Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2016 May 17;10:217. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00217

Reading Aloud and Solving Simple Arithmetic Calculation Intervention (Learning Therapy) Improves Inhibition, Verbal Episodic Memory, Focus Attention and Processing Speed in Healthy Elderly People: Evidence from a Randomized Controlled Trial

Rui Nouchi 1,2,3,*, Yasuyuki Taki 4,5,6, Hikaru Takeuchi 6, Takayuki Nozawa 3,7, Atsushi Sekiguchi 5,8, Ryuta Kawashima 3,6,7,9
PMCID: PMC4868921  PMID: 27242481

Abstract

Background: Previous reports have described that simple cognitive training using reading aloud and solving simple arithmetic calculations, so-called “learning therapy”, can improve executive functions and processing speed in the older adults. Nevertheless, it is not well-known whether learning therapy improve a wide range of cognitive functions or not. We investigated the beneficial effects of learning therapy on various cognitive functions in healthy older adults.

Methods: We used a single-blinded intervention with two groups (learning therapy group: LT and waiting list control group: WL). Sixty-four elderly were randomly assigned to LT or WL. In LT, participants performed reading Japanese aloud and solving simple calculations training tasks for 6 months. WL did not participate in the intervention. We measured several cognitive functions before and after 6 months intervention periods.

Results: Compared to WL, results revealed that LT improved inhibition performance in executive functions (Stroop: LT (Mean = 3.88) vs. WL (Mean = 1.22), adjusted p = 0.013 and reverse Stroop LT (Mean = 3.22) vs. WL (Mean = 1.59), adjusted p = 0.015), verbal episodic memory (Logical Memory (LM): LT (Mean = 4.59) vs. WL (Mean = 2.47), adjusted p = 0.015), focus attention (D-CAT: LT (Mean = 2.09) vs. WL (Mean = −0.59), adjusted p = 0.010) and processing speed compared to the WL control group (digit symbol coding: LT (Mean = 5.00) vs. WL (Mean = 1.13), adjusted p = 0.015 and Symbol Search (SS): LT (Mean = 3.47) vs. WL (Mean = 1.81), adjusted p = 0.014).

Discussion: This randomized controlled trial (RCT) can be showed the benefit of LT on inhibition of executive functions, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed in healthy elderly people. Our results were discussed under overlapping hypothesis.

Keywords: learning therapy, cognitive training, reading aloud, simple calculation, transfer effect

Background

Cognitive function changes during one’s lifetime (Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004). Previous reports have described that older adults showed a decline in memory performance (Salthouse, 2003), attentional process (Yakhno et al., 2007), processing speed (Salthouse, 1996) and executive functions (Royall et al., 2004). Defining the term executive function is important. The term “executive function” is used as an umbrella for various complex cognitive processes and sub-processes (Elliott, 2003). Executive function includes the high-level cognitive processes that facilitate new ways of behaving and which optimize one’s approach to unfamiliar circumstances (Gilbert and Burgess, 2008). Three main components in executive functions are “updating (constant monitoring and rapid addition/deletion of working-memory contents), shifting (switching flexibly between tasks or mental sets) and inhibition (deliberate overriding of dominant or proponent responses)” (Miyake et al., 2000; Miyake and Friedman, 2012). As described herein, we use executive function as a collective term includes updating, shifting and inhibition.

Previous studies have demonstrated improvements of cognitive functions in older adults (Verhaeghen et al., 1992; Ball et al., 2002; Clare and Woods, 2004; Edwards et al., 2005; Carretti et al., 2007; Green and Bavelier, 2008; Lustig et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010; Nouchi et al., 2012a; Ballesteros et al., 2014). Cognitive training is an intervention program that provides structured practice on tasks related to aspects of cognitive function. There are many types of cognitive training such as working memory training (Richmond et al., 2011), processing speed training (Edwards et al., 2005, 2009), memory strategic training (Verhaeghen et al., 1992; Mahncke et al., 2006a; Carretti et al., 2007) and brain training game (Nouchi et al., 2012b; Anguera et al., 2013; Lampit et al., 2014; Toril et al., 2014). Developing cognitive training and validating the evidence of cognitive training have been at the forefront of scientific efforts (Nouchi and Kawashima, 2014).

Recently, cognitive training using reading aloud and solving of simple arithmetic calculations, so-called learning therapy (LT), has been developed (Kawashima et al., 2005; Uchida and Kawashima, 2008; Nouchi et al., 2012b). We were motivated to develop the new cognitive training based on following reasons. First, a previous study suggested that cognitive training should be designed in accordance with neuro-scientific evidence (Papp et al., 2009). Consequently, training tasks of learning therapy are created based on recent neuroimaging findings. Learning therapy uses reading text aloud and making simple calculations. Neuroimaging studies show that reading sentences with a voice (Miura et al., 2003, 2005) and simple arithmetic operations (Kawashima et al., 2004; Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011) activated the frontal, temporal and parietal cortices. Second, psychological stress related cognitive training reduced the improvements of cognitive functions after cognitive training (McAvinue et al., 2013). We selected extremely simple and easy tasks to do during intervention periods. Additionally, the training tasks are more familiar activities for older people because reading and calculation are parts of everyday life. Moreover, the training tasks use only papers and pencils. Consequently, elderly people can comprehend and perform training tasks. Third, previous results of studies have suggested that multiple components in training tasks facilitate to improvement of cognitive functions (Stuss et al., 2007; Green and Bavelier, 2008). Reading aloud and solving arithmetic problems are accomplished using a combination of cognitive processes including memory, attention and executive functions (inhibition, shifting and updating; Nouchi et al., 2012b). Therefore, we prepared reading aloud and mathematical calculations tasks as training tasks of learning therapy. Learning therapy is expected to have greater effects than previous cognitive training because it alleviates the difficulties mentioned above.

An earlier report described that learning therapy improves executive functions, episodic memory and processing speed (Kawashima et al., 2005; Uchida and Kawashima, 2008; Yoshida et al., 2014). However, there is no study which investigates the positive effects of learning therapy on various cognitive functions (e.g., memory and attention) in elderly people. Previous studies did not measure any cognitive function except for executive functions and processing speed. It is necessary to test the effectiveness of learning therapy on diverse cognitive functions, because various cognitive functions are necessary to support our actions and behaviors in everyday life.

As we mentioned before, the previous studies using learning therapy showed only improvements of executive functions, episodic memory and processing speed (Kawashima et al., 2005; Uchida and Kawashima, 2008; Yoshida et al., 2014). It is still unclear whether or not learning therapy can facilitate performance of other cognitive domains such as episodic memory, short term memory, working memory, reading ability and attention. Thus, this study was designed to investigate whether or not learning therapy can improve diverse cognitive functions in elderly people. There were essential differences from previous studies. First, the current study was aimed to validate the previous finding using the different cognitive tests in the same domains such as executive functions and processing speed. For example, previous studies used: (1) Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) as the executive functional measure (Kawashima et al., 2005; Uchida and Kawashima, 2008); (2) the digit symbol coding as the processing speed measure (Kawashima et al., 2005; Uchida and Kawashima, 2008); and (3) the word list as the episodic memory measure (Yoshida et al., 2014). On the other hand, we newly used Stroop and verbal fluency tests for the executive functional measure, Symbol Search (SS) for the processing speed andLogical Memory (LM) and face-name association memory for the episodic memory measure. Second, we measure other cognitive domains such as focus attention, short-term memory and working memory which did not measure in the previous articles. Thus, we can firstly investigate the beneficial effects of LT on a wide range of cognitive functions in one study.

To elucidate the beneficial effects of learning therapy on a wide range of cognitive function, we conducted single-blinded randomized controlled trials (RCT) using learning therapy. Testers who conducted psychological tests were blinded to the study hypothesis and the group membership (learning therapy group or not). To test the benefits of learning therapy, we assessed a broad range of cognitive functions. We measured inhibition and shifting of executive functions, verbal and facial episodic memory, short-term memory, working memory, reading ability, focus attention and processing speed.

Based on previous studies using cognitive training related to reading and mathematical calculations, we hypothesized that learning therapy would improve the inhibition performance in executive functional domain, verbal episodic memory in episodic memory domain, focus attention and processing speed in the healthy elderly people. Several reasons exist: (1) regarding the inhibition performance, a previous study (Uchida and Kawashima, 2008) found that performance of FAB (Dubois et al., 2000) which measured executive functions improved after learning therapy. Especially, the sub score (sensitivity to an interference task) of FAB was increased after learning therapy. The sensitivity to an interference task requiring behavioral self-regulation under verbal commands conflicts with sensory information. It may be highly correlated with the inhibition ability in executive functions. This task is similar to the Stroop task. Cognitive training using calculation (Takeuchi et al., 2011) and video game training (Nouchi et al., 2013), which included reading aloud and mental calculations, in young adults showed the improvements of the Stroop task. Consequently, we hypothesized that learning therapy can improve the inhibition performance measured by Stroop task performance; (2) for episodic memory, a previous study using working memory training, which included calculation training, demonstrated improvements of episodic memory (McAvinue et al., 2013). An earlier long-term intervention study using reading aloud and calculation revealed improvements of verbal episodic memory as measured by word lists (Yoshida et al., 2014). Consequently, we hypothesized that learning therapy would engender improvements of verbal episodic memory performance measured by LM test; (3) for focus attention, to solve mathematical calculations and reading sentences, it is necessary to examine the present information (focus attention) specifically. We assume that the focus attention ability would be improved by simple calculation and reading aloud tasks; and (4) for processing speed, our report of a previous study described improvements of processing speed measured by digit symbol coding (Uchida and Kawashima, 2008). Additionally, video game training using reading aloud and calculations also showed improvements of processing speed measured by SS (Nouchi et al., 2013). Based on the previous finding, we hypothesized that processing speed measured by SS and digit symbol coding. Therefore, we assumed that learning therapy engenders improvements of inhibition, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed compared to the control group.

Methods

Randomized Controlled Trial Design and Setting of this Trial

The following information was the same as our protocol article (Nouchi et al., 2012b). This study was a RCT. This study “conducted in Sendai city, Miyagi prefecture, Japan. Written informed consent to participate in the study was obtained from each participant based on the Declaration of Helsinki before enrolment. Informed consent were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine (ref. 2011-153). This study was registered in the University Hospital Medical Information Network (UMIN) Clinical Trial Registry (UMIN000006998)” (Nouchi et al., 2012b).

To assess the impact of learning therapy on widely diverse cognitive functions in healthy elderly people, we used a single-blinded intervention with two parallel groups: a learning therapy group and a waiting list (WL) control group. Testers were blind to the study’s hypothesis and the group membership of participants. The Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) statement (http://www.consort-statement.org/home/) was used as a framework for developing the study methodology (see Supplementary Material 1). The trial design is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Consolidated standards of reporting trials (CONSORT) flowchart.

Participants

Seventy-two participants were recruited from Sendai city through advertisements in the local town paper (Kahoku weekly; Figure 1). A procedure of recruitment of participants was the same as our previous study (Nouchi et al., 2014). Sample size calculation was written in Supplementary Material 2. Interested participants were screened using a semi-structured telephone interview, during which we asked participants for their demographic information (e.g., age and gender) and asked the same questions related to our inclusion and exclusion criteria (e.g., past medical histories of disease known to affect the central nervous system and using medications known to interfere with cognitive functions). “After the telephone interview, four people were excluded because they reported taking medications known to interfere with cognitive function (including benzodiazepines, antidepressants and other central nervous agents)” (Nouchi et al., 2012b). Two people were excluded based on severe hypertension (systolic blood pressure over 180, diastolic blood pressure over 110). Two participants declined to participate before a random assignment. All included participants (n = 64) were invited to visit Tohoku University for more detailed screening assessment (JART) and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) and to provide written informed consent. No participant was excluded based on JART scores. Randomization was conducted after receiving the informed consent statement from participants. Before the intervention period, we measured reading abilities using a sub-scale of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) such as Vocabulary (Wechsler, 1997) and arithmetic abilities using a sub-scale of WAIS such as Arithmetic (Wechsler, 1997). Table 1 presents the baseline characteristics. There were no significant differences between the groups in all data (two-sample t-test, p > 0.10). The scores of MMSE and IQ were within the range of normality.

Table 1.

Characteristics of participants in the learning therapy group and the waiting list control groups.

Learning therapy group Wait list control group
Mean SD Mean SD p-value
Age (years) 72.81 (6.18) 71.38 (4.92) 0.31
Education (years) 13.25 (2.00) 12.69 (1.40) 0.20
MMSE (score) 28.34 (0.86) 28.44 (0.76) 0.65
Arithmetic from WAIS (score) 11.44 (2.42) 11.13 (2.42) 0.61
Vocabulary WAIS (score) 31.19 (4.37) 31.03 (4.65) 0.89
JART (score) 19.97 (3.47) 19.21 (4.30) 0.44
Estimated IQ from JART (score) 109.91 (7.03) 108.50 (8.61) 0.48

No significant difference was found between combination exercise training and the waiting list control groups (two-sample t-test, p > 0.10). JART, Japanese Reading test; SD, standard deviation; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III.

Details of sample size calculation was described in the protocol of this study (Nouchi et al., 2012b).

Learning Therapy Group (Cognitive Intervention Group)

The cognitive intervention method was the same as that used in our previous study with learning therapy for healthy older adults (Uchida and Kawashima, 2008). Training tasks use two simple tasks (solving arithmetic and Japanese language problems) with systematized basic problems in arithmetic and reading (Uchida and Kawashima, 2008). The example of training tasks showed in Figures 2, 3. The lowest level of difficulty in simple calculation was simple addition (e.g., 1 + 3). The highest level of calculation was three figure number division (e.g., 156 ÷ 3). The lowest level of difficulty in reading aloud was reading simple sentences of 17 characters such as Japanese Haiku poems. The highest level was reading fairy tales aloud (about 100–120 characters per page). The difficulties of the Japanese Language problems were based on the number of Japanese characters per page because it takes more time to read aloud sentences with more characters.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

An example of the training task for reading aloud.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

An example of the training task for simple calculation.

Before intervention, we measured the percentages of correct answers and the time it takes to solve the diagnostic tests. The diagnostic tests consisted of 70 simple arithmetic and 16 simple reading tests. Based on the diagnostic tests, the appropriate level and workload in training tasks were set. In these tasks, participant was able to solve the problems with ease and without mental stress within 15 min. The difficulty level of training tasks and workloads did not change during the intervention period.

The cognitive intervention was scheduled to be conducted for 23 weeks. Participants in the cognitive intervention group were asked to go to a classroom at Tohoku University once a week. They were instructed to complete five sheets of each task prepared for each participant for that day. The daily learning time for the two tasks was approximately 15 min. They were required to complete five sheets of each task as homework for 4–6 days a week.

Waiting List Control Group (No Cognitive Intervention Group)

The following texts were the same as our previous protocol article (Nouchi et al., 2012b). “The waiting list control group received no intervention. Those participants were informed by letter that they were scheduled to receive an invitation to participate after a waiting period of 6 months. No placebo was used for the social contact group” (Nouchi et al., 2012b). Results of previous intervention studies (Clark et al., 1997; Mahncke et al., 2006) showed that “a placebo group was unnecessary for study of this type because no difference existed in cognitive or functional improvement between the placebo and no-social-contact groups (control group)” (Nouchi et al., 2012b). After a waiting period of 6 months, 26 elderly people in the waiting list group participated in the learning therapy. Six elderly people declined to receive the learning therapy because three elderly people dropped out during the waiting period and three elderly did not want to receive the learning therapy after the waiting period.

There were several reasons why we used the waiting list control group. To investigate beneficial effects of cognitive training, we should use certain control conditions because of reductions in the influence of practice effects and cognitive declines by aging. Several studies used no-contact or waiting list groups (Levine et al., 2007; Basak et al., 2008; Shatil et al., 2010; McDougall and House, 2012). On the other hand, many activities were selected for active control conditions (Buiza et al., 2008; Peretz et al., 2011; Nouchi et al., 2012a). For instance, there were many types of activities in the active control groups such as selecting letters in the newspaper (Herrera et al., 2012), leaning trivia using web sites (Richmond et al., 2011a) and video games (Nouchi et al., 2012a). From a methodological perspective, it would be better to use an other intervention program as an active control group in the cognitive training studies compared to use a waiting list control group. However, to use an active control group, we should consider an ethical issue. Using the active control group should be “a reasonable balance of the level of benefit to the time investment required of participants in the treatment group” (Street and Luoma, 2002). An active control group with meaningless and worthless tasks is not suitable. In the cognitive training research field, there is no standard and appropriate task for the active control group. The choice of an active control condition must be considered within costs, research questions and ethical considerations. Additionally, results of previous intervention studies (Clark et al., 1997; Mahncke et al., 2006) reported that a placebo group was unnecessary for study of this type because no difference existed in cognitive or functional improvement between the placebo and no-social-contact groups (control group). Moreover, using the waiting list control group had “the advantage of letting everyone in the study receive the new intervention (sooner or later)” (Nouchi et al., 2014). Therefore, we decided to the waiting list control group.

Cognitive Function Measures

The following texts were the same as our previous article (Nouchi et al., 2012b). To test the positive effects of learning therapy on performance in cognitive functions, we used several cognitive measures (Table 2). “Measures of the cognitive functions were divisible into seven categories (executive functions, episodic memory, short-term memory, working memory, reading ability, attention and processing speed)” (Nouchi et al., 2012b). Executive functions were measured by the Stroop Test (ST; Hakoda and Sasaki, 1990; Watanabe et al., 2011) and Verbal fluency task (VFT; Ito et al., 2004). The ST had ST and reverse ST (rST) conditions. For ST, participants were required to select a word corresponding to the ink color in the color–word combination. For rST participants were asked to choose a color item corresponding to a color–word combination’s semantic meaning or selection. For VFT, we used Japanese version’s letter fluency task (LFT) and category fluency (CFT) task. Participants were asked to generate many words beginning with the specific letter (LFT) or many words of a category (CFT). Episodic memory was measured using LM (Wechsler, 1987) and First and Second Names (FSN; Wilson et al., 1985). For LM, participants were asked to memorize the short story. For FSN, participants were asked to memorize FSN with faces. Verbal short-term memory was measured by Digit Span Forward (DS-F; Wechsler, 1997). For DS-F, participants were asked to memorize numbers and repeat the numbers. Working memory was measured using Digit Span Backward (DS-B; Wechsler, 1997). For DS-B, participants were require to memorize digit numbers and answer the number in reverse order. Reading ability was measured by the Japanese Reading Test (JART; Matsuoka et al., 2006). JART was reading test of 25 Japanese Kanji. The Digit Cancellation Task (D-CAT) was used to measure focus attention (Hatta et al., 2000). For D-CAT, participants were asked to check the target number in 12 rows of 50 digits. Digit Symbol Coding (Cd; Wechsler, 1997) and SS (Wechsler, 1997) were used for processing speed tests. For Cd, participants were required to write the symbol corresponding to the number. For SS, participants were required to search the target symbols in five symbols. Details of all tasks are described in the Supplementary Material 2. We assessed these cognitive function measures before and after the intervention period (6 months).

Table 2.

Summary of cognitive function measures.

Cognitive function Task
Executive functions Verbal fluency task
Stroop test
Attention Digit cancellation task
Episodic memory Logical memory
First and second names
Short-term memory Digit span forward
Working memory Digit span backward
Reading ability Japanese reading test
Processing speed Digit symbol coding
Symbol Search

Analysis

This study was designed to evaluate the beneficial effect of learning therapy in elderly people. The following analysis method was also presented in our protocol article (Nouchi et al., 2012b). We calculated the change score (post-training score minus pre-training score) in all cognitive function measures. We conducted ANCOVA for the change scores in each cognitive test. The change scores were the dependent variable. Groups (learning therapy, waiting list control) were the independent variable. Pre-training scores in the dependent variable, sex and age were the covariates to exclude the possibility that any pre-existing difference of measure between groups affects the result of each measure and adjust for background characteristics. Significance was inferred for p < 0.05. We used Storey’s False discovery rate (FDR) correction methods to adjust the p values (Storey, 2002). Moreover, this report describes eta squared (η2) as an index of effect size (Cohen, 1988). Missing data were imputed using the expectation-maximization method, as implemented in the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Missing Value Analysis. It imputed missing values using maximum likelihood estimation with observed data in an iterative process (Dempster et al., 1977; Nouchi et al., 2012b). All randomized participants were included in the analyses in line with their allocation, irrespective of how many sessions they completed (intention-to-treat principle). All analyses were conducted using software (SPSS ver. 18 or higher; SPSS Inc.).

Results

Sixty-four elderly people participated in this RCT (learning therapy and waiting list control). Thirty of the 32 members of the learning therapy group and 29 of the 32 members of the waiting list control group were completed. We imputed missing values of two participants in the learning therapy group and three participants in the waiting list control group using intention-to-treat principle (see “Analysis” Section). The pre-training and post-training scores in cognitive functions are presented in Table 3.

Table 3.

Cognitive function scores before and after training period in both groups.

Learning therapy group Wait list control group
Pre Post Pre Post
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD p-value
Executive functions
  LFT (number) 8.56 (2.53) 9.38 (2.64) 9.59 (3.35) 10.28 (2.82) 0.17
  CFT (number) 11.06 (2.84) 11.72 (2.77) 11.41 (2.08) 11.44 (3.26) 0.58
  rST (number) 27.44 (6.87) 31.31 (5.87) 28.63 (6.47) 29.84 (6.66) 0.48
  ST (number) 41.41 (9.00) 44.63 (8.29) 40.78 (9.15) 42.38 (8.73) 0.78
Attention
  D-CAT (number) 26.31 (5.87) 28.41 (4.98) 26.84 (5.48) 26.25 (5.47) 0.71
Episodic memory
  LM (score) 7.31 (2.22) 11.91 (2.80) 8.09 (2.84) 10.56 (2.94) 0.23
  FSN (score) 2.97 (1.60) 3.34 (1.89) 3.66 (1.79) 4.00 (2.06) 0.11
Short-term memory
  DS-F (score) 5.13 (0.87) 4.97 (1.18) 5.56 (1.37) 5.34 (1.33) 0.16
Working memory
  DS-B (score) 4.09 (0.86) 3.97 (0.82) 4.38 (1.29) 4.13 (1.13) 0.31
Processing speed
  Cd (number) 64.56 (13.15) 69.56 (13.21) 66.84 (11.48) 67.97 (11.37) 0.61
  SS (number) 33.00 (6.15) 36.47 (5.51) 32.81 (6.17) 34.63 (6.41) 0.90
Verbal ability
  JART (score) 19.97 (3.47) 20.34 (3.30) 19.25 (4.30) 19.50 (4.15) 0.46

Group comparison (two sample t-tests) of the pre-training scores revealed no significant difference in any measure of cognitive functions between the learning therapy group and the waiting list control group (p > 0.10). Pre, pre-training; post, post-training; SD, standard deviation. Executive functions were measured using the Stroop Test (ST) and Verbal fluency task (VFT). Attention was measured using the Digit Cancellation Task (D-CAT). Episodic Memory was measured using Logical Memory (LM) and First and Second Names (FSN). Short-term memory was measured using Digit Span Forward (DS-F). Working memory was measured using Digit span Backward (DS-B). Processing speed was measured using Digit Symbol Coding (Cd) and Symbol Search (SS). Reading ability was measured using the Japanese Reading Test (JART).

To test the positive effect of the learning therapy on the improvement of cognitive functions, we did ANCOVA for the change scores in each cognitive test (Table 4). We found significant group differences in four cognitive domains (executive functions, episodic memory, attention and processing speed). For executive functions, the learning therapy group improved the ST score (learning therapy (Mean = 3.88) vs. waiting list control (Mean = 1.22), F(1,59) = 7.35, η2 = 0.11, adjusted p = 0.013) and the rST score (learning therapy (Mean = 3.22) vs. waiting list control (Mean = 1.59), F(1,59) = 8.72, η2 = 0.09, adjusted p = 0.015). For episodic memory, the learning therapy group improved the LM score (learning therapy (Mean = 4.59) vs. waiting list control (Mean = 2.47), F(1,59) = 9.72, η2 = 0.12, adjusted p = 0.015). For focus attention, learning therapy group improved the D-CAT score (learning therapy (Mean = 2.09) vs. waiting list control (Mean = -0.59), F(1,59) = 12.23, η2 = 0.14, adjusted p = 0.010). For processing speed, learning therapy group improved the Cd score (learning therapy (Mean = 5.00) vs. waiting list control (Mean = 1.13), F(1,59) = 9.85, η2 = 0.13, adjusted p = 0.015) and the SS score (learning therapy (Mean = 3.47) vs. waiting list control (Mean = 1.81), F(1,59) = 7.81, η2 = 0.10, adjusted p = 0.014). These results demonstrate that the learning therapy led to improvements of inhibition performance in executive functions, verbal episodic memory’s measures, focus attentional measure and all processing speed’s measures.

Table 4.

Change scores in cognitive functions measures of both groups.

Learning therapy Wait list control
Mean SD Mean SD Effect size (η2) p-value Adjusted p-value Results of ANCOVA
Executive functions
  LFT (number) 0.81 (1.82) 0.69 (2.21) 0.00 0.607 0.759 ns
  CFT (number) 0.66 (2.59) 0.03 (2.79) 0.01 0.396 0.566 ns
  rST (number) 3.22 (1.90) 1.59 (2.53) 0.09 0.009 0.015 LT > WL
  ST (number) 3.88 (3.44) 1.22 (3.90) 0.11 0.005 0.013 LT > WL
Episodic memory
  LM (score) 4.59 (2.75) 2.47 (2.30) 0.12 0.003 0.015 LT > WL
  FSN (score) 0.38 (1.79) 0.34 (1.81) 0.00 0.650 0.650 ns
Short-term memory
  DS-F (score) (0.16) (1.05) (0.22) (0.83) 0.01 0.457 0.784 ns
Working memory
  DS-B (score) (0.13) (0.75) (0.25) (1.34) 0.00 0.862 0.781 ns
Attention
  D-CAT (number) 2.09 (2.99) (0.59) (3.15) 0.14 0.001 0.010 LT > WL
Processing speed
  Cd (number) 5.00 (4.31) 1.13 (4.77) 0.13 0.003 0.015 LT > WL
  SS (number) 3.47 (2.98) 1.81 (2.60) 0.10 0.007 0.014 LT > WL
Verbal ability
  JART (score) 0.38 (1.50) 0.25 (1.30) 0.00 0.622 0.691 ns

Change scores were calculated by subtracting the pre-cognitive measure score from the post-cognitive measure score. We conducted analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for the change scores in each of cognitive tests. In the ANCOVA, pre-training scores in each cognitive test, sex and age were the covariates. Significance was inferred for p < 0.05. The p values were adjusted using FDR method. Moreover, this report describes eta square (η2) as an index of effect size. As a descriptive index of strength of association between an experimental factor (main effect or interaction effect) and a dependent variable, η2 is defined as the proportion of total variation attributable to the factor, and it ranges in value from 0 to 1. η2 ≥ 0.01 is regarded as a small effect, η2 ≥ 0.06 as a medium effect, and η2 ≥ 0.14 as a large effect. LT, learning therapy; WL, waiting list control group; SD, standard deviation. Executive functions were measured using the Stroop Test (ST) and Verbal fluency task (VFT). Episodic Memory was measured using Logical Memory (LM) and First and Second Names (FSN). Short-term memory was measured using Digit Span Forward (DS-F). Working memory was measured using Digit span Backward (DS-B). Attention was measured using the Digit Cancellation Task (D-CAT). Processing speed was measured using Digit Symbol Coding (Cd) and Symbol Search (SS). Reading ability was measured using the Japanese Reading Test (JART).

Discussion

We investigated the positive effects of learning therapy on diverse cognitive functions in healthy elderly people. Results showed clearly that the learning therapy group exhibited improved inhibition performance of executive functions measured by ST and rST, verbal episodic memory measured by LM, focus attention measured by D-CAT and processing speed measured by SS and Cd compared to the waiting list control group. These results supported our hypothesis. The improvements of Cd performance of processing speed after learning therapy are consistent with previous evidence (Kawashima et al., 2005; Uchida and Kawashima, 2008). However, this study extends the previous finding by demonstrating improvements of focus attention and by replicating improvement of inhibition process and processing speed using different cognitive functional measures such as ST, rST and SS after learning therapy.

The overlapping hypothesis can explain the present results (Nouchi et al., 2012a, 2013, 2014). The overlapping hypothesis hypothesizes that improvements of cognitive functions by a certain type of training would occur if the processes during both cognitive training tasks (e.g., reading aloud and calculation simple arithmetic problem) and untrained tasks (e.g., measures of cognitive functions) were overlapped and were involved in similar cognitive processes. In this study, participants were required to undertake learning therapy, which included a reading aloud task and a simple arithmetic calculation problem task. As we described in the background reading aloud and simple arithmetic calculation involve many cognitive components and processes. To perform these tasks, inhibition process related to executive functions, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed are expected to be recruited. For instance, attentions are necessary to modulate one’s own voice during reading sentences aloud and inhibition ability is important to select and use rules of mathematics during calculation. Long-term Memory (semantic and verbal episodic memory) is expected to be necessary to read a sentence and to comprehend the sentence during reading aloud (LaBerge and Samuels, 1974; Myers et al., 2000). When we read a sentence, we construct a mental representation, known as the situation model (Kintsch, 1994; Zwaan and Radvansky, 1998). To facilitate understanding and integration of incoming information from sentence, we use related information from verbal long-term memory (Cook et al., 1998). Both semantic and verbal episodic memories are important to create and elaborate the situation model (Van Dijk and Kintsch, 1983; Kintsch et al., 1999). Some previous studies showed that semantic memory has the most important role in reading and comprehending the sentences (Garrod and Terras, 2000). Especially, the patient with episodic memory dysfunctions has no problem with reading (Tulving et al., 1988). Thus, most theories assume that comprehending and reading a sentence rely on more on semantic rather than on episodic memory. However, for healthy people, verbal episodic memory may influence reading processing as early as general world knowledge (Myers et al., 2000). One previous study reported that the verbal episodic memory for prior text facilitated the reading process (Haviland and Clark, 1974). Verbal episodic memory for prior texts is reactivated automatically when we encounter the related information during reading sentences (Myers and O’Brien, 1998). Consequently, verbal episodic memory is also necessary to read and comprehend texts as well as semantic memory. Focus attention is expected to be necessary to perform each task with concentration. Processing speed has a important role to solve mathematical problems and to read as fast as possible during a training period. The overlapping hypothesis explains the improvements of inhibition in executive functions, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed after learning therapy that follows. First, it would take the mental processes described above to perform the reading aloud task and the simple arithmetic calculation problem task. Second, both these training tasks and the cognitive tests can share the similar cognitive processes. Third, the cognitive processes described above are expected to be facilitated by these training tasks. Therefore, inhibition performance in executive functions, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed were improved after these training tasks.

Considering common elements of improved cognitive functional measures, it may be true that learning therapy generally improved the speed of processing related to task performance. Improved cognitive functional measures such as ST, rST, D-CAT, SS and Cd required participants to do the task as quickly as possible during a limited time (please see “Methods” Section). Two training tasks also require that participants complete materials of two training tasks as quickly as possible. Based on the overlapping hypothesis, both cognitive training tasks such as reading aloud and simple calculations and the psychological tests which measured the cognitive functions can share the elements of speed of processing. Therefore, performances of cognitive functional measures that require speed of participants were improved by learning therapy.

It is important to consider dissociations of improvements in executive functions and episodic memory measures. The benefits of learning therapy did not generalize to all tasks in the same functions. In this study, the learning therapy group improved inhibition performance of executive functions (rST and ST) but not other shifting performance of executive functions (LFT and CFT). Additionally, the learning therapy improved verbal episodic memory measure such as LM, but not the facial episodic memory measure such as FSN. According to the overlapping hypothesis, the possibility exists that these training tasks (reading aloud and calculation simple arithmetic problems) and cognitive function measures such as LFT, CFT and FSN might not share the same cognitive processes. For instance, previous studies showed that LFT and CFT require both the generation of words within a subcategory (clustering) and the ability to shift to a new category when a subcategory is exhausted (switching; Troyer et al., 1998). FSN requires a face recognition process and an association between FSN and between faces and names (Wilson et al., 1985). As described earlier herein, these cognitive processes (clustering, switching, face recognition and association) did not include cognitive processes during reading aloud and arithmetic calculation problems. Consequently, the respective levels of performance of LFT, CFT and FSN were not improved by learning therapy.

Dissociation result of episodic memory can also be explained by the following ideas. For episodic memory, participants may become more familiar with the story structure by reading aloud training. Therefore, participants would be able to use the knowledge to encode information better after the reading aloud training. This idea can predict only the improvement of LM, but not the FSN.

From the perspective of definition of executive functions, three main components exist in executive functions. Based on this, learning therapy improved only the inhibition ability in executive functions, but not the shifting ability. The improvement of the inhibition ability after reading aloud and simple calculation training was consistent with our previous results (Uchida and Kawashima, 2008; Nouchi et al., 2013). Our dissociation results of executive functions suggest that it is necessary to prepare multiple cognitive measures in each cognitive domain such as executive functions and episodic memory. Moreover, in future studies, we have to focus on one area of cognitive domain (e.g., working memory) and use varied measures for the specific cognitive domain (operating span, reading span, n-back and digit (spatial) span). It may reveal a great deal about the impact of cognitive intervention on cognitive functions in the elderly people.

In addition, discussion of negative findings of no improvements of verbal ability, short-term memory and working memory is important. For verbal ability measured by JART, participants must read Kanji compound words. The difficulty level of the kanji words in JART is higher than that in the junior high school. However, the difficulty level of kanji in the reading aloud task would be lower than that in JART because our texts for the reading aloud task are based on first-grade to fourth-grade elementary school students (please see “Methods” Section). Thus, the participants did not learn how to read difficult kanji through intervention. Therefore, learning therapy did not show the improvement of JART performance. In addition, we found no significant improvements of short-term and working memory measured by DS-F and DS-B. No improvements of short-term and working memory performance would come from the lack of adaptive elements for the training tasks. Some previous studies demonstrated that intensive adaptive training would facilitate improvements of cognitive functions (Takeuchi et al., 2011; McAvinue et al., 2013). One previous study using mental calculation training in young adults reported that the intensive adaptive training using mental calculation led to improvement of short-term memory performance, but not the no intention adaptive training using mental calculation (Takeuchi et al., 2011). In the present study, we set the appropriate difficulties level of both task based on the diagnostic tests. However, the difficulty level of both training tasks did not change through intervention periods. Thus, learning therapy showed no improvement of short-term and working memory performance. Future studies demand additional detailed studies to ascertain whether learning therapy using the intensive adaptive training would improve short-term and working memory performance.

This study has several strength points compared to earlier studies assessing the effects of cognitive training for elderly people. As we described in our protocol article (Nouchi et al., 2012b), we used easy to learn training tasks such as reading sentence and simple calculation using a paper and pencil. Most training tasks in previous studies were complex tasks using computers (Berry et al., 2010; Li et al., 2010). Using computers might make it easy to record data precisely and to control tasks. Nevertheless, elderly people often have difficulty using computers (Wagner et al., 2010). Recent studies have demonstrated that some barriers to computer use exist, such as lack of interest, lack of knowledge and fear of using computer (Saunders, 2004; Peacock and Künemund, 2007). The difficulty of using computers is one main reason of frustration and other negative emotions, possibly reducing their motivation to continue. On the other hand, our training tasks are easy to do for elderly people. Therefore our training tasks were expected to encourage their willingness.

This study has a limitation. We do not use other cognitive training as an active control group. We used the waiting list control group because previous studies revealed no differences in cognitive or functional improvement between the active control group and the non-training control group (Clark et al., 1997; Mahncke et al., 2006). However, from a study design perspective, it would be suitable to use the active control group to prove the effectiveness of learning therapy. Future intervention studies should be conducted to compare learning therapy to other cognitive training such as working memory training or training programs of other types such as exercise training.

Conclusion

In summary, this study was designed to investigate the beneficial effects of learning therapy on diverse cognitive functions in healthy elderly people. This report is the first of a study revealing the beneficial effects of learning therapy on inhibition performance of executive functions, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed in healthy elderly people. On the other hand, the learning therapy did not improve shifting performance of executive function, facial episodic memory, verbal short-term memory, verbal working memory and reading ability. These positive and negative findings would be important for people who want to use the learning therapy. If people want to improve inhibition performance, verbal episodic memory, focus attention and processing speed, this intervention program would work well. If people want to improve other cognitive domains (e.g., working memory), other cognitive training program such as working memory training would be well for them. This study had some limitations, but produced important and sufficient evidence demonstrating the effectiveness of learning therapy. Given that cognitive functions are important to do daily life activities (Cahn-Weiner et al., 2000, 2002; Lee et al., 2005), our results suggest that cognitive functions in the elderly are enhanced through daily life activities such as reading aloud and calculating simple arithmetic problems.

Author Contributions

RN designed, developed the study protocol and calculated the sample size. RN conducted the study. RN wrote the manuscript with YT, HT, TN, AS and RK. RK also provided advice related to the study protocol. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

Learning therapy was developed by RK and the Kumon Institute of Education. However, RK derives no income from Kumon Institute of Education and Society for Learning Therapy. RK has no other competing interests. All other authors have declared no competing interests.

Acknowledgments

This study was an industry–academic collaboration of Tohoku University: Smart Aging Square (http://www2.idac.tohoku.ac.jp/dep/sairc/square.html). This study was supported by the Kumon Institute of Education. This study is also supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 15H05366 (Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (A)) and Research Grant of Frontier Research Institute for Interdisciplinary Science (FRIS), Tohoku University. Funding sources of the trial have no involvement in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or writing of articles. We thank A. Kasagi and H. Nouchi for recruiting the participants, testers for performing psychological tests, supporters for conducting learning therapy, the participants and all of our other colleagues in IDAC, Tohoku University for their support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00217/abstract

References

  1. Anguera J. A., Boccanfuso J., Rintoul J. L., Al-Hashimi O., Faraji F., Janowich J., et al. (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 501, 97–101. 10.1038/nature12486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arsalidou M., Taylor M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. Neuroimage 54, 2382–2393. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ball K., Berch D. B., Helmers K. F., Jobe J. B., Leveck M. D., Marsiske M., et al. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288, 2271–2281. 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ballesteros S., Prieto A., Mayas J., Toril P., Pita C., Ponce de León L., et al. (2014). Brain training with non-action video games enhances aspects of cognition in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:277. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Basak C., Boot W. R., Voss M. W., Kramer A. F. (2008). Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol. Aging 23, 765–777. 10.1037/a0013494 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berry A. S., Zanto T. P., Clapp W. C., Hardy J. L., Delahunt P. B., Mahncke H. W., et al. (2010). The influence of perceptual training on working memory in older adults. PLoS One 5:e11537. 10.1371/journal.pone.0011537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buiza C., Etxeberria I., Galdona N., Gonzalez M. F., Arriola E., López de Munain A., et al. (2008). A randomized, two-year study of the efficacy of cognitive intervention on elderly people: the Donostia Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 23, 85–94. 10.1002/gps.1846 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cahn-Weiner D. A., Boyle P. A., Malloy P. F. (2002). Tests of executive function predict instrumental activities of daily living in community-dwelling older individuals. Appl. Neuropsychol. 9, 187–191. 10.1207/s15324826an0903_8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cahn-Weiner D. A., Malloy P. F., Boyle P. A., Marran M., Salloway S. (2000). Prediction of functional status from neuropsychological tests in community-dwelling elderly individuals. Clin. Neuropsychol. 14, 187–195. 10.1076/1385-4046(200005)14:2;1-z;ft187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carretti B., Borella E., De Beni R. (2007). Does strategic memory training improve the working memory performance of younger and older adults? Exp. Psychol. 54, 311–320. 10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Clark F., Azen S. P., Zemke R., Jackson J., Carlson M., Mandel D., et al. (1997). Occupational therapy for independent-living older adults. A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 278, 1321–1326. 10.1001/jama.1997.03550160041036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Clare L., Woods R. T. (2004). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 14, 385–401. 10.1080/09602010443000074 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cook A. E., Halleran J. G., O’Brien E. J. (1998). What is readily available during reading? A memory–based view of text processing. Discourse Process. 26, 109–129. 10.1080/01638539809545041 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dempster A. P., Laird N. M., Rubin D. B. (1977). Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B Methodol. 39, 1–38. [Google Scholar]
  16. Dubois B., Slachevsky A., Litvan I., Pillon B. (2000). The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology 55, 1621–1626. 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1621 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Edwards J. D., Delahunt P. B., Mahncke H. W. (2009). Cognitive speed of processing training delays driving cessation. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 64, 1262–1267. 10.1093/gerona/glp131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Edwards J. D., Wadley V., Vance D., Wood K., Roenker D., Ball K. (2005). The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment. Health 9, 262–271. 10.1080/13607860412331336788 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Elliott R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. Br. Med. Bull. 65, 49–59. 10.1093/bmb/65.1.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Garrod S., Terras M. (2000). The contribution of lexical and situational knowledge to resolving discourse roles: bonding and resolution. J. Mem. Lang. 42, 526–544. 10.1006/jmla.1999.2694 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gilbert S. J., Burgess P. W. (2008). Executive function. Curr. Biol. 18, R110–R114. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Green C., Bavelier D. (2008). Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychol. Aging 23, 692–701. 10.1037/a0014345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hakoda Y., Sasaki M. (1990). Group version of the stroop and reverse-stroop test: the effects of reaction mode, order and practice. Kyoiku Shinrigaku Kenkyu (Jpn. J. Educ. Psychol.) 38, 389–394. [Google Scholar]
  25. Hatta T., Ito Y., Yoshizaki K. (2000). D-CAT Manual (Screening Test for Attention). Osaka: Union Press. [Google Scholar]
  26. Haviland S. E., Clark H. H. (1974). What’s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 13, 512–521. 10.1016/s0022-5371(74)80003-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hedden T., Gabrieli J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 87–96. 10.1038/nrn1323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Herrera C., Chambon C., Michel B. F., Paban V., Alescio-Lautier B. (2012). Positive effects of computer-based cognitive training in adults with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 50, 1871–1881. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ito E., Hatta T., Ito Y., Kogure T., Watanabe H. (2004). Performance of verbal fluency tasks in Japanese healthy adults: effect of gender, age and education on the performance. Shinkei Shinrigaku Kenkyu (Jpn. J. Neuropsychol.) 20, 254–263. [Google Scholar]
  30. Jean L., Bergeron M.-È., Thivierge S., Simard M. (2010). Cognitive intervention programs for individuals with mild cognitive impairment: systematic review of the literature. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 281–296. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181c37ce9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kawashima R., Okita K., Yamazaki R., Tajima N., Yoshida H., Taira M., et al. (2005). Reading aloud and arithmetic calculation improve frontal function of people with dementia. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 60, 380–384. 10.1093/gerona/60.3.380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kawashima R., Taira M., Okita K., Inoue K., Tajima N., Yoshida H., et al. (2004). A functional MRI study of simple arithmetic–a comparison between children and adults. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 18, 227–233. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kintsch W. (1994). Text comprehension, memory and learning. Am. Psychol. 49, 294–303. 10.1037/0003-066x.49.4.294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kintsch W., Patel V. L., Ericsson K. A. (1999). The role of long-term working memory in text comprehension. Psychologia 42, 186–198. [Google Scholar]
  35. LaBerge D., Samuels S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cogn. Psychol. 6, 293–323. 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lampit A., Hallock H., Valenzuela M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 11:e1001756. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lee Y., Kim J. H., Lee K. J., Han G., Kim J. L. (2005). Association of cognitive status with functional limitation and disability in older adults. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 17, 20–28. 10.1007/bf03337716 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Levine B., Stuss D. T., Winocur G., Binns M. A., Fahy L., Mandic M., et al. (2007). Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: effects on strategic behavior in relation to goal management. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 143–152. 10.1017/s1355617707070178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Li K. Z., Roudaia E., Lussier M., Bherer L., Leroux A., McKinley P. A. (2010). Benefits of cognitive dual-task training on balance performance in healthy older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 65, 1344–1352. 10.1093/gerona/glQ161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lustig C., Shah P., Seidler R., Reuter-Lorenz P. A. (2009). Aging, training and the brain: a review and future directions. Neuropsychol. Rev. 19, 504–522. 10.1007/s11065-009-9119-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mahncke H. W., Connor B. B., Appelman J., Ahsanuddin O. N., Hardy J. L., Wood R. A., et al. (2006). Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: a randomized, controlled study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 12523–12528. 10.1073/pnas.0605194103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Matsuoka K., Uno M., Kasai K., Koyama K., Kim Y. (2006). Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease using Japanese ideographic script (Kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 60, 332–339. 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01510.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McAvinue L. P., Golemme M., Castorina M., Tatti E., Pigni F. M., Salomone S., et al. (2013). An evaluation of a working memory training scheme in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:20. 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McDougall S., House B. (2012). Brain training in older adults: evidence of transfer to memory span performance and pseudo-Matthew effects. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 19, 195–221. 10.1080/13825585.2011.640656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Miura N., Iwata K., Watanabe J., Sugiura M., Akitsuki Y., Sassa Y., et al. (2003). Cortical activation during reading aloud of long sentences: fMRI study. Neuroreport 14, 1563–1566. 10.1097/00001756-200308260-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Miura N., Watanabe J., Iwata K., Sassa Y., Riera J., Tsuchiya H., et al. (2005). Cortical activation during reading of ancient versus modern Japanese texts: fMRI study. Neuroimage 26, 426–431. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Miyake A., Friedman N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. 10.1177/0963721411429458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A., Wager T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Myers J. L., Cook A. E., Kambe G., Mason R. A., O’Brien E. J. (2000). Semantic and episodic effects on bridging inferences. Discourse Process. 29, 179–199. 10.1207/s15326950dp2903_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Myers J. L., O’Brien E. J. (1998). Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Process. 26, 131–157. 10.1080/01638539809545042 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nouchi R., Kawashima R. (2014). Improving cognitive function from children to old age: a systematic review of recent smart ageing intervention studies. Adv. Neurosci. 2014:235479 10.1155/2014/235479 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nouchi R., Taki Y., Takeuchi H., Hashizume H., Akitsuki Y., Shigemune Y., et al. (2012a). Brain training game improves executive functions and processing speed in the elderly: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 7:e29676. 10.1371/journal.pone.0029676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nouchi R., Taki Y., Takeuchi H., Hashizume H., Nozawa T., Kambara T., et al. (2013). Brain training game boosts executive functions, working memory and processing speed in the young adults: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 8:e55518. 10.1371/journal.pone.0055518 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nouchi R., Taki Y., Takeuchi H., Hashizume H., Nozawa T., Sekiguchi A., et al. (2012b). Beneficial effects of reading aloud and solving simple arithmetic calculations (learning therapy) on a wide range of cognitive functions in the healthy elderly: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 13:32. 10.1186/1745-6215-13-32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nouchi R., Taki Y., Takeuchi H., Sekiguchi A., Hashizume H., Nozawa T., et al. (2014). Four weeks of combination exercise training improved executive functions, episodic memory and processing speed in healthy elderly people: evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Age (Dordr.) 36, 787–799. 10.1007/s11357-013-9588-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Papp K. V., Walsh S. J., Snyder P. J. (2009). Immediate and delayed effects of cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: a review of current literature and future directions. Alzheimers Dement. 5, 50–60. 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Peacock S. E., Künemund H. (2007). Senior citizens and Internet technology. Eur. J. Ageing 4, 191–200. 10.1007/s10433-007-0067-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Peretz C., Korczyn A. D., Shatil E., Aharonson V., Birnboim S., Giladi N. (2011). Computer-based, personalized cognitive training versus classical computer games: a randomized double-blind prospective trial of cognitive stimulation. Neuroepidemiology 36, 91–99. 10.1159/000323950 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Richmond L. L., Morrison A. B., Chein J. M., Olson I. R. (2011). Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychol. Aging 26, 813–822. 10.1037/a0023631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Royall D. R., Palmer R., Chiodo L. K., Polk M. J. (2004). Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: the Freedom House Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 52, 346–352. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52104.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Salthouse T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 103, 403–428. 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Salthouse T. A. (2003). Memory aging from 18 to 80. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 17, 162–167. 10.1097/00002093-200307000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Saunders E. J. (2004). Maximizing computer use among the elderly in rural senior centers. Educ. Gerontol. 30, 573–585. 10.1080/03601270490466967 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shatil E., Metzer A., Horvitz O., Miller A. (2010). Home-based personalized cognitive training in MS patients: a study of adherence and cognitive performance. NeuroRehabilitation 26, 143–153. 10.3233/NRE-2010-0546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Storey J. D. (2002). A direct approach to false discovery rates. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Stat. Methodol. 64, 479–498. 10.1111/1467-9868.00346 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Street L. L., Luoma J. B. (2002). Control groups in psychosocial intervention research: ethical and methodological issues. Ethics Behav. 12, 1–30. 10.1207/s15327019eb1201_1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stuss D. T., Robertson I. H., Craik F. I., Levine B., Alexander M. P., Black S., et al. (2007). Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: a randomized trial to evaluate a new protocol. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 120–131. 10.1017/s1355617707070154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Takeuchi H., Taki Y., Sassa Y., Hashizume H., Sekiguchi A., Fukushima A., et al. (2011). Working memory training using mental calculation impacts regional gray matter of the frontal and parietal regions. PLoS One 6:e23175. 10.1371/journal.pone.0023175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Toril P., Reales J. M., Ballesteros S. (2014). Video game training enhances cognition of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Aging 29, 706–716. 10.1037/a0037507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Troyer A. K., Moscovitch M., Winocur G., Alexander M. P., Stuss D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: the effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 36, 499–504. 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00152-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tulving E., Schacter D. L., McLachlan D. R., Moscovitch M. (1988). Priming of semantic autobiographical knowledge: a case study of retrograde amnesia. Brain Cogn. 8, 3–20. 10.1016/0278-2626(88)90035-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Uchida S., Kawashima R. (2008). Reading and solving arithmetic problems improves cognitive functions of normal aged people: a randomized controlled study. Age (Dordr.) 30, 21–29. 10.1007/s11357-007-9044-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Van Dijk T. A., Kintsch W. (1983). Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  74. Verhaeghen P., Marcoen A., Goossens L. (1992). Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Aging 7, 242–251. 10.1037/0882-7974.7.2.242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wagner N., Hassanein K., Head M. (2010). Computer use by older adults: a multi-disciplinary review. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 870–882. 10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Watanabe M., Hakoda Y., Matsumoto A. (2011). Group version of the stroop and reverse-stroop test: an asymmetric developmental trait in two kinds of interference. Kyusyu Daigaku Shinrigaku Kenkyu (Kyushu Univ. Psychol. Res.) 12, 41–50. 10.15017/20075 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wechsler D. A. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wechsler D. A. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Third edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  79. Wilson B. A., Cockburn J., Baddeley A. D. (1985). The Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test. Reading, England: Thamas Valley Test Company. [Google Scholar]
  80. Yakhno N. N., Zakharov V. V., Lokshina A. B. (2007). Impairment of memory and attention in the elderly. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 37, 203–208. 10.1007/s11055-007-0002-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Yoshida H., Sun K., Tsuchida N., Ohkawa I. (2014). Do practices of learning activities improve the cognitive functioning of healthy elderly adults? From the viewpoint of a transfer effect. Shinrigaku Kenkyu 85, 130–138. 10.4992/jjpsy.85.13013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zwaan R. A., Radvansky G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol. Bull. 123, 162–185. 10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES