Abstract
Current gold standard treatments for eating disorders (EDs) lack satisfactory efficacy, and traditional psychological treatments do not directly address executive functioning deficits underpinning ED pathology. The goal of this paper is to explore the potential for enhancing ED treatment outcomes by improving executive functioning deficits that have been demonstrated to underlie eating pathology. To achieve our objective, we (1) review existing evidence for executive functioning deficits that underpin EDs and consider the extent to which these deficits could be targeted in neurocognitive training programs, (2) present the evidence for the one ED neurocognitive training program well-studied to date (Cognitive Remediation Therapy), (3) discuss the utility of neurocognitive training programs that have been developed for other psychiatric disorders with similar deficits, and (4) provide suggestions for the future development and research of neurocognitive training programs for EDs. Despite the fact that the body of empirical work on neurocognitive training programs for eating disorders is very small, we conclude that their potential is high given the combined evidence for the role of deficits in executive functioning in EDs, the initial promise of Cognitive Remediation Training, and the success in treating related conditions with neurocognitive training. Based on the evidence to date, it appears that the development and empirical evaluation of neurocognitive training programs for EDs is warranted.
Keywords: Neurocognitive training, Inhibitory control training, Eating disorders
Current treatments for eating disorders
Eating disorders (EDs) are serious psychiatric illnesses and the efficacy of existing cognitive behavioral treatments is unsatisfactory (Brownley et al., 2007; Bulik et al., 2007; Shapiro et al., 2007). Innovative treatment methods may be needed to improve outcomes. Anorexia nervosa (AN) has the highest mortality rate of all psychiatric conditions (Birmingham et al., 2005), but the efficacy of existing treatments for adults with AN is limited (Carter et al., 2011; Zipfel et al., 2014). Family-based treatment for adolescents with AN is considered an effective treatment, though its efficacy is moderate at best, with a large percentage of adolescents showing continued symptoms after a full dose of treatment (APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice, 2006). Generally, AN patients are often resistant to beginning treatment (Serpell et al., 1999; Vitousek, Watson, & Wilson, 1998), have poor treatment adherence and acceptance (Halmi et al., 2005), and commonly drop out of treatment (Halmi et al., 2005; Kahn & Pike, 2001; Mahon, 2000; Surgenor, Maguire, & Beumont, 2004). Treatments for bulimia nervosa (BN) and binge eating disorder (BED) are more effective than treatments for AN, but are well short of satisfactory (Kass, Kolko, & Wilfley, 2013). Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) for BN and BED, including an enhanced, transdiagnostic version, Cognitive Behavioral Therapy-Enhanced (CBT-E), demonstrates the best outcomes to date for these disorders (Byrne et al., 2011; Excellence, N.I.f.C., 2011; Fairburn et al., 2009; Hay et al., 2009; Shapiro et al., 2007; Wonderlich et al., 2013). Interpersonal Psychotherapy (IPT) for BN also has strong empirical support, but has consistently yielded outcomes that are comparable or slightly worse than those of CBT-E (Spielmans et al., 2013). However, one of the most comprehensive and recent studies of CBT-E found that by the end of treatment, only 38.6% of patients with BN met remission criteria and by 60-weeks follow-up, 45.6% met remission criteria (Byrne et al., 2011). Although CBT and IPT are relatively effective treatments for BN, the fact that over 50% of patients are partially or fully symptomatic after CBT-E suggests additional room for improvement (Fairburn et al., 2009).
Aims of the current paper
Deficits in neurocognitive functioning (described in greater detail below) have been hypothesized to contribute to the development and maintenance of eating pathology. Addressing these deficits may be one method for improving existing treatment of EDs. This paper will explore the potential for enhancing treatment outcomes through the training of neurocognitive deficits that appear to underlie eating pathology. First, we will briefly review existing evidence for executive functioning deficits that underpin EDs, and briefly present the evidence for the one ED neurocognitive training program well-studied to date (Cognitive Remediation Therapy). We will then (1) discuss the utility of neurocognitive training programs that have been developed for other psychiatric disorders with similar deficits, and (2) provide suggestions for the future development and research of neurocognitive training programs for EDs.
Neurocognitive deficits in eating disorders
Many of the observed neurocognitive deficits implicated in EDs occur in the area of executive function (EF; Kanakam & Treasure, 2013; van Elburg & Treasure, 2013; Van den Eynde et al., 2011). EF is an umbrella term that refers to a set of neuropsychological processes (primarily centered in prefrontal regions) that govern higher-level, goal-directed behavior (Miyake et al., 2000). A set of meta-analyses (Lang et al., 2014; Lopez et al., 2008; Roberts et al., 2007; Wu et al., 2013, 2014) provide strong evidence that deficits in set-shifting (i.e., difficulty flexibly adjusting behavior and cognitions in order to achieve goals in accordance with changing rules or situational demands; Roberts, Tchanturia, & Treasure, 2010; Roberts et al., 2007; Tchanturia et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2014), central coherence (i.e., biased emphasis on small details and a weak ability to integrate information into a gestalt; Lopez et al., 2009), and working memory (i.e., the inability to temporarily store and attend to goal-relevant information, while ignoring distracting or irrelevant informational inputs; Duchesne et al., 2010; Svaldi, Brand, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2010; Zakzanis, Campbell, & Polsinelli, 2010) are present across ED diagnostic groups, and that the magnitude of impairment is similar among AN and BN subtypes (Lang et al., 2014; Roberts et al., 2007). Additionally, poor inhibitory control (i.e., the ability to inhibit a prepotent response) has been observed in EDs characterized by binge eating (Fischer, Smith, & Anderson, 2003; Rosval et al., 2006; Wu et al., 2013). Converging evidence that deficits in EF are present in adolescents who have a short duration of ED illness (Darcy et al., 2012, 2014; Fitzpatrick et al., 2012), and that deficits remain following symptom remission (Holliday et al., 2005; Lopez et al., 2009; Roberts et al., 2007, 2010), suggest that these traits could be causal rather than consequential (though there is likely to be some degree of bi-directionality) (Kanakam & Treasure, 2013).
Most existing ED therapies do not directly address potential EF maintenance factors, and thus perhaps miss a vital treatment target. Moreover, deficits in EF may interfere with the ability to successfully engage in and benefit from behavioral treatment (Fowler et al., 2006; Lena & Fiocco, 2004; Svaldi et al., 2010). Experts in the field have recently called for the development of treatments that directly target relevant maintenance factors (such as EF) that are not addressed in CBT, with the hope of improving treatment outcomes (Wonderlich, 2013). Neurocognitive training, described below, is one possible adjunctive treatment method to target EF, which has the potential to enhance outcomes for EDs. The degree to which traditional therapies such as CBT affect EF is unknown, although this is an area ripe for future investigation. It should be noted that other treatment approaches, such as repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS) and direct current stimulation (Grall-Bronnec & Sauvaget, 2014), can target brain regions subserving EF (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and may be viable methods for targeting EF deficits. These interventions represent potential treatments for EDs, but are somewhat different from the training approaches described in the remainder of the article in that they noninvasively deliver stimulation to small regions of the brain and cannot be defined as training approaches. For the remainder of the paper we focus specifically on neurocognitive training paradigms, although additional research on alternative approaches for improving EF in EDs may suggest other viable options.
Neurocognitive training for related conditions
A growing body of work has demonstrated that psychiatric illness is associated with dysfunction across prefrontal, fronto-limbic, and fronto-striatal neural systems (Vinogradov, Fisher, & de Villers-Sidani, 2011). These systems are associated with a diverse range of cognitive functioning including perception, cognition, social interactions, emotion regulation, and motivation (Eisenberg & Berman, 2010; Hartley & Phelps, 2010; Koob & Volkow, 2010; Price & Drevets, 2010; Vinogradov, Fisher, & Nagarajan, 2013). The plasticity observed in neural circuitry across the lifespan, combined with alterations in neural functioning associated with an individual's specific learning history, supports the assumption that brain functions can be trained (Vinogradov et al., 2013). The associations among dysfunction in neural systems, deficits in neurocognition, maladaptive behaviors, and cognitions have prompted researchers to examine whether interventions that improve neural functioning can result in improvements in psychiatric symptoms.
Although preliminary, initial reviews suggest that neurocognitive training can produce alterations in brain regions, neural circuitry, and behaviors, at least for behaviors similar to the training paradigm and potentially to more extended real-world behaviors (Vinogradov et al., 2013). Cognitive remediation emerged as a possible adjunct to standard treatments for schizophrenia over 50 years ago, and reviews assessing a wide variety of rehabilitation approaches have documented a moderate effect size of d = 0.41 for cognitive improvement and d = 0.36 for functional outcome, providing support for the efficacy of this approach in psychiatric populations (McGurk et al., 2007). Despite a strong rationale for investigating neurocognitive training in other disorders with observed neuropsychological deficits, neurocognitive training has only recently been investigated in disorders beyond schizophrenia. At this time, the only existing direct EF training approach that has been tested with EDs is Cognitive Remediation Therapy (CRT).
Cognitive remediation therapy for AN
At this time, most research on CRT has focused solely on AN, which is briefly discussed below.
Description of CRT
CRT is not designed as a stand-alone treatment for AN (i.e., its aim is not on weight gain or improving disordered eating cognitions); rather, it is designed as an adjunctive treatment, with the aim of (1) encouraging retention in targeted AN treatment by adding an engaging and interactive therapy component, and (2) decreasing cognitive rigidity in the hope of facilitating better utilization of skills provided in traditional therapies. CRT is an in-person intervention that aims to increase cognitive flexibility by identifying problems with inflexible thinking and practicing tasks meant to increase cognitive and behavioral flexibility (Easter & Tchanturia, 2011). With the exception of one study that examined CRT in the form of 21 sessions of computerized set-shifting trainings in conjunction with nine in-person sessions (Brockmeyer et al., 2014), CRT has typically been delivered for 8–10 weekly in-person sessions, and is conducted either individually or in group format. ED-related behaviors are not directly discussed, as the content of the group focuses on cognitive process and not on the specific content of cognitions. (The format and content of CRT has been comprehensively reviewed elsewhere; cf., Tchanturia, 2014.)
Preliminary data for CRT
At this time, 14 studies have investigated the use of CRT for the treatment of AN (see reviews by Tchanturia and colleagues; Tchanturia, Lloyd, & Lang, 2013; Tchanturia, Lounes, & Holttum, 2014). Overall, CRT has demonstrated acceptability and feasibility for use in both inpatient and outpatient settings (Easter & Tchanturia, 2011; Tchanturia et al., 2013) and dropout rates range from 10 to 15%, as compared to approximately 60% for most treatment trials for AN (Tchanturia et al., 2013). Although results are mixed, several studies have revealed medium-to-large effect sizes in improvements in cognitive flexibility from pre- to post-treatment (Brockmeyer et al., 2014; Lock et al., 2013; Tchanturia et al., 2013). However, there is minimal support for the long-term transfer of such improvements in cognitive flexibility to symptom improvement (Lock et al., 2013). In addition, formal assessment of the putative mediators (set-shifting and central coherence) of CRT has yet to be formally examined.
Limitations of existing research on CRT
Although this research provides initial promise for CRT as an acceptable treatment for AN, several limitations exist in the current literature. First, there is a notable lack of a control group in most studies, which raises concerns regarding confounds (e.g., expectancy effects, regression to the mean) in addition to the critical question of whether improvements in cognitive flexibility may be attributable to practice effects (both from baseline assessment and tasks throughout the course of CRT) rather than true improvements in cognitive flexibility. Also, because most studies of CRT have been conducted in an inpatient setting, it is possible that improvements in cognitive flexibility could be attributable to weight gain achieved through treatment, as starvation/malnourishment can cause impairments in brain function and neuropsychological performance (McCormick et al., 2008). One study of the existing 14 on CRT included computerized set-shifting trainings as part of the CRT training program (Brockmeyer et al., 2014), but otherwise the adaptations on the traditional in-person format and frequency of CRT have been minimal.
Also unknown is whether improvements in cognitive flexibility achieved through CRT persists over time. Some of the initial work has failed to demonstrate long-term improvements in neurocognitive performance (Tchanturia et al., 2013). Additionally, further investigation is needed to determine how best to integrate CRT with other interventions and to understand whether integration produces a synergistic effect. Finally, research on CRT for ED has generally been limited to AN samples, with little work having been conducted with BN and BED.
Extending neurocognitive training paradigms for treatment of conditions related to ED
Although CRT shows promise as an adjunctive treatment for ED, the current paradigm targets only cognitive flexibility. As we have discussed, EF deficits in EDs also include poor inhibitory control and working memory. Training in these domains has shown promise in the treatment of other psychiatric disorders (Kurtz et al., 2007) and in populations who experience subclinical eating problems (Houben & Jansen, 2011; Veling, Aarts, & Papies, 2011). Of note, most of the programs described below are different from most investigations of CRT in that that they involve automated, computer-based, high-frequency practice sessions. These trainings require repeated and rapid practice of tasks that target underlying implicit processes involved in executive control. Extensive evidence exists to indicate that intensive and frequent drill-and-practice sessions (delivered by a computer) are the most effective at improving deficits, particularly when administered for several weeks (Vinogradov et al., 2011). To date, computer-assisted cognitive remediation treatments have yielded little clinical or functional benefit to patients without concurrent functional or behavioral skills training (Bowie et al., 2012; Spierer, Chavan, & Manuel, 2013). However, based on two meta-analyses of general cognitive remediation training for schizophrenia (which included studies designed to address multiple deficits and computer-assisted training studies), computer-based training can augment the effects of behavioral therapy when the two modalities are administered concurrently (McGurk et al., 2007; Wykes et al., 2011). Based on these results, and the overlap in types of EF deficits targeted in the training programs described above, we propose computer-based neurocognitive training as an adjunctive therapy to enhance existing evidence-based treatments.
CRT for EDs primarily targets explicit decision-making skills through verbal discussion of these processes with a trained therapist, and fails to reach the frequency or intensity of practice that may be required for long-lasting changes in EF. Computer-based training can be administered at high frequencies at very little cost, and moreover can be delivered in the home. Both strength and duration of improvements in neurocognitive deficits have been shown to be associated with training session dosage (McGurk et al., 2007), suggesting the potential for computer-based neurocognitive training for EDs. Moreover, the low cost of delivery and automated format of delivery make this type of treatment highly disseminable. The potential applications of computerized cognitive training for other psychiatric disorders to EDs are described below.
Cognitive flexibility training
Implementation of computerized training of cognitive flexibility may help to address the limitations of current CRT paradigms for EDs. These trainings would provide direct, implicit training of cognitive flexibility, and may yield greater improvements than explicit, discussion-based training. In addition, improvements from these computerized sessions may have a “bottom-up” effect and improve patients' ability to engage with treatment and reduce symptomatic behavior during treatment. For example, in AN, increased flexibility allows patients to generate alternatives to rigid food and exercise rules, thereby improving one's ability to comply with treatment recommendations for more flexible and varied eating patterns.
Computerized cognitive training has shown promise in the treatment of other psychiatric disorders that are characterized by impaired cognitive flexibility, including major depressive disorder, schizophrenia, and substance use disorders. Among patients with depression, computerized cognitive flexibility training (with no therapist intervention) yielded improvements in cognitive flexibility which also generalized to other EF domains (Elgamal et al., 2007). However, the training failed to yield improvements in depression symptoms, and other results suggest that clinical benefits of such a training can only be observed when administered in conjunction with psychotherapy (Bowie et al., 2013). Several studies have demonstrated that high-frequency, computerized cognitive training ameliorates deficits in cognitive flexibility among those with schizophrenia (Bellucci, Glaberman, & Haslam, 2003; Dickinson et al., 2010; Urben et al., 2012). Results on generalization of cognitive gains to clinical and psychosocial improvements are mixed (Cavallaro et al., 2009; Urben et al., 2012). Indeed, most studies that yielded improved behavioral outcomes were confounded by the inclusion of psychotherapeutic intervention, which included practice of specific behavioral strategies to improve psychosocial functioning (Bowie & Gupta, 2013; Wykes et al., 2011). Among substance abuse patients, two studies have shown that computer-assisted cognitive training (which included a cognitive flexibility component) enhanced engagement in treatment and subsequent abstinence rates (Fals-Stewart & Lam, 2010; Grohman & Fals-Stewart, 2003). Overall, in conjunction with one study that has used a computerized CRT training for AN (Brockmeyer et al., 2014), these results suggest that computerized training has the potential to address deficits in cognitive flexibility among EDs. The extent to which neurocognitive gains may augment the efficacy of standard behavioral treatment is still debated, but it is likely that a combination of computer training and clinician-guided therapy may have the most benefit.
Inhibitory control training
Inhibitory control training (ICT), also known as response inhibition training, has been used to reduce a range of problematic and impulsive behaviors (Houben & Jansen, 2011; Houben et al., 2011; Jones & Field, 2013) among non-clinical samples, but the most relevant successes have been those that reduce disinhibited eating. Food-cue-specific ICT paradigms have shown preliminary efficacy at reducing impulsive responses to visual cues for highly palatable foods (Houben, 2011; Houben, Wiers, & Jansen, 2011). Training also generalizes to reduced consumption of the trained stimulus, such that certain “trigger” items (e.g., candy, potato chips) can be targeted for inhibition and reduced consumption (Veling et al., 2011; Veling, Aarts, & Stroebe, 2013). However, caution must be used when interpreting these preliminary results; the majority of ICT studies for eating behavior have included only non-clinical samples, and are limited methodologically. For example, Houben and colleagues used a paradigm in which the control group received an intervention which appeared to actually train subjects to be more impulsive (Houben, 2011; Houben & Jansen, 2011), and the efficacy of the ICT intervention may have been therefore exaggerated in their study results. Indeed, two other studies have found no transfer of cognitive gains to improvements in eating behavior (Guerrieri, Nederkoorn, & Jansen, 2007, 2012). Recent studies have improved upon these initial paradigms with better-controlled studies which compared effects of cue-specific and/or general inhibition training to more valid control conditions (Jones & Field, 2013; Veling et al., 2013). Cue-specific ICT has been found to facilitate weight loss (Allom & Mullan, 2015; Veling et al., 2014), and has also been shown to reduce laboratory selection (Koningsbruggen et al., 2014; Veling et al., 2013) and consumption (Houben & Jansen, 2015) of palatable foods. However, only one of these studies included a follow-up period of at least one week, and results showed abatement of cognitive gains over time (Allom & Mullan, 2015). Therefore, the durability of these training effects has yet to be demonstrated. While IC training has shown preliminary promise in reducing impulsive eating behavior, evidence for treatment effects must be extended to clinical populations and improved to yield long-term influence on behavior.
The impulsive eating behavior addressed by these studies is similar in nature to the impulsive and disinhibited behavior that characterizes binge-eating-spectrum pathology (BN, BED, AN B/P), and an exploration of the utility of ICT as a treatment adjunct appears warranted. The stimulus-specific nature of ICT paradigms may allow for individualized training programs that include response inhibition training for an individual's most frequent “trigger” foods that are typically eaten under such emotional circumstances. However, binge eating is more entrenched and emotionally complex than disinhibited eating experienced by dieters (Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011; Munsch et al., 2012; Schag et al., 2013). Although food-cue-specific inhibitory control deficits among ED patients who engage in binge eat are well-documented (and are qualitatively similar to non-ED dieters' deficits), bulimic symptoms are also frequently tied to a negative affective state, rather than a simple desire for a “tempting” food (Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011). Existing ICT paradigms therefore may require modification in order to address impulsive binge eating and purging behaviors, which often occur specifically under a negative affective state (Haedt-Matt & Keel, 2011). Of note, results from one study of ICT for alcohol consumption suggest that training may modify affective associations with the target stimulus (which would be binge foods for BN or BED) (Houben et al., 2011). Taken together, existing evidence suggests that exploration of the efficacy of ICT for binge-eating pathology (perhaps as an adjunctive therapy to CBT) is justified.
Working memory training
Improvements during treatment for EDs are associated with concurrent improvements in working memory (Lena & Fiocco, 2004; Moser et al., 2003), and it is likely that observed working memory deficits in ED patients interfere with progress in treatment. For example, individuals with poor working memory may experience greater preoccupation with thoughts about food, weight, and shape, which may result in greater difficulty in processing new and particularly complex information, such as that delivered during a psychotherapy session (Kemps et al., 2006).
Working memory capacity may be of particular importance for successful ED treatment, and has the potential to provide both direct and indirect benefits (Morrison & Chein, 2011). For example, an individual must engage in self-regulation in order to reduce the occurrence of symptomatic behavior. Basic working memory training could be provided in conjunction with a specific plan for reducing symptoms and normalizing eating behavior, and could directly enhance patients' ability to follow through with treatment guidelines (such as resisting the urge to binge, or following a clinician-prescribed dietary plan). Training may also support the processing of complex information (Carretti, Borella, & De Beni, 2007). Indirectly, improved ability to engage with treatment guidelines (in and out of therapy sessions) may result in improved self-efficacy and improve motivation for recovery.
Working memory trainings have been developed and tested for a range of psychopathology, with much of the extant work focused on targeting working memory deficits in children with ADHD (Klingberg, 2010). This work, much of which is conducted by Klingberg and colleagues, has demonstrated the short-term efficacy of computerized trainings in improving working memory capacity in those with ADHD (Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg, 2002; Klingberg et al., 2005), and often, the transfer of effects to other domains, such as response inhibition (Klingberg et al., 2005). In addition, working training appears to yield increases in neural activity in prefrontal and parietal areas, providing evidence for training-induced plasticity in the neural regions subserving working memory (Olesen, Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2004). Growing empirical support exists for the utility of working memory training in the treatment of other adult psychiatric disorders with executive deficits similar to those identified in EDs (Fisher et al., 2010; Morrison & Chein, 2011; Wolinsky et al., 2009), including schizophrenia. Similar to EDs, schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder in which working memory deficits are well-established and known to interfere with treatment efficacy and interpersonal functioning in affected patients (Green et al., 2000). Some results suggest that working memory training may yield greater transfer effects and may promote generalization of skills and improved behavioral outcomes compared to other neurocognitive training paradigms, though the extent of far transfer from working memory training is still debated (Jaeggi et al., 2014). Computer-based working memory training for patients with schizophrenia has been shown to yield specific cognitive improvement (Fisher et al., 2010). As with CRT, cognitive gains appear to transfer to psychosocial functioning only with concurrent behavioral skills training (Medalia & Saperstein, 2013). Thus, computer training is likely best suited as an adjunct to CBT-E.
Working memory training has also been shown to reduce behavioral impulsivity during decision-making tasks in individuals with stimulant dependence, and to decrease alcohol consumption in problem drinkers at one-month follow-up (Bickel et al., 2011; Houben et al., 2011), but replication is required in order to determine whether these results generalize to substance abuse patient populations. Impulsivity is known to underlie several eating disorder behaviors, including binge eating and purging (Wonderlich, Connolly, & Stice, 2004), and there is also substantial co-morbidity among EDs and substance use (Dawe & Loxton, 2004). Therefore, effective training paradigms for substance use are also likely to be effective for EDs if adapted appropriately for AN, BN, and BED.
Future directions
The existing body of research suggests that impairments in EF, particularly in the domains of set-shifting, central coherence, inhibitory control, and working memory, are related to the development and maintenance of disordered eating behavior (Dickinson et al., 2010; Lopez et al., 2009). CRT shows initial promise for improving EF, though more research is needed to establish whether these programs result in symptom reduction or greater responsiveness to conventional behavioral treatment (McGurk et al., 2007; Vinogradov et al., 2011). Moreover, at this time, the putative mediators (set-shifting and central coherence) of CRT have yet to be formally examined. For example, Dingemans and colleagues (2014) reported significant improvements in quality of life and ED symptomology in the treatment as usual + CRT versus the treatment as usual group, but changes in neuropsychological functioning were equivalent between groups, indicating CRT may be operating through a mechanism other than improving cognitive flexibility. It may be that CRT affects other EF domains beyond set-shifting and central coherence, indicating that paradigms specifically targeting each deficit may not be necessary to improve treatment response. However, it is also possible that CRT may be operating through other non-specific therapy effects. Identifying the mechanisms of action in CRT will help to better understand how symptom improvements are effected as well as to refine the treatment to include those components that effect change.
Dingemans and colleagues also observed differences in response to CRT such that those with poorer set-shifting at baseline had greater quality of life outcomes in the CRT condition. These results suggest that CRT, and perhaps neurocognitive training programs more generally, yields the most benefit for those with the most prominent deficits. These results also raise the intriguing possibility that strengthening executive capacities could benefit individuals more generally in their everyday lives. Future research should continue to identify who may potentially benefit most from adjunctive neurocognitive training programs, with particular foci on identifying baseline severity as a potential predictor of response and examination of overall quality of life as an outcome variable. Finally, though some work has begun to establish how changes in cognitive flexibility are related to underlying neurobiological mechanisms (Garrett et al., 2014), there is little known about how and whether CRT specifically alters these mechanisms. Evaluating neurobiological changes as a result of CRT treatment will help to evaluate the specific changes that then impact executive function. Additional future research directions specific to CRT have been published elsewhere, cf., Tchanturia (2014).
Although relatively minimal work has been conducted in the area of neurocognitive training for eating pathology beyond CRT, computer-based trainings have been used to train the same types of neurocognitive deficits in related conditions with similar symptom patterns (Dickinson et al., 2010; Wykes et al., 2011) and may similarly prove beneficial for eating pathology. More work is needed to understand whether these computer-based training programs similarly impact EF deficits in EDs, and whether changes are associated with symptom change. The only existing study utilizing computer-based model of neurocognitive training for EDs demonstrated promising findings for the utility of computer-based training (Brockmeyer et al., 2014). Given that computerized neurocognitive training paradigms are potentially more disseminable than in-person CRT, future research should continue to examine preliminary and/or analogue effects of computerized neurocognitive trainings on EF and symptom change in individuals with eating pathology.
Across neurocognitive training programs, there is minimal knowledge about how long and how often neurocognitive training should be administered to be most effective or how to best integrate programs into existing treatment. According to Vinogradov and colleagues (2011), trainings must occur on a daily basis for many weeks or months before neurocognitive changes occur to the extent that the changes will persist and transfer to behavior outside of the task being trained; however, what dose specifically is required remains unknown. For EDs in particular, the available literature to inform these types of decisions when designing a training program is sparse. Moreover, little is known about the long-term maintenance of skills acquired from neurocognitive training (McGurk et al., 2007; Vinogradov et al., 2011). Computerized neurocognitive training paradigms will not take the place of traditional behavioral treatments, but may serve as a valuable adjunctive intervention. Further research should examine the additive benefit of such paradigms to traditional psychosocial treatments, such as CBT or even psychopharmacological interventions.
In sum, in order to develop more effective training programs, additional research is needed to better understand (1) which neurocognitive deficits contribute to symptoms and are amenable to training programs, (2) which training programs promote maximal gains and generalization (and how behavioral skills training might maximize potential for clinical benefit), (3) the intensity and duration of training that are necessary to observe improvement, and (4) the potential utility of training paradigms that allow patients to engage in more frequent and consistent training (e.g. at home computerized training programs). Lastly, we need additional research on how best to incorporate cognitive training programs into existing psychosocial treatments to promote long-term behavior change. Without additional research in these domains, the creation of empirically-based neurocognitive training programs that can promote long-term reductions in disordered eating symptoms will be limited. It is our hope that the successes of neurocognitive training programs within other psychiatric disorders and the strong rationale for incorporating these techniques into treatments for eating disorders will spur others to continue work in this promising area.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest disclosure: The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- Allom V, Mullan B. Two inhibitory control training interventions designed to improve eating behaviour and determine mechanisms of change. Appetite. 2015;89:282–290. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice Evidence-based practice in psychology. American Psychologist. 2006;61:271–285. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellucci DM, Glaberman K, Haslam N. Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation reduces negative symptoms in the severely mentally ill. Schizophrenia Research. 2003;59(2):225–232. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00402-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel W, et al. Remember the future. Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69(3):260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birmingham C, et al. The mortality rate from anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2005;38(2):143–146. doi: 10.1002/eat.20164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Gupta M, Holshausen K. Cognitive remediation therapy for mood disorders. Rationale, early evidence, and future directions. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie. 2013;58(6):319–325. doi: 10.1177/070674371305800603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, et al. Combined cognitive remediation and functional skills training for schizophrenia. Effects on cognition, functional competence, and real-world behavior. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169(7):710–718. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, et al. Cognitive remediation for treatment-resistant depression. Effects on cognition and functioning and the role of online homework. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2013;201(8):680–685. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31829c5030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brockmeyer T, et al. Training cognitive flexibility in patients with anorexia nervosa. A pilot randomized controlled trial of cognitive remediation therapy. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2014;47(1):24–31. doi: 10.1002/eat.22206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brownley KA, et al. Binge eating disorder treatment. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(4):337–348. doi: 10.1002/eat.20370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bulik CM, et al. Anorexia nervosa treatment. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(4):310–320. doi: 10.1002/eat.20367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne SM, et al. The effectiveness of enhanced cognitive behavioural therapy for eating disorders. An open trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2011;49(4):219–226. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carretti B, Borella E, De Beni R. Does strategic memory training improve the working memory performance of younger and older adults? Experimental Psychology. 2007;54(4):311–320. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter FA, et al. The long-term efficacy of three psychotherapies for anorexia nervosa. A randomized, controlled trial. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2011;44(7):647–654. doi: 10.1002/eat.20879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavallaro R, et al. Computer-aided neurocognitive remediation as an enhancing strategy for schizophrenia rehabilitation. Psychiatry Research. 2009;169(3):191–196. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darcy AM, et al. Set-shifting among adolescents with bulimic spectrum eating disorders. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2012;74(8):869. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31826af636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darcy AM, et al. Central coherence in adolescents with bulimia nervosa spectrum eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2014 doi: 10.1002/eat.22340. doi:10.1002/eat.22340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawe S, Loxton NJ. The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28(3):343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson D, et al. A randomized, controlled trial of computer-assisted cognitive remediation for schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167(2):170–180. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingemans AE, Danner UN, Donker JM, Aardoom JJ, van Meer F, Tobias K, van Furth EF. The effectiveness of cognitive remediation therapy in patients with a severe or enduring eating disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2014;83(1):29–36. doi: 10.1159/000355240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duchesne M, et al. Assessment of executive functions in obese individuals with binge eating disorder. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria (Sao Paulo, Brazil: 1999) 2010;32(4):381–388. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462010000400011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Easter A, Tchanturia K. Therapists' experiences of cognitive remediation therapy for anorexia nervosa. Implications for working with adolescents. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;16(2):233–246. doi: 10.1177/1359104511401185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg D, Berman K. Executive function, neural circuitry, and genetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):258–277. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgamal S, et al. Successful computer-assisted cognitive remediation therapy in patients with unipolar depression. A proof of principle study. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37(09):1229–1238. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Excellence N.I.f.C. CG9 eating disorders review recommendations. London, England: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Fairburn C, et al. Transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioral therapy for patients with eating disorders. A two-site trial with 60-week follow-up. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166(3):311. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08040608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fals-Stewart W, Lam WK. Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation for the treatment of patients with substance use disorders. A randomized clinical trial. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2010;18(1):87. doi: 10.1037/a0018058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer S, Smith GT, Anderson KG. Clarifying the role of impulsivity in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2003;33(4):406–411. doi: 10.1002/eat.10165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher M, et al. Neuroplasticity-based cognitive training in schizophrenia. An interim report on the effects 6 months later. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2010;36(4):869–879. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzpatrick KK, et al. Set-shifting among adolescents with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2012;45(7):909–912. doi: 10.1002/eat.22027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fowler L, et al. Profile of neurocognitive impairments associated with female in-patients with anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(4):517–528. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett AS, et al. Predicting clinical outcome using brain activation associated with set-shifting and central coherence skills in anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2014;57:26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grall-Bronnec M, Sauvaget A. The use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for modulating craving and addictive behaviours. A critical literature review of efficacy, technical and methodological considerations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;47:592–613. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, et al. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2000;26(1):119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grohman K, Fals-Stewart W. Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation with substance-abusing patients. Effects on treatment response. Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation. 2003;21(4):10–17. [Google Scholar]
- Guerrieri R, Nederkoorn C, Jansen A. The interaction between impulsivity and a varied food environment. Its influence on food intake and overweight. International Journal of Obesity. 2007;32(4):708–714. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guerrieri R, Nederkoorn C, Jansen A. Disinhibition is easier learned than inhibition. The effects of (dis) inhibition training on food intake. Appetite. 2012;59(1):96–99. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK. Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating. A meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137(4):660. doi: 10.1037/a0023660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halmi K, et al. Predictors of treatment acceptance and completion in anorexia nervosa. Implications for future study designs. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(7)):776. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.7.776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartley C, Phelps E. Changing fear. The neurocircuitry of emotion regulation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):136–146. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hay P, et al. Psychological treatments for bulimia nervosa and binging. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009;(4):CD000562. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000562.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holliday J, et al. Is impaired set-shifting an endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(12):2269–2275. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K. Overcoming the urge to splurge. Influencing eating behavior by manipulating inhibitory control. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2011;42(3):384–388. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Jansen A. Training inhibitory control. A recipe for resisting sweet temptations. Appetite. 2011;56(2):345–349. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Jansen A. Chocolate equals stop. Chocolate-specific inhibition training reduces chocolate intake and go associations with chocolate. Appetite. 2015;87:318–323. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Wiers R, Jansen A. Getting a grip on drinking behavior training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science. 2011;22(7):968–975. doi: 10.1177/0956797611412392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, et al. Resisting temptation. Decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011;116(1):132–136. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeggi SM, et al. The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition. 2014;42(3):464–480. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0364-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones A, Field M. The effects of cue-specific inhibition training on alcohol consumption in heavy social drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2013;21(1):8. doi: 10.1037/a0030683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn C, Pike K. In search of predictors of dropout from inpatient treatment for anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2001;30(3):237–244. doi: 10.1002/eat.1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanakam N, Treasure J. A review of cognitive neuropsychiatry in the taxonomy of eating disorders. State, trait, or genetic? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18(1–2):83–114. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2012.682362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kass A, Kolko R, Wilfley D. Psychological treatments for eating disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2013;26(6):549–555. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328365a30e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemps E, et al. Selective working memory deficits in anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review. 2006;14(2):97–103. [Google Scholar]
- Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14(7):317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2002;24(6):781–791. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.6.781.8395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingberg T, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD. A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44(2):177–186. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koningsbruggen GM, et al. Comparing two psychological interventions in reducing impulsive processes of eating behaviour. Effects on self-selected portion size. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2014;19(4):767–782. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob G, Volkow N. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurtz MM, et al. Computer-assisted cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. What is the active ingredient? Schizophrenia Research. 2007;89(1):251–260. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang K, et al. Central coherence in eating disorders. An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2014;0:1–13. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2014.909606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lena S, Fiocco A, Leyenaar J. The role of cognitive deficits in the development of eating disorders. Neuropsychology Review. 2004;14(2):99–113. doi: 10.1023/b:nerv.0000028081.40907.de. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lock J, et al. Is outpatient cognitive remediation therapy feasible to use in randomized clinical trials for anorexia nervosa? International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(6):567–575. doi: 10.1002/eat.22134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez C, et al. Central coherence in eating disorders. A systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38(10):1393–1404. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez C, et al. Weak central coherence in eating disorders. A step towards looking for an endophenotype of eating disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2009;31(1):117–125. doi: 10.1080/13803390802036092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahon J. Dropping out from psychological treatment for eating disorders. What are the issues? European Eating Disorders Review. 2000;8(3):198–216. [Google Scholar]
- McCormick L, et al. Implications of starvation-induced change in right dorsal anterior cingulate volume in anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2008;41(7):602–610. doi: 10.1002/eat.20549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGurk S, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(12):1791–1802. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medalia A, Saperstein AM. Does cognitive remediation for schizophrenia improve functional outcomes? Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2013;26(2):151–157. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32835dcbd4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks. A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison AB, Chein JM. Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2011;18(1):46–60. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0034-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser DJ, et al. Neuropsychological functioning pretreatment and posttreatment in an inpatient eating disorders program. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2003;33(1):64–70. doi: 10.1002/eat.10108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munsch S, et al. Binge eating in binge eating disorder. A breakdown of emotion regulatory process? Psychiatry Research. 2012;195(3):118–124. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(1):75–79. doi: 10.1038/nn1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price J, Drevets W. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):192–216. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts ME, Tchanturia K, Treasure JL. Exploring the neurocognitive signature of poor set-shifting in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2010;44(14):964–970. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts ME, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37(08):1075–1084. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707009877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosval L, et al. Impulsivity in women with eating disorders. Problem of response inhibition, planning, or attention? International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2006;39(7):590–593. doi: 10.1002/eat.20296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schag K, et al. Food-related impulsivity in obesity and Binge Eating Disorder. A systematic review. Obesity Reviews. 2013;14(6):477–495. doi: 10.1111/obr.12017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serpell L, et al. Anorexia nervosa. Friend or foe? International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1999;25(2):177–186. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199903)25:2<177::aid-eat7>3.0.co;2-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro JR, et al. Bulimia nervosa treatment. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(4):321–336. doi: 10.1002/eat.20372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro J, et al. Bulimia nervosa treatment. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(4):321–336. doi: 10.1002/eat.20372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielmans GI, et al. Specificity of psychological treatments for bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder? A meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013;33(3):460–469. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spierer L, Chavan CF, Manuel AL. Training-induced behavioral and brain plasticity in inhibitory control. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7(427) doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surgenor L, Maguire S, Beumont P. Drop-out from inpatient treatment for anorexia nervosa. Can risk factors be identified at point of admission? European Eating Disorders Review. 2004;12(2):94–100. [Google Scholar]
- Svaldi J, Brand M, Tuschen-Caffier B. Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder. Appetite. 2010;54(1):84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia K. Cognitive Remediation Therapy (CRT) for eating and weight disorders. Routledge; New York, NY: 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia K, Lloyd S, Lang K. Cognitive remediation therapy for anorexia nervosa. Current evidence and future research directions. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(5):492–495. doi: 10.1002/eat.22106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia K, Lounes N, Holttum S. Cognitive remediation in anorexia nervosa and related conditions. A systematic review. European Eating Disorders Review. 2014;22(6):454–462. doi: 10.1002/erv.2326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia K, et al. Cognitive flexibility and clinical severity in eating disorders. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(6):e20462. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urben S, et al. Computer-assisted cognitive remediation in adolescents with psychosis or at risk for psychosis. A 6-month follow-up. Acta Neuropsychiatrica. 2012;24(6):328–335. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2012.00651.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Elburg A, Treasure J. Advances in the neurobiology of eating disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2013;26(6):556–561. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328365a2e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Eynde F, et al. Neurocognition in bulimic eating disorders. A systematic review. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2011;124(2):120–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veling H, Aarts H, Papies EK. Using stop signals to inhibit chronic dieters' responses toward palatable foods. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2011;49(11):771–780. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veling H, Aarts H, Stroebe W. Using stop signals to reduce impulsive choices for palatable unhealthy foods. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2013;18(2):354–368. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.2012.02092.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veling H, et al. Targeting impulsive processes of eating behavior via the internet. Effects on body weight. Appetite. 2014;78:102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinogradov S, Fisher M, de Villers-Sidani E. Cognitive training for impaired neural systems in neuropsychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;37(1):43–76. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinogradov S, Fisher M, Nagarajan S. Cognitive training in schizophrenia. Golden age or wild west? Biological Psychiatry. 2013;73(10):935–937. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vitousek K, Watson S, Wilson G. Enhancing motivation for change in treatment-resistant eating disorders. Clinical Psychology Review. 1998;18(4):391–420. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00012-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolinsky FD, et al. The ACTIVE cognitive training interventions and the onset of and recovery from suspected clinical depression. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2009;64(5):577–585. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wonderlich S. Eating disorders research society. Bethesda, MD: 2013. Negative reinforcement models and new treatments for bulimia nervosa. [Google Scholar]
- Wonderlich SA, Connolly KM, Stice E. Impulsivity as a risk factor for eating disorder behavior. Assessment implications with adolescents. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2004;36(2):172–182. doi: 10.1002/eat.20033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wonderlich S, et al. A randomized controlled comparison of integrative cognitive-affective therapy (ICAT) and enhanced cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT-E) for bulimia nervosa. Psychological Medicine. 2013:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu M, et al. Inhibitory control in bulimic-type eating disorders. A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(12):e83412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu M, et al. Set-shifting ability across the spectrum of eating disorders and in overweight and obesity. A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2014;26:1–21. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wykes T, et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia. Methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168(5):472–485. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zakzanis KK, Campbell Z, Polsinelli A. Quantitative evidence for distinct cognitive impairment in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of Neuropsychology. 2010;4(1):89–106. doi: 10.1348/174866409X459674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zipfel S, et al. Focal psychodynamic therapy, cognitive behaviour therapy, and optimised treatment as usual in outpatients with anorexia nervosa (ANTOP study). Randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2014;383(9912):127–137. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61746-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]