eight young adult functioning domains showed at least one significantly elevated measure of impairment among maltreated participants relative to nonmaltreated participants, when adjusting for demographic, prenatal, and conceptually linked baseline variables, and with control of false discovery rate. The most salient pattern was of increased internalizing and self-harm-related symptoms, including elevated suicide attempts, higher levels of bulimia/eating disorder symptoms, and lower self-esteem among maltreated participants. These findings clearly support the contention that child or adolescent maltreatment specifically is an important risk factor for maladaptive functioning in young adulthood among women with childhood ADHD, particularly with respect to internalizing symptoms and suicidal behavior. We did not find evidence that these findings were unique to the ADHD participants, but given the small number of comparison participants with maltreatment, our moderational tests were underpowered.

The impairments of the maltreated participants are conceptually and empirically interrelated. In both the adjusted and nonadjusted analyses, the maltreated subgroup showed elevations on nearly all of the internalizing measures in young adulthood, including increased risk for anxiety disorder and greater depressive symptoms. These are in line with the findings of Keyes et al. (2012) that underlying vulnerability to internalizing symptomatology mediates the link between certain types of childhood maltreatment and mental health in women. Strikingly, the maltreated participants also displayed a higher rate of suicide attempts, as well as lower self-esteem and greater eating disorder and bulimia symptoms. These domains are clearly interlinked in women (Casper, 1998; Waxman, 2009). The findings extend prior research (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2010; Hinshaw et al., 2012) by suggesting that the particularly high risk of internalizing and self-harmful behavior patterns found in girls and women with ADHD may be related, at least in part, to experiences with childhood maltreatment. Of course, without a male comparison group, we cannot claim that this pattern of findings is unique to females. Still, the findings indicate that, among females with ADHD, maltreatment-related impairments concentrate in the domains of internalizing and self-harm.

Within the self-harm domain, however, no significant differences emerged with respect to NSSI behavior; within the internalizing symptoms domain, risk for diagnosis of a depressive disorder did not differ significantly by maltreatment status. NSSI, which involves deliberate bodily self-harm with the absence of lethal intent (Nock, 2009), was common in the sample (43.7%), and it is plausible that differences among the maltreated and nonmaltreated participants were detectable only in the more extreme domain of actual suicidal behavior. Despite the extensive comorbidity between depression and anxiety, it is unclear why the maltreated and nonmaltreated groups did not differ with respect to categorical depression. It is possible that one or more “third variables,” such as peer victimization (e.g., Hamilton et al., 2013), or a certain type of abuse, might provide specific risk for major depressive disorder.

In addition, within the externalizing symptoms domain, significant differences did not emerge. Externalizing behavior was common in this sample; we may have lacked the power to detect significant findings related to maltreatment in this domain. This finding stands in contrast to the increased maltreatment-related CD and delinquency found in De Sanctis et al.'s (2012) mostly male ADHD sample, suggesting the importance of considering gender in maltreatment-related outcomes for individuals with ADHD. Consistent with findings showing sex differences in manifestations of externalizing behavior (e.g., Archer, 2004; Crick & Grotpeter, 1995), one study found that maltreatment was associated with relational aggression (e.g., covert manipulation and bullying) in girls, and physical aggression in boys (Cullerton-Sen et al., 2008). That study suggested that maltreated children may be especially likely to engage in aggressive behaviors typical of their same-sex peer groups. Thus, it is plausible that our externalizing measures failed to capture the specific behaviors suited to our female sample.

In addition, the measures in substance use, academic achievement, and service utilization did not attain significance in either the unadjusted or the adjusted analyses after applying the BH correction. Still, without exception, all measures examined were in the expected direction (i.e., indicating greater impairment) for maltreated as compared to the nonmaltreated participants.

Although naturalistic designs do not allow for testing direction of effects, the findings raise important etiological questions about the directionality of the association between maltreatment and ADHD. More than half of the instances of maltreatment preceded the ascertainment of ADHD status at Wave 1. Thus, the extent to which ADHD symptoms in such cases were a product of the maltreatment is unclear. ADHD and child maltreatment share a number of risk factors, such as family disorganization and parenting stress, poor parent–child relationships, parental substance abuse and/or mental health issues, and parental characteristics, such as low education, single parenthood, and low income (CDC, 2012b; Johnston, Mash, Miller, & Ninowski, 2012). Moreover, several potential bidirectional or interactive relationships could explain the link between childhood maltreatment and ADHD. ADHD may predispose some children to a greater likelihood of maltreatment, because of ADHD-related interpersonal and self-regulatory problems that can be stressful for parents and potentially precipitate negative parenting practices such as abuse (Deault, 2010; Ford et al., 2000; Glatz, Stattin, & Kerr, 2011). Conversely, however, it is possible that maltreatment may place children at risk for symptomatic profiles paralleling those in ADHD, such as difficulty concentrating and physiological hyperreactivity (Ford et al., 2000). Symptoms of maltreatment, such as hypervigilance or extreme passivity, may sometimes be inappropriately attributed to “attention deficit” (Stirling & Amaya-Jackson, 2008). Evidence from neurobiology and neuroimaging studies demonstrates overlapping brain–behavior pathways observed in ADHD and child maltreatment (Dahmen, Putz, Herpertz-Dahlmann, & Konrad, 2012; Tomoda et al., 2009).

Nevertheless, the present findings have several important clinical and public health implications for girls and young women. Clinicians would profit from consistently assessing childhood maltreatment in patients with suspected or diagnosed ADHD, especially those presenting with considerable comorbid internalizing symptoms. Whereas mental health professionals sometimes lack awareness of the signs of child maltreatment and fail to recognize it (Gilbert et al., 2009), the systematic use of screening tools and specialized training has been shown to increase the detection rate (Carter, Bannon, Limbert, Docherty, & Barlow, 2006; Louwers et al., 2012). The proactive use of screening procedures by clinicians may reveal otherwise undetected child maltreatment in especially vulnerable populations such as girls with ADHD. In addition, considering that treatment approaches for ADHD and childhood trauma differ substantially, screening for maltreatment should help guide therapeutic interventions aimed at individuals with co-occurring ADHD and maltreatment. For instance, medication is typically indicated as a front-line treatment for ADHD (American Academy of American Academy of Pediatrics, 2011; Santosh et al., 2005), whereas psychosocial interventions have been prioritized for treatment of child maltreatment and posttraumatic stress disorder (American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry & Cohen, 1998). Furthermore, compared to their nonmaltreated counterparts, children with histories of maltreatment have been shown to present and respond differently to therapy (Lau & Weisz, 2003), so modifications of existing treatments may be needed in order to address their unique needs. Finally, the findings underscore the importance of providing support not only to the child with ADHD but also to her caregivers, in order to reduce the risk of child maltreatment. Parent interventions for children with both ADHD (e.g., Zwi, Jones, Thorgaard, York, & Dennis, 2011) and maltreatment (e.g., Prinz, Sanders, Shapiro, Whitaker, & Lutzker, 2009) have been shown to mitigate risk factors for maltreatment, such as parental stress.

Because of our conceptual interest in examining correlates of maltreatment in an already high-risk group (i.e., girls/young women with ADHD) and because the small size of our maltreated comparison group reduced statistical power to examine correlates of maltreatment in it, our evaluation of differential predictions from maltreatment to impairments in the ADHD versus comparison samples was exploratory. We found that the direction and effect sizes of predictions to impairment in the comparison group were quite similar to those in the ADHD group (additional data available on request).

Certain limitations of the current study should be considered in interpreting the findings. First, the sample was not designed to be representative of the population, although it may reasonably reflect the demographic nature of ADHD in the San Francisco Bay Area (see Hinshaw, 2002). Second, although the overall retention rate was high (93%), certain measures had more loss of data, and the few participants lost to follow-up were poorer and had marginally lower IQ scores. Third, there may have been misclassification bias, because maltreatment was not directly or systematically queried at the outset of this longitudinal investigation. We determined maltreatment status via chart review, which may have led to false negatives in terms of ascertaining maltreatment status. Furthermore, all forms of information relating to maltreatment (i.e., parent reports, youth self-reports, and official records) are subject to biases and limitations (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005). For instance, parents may underreport maltreatment histories because of shame, denial, and fear of future legal consequences, as well as limited awareness or inaccurate recall of the abusive event (Chan, 2012; Cicchetti & Olsen, 1990). In turn, youth may underreport or minimized abuse because of a sense of loyalty to the perpetrator or psychological processes, such as dissociation, that may hinder recall of the maltreatment event (Chan, 2012). Official records also have been found to underreport maltreatment (Tyler, Smith, & Ellis, 2006), in part because authorities are not privy to all acts of maltreatment. Although any single information source may have been imperfect, we relied whenever possible on multiple sources of information to determine maltreatment status; this multipronged approach tends to yield the most accurate account of maltreatment (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005). Fourth, although the sample is the largest of females with childhood ADHD in existence to our knowledge, the maltreated subsample was of modest size, limiting power to detect more subtle long-term correlates of maltreatment.

In order to isolate the sequelae of maltreatment, we utilized stringent covariates in our analyses. Because, however, of the above-noted timing of maltreatment in relation to ADHD (i.e., many maltreatment instances were contemporaneous with or preceded our Wave 1 ADHD diagnoses), covariates linked to demographics, birth-related circumstances, and childhood comorbidities are an imperfect strategy. Wave 1 comorbidity covariates could represent partial consequences of maltreatment rather than any truly preexisting characteristics. Overall, the selection of covariates required a judicious but imperfect balance between seeking specificity of maltreatment-related impairments versus guarding against statistical overcontrol.

The low frequencies of the “pure” individual maltreatment types made it impossible to examine the separate correlates of physical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect. In addition, important aspects of maltreatment (e.g., severity and chronicity) could not be reliably coded. Moreover, additional traumas such as emotional abuse and exposure to domestic violence were not amenable to reliable coding. Future investigations could analyze the long-term implications of maltreatment on other criterion domains such as neurocognitive functioning, as well as possible mediators of outcome (e.g., social relationships) and protective factors that spur resilient functioning.

Still, few studies have examined the consequences of maltreatment among individuals with ADHD; to our knowledge, this is the first to do so with a longitudinal, all-female sample. The well-characterized, carefully diagnosed, and ethnically and socioeconomically diverse nature of the sample increases the generalizability of the findings, as does the high retention rate (93% over the 10-year interval). In addition, the inclusion of multiple-source, multiple-informant instruments and a broad range of criterion variables enabled examination of the diffuse correlates of maltreatment as well as patterns across symptoms and impairments. Finally, the longitudinal design and stringent application of covariates suggests that the adjusted results reflect the independent correlates of maltreatment rather than related confounds, given the caveats noted above. The use of BH corrections in both the unadjusted and the adjusted analyses militates against the likelihood of Type 1 error.

In summary, although explicit sex-comparison investigations are necessary, some of the serious psychosocial problems experienced by women with ADHD, particularly in terms of internalizing symptomatology and suicide risk, may be in part attributable to experiences with maltreatment. We argue for the need for improved detection of and intervention with childhood abuse and neglect in females with ADHD.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funding from National Institute of Mental Health Grant 45064 (to S.P.H.) and a Berkeley Fellowship (to M.D.G.). We gratefully acknowledge the young women who participated in this longitudinal investigation, their caregivers, and the many graduate students and staff who have made this research possible. We also thank Jarrod Butler, Ronald Sahyouni, Mariah Tate, and Allison Briscoe-Smith.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA adult forms and profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; Burlington, VT: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. Cohen J. Practice parameters for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1998;37:4S–26S. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199810001-00002. doi:10.1097/ 00004583-199810001-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Academy of Pediatrics ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pedatrics. 2011;128:1–16. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2654. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-2654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Author; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  5. Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Bremner JD, Walker JD, Whitfield C, Perry BD, et al. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2006;256:174–186. doi: 10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4. doi:10.1007/s00406-005-0624-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Archer J. Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology. 2004;8:291–322. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291. [Google Scholar]
  7. Babinski DE, Pelham WEJ, Molina BSG, Gnagy EM, Wasch-busch DA, Yu J, et al. Late adolescent and young adult outcomes of girls diagnosed with ADHD in childhood: An exploratory investigation. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2011;15:204–214. doi: 10.1177/1087054710361586. doi:10.1177/1087054710361586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bakeman R, Gottman JM. Assessing observer agreement. In Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. 2nd ed. Cambridge Univeristy Press; New York: 1997. pp. 56–80. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bardone-Cone AM, Maldonado CR, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, Wonderlich SA, Joiner TEJ, et al. Revisiting differences in individuals with bulimia nervosa with and without a history of anorexia nervosa: Eating pathology, personality, and maltreatment. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2008;41:697–704. doi: 10.1002/eat.20567. doi:10.1002/eat.20567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barkley RA. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Guilford Press; New York: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  11. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri WF. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories—IA and —II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1996;67:588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beers SR, De Bellis MD. Neuropsychological function in children with maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:483–486. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.483. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  14. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Monuteaux MC. Differential effect of environmental adversity by gender: Rutter's index of adversity in a group of boys and girls with and without ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:1556–1562. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.9.1556. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.9.1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Biederman J, Petty CR, Monuteaux MC, Fried R, Byrne D, Mirto T, et al. Adult psychiatric outcomes of girls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: 11-year follow-up in a longitudinal case-control study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167:409–417. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050736. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09050736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Biederman J, Petty CR, O'Connor KB, Hyder LL, Faraone SV. Predictors of persistence in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Results from an 11-year controlled follow-up study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2012;125:147–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01797.x. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01797.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Biederman J, Petty CR, Spencer TJ, Woodworth KY, Bhide P, Zhu J, et al. Examining the nature of the comorbidity between pediatric attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2012;128:78–87. doi: 10.1111/acps.12011. doi:10.1111/acps.12011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bird HR. The assessment of functional impairment. In: Shaffer D, Lucas CP, Richters JE, editors. Diagnostic assessment in child and adolescent psychopathology. Guilford Press; New York: 1999. pp. 209–229. [Google Scholar]
  19. Briere J, Jordan CE. Childhood maltreatment, intervening variables, and adult psychological difficulties in women: An overview. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse. 2009;10:375–388. doi: 10.1177/1524838009339757. doi:10.1177/152483800 9339757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Briscoe-Smith AM, Hinshaw SP. Linkages between child abuse and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in girls: Behavioral and social correlates. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2006;30:1239–1255. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.04.008. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Carroll XDP, Yi H, Liang Y, Pang K, Leeper-Woodford S, Riccardi P, et al. Family environmental factors associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Chinese children: A case-control study. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e50543. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Carter YH, Bannon MJ, Limbert C, Docherty A, Barlow J. Improving child protection: A systematic review of training and procedural interventions. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2006;91:740–743. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.092007. doi:10.1136/adc.2005.092007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Casper RC. Depression and eating disorders. Depression and Anxiety. 1998;8:96–104. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1998)8:1+<96::AIDDA15>3.0.CO;2-4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. CDC. Child maltreatment: Facts at a glance. US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Atlanta, GA: 2012a. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/pdf/cm_datasheet2012-a.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  25. CDC. Child maltreatment: Risk and protective factors. US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; Atlanta, GA: 2012b. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/childmaltreatment/riskprotectivefactors.html. [Google Scholar]
  26. Chan KL. Comparison of parent and child reports on child maltreatment in a representative household sample in Hong Kong. Journal of Family Violence. 2012;27:11–21. doi: 10.1007/s10896-011-9405-1. doi:10.1007/s10896-011-9405-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chronis-Tuscano A, Molina SG, Pelham WE, Applegate B, Dahlke A, Overmeyer M, et al. Very early predictors of adolescent depression and suicide attempts in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:1044–1051. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.127. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cicchetti D, Olsen K. The developmental psychopathology of child maltreatment. In: Lewis M, Miller SM, editors. Handbook of developmental psychopathology. Plenum Press; New York: 1990. pp. 261–279. [Google Scholar]
  29. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:597–600. doi:10.1017/S0954579400007318. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. Child maltreatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2005:1,409–438. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144029. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Claes L, Vandereycken W, Vertommen H. Self-injurious behavior in eating-disordered patients. Eating Behaviors. 2001;2:263–272. doi: 10.1016/s1471-0153(01)00033-2. doi:10.1016/S1471-0153(01)00033-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development. 1995;66:710–722. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00900.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00900.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cullerton-Sen C, Cassidy AR, Murray-Close D, Cicchetti D, Crick NR, Rogosch FA. Childhood maltreatment and the development of relational and physical aggression: The importance of a gender-informed approach. Child Development. 2008;79:1236–1751. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01222.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01222.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dahmen B, Putz V, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Early pathogenic care and the development of ADHD-like symptoms. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2012;119:1023–1036. doi: 10.1007/s00702-012-0809-8. doi:10.1007/s00702-012-0809-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Deault LC. A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2010;41:168–192. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0159-4. doi:10.1007/s10578-009-0159-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. De Sanctis VA, Nomura Y, Newcorn JH, Halperin JM. Childhood maltreatment and conduct disorder: Independent predictors of criminal outcomes in ADHD youth. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2012;36:782–789. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.08.003. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. De Sanctis VA, Trampush JW, Harty SC, Marks DJ, Newcorn JH, Miller CJ, et al. Childhood maltreatment and conduct disorder: Independent predictors of adolescent substance use disorders in youth with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2008;37:785–793. doi: 10.1080/15374410802359650. doi:10.1080/15374410802359650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dong M, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, et al. The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2004;28:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dube SR, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Chapman DP, Williamson DF, Giles WH. Childhood abuse, household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: Findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2001;286:3089–3096. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.24.3089. doi:10.1001/jama.286.24.3089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, Chapman DP, Giles WH, Anda RF. Childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction and the risk of illicit drug use: The Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Pediatrics. 2003;111:564–572. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.3.564. doi:10.1542/peds.111.3.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Edwards VJ, Holden GW, Felitti VJ, Anda RF. Relationship between multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health in community respondents: Results from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1453–1460. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1453. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Elliott DS, Huizinga D, Ageton S. Explaining delinquency and drug use. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 1985. [Google Scholar]
  43. Fleiss JL. Statistical methods for rates and proportions. Wiley; New York: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  44. Ford JD, Racusin R, Ellis CG, Daviss WB, Reiser J, Fleischer A, et al. Child maltreatment, other trauma exposure, and posttraumatic symptomatology among children with oppositional defiant and at tention deficit hyperactivity disorders. Child Maltreatment. 2000;5:205–217. doi: 10.1177/1077559500005003001. doi:10.1177/1077559500005003001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Franke B, Faraone SV, Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Bau CHD, Ramos-Quiroga JA, et al. The genetics of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults, a review. Molecular Psychiatry. 2012;17:960–987. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.138. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Galera C, Cote SM, Bouvard MP, Pingault JB, Melchior M, Michel G, et al. Early risk factors for hyperactivity–impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. JAMA Psychiatry. 2011;68:1267–1275. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.138. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Garner DM. Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI-2) 2nd ed. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  48. Garner DM, Garfinkel PE. The Eating Attitudes Test: An index of the symptoms of anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine. 1979;9:273–279. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700030762. doi:10.1017/S0033291700030762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Garner DM, Olmstead MP, Bohr Y, Garfinkel PE. The Eating Attitudes Test: Psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine. 1982;12:871–878. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700049163. doi:10.1017/S0033291700049163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gilbert R, Kemp A, Thoburn J, Sidebotham P, Radford L, Glaser D, et al. Recognising and responding to child maltreatment. Lancet. 2009;373:167–180. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61707-9. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61707-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Glatz T, Stattin H, Kerr M. Parents' reactions to youths' hyper-activity, impulsivity, and attention problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:1125–1135. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9541-3. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9541-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009. 186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gunnar MR, Wenner JA, Thomas KM, Glatt CE, McKenna MC, Clark AG. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism moderates early deprivation effects on attention problems. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24:1215–1223. doi: 10.1017/S095457941200065X. doi:10.1017/S095457941200065X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hamilton JL, Shapero BG, Stange JP, Hamlat EJ, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. Emotional maltreatment, peer victimization, and depressive versus anxiety symptoms during adolescence: Hopelessness as a mediator. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2013;42:332–347. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.777916. doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.777916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Harter S. The Perceived Competence Scale for Children. Child Development. 1982;53:87–97. doi:10.2307/1129640. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Harter S. Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. University of Denver; 1988. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  57. Hinshaw SP. Preadolescent girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I. Background characteristics, comorbidity, cognitive and social functioning, and parenting practices. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:1086–1098. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.5.1086. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.5.1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Sami N, Fargeon S. Prospective follow-up of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adolescence: Evidence for continuing cross-domain impairment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:489–499. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.489. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hinshaw SP, Owens EB, Zalecki C, Huggins SP, Montenegro-Nevado AJ, Schrodek E, et al. Prospective follow-up of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into early adulthood: Continuing impairment includes elevated risk for suicide attempts and self-injury. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80:1041–1051. doi: 10.1037/a0029451. doi:10.1037/a0029451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Hurley JB, Palmer RL, Stretch D. The specificity of the Eating Disorders Inventory: A reappraisal. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1990;9:419–424. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(199007) 9:4<419::AID-EAT2260090408>3.0.CO;2-O. [Google Scholar]
  61. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Mac OS, Microsoft Windows. Version 20.0 [Computer software] IBM Corp.; Armonk, NY: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  62. Jessor R, Donovan JE, Costa FM. Health Behavior Questionnaire. University of Colorado, Institute of Behavioral Science; Boulder, CO: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  63. Johnston C, Mash EJ, Miller N, Ninowski JE. Parenting in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clinical Psychology Review. 2012;32:215–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.007. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Keyes KM, Eaton NR, Krueger RF, McLaughlin KA, Wall MM, Grant BF, et al. Childhood maltreatment and the structure of common psychiatric disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;200:107–115. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.093062. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.111.093062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lansford JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Crozier J, Kaplow J. A 12-year prospective study of the long-term effects of early child physical maltreatment on psychological, behavioral, and academic problems in adolescence. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2002;156:824–830. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.8.824. doi:10.1001/archpedi.156.8.824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Lau AS, Weisz JR. Reported maltreatment among clinic-referred children: Implications for presenting problems, treatment attrition, and long-term outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:1327–1334. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000085754.71002.14. doi:10.1097/01.CHI.0000085754.71002.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Leeb RT, Paulozzi L, Melanson C, Simon T, Arias I. Child maltreatment surveillance: Uniform definitions for public health and recommended data elements, Version 1.0. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control; Atlanta, GA: 2008. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ViolencePrevention/pdf/CM_Surveillance-a.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  68. Louwers EC, Korfage IJ, Affourtit MJ, Scheewe DJ, van de Merwe MH, Vooijs-Moulaert AF, et al. Effects of systematic screening and detection of child abuse in emergency departments. Pediatrics. 2012;130:457–464. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3527. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-3527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. McCrory E, De Brito SA, Viding E. Research review: The neurobiology and genetics of maltreatment and adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:1079–1095. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02271.x. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02271.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Molina BSG, Pelham WE. Childhood predictors of adolescent substance use in a longitudinal study of children with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:497–507. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.497. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. National Household Survey on Drug Abuse . US Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service, and Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse; Washington, DC: 1992. OMB No. 0930-0110. [Google Scholar]
  72. Nigg JT. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and adverse health outcomes. Clinical Psychology Revue. 2013;33:215–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.005. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Nikolas MA, Burt SA. Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:1–17. doi: 10.1037/a0018010. doi:10.1037/a0018010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Nock MK. Why do people hurt themselves? New insights into the nature and functions of self-injury. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18:78–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01613.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01613.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ouyang L, Fang X, Mercy J, Perou R, Grosse SD. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and child maltreatment: A population-based study. Journal of Pediatrics. 2008;153:851–856. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.06.002. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Blanz B, Schmidt MH. Psychosocial adversities underestimated in hyperkinetic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:259–263. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Prinz RJ, Sanders MR, Shapiro CJ, Whitaker DJ, Lutzker JR. Population-based prevention of child maltreatment: The U.S. Triple P System population trial. Prevention Science. 2009;10:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s11121-009-0123-3. doi:10.1007/s11121-009-0123-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Richards LM. It is time for a more integrated bio-psycho-social approach to ADHD. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1177/1359104512458228. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1359104512458228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Santosh PJ, Taylor E, Swanson J, Wigal T, Chuang S, Davies M, et al. Refining the diagnoses of inattention and overactivity syndromes: A reanalysis of the Multimodal Treatment study of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) based on ICD-10 criteria for hyperkinetic disorder. Clinical Neuroscience Research. 2005;5:307–314. doi:10.1016/j.cnr.2005.09.010. [Google Scholar]
  80. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Lucas CP, Dulcan MK, Schwab-Stone ME. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:28–38. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014. doi:10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Silverman AB, Reinherz HZ, Giaconia RM. The long-term sequelae of child and adolescent abuse: A longitudinal community study. Child Abuse and Neglect. 1996;20:709–723. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(96)00059-2. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(96)00059-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Slade EP, Wissow LS. The influence of childhood maltreatment on adolescents' academic performance. Economics of Education Review. 2007;26:604–614. doi: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2006.10.003. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2006.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Stirling JJ, Amaya-Jackson L. Understanding the behavioral and emotional consequences of child abuse. Pediatrics. 2008;122:667–673. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1885. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-1885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sugaya L, Hasin DS, Olfson M, Lin KH, Grant BF, Blanco C. Child physical abuse and adult mental health: A national study. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2012;25:384–392. doi: 10.1002/jts.21719. doi:10.1002/jts.21719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Swanson JM. School-based assessments and interventions for ADD students. K. C. Publishing; Irvine, CA: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  86. Tomoda A, Suzuki H, Rabi K, Sheu YS, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Reduced prefrontal cortical gray matter volume in young adults exposed to harsh corporal punishment. NeuroImage. 2009;47:T66–T71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.005. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Tyler KM, Smith DR, Ellis PM. Child neglect: Developmental consequences, intervention, and policy implications. Child and Youth Care Forum. 2006;35:1–20. doi:10.1007/s10566-005-9000-9. [Google Scholar]
  88. Waxman SE. A systematic review of impulsivity in eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review. 2009;17:408–425. doi: 10.1002/erv.952. doi:10.1002/erv.952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children. 2nd ed. Psychological Corporation; New York: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  90. Wechsler D. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, Second Version. Psychological Corporation; New York: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  91. What Works Clearinghouse What Works Clearinghouse: Procedures and standards handbook (version 2.1) 2008 Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/reference_resources/wwc_procedures_v2_1_standards_handbook.pdf.
  92. Williams VSL, Jones LV, Tukey JW. Controlling error in multiple comparisons with examples from state-to-state differences in educational achievement. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 1999;24:42–69. doi:10.3102/10769986024001042. [Google Scholar]
  93. Zwi M, Jones H, Thorgaard C, York A, Dennis JA. Parent training interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children aged 5 to 18 years. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2011. 2011 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003018.pub3. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003018.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES