Abstract
The network scale-up method is a promising technique that uses sampled social network data to estimate the sizes of epidemiologically important hidden populations, such as sex workers and people who inject illicit drugs. Although previous scale-up research has focused exclusively on networks of acquaintances, we show that the type of personal network about which survey respondents are asked to report is a potentially crucial parameter that researchers are free to vary. This generalization leads to a method that is more flexible and potentially more accurate. In 2011, we conducted a large, nationally representative survey experiment in Rwanda that randomized respondents to report about one of 2 different personal networks. Our results showed that asking respondents for less information can, somewhat surprisingly, produce more accurate size estimates. We also estimated the sizes of 4 key populations at risk for human immunodeficiency virus infection in Rwanda. Our estimates were higher than earlier estimates from Rwanda but lower than international benchmarks. Finally, in this article we develop a new sensitivity analysis framework and use it to assess the possible biases in our estimates. Our design can be customized and extended for other settings, enabling researchers to continue to improve the network scale-up method.
Keywords: acquired immunodeficiency syndrome, epidemiologic methods, HIV, network sampling, population size estimation, social networks, survey research
Many important problems in science and policy require estimates of the sizes of hidden populations. For example, in order to respond to the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)/acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS) epidemic, scientists and policy-makers need information about the sizes of key populations at risk for the disease: men who have sex with men, female sex workers, male clients of female sex workers, and people who inject illicit drugs (1). These size estimates are critical to designing HIV services, evaluating the outcomes of HIV-related interventions, and predicting the future course of the HIV/AIDS epidemic (1–3).
Unfortunately, traditional survey techniques are not well-suited to making accurate size estimates for hidden populations (2, 4). One promising alternative is the network scale-up method, which is based on the idea that ordinary people have embedded within their personal networks information that can be used to estimate the sizes of hidden populations, if that information can be properly collected, aggregated, and adjusted (4–6). The network scale-up method has many advantages over other population size estimation procedures, as has been described in detail elsewhere (4, 7): It can easily be standardized across times and locations; it can be used to concurrently produce estimates for several different hidden populations; it can be partially self-validating because it can easily be applied to populations of known size; it can produce estimates at the national, regional, or city level; it does not require respondents to report that they themselves are members of a stigmatized group; and it can be easily added to existing survey instruments. Because of its appeal, the network scale-up method has been used in studies around the world, and its use is accelerating (see Table 1 of the article by Feehan and Salganik (8)).
The current study makes 3 main contributions. First, we show that the type of network about which respondents are asked to report is a potentially crucial parameter that researchers are free to vary. Previous scale-up studies have asked respondents to report about their network of acquaintances, but there is no reason to think that this particular network will produce optimal or even near-optimal estimates. In order to explore this important issue, in 2011 we conducted a large, nationally representative survey experiment in Rwanda (3). By 1) randomizing respondents to report about one of 2 different types of personal networks and 2) estimating quantities whose true size was known, we were able to show that, somewhat surprisingly, asking respondents for less information can produce more accurate size estimates. The methodology we developed means that our study design can be replicated, customized, and extended in other settings, enabling cumulative improvement of the scale-up method over time.
Second, we use the results of our survey to produce substantively important hidden population size estimates for 4 key populations at increased risk of HIV infection in Rwanda: female sex workers, male clients of female sex workers, men who have sex with men, and people who inject illicit drugs. Sub-Saharan Africa is central to global efforts to combat HIV/AIDS, and Rwanda is an important example of the challenge that many countries in the region face: In 2010, the national prevalence of HIV infection among Rwandan adults was estimated to be 3%, yet little was known about the sizes of key populations at increased risk for HIV (9). Our study demonstrates that use of the scale-up method is feasible in Rwanda and is likely to be feasible in developing countries all over the world.
Third, we build on recent statistical research to introduce a new framework for sensitivity analysis that enables researchers to calculate estimates under different assumptions about potential biases. This framework for sensitivity analysis has been derived from first principles, meaning that it accounts for all of the sources of bias that have been previously discussed (but not resolved) in the scale-up literature; our framework also identifies new potential sources of bias not previously considered. This sensitivity framework can be used in future scale-up studies, regardless of whether they have an embedded experiment. We illustrate the framework by applying it to our estimates of the sizes of 4 key populations at increased risk of HIV infection in Rwanda.
METHODS
The network scale-up method
As Bernard et al. (4) have described elsewhere, network scale-up estimates come from survey data collected from a representative sample of the general population. Respondents are asked about their social connections with people in several hidden populations (e.g., “How many female sex workers do you know?”) and their connections with people in groups of known size (e.g., “How many teachers do you know?”). The responses are combined to produce estimates of the sizes of hidden populations using the basic scale-up estimator (5):
(1) |
where is the estimated size of a hidden population, N is the size of the total population, πi is the probability of inclusion for the ith survey respondent, yi,H is the number of members of the hidden population respondent i reports being connected with, and is the estimated size of the personal network of respondent i (4, 8, 10). The personal network size estimates come from the known population estimator, which is based on the number of connections respondents report having with the groups of known size:
(2) |
where yi,j is the number of people respondent i reports knowing in population j and Nj is the total size of known population j (5). The estimators in equations 1 and 2 will be consistent and unbiased under conditions described elsewhere (8). Intuitively, the basic scale-up estimator (equation 1) is like a sample proportion, but rather than being taken over all respondents, it is taken over all the members of respondents’ personal networks.
Definition of a social tie
In order to use the scale-up method in practice, researchers need to define what it means to be socially connected to another person. These connections are called ties in the social networks literature (11), and they can also be referred to as edges or links.
Almost all previous scale-up surveys have followed the definition of a social tie used in the original scale-up study (12): Two people are considered connected if they both know each other by sight and by name and have been in contact during the past 2 years (see Table 1). However, there is no particular reason to believe that this widely used definition leads to the best possible estimates. In fact, consideration of all possible definitions of a social tie along a continuum from very weak to very strong (13) highlights the fact that choosing a specific tie definition probably induces a trade-off between the quality and quantity of information collected from each respondent.
Table 1.
Acquaintance Definition (n = 2,236) | Meal Definition (n = 2,433) |
---|---|
1. People of all ages who live in Rwanda | 1. People of all ages who live in Rwanda |
2. People the respondent knows, by sight and by name, and who also know the respondent by sight and name | 2. People the respondent knows, by sight and by name, and who also know the respondent by sight and name |
3. People the respondent has had some contact with—either in person, over the phone, or on the computer in the previous 12 months | 3. People the respondent has shared a meal or drink with in the past 12 months, including family members, friends, coworkers, or neighbors, as well as meals or drinks taken at any location, such as at home, at work, or in a restaurant |
In order to understand this trade-off more carefully, it is useful to embed it within the total survey error framework. As with other survey-based methods, error in scale-up estimates can be decomposed into 2 broad categories: sampling error and nonsampling error (14). Sampling error arises from the fact that researchers interview only a sample of people rather than an entire population, and nonsampling error arises from all other sources of error, such as incompleteness in the sampling frame, mistakes in data processing, and inaccuracies in responses (see Groves and Lyberg (14) for a more detailed review). Previous network research leads us to predict that both sampling error and nonsampling error will depend on which definition of a social tie researchers choose for their survey. One set of findings suggests that nonsampling error will vary by tie strength, because people have more accurate information about their strong ties than their weak ties (15–19). A second set of findings suggests that sampling error will vary by tie strength, because people have more weak ties than strong ties (20, 21). Therefore, unless these two forces completely balance, total survey error will be a function of tie strength (see Figure 1 for a possible example).
The possible relationship between tie strength and total error means that by exploring other definitions of social ties, researchers can potentially obtain more accurate estimates at no additional cost. We assessed this possibility empirically by conducting a survey experiment to compare the accuracy of estimates made using 2 different tie definitions.
Survey experiment for evaluating tie definitions
In our survey experiment (3), each household was randomly assigned to one of 2 experimental arms. The questionnaires used in both arms were identical except for the definition of a social tie (Table 1). The first arm's tie definition, which we called the acquaintance definition, was modeled after the one used in all previous scale-up studies (4). The second arm's tie definition, which we called the meal definition, had never been used before. We designed the meal definition, in consultation with Rwandan researchers and policy-makers, so that 1) it would represent a stronger tie than the acquaintance definition and 2) it would be nested within the acquaintance definition, meaning that anyone who was connected to a respondent under the meal definition was also, by definition, connected to the respondent under the acquaintance definition (but not vice versa). We predicted that using the meal definition, we would learn about fewer people in each interview but the information we received would produce better size estimates.
Internal consistency checks for evaluating tie definitions
The information collected about ties to groups of known size (Table 2) makes it possible to check the accuracy of the scale-up estimates for these groups of known size (10), a practice known in the scale-up literature as internal consistency checks (4, 10). In order to do this, for each group of known size we 1) pretend its size is not known; 2) use the remainder of the groups of known size to estimate the respondents’ personal network sizes (equation 2); and 3) apply the scale-up method to estimate the size of the held-out group (equation 1). These internal consistency checks provide a natural method for deciding which of the tie definitions leads to more accurate estimates, at least for the groups of known size.
Table 2.
Group Name | Group Size (No. of Persons) | Source |
---|---|---|
Priests | 1,004 | Catholic Church |
Nurses or physicians | 7,807 | Ministry of Health |
Twahirwa | 10,420 | ID databasea |
Mukandekezi | 10,520 | ID database |
Nyiraneza | 21,705 | ID database |
Male community health workers | 22,000 | Ministry of Health |
Ndayambaje | 22,724 | ID database |
Murekatete | 30,531 | ID database |
Nsengimana | 32,528 | ID database |
Mukandayisenga | 35,055 | ID database |
Widowers | 36,147 | RDHS (2005, 2007, 2010) |
Ndagijimana | 37,375 | ID database |
Bizimana | 38,497 | ID database |
Nyirahabimana | 42,727 | ID database |
Teachers | 47,745 | Ministry of Education |
Nsabimana | 48,560 | ID database |
Divorced men | 50,698 | RDHS (2005, 2007, 2010) |
Mukamana | 51,449 | ID database |
Incarcerated people | 68,000 | ICRC 2010 report |
Women who smoked | 119,438 | RDHS (2005) |
Muslims | 195,449 | RDHS (2005, 2007, 2010) |
Women who gave birth in last 12 months | 256,164 | RDHS (2010) |
Abbreviations: ICRC, International Committee of the Red Cross; ID, identity; RDHS, Rwanda Demographic and Health Survey.
a The ID database denotes groups of names from the Rwandan national identity card database.
Linear blending for size estimates
Our survey produced 2 estimates for the size of each key population, 1 from each tie definition. However, policy-makers typically require a single consensus estimate. Therefore, in Web Appendix 1 (available at http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/), we formally derive a linear blending technique to combine the results from both arms into a single consensus estimate, much like a meta-analysis (22). We consider all possible combinations of the estimates with the form
(3) |
Web Appendix 1 proves that, under the assumption that the estimators from each experimental arm are unbiased and uncorrelated, the optimal linear blending weight is
(4) |
where is the sampling variance for the acquaintance definition estimate and is the sampling variance for the meal definition estimate. In narrative terms, equation 4 says that the larger the variance of the acquaintance definition's estimate relative to the variance of the meal definition's estimate, the more weight the meal definition estimate gets in the blended estimate. In practice, we use equation 4 by plugging in sample-based estimates of and
Framework for sensitivity analysis
There is a large body of literature describing many potential sources of bias in the basic scale-up estimator (4, 8, 23–29). Therefore, following a long tradition in epidemiology (30–32), we develop a framework for sensitivity analysis below. Our framework allows other researchers to calculate estimates under different assumptions about possible biases.
Using results developed by Feehan and Salganik (8), we consider the multiplicative bias of the basic scale-up estimator:
(5) |
where α is an overall adjustment factor that captures all biases in the basic scale-up estimator. Web Appendix 2 shows that α can be written as the product of 4 quantities:
(6) |
Each of the 4 adjustment factors is defined precisely in Web Appendix 2 and could potentially be estimated empirically. Together, the 4 adjustment factors account for the nonsampling errors discussed in the scale-up literature. In previous studies, investigators who have reported estimates from the basic scale-up estimators have implicitly assumed that the product of these 4 quantities is 1.
The 4 terms in equation 6 can be divided into 2 groups: reporting terms (ηF and τF), which summarize the accuracy of respondents' reports, and structural terms (δF and φF), which summarize the differences between the hidden population, the survey respondents, and the entire population. The precision, ηF, quantifies respondents' tendency to give false-positive reports (e.g., if respondents report that some members of their personal network are sex workers when they are not). On the other hand, the true positive rate, τF, quantifies respondents' tendency to give true-positive reports (e.g., if respondents are connected to sex workers and able to report this correctly). In general, we would expect the true positive rate to be less than 1, because members of hidden populations might attempt to keep this information secret from other people. The degree ratio, δF, quantifies whether members of hidden populations tend to have smaller personal networks than the survey respondents (e.g., if the average sex worker has fewer connections to adults than the average adult). Finally, the frame ratio, φF, quantifies whether survey respondents (e.g., adults) tend to have larger personal networks than the entire population (e.g., adults and children).
In addition to assessing the sensitivity of our estimates, our sensitivity framework (equation 6) enables our estimates to be improved over time as more is learned. If additional studies are conducted to estimate any of the quantities in equation 6 (ηF, φF, δF, and τF) (7, 8, 33), then, using the information given in Web Tables 1 and 2 and the procedure shown in Web Appendix 2, these new estimates can be combined with the results from our study to produce improved estimates of the sizes of hidden populations that are less dependent on assumptions about unobserved quantities.
Data collection and processing
To conduct our survey experiment, we collected original data using the same survey infrastructure as the 2010 Rwanda Demographic and Health Survey (3). The Demographic and Health Surveys Program, funded by the US Agency for International Development, is one of the largest and most widely used sources of reliable information about international health. Since 1984, the Demographic and Health Surveys Program has conducted over 260 surveys in more than 85 developing countries, and it has well-established protocols for developing questionnaires, training interviewers, supervising interviews, and processing data and for overall supervision of household-based surveys (34, 35). By using the infrastructure of the Demographic and Health Surveys Program, we ensured that our research design can be used in face-to-face surveys in developing countries around the world.
Our sample was drawn from the preparatory frame constructed for the 2012 Rwanda Census, which contained a complete list of 14,837 villages, which are the smallest administrative units in the country. We used a stratified, 2-stage cluster design with these villages as the primary sampling units.
We conducted interviews with 4,669 respondents from 2,125 households in 130 villages (household response rate: 99%; individual response rate: 97%) (3). All household members aged 15 years or more were interviewed in each selected household. Eight survey teams—each consisting of 1 supervisor, 2 male interviewers, and 2 female interviewers—conducted these interviews between June and August of 2011. Upon arriving in each sampled village, the survey team first updated the list of households. A number was assigned to each household in the updated listing, and the supervisor used a randomly pregenerated table to select households and assign them to tie definitions. Balance checks suggested that our randomization procedure was implemented according to the study design (Web Appendix 3, Web Table 3, and Web Figure 1). All interviews were conducted face-to-face in Kinyarwanda (the official language of Rwanda) in the respondent's home. All responses were recorded on paper forms, and data were entered twice using CSPro software (US Census Bureau, Washington, DC). Table 2 shows the definitions and data sources of the 22 known populations that we used to estimate the sizes of respondents' personal networks (equation 2). Full details on the sampling plan and the survey instrument have been provided elsewhere (3).
The survey protocol, including questionnaires and other instruments, was reviewed and approved by the Rwanda National Ethics Committee (Kigali, Rwanda), the Rwanda National Institute of Statistics (Kigali, Rwanda), and the institutional review boards of ICF International (Fairfax, Virginia), Princeton University (Princeton, New Jersey), the International Center for Research on Women (Washington, DC), and the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (Atlanta, Georgia). All participants were informed about their free choice to participate and their right to withdraw at any time during the study. Interviewers secured written consent from all respondents before the interview.
When analyzing our data, consistent with common scale-up practice, we truncated extreme outliers by top-coding all responses about connections with groups of known or unknown size at 30 (29), which affected 0.2% of responses. We used the rescaled bootstrap technique of Rao and Wu (36) to produce estimates of sampling uncertainty that accounted for the complex sample design (8) (see Web Appendix 4 and Web Figure 2). Unless otherwise noted, all estimates were produced using weights to account for the complex sample design. All of our calculations were done in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria) (37), using the following packages: networkreporting (38), plyr (39), ggplot2 (40), stringr (41), stargazer (42), RItools (43), and car (44). Our data set and a copy of the survey instrument are freely available for download (3), and code with which to replicate our analyses is available online (https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/CCC6HF).
RESULTS
For each of the 22 groups of known size, respondents to the meal definition reported fewer average connections, indicating that our survey experiment manipulated responses (Web Figure 1). Using these reports to estimate the average size of respondents' personal networks (equation 2) showed that the meal definition elicited information about 60% fewer people than the acquaintance definition: The estimated mean network size for the meal definition was 108 persons (95% confidence interval: 99, 118), while for the acquaintance definition it was 251 persons (95% confidence interval: 237, 267) (Figure 2).
Internal consistency checks
The internal consistency checks, which compared the performance of both tie definitions in estimating the sizes of known populations, showed that in most cases, the estimates were close to the true values (Figure 3). However, there were a few exceptions: The most notable ones were teachers (whose size was overestimated), Muslims (whose size was underestimated), and women who gave birth in the past 12 months (whose size was underestimated). On average, across the 22 groups of known size, estimates derived from the meal definition had lower mean squared errors than estimates from the acquaintance definition (2-sided P < 0.001)—a result that was robust to the specific error metric used (Web Figure 3).
Size estimates for hidden populations and sensitivity analysis
Figure 4 shows the estimated sizes of 4 key populations at risk for HIV infection in Rwanda: male clients of female sex workers, female sex workers, men who have sex with men, and people who inject illicit drugs. As described in more detail in the Discussion, these estimates were consistently higher than earlier estimates in Rwanda, but they were generally lower than international benchmarks.
In the absence of empirical evidence about the magnitude of possible biases in the basic scale-up estimator, Figure 4 assumes that the estimates from each tie definition are unbiased (α = 1). In Figure 5, we relax this assumption using our framework for sensitivity analysis to show how the blended estimate depends on the assumed multiplicative bias (α) for each hidden population. For example, if we considered the estimate for the number of female sex workers, and if the meal estimator were unbiased (α = 1) but the acquaintance estimator tended to be too small (α = 1.5), panel A of Figure 5 shows that the blended size estimate should be about 40,000, which is higher than the estimate obtained under the assumption that both arms are unbiased. In general, Figure 5 shows that higher values of α lead to higher estimates. However, the exact nature of the relationship between assumed values for α and size estimates depends on the sampling variance of the estimator from each arm. Web Appendix 2 shows how researchers can use our sensitivity framework to combine our data with any set of assumptions about ηF, φF, δF, and τF to produce adjusted estimates (see Web Tables 1 and 2 for an example).
DISCUSSION
Findings
Our survey experiment demonstrated that the definition of a social tie which respondents are asked to report about is a potentially critical parameter that researchers can vary. There appears to be a trade-off between quantity and quality of network reports: For the groups of known size, the internal consistency checks revealed that the meal definition outperformed the acquaintance definition that has been used in all previous scale-up studies. These results show that learning about fewer people can, somewhat surprisingly, lead to more accurate estimates. Our study, therefore, makes the clear and falsifiable prediction that future scale-up studies will produce more accurate estimates using stronger definitions of social ties.
We also used our survey to estimate the sizes of 4 populations at risk for HIV infection in Rwanda. Our blended scale-up estimates were consistently higher than earlier estimates from Rwanda made using other methods (Figure 4). For female sex workers, our estimate was higher than 3 earlier estimates—1 made using mapping (3), 1 made using enumeration (45), and 1 using capture-recapture (3). Our estimate for the number of male clients of female sex workers was higher than an earlier direct estimate (3, 45, 46). To our knowledge, there have been no previous estimates of the numbers of people who inject drugs and men who have sex with men in Rwanda. On the other hand, our estimates are comparable to or lower than benchmark estimates from the Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS) (47), which were derived from published literature on population size estimates from around the world. Our estimated number of female sex workers was within the range given by the UNAIDS benchmarks. However, our estimates for the number of male clients of female sex workers and the number of injecting drug users were both lower than the UNAIDS benchmarks. There are no UNAIDS benchmarks available for men who have sex with men in Africa. Additional details about these comparisons are presented in Web Appendix 5 (also see Web Tables 4–7 and Web Figure 4).
Finally, we have introduced a framework for sensitivity analysis that enables scale-up researchers to calculate estimates under different assumptions about possible biases. Our framework is derived from first principles, enabling it to account for all of the sources of potential bias, only some of which were previously discussed in the scale-up literature. We have demonstrated the framework in action by assessing the sensitivity of our basic scale-up estimates for the sizes of 4 populations at risk of HIV infection in Rwanda. Web Appendix 2 contains a step-by-step guide to using the framework for sensitivity analysis, which can be applied in other studies regardless of whether a tie definition experiment has been conducted.
Limitations
Our study had several important limitations. The fact that the meal definition worked better than the acquaintance definition on average across the 22 groups of known size in Rwanda does not guarantee that it will work better for hidden populations in Rwanda or hidden populations in other countries.
Further, we do not claim that the meal definition is optimal. In fact, we hope future research explores a wider range of tie definitions with the goal of developing an empirical understanding of the general relationship between tie strength and total survey error. We recommend that future studies explore tie definitions from a parameterized family that is sufficiently flexible for use in many different countries. For example, the meal and acquaintance tie definitions are members of a 2-parameter family, where 1 parameter is an interaction type (e.g., had some contact, shared a meal or drink) and 1 parameter is a time window (e.g., in the past year). By focusing on a parameterized family of tie definitions, it will be possible to compare and combine results from different studies, enabling knowledge about the method to build up as more scale-up studies are conducted.
Empirically, our blended estimates (Figure 4) depended on the assumption that the basic scale-up estimator was unbiased in this setting. However, our framework for sensitivity analysis shows that different plausible assumptions about the bias can lead to a wide range of possible estimates (Figure 5). Therefore, we recommend that in future studies investigators attempt to measure these possible biases directly (e.g., see Salganik et al. (33) and Maghsoudi et al. (48)) so that estimates are less dependent on assumptions.
Conclusion
Estimating the sizes of hidden populations such as sex workers and people who inject drugs is a critical problem in many settings. The network scale-up method is a promising approach, and in this paper we show that it is more flexible and potentially more accurate than had been previously realized. Further, the methodology we developed and deployed—a survey experiment with blending (Figure 6)—provides a general template that future researchers can replicate, customize, and extend. If this design becomes standard, future studies will provide both estimates about specific hidden populations and more general insights that will lead to cumulative methodological improvement.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Author affiliations: Office of Population Research, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey (Dennis M. Feehan, Matthew J. Salganik); Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, College of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Rwanda, Kigali, Rwanda (Aline Umubyeyi); Strategic Information and Evaluation Department, Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS, Geneva, Switzerland (Mary Mahy); Division of Global HIV and TB, Center for Global Health, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia (Wolfgang Hladik); and Department of Sociology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey (Matthew J. Salganik).
The study “Estimating the Size of Populations through a Household Survey” (2011 ESPHS) (3) was conducted in Rwanda from June 2011 to August 2011 by the School of Public Health, University of Rwanda, in collaboration with the Rwanda Biomedical Center/Institute of HIV/AIDS, Disease Prevention and Control Department, with the assistance of the National Institute of Statistics of Rwanda. Technical assistance and funding for the project was provided by the Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS and ICF International (Calverton, Maryland) through the MEASURE Demographic and Health Surveys Program, a US Agency for International Development (USAID)-funded project providing support and technical assistance in the implementation of population and health surveys in countries worldwide. Additional assistance was provided by the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Princeton University, and the University of Florida. The government of Japan, the United Nations Joint Team on HIV of Kigali, Rwanda, and USAID provided additional funding. The research was also supported by grants to M.J.S. from the US National Science Foundation (grant CNS-0905086) and the US National Institutes of Health (grants R01-HD062366, R01-HD075666, and R24-HD047879).
We thank Bernard Barrere and Dr. Patrick Ndimubanzi for helping to design and conduct the study, and we thank Drs. Drew Baughman, Steve Gutreuter, Meade Morgan, Russ Bernard, Chris McCarty, and Noreen Goldman for valuable conversations and comments.
The opinions expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the government of Rwanda, the funding agencies, or the collaborating organizations.
Some of this work was performed while M.J.S. was an employee of Microsoft Research (New York, New York).
REFERENCES
- 1.Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS; WHO Working Group on Global HIV/AIDS and STI Surveillance. Guidelines on Estimating the Size of Populations Most At Risk to HIV. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS; WHO Working Group on Global HIV/AIDS and STI Surveillance. Guidelines on Surveillance Among Populations Most At Risk for HIV. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rwanda Biomedical Center/Institute of HIV/AIDS, Disease Prevention and Control Department; School of Public Health, University of Rwanda; Joint United Nations Programme on HIVAIDS et al. Estimating the Size of Populations Through a Household Survey (ESPHS)—Rwanda 2011. Calverton, MD: ICF International; 2012. http://www.dhsprogram.com/pubs/pdf/FR261/FR261.pdf. Accessed August 9, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bernard HR, Hallett T, Iovita A et al. . Counting hard-to-count populations: the network scale-up method for public health. Sex Transm Infect. 2010;86(suppl 2):ii11–ii15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Killworth PD, McCarty C, Bernard HR et al. . Estimation of seroprevalence, rape, and homelessness in the United States using a social network approach. Eval Rev. 1998;222:289–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bernard HR, Johnsen EC, Killworth PD et al. . Estimating the size of an average personal network and of an event subpopulation. In: Kochen M, ed. The Small World. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation; 1989:159–175. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Salganik MJ, Fazito D, Bertoni N et al. . Assessing network scale-up estimates for groups most at risk of HIV/AIDS: evidence from a multiple-method study of heavy drug users in Curitiba, Brazil. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;17410:1190–1196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Feehan DM, Salganik MJ. Generalizing the network scale-up method: a new estimator for the size of hidden populations. Sociol Methodol. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS. Global Report: UNAIDS Report on the Global AIDS Epidemic 2013. Geneva, Switzerland: Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Killworth PD, Johnsen EC, McCarty C et al. . A social network approach to estimating seroprevalence in the United States. Soc Netw. 1998;201:23–50. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wasserman S, Faust K. Social Network Analysis. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bernard HR, Johnsen EC, Killworth PD et al. . Estimating the size of an average personal network and of an event subpopulation. In: Kochen M, ed. The Small World. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation; 1989:159–175. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Granovetter M. The strength of weak ties. Am J Sociol. 1973;786:1360–1380. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Groves RM, Lyberg L. Total survey error: past, present, and future. Public Opin Q. 2010;745:849–879. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Marsden PV. Network data and measurement. Annu Rev Sociol. 1990;16:435–463. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Marin A. Are respondents more likely to list alters with certain characteristics? Implications for name generator data. Soc Netw. 2004;264:289–307. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brewer DD. Forgetting in the recall-based elicitation of personal and social networks. Soc Netw. 2000;221:29–43. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brewer DD, Webster CM. Forgetting of friends and its effects on measuring friendship networks. Soc Netw. 1999;214:361–373. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sudman S. Experiments in measuring neighbor and relative social networks. Soc Netw. 1988;101:93–108. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Goel S, Mason W, Watts DJ. Real and perceived attitude agreement in social networks. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;994:611–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.DiPrete TA, Gelman A, McCormick T et al. . Segregation in social networks based on acquaintanceship and trust. Am J Sociol. 2011;1164:1234–1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. 1st ed Orlando, FL: Academic Press, Inc.; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Killworth PD, McCarty C, Bernard HR et al. . Two interpretations of reports of knowledge of subpopulation sizes. Soc Netw. 2003;252:141–160. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kadushin C, Killworth PD, Bernard HR et al. . Scale-up methods as applied to estimates of heroin use. J Drug Issues. 2006;362:417–440. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Maltiel R, Raftery AE, McCormick TH. Estimating population size using the network scale up method. Ann Appl Stat. 2015;93:1247–1277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.McCormick TH, Zheng T. Adjusting for recall bias in “How many X's do you know?” surveys. New York, NY: Columbia University; 2007. http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~tyler/pubs/mccormick_recall.pdf. Accessed March 3, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Killworth PD, McCarty C, Johnsen EC et al. . Investigating the variation of personal network size under unknown error conditions. Sociol Methods Res. 2006;351:84–112. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Johnsen EC, Bernard HR, Killworth PD et al. . A social network approach to corroborating the number of AIDS/HIV+ victims in the US. Soc Netw. 1995;17(3-4):167–187. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zheng T, Salganik MJ, Gelman A. How many people do you know in prison? Using overdispersion in count data to estimate social structure in networks. J Am Stat Assoc. 2006;101474:409–423. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Greenland S. Basic methods for sensitivity analysis of biases. Int J Epidemiol. 1996;256:1107–1116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Greenland S. The impact of prior distributions for uncontrolled confounding and response bias: a case study of the relation of wire codes and magnetic fields to childhood leukemia. J Am Stat Assoc. 2003;98461:47–54. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Greenland S. Multiple-bias modelling for analysis of observational data. J R Stat Soc Ser A Stat Soc. 2005;1682:267–306. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Salganik MJ, Mello MB, Abdo AH et al. . The game of contacts: estimating the social visibility of groups. Soc Netw. 2011;331:70–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Short Fabic M, Choi Y, Bird S. A systematic review of Demographic and Health Surveys: data availability and utilization for research. Bull World Health Organ. 2012;908:604–612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Corsi DJ, Neuman M, Finlay JE et al. . Demographic and Health Surveys: a profile. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;416:1602–1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rao JN, Wu CFJ. Resampling inference with complex survey data. J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83401:231–241. [Google Scholar]
- 37.R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. http://www.R-project.org/. Accessed March 3, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Feehan DM, Salganik MJ. The networkreporting package. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. http://cran.r-project.org/package=networkreporting. Accessed March 3, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wickham H. The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. J Stat Softw. 2011;401:1–29. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wickham H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wickham H. stringr: Make it easier to work with strings. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=stringr. Accessed August 9, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hlavac M. stargazer: LaTeX code and ASCII text for well-formatted regression and summary statistics tables. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=stargazer. Accessed August 9, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bowers J, Fredrickson M, Hansen B. RItools: randomization inference tools. (R Package, Version 01-11) Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2010. http://www.jakebowers.org/RItools.html. Accessed June 2, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fox J, Weisberg S. An R Companion to Applied Regression. 2nd ed Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2011. http://socserv.socsci.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion. Accessed August 9, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mutagoma M, Kayitesi C, Gwiza A et al. . Estimation of the size of the female sex worker population in Rwanda using three different methods. Int J STD AIDS. 2015;2611:810–814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rwanda Biomedical Center. Behavioral and biological surveillance survey among female sex workers. Kigali, Rwanda: Center for Treatment and Research on AIDS, Malaria, Tuberculosis and Other Epidemics, Rwanda Ministry of Health; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS. Quick Start Guide for Spectrum. Geneva, Switzerland: Joint United Nations Programme on HIV/AIDS; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Maghsoudi A, Baneshi MR, Neydavoodi M et al. . Network scale-up correction factors for population size estimation of people who inject drugs and female sex workers in Iran. PLoS One. 2014;911:e110917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.