Skip to main content
Frontiers in Pediatrics logoLink to Frontiers in Pediatrics
. 2016 Jun 15;4:62. doi: 10.3389/fped.2016.00062

Influence of Yoga-Based Personality Development Program on Psychomotor Performance and Self-efficacy in School Children

Madhusudan Das 1, Singh Deepeshwar 1,*, Pailoor Subramanya 1, Nandi Krishnamurthy Manjunath 1
PMCID: PMC4908105  PMID: 27379220

Abstract

Selective attention and efficacy are important components of scholastic performance in school children. While attempts are being made to introduce new methods to improve academic performance either as part of curricular or extracurricular activities in schools, the success rates are minimal. Hence, this study assessed the effect of yoga-based intervention on psychomotor performance and self-efficacy in school children. Two hundred ten school children with ages ranging from 11 to 16 years (mean age ± SD; 13.7 ± 0.8 years) satisfying the inclusion and exclusion criteria were recruited for the 10-day yogä program. An equal number of age-matched participants (n = 210; mean ± SD; 13.1 ± 0.8 years) were selected for the control group. Participants were assessed for attention and performance at the beginning and end of 10 days using trail making task (TMT) A and B, and self-efficacy questionnaire. The yoga group showed higher self-efficacy and improved performance after 10 days of yoga intervention. The performance in TMT-A and -B of the yoga group showed a significantly higher number of attempts with a reduction in time taken to complete the task and a number of wrong attempts compared with control group. Results suggest that yoga practice enhances self-efficacy and processing speed with fine motor coordination, visual–motor integration, visual perception, planning ability, and cognitive performance.

Keywords: yoga, self-efficacy, attention, trail making test, academic performance

Introduction

Scholastic performance in school children depends on multiple factors. Memory, attention, and motor speed are some of the important intrinsic factors, which play a major role in an individual’s performance. Motivation to perform can also be influenced by self-efficacy (1). Self-efficacy is defined as “an individual’s judgment of his/her own capabilities to organize and execute the tasks to achieve optimal performance” (2). High self-efficacy is related to a number of positive physical, social, and psychological outcomes. A longitudinal study of 390 adolescents, reported lower levels of depression and delinquency with positively correlated emotional self-efficacy (the perceived ability to handle negative emotions and express positive emotions) (3). Another study on low self-efficacy in students and patients reported severity of social anxiety and associated social impairment (4). Therefore, attempts are being made to create structured activities within the curriculum, which can enhance an individual’s scholastic behavior and performance. But, these programs with focused objectives have resulted in improvement of the student’s performance, but not behavior.

Yoga is an ancient Indian discipline that aims at developing an integrated personality, where the growth of physical, mental, social, and spiritual planes is equally focused (5, 6). Manjunath and Telles reported that practicing yoga (including postures, regulated breathing, relaxation techniques, and meditation) can improve the ability to plan and execute a given cognitive task in school children (7). Furthermore, several studies have demonstrated the positive impact of yoga in terms of enhanced attention, concentration, and memory (visual and spatial memory) (8, 9) in school children. It was demonstrated that the combination of yoga practices can improve motor speed (10), while an individual technique (called cyclic meditation) involving focused attention can improve perception (11).

In addition, the beneficial effects of yoga in improving hand steadiness, which is suggestive of better attention and concentration (8) has been documented. Furthermore, a recent report suggested an improvement in motor skills and visual discrimination following the practice of a yoga-based breathing technique (Kapalabhati), compared with breath awareness (12).

It is evident from these earlier studies that combination of yoga practices in general, and selected individual yoga techniques in isolation, can positively influence an individual’s perception and motor performance. The studies mentioned above provide some evidence for the use of yoga in improving motor function, attention, and perception, which are important for scholastic performance. However, there are no comprehensive studies with larger sample size, controlled environment, and longer duration of intervention available to understand the influence of a customized yoga module on an individual’s performance and behavior.

Hence, this study was designed to evaluate changes in psychomotor performance and self-efficacy following a specially designed personality development yoga camp for school children.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Four hundred twenty healthy school children with ages ranging between 13 and 16 years were recruited. Two hundred ten children (M = 13.78; SD = 0.89) were from a self-selected 10-day yoga-based “Personality Development Camp” (PDC) at a yoga institution, in South India. An equal number of age-matched subjects (M = 13.08; SD = 0.84) without any experience of yoga were selected as control group from a school within the same locality. The inclusion criteria included (i) between 13 and 16 years of age, (ii) English as the medium of instruction in the school, (iii) willingness to participate in the study, and (iv) no hospitalization in the last 3 months. The exclusion criteria included (i) any history of neurological or psychiatric disturbances and (ii) learning disability if any (i.e., slow learner). The study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee (Res/IEC-SVYASA/003/2013) of the yoga institution. Since the selected age group included minors (below 18 years of age), signed informed consent was obtained from their parents and guardian (Principal of the School). Demographic details of the participants are given in Table 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the participants.

Participants Yoga group
Control group
n M ± SD n M ± SD
Age (years) 210 13.78 ± 0.89 210 13.08 ± 0.84
Gender
Male 140 13.75 ± 0.91 124 13.18 ± 0.83
Female 70 13.84 ± 0.88 86 12.93 ± 0.83
Socioeconomic status
High 48 (23%) 32 (15.24%)
Middle 148 (70.5%) 162 (77.14%)
Lower 14 (6.5%) 6 (6.7%)
Educational status
Up to 6 years 52 (24.76%) 63 (30%)
Up to 8 years 123 (58.57%) 119 (56.67%)
Up to 10 years 35 (16.67%) 28 (13.33%)

Design

Eligible subjects from the PDC camp at the yoga institution who were willing to participate in the study were stratified based on age and academic status and were assigned to the yoga group. The yoga group and the control group were assessed before and immediately after 10 days. They were assessed similar to the experimental group on days 1 and 10 during which time the subjects were asked to continue with their normal routine.

Intervention

The 10-day “Personality Development Camp” (PDC) of 10 h per day was designed for children below 17 years of age. The 10-h routine consisted of physical postures (Asanas), voluntary regulated breathing (Pranayama), meditation (Dhyana), relaxation techniques, internal cleansing practices (Kriyas), and reciting hymns from traditional yoga texts, music, yoga games, and happy assembly. Kriyas are yogic cleansing exercises, which are performed to cleanse the body and assist with the natural removal of waste products (13). Details of the intervention are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Summarized 10-day Yoga intervention program.

Sl no. Name of intervention Duration
1. Asana session
  • Standing posture

  • Sitting postures

  • Prone posture

  • Inverted postures

  • Supine postures

2 h
2. Pranayama session
  • Sectional breathing

  • Thoracic breathing

  • Diaphragm breathing

  • Hyperventilation (Bhastrika and Kapalbhati)

  • Balancing of the breath (NadiShuddhi, AnulomVilom)

1 h
3. Cleansing techniques (Kriyas)
  • Trataka (eye-cleansing techniques)

  • Sutra Neti and JalaNeti (nasal tract-cleansing techniques)

  • VamanDhouti and LaghuShankaPrakshalana (GI tract-cleansing techniques)

Twice
4. Gita Chanting session
  • Chanting (18 verses from Bhagavad Gita)

  • Yogic discourse

1 h
5. Creativity hour
  • Karma yoga

  • Designing and arts

  • Tree plantation

  • Debate

  • Stories

2 h
6. Bhajan session
  • Devotional songs

  • Patriotic songs

1 h
7. Game session
  • Yogic games

  • Group awareness

1 h
8. Happy assembly
  • Cultural program

  • Team work

2 h

Assessments

Each subject was assessed in two sessions, one using trail making tasks-A and -B (TMT) (14) and the second using self-efficacy questionnaire for children (SEQ-C) (15). Although trail making tasks are very simple, it has been hypothesized that they reflect various cognitive processes including attention, visual search and scanning, sequencing and shifting, psychomotor speed, abstraction, flexibility, ability to execute and modify a plan of action, and ability to maintain two trains of thoughts simultaneously (12).

During the TMT session, subjects were assessed individually by seating them on a comfortable chair. Trail making task “A” involved subjects drawing lines connecting 25 consecutive circled numbers in a numerical sequence (i.e., 1–2–3, etc.) as rapidly as possible. In task “B,” the subjects were directed to draw lines to connect 12 consecutive circled numbers and 12 consecutive letters in an alternate numeric and alphabetic sequence (i.e., 1–A–2–B, etc.) as rapidly as possible. Time (in seconds) taken for completing the task was noted by using a stop watch. The errors committed during the task completion were scored removing accurately (16).

Self-efficacy questionnaire for children was developed by Muris (15) to assess general self-efficacy across three domains: academic, social, and emotional situations. The SEQ-C is a 24-item self-reported measure with eight items for each domain. Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 being “not at all” and 5 being “very well.” Both assessments were arranged on separate days at the same time for the two groups (i.e., Yoga and Control). The SEQ-C was administered as a group test, and the subjects were seated comfortably.

Procedure

The TMT and self-efficacy questionnaire (SEQs) questionnaires were administered on children of both the groups (yoga and control) on Day 1 and Day 10 of personality development yoga camp. The data collection lasted for 20 min. The yoga groups underwent for 10 days yoga practices, whereas control group were asked to continue with their normal routine. The yoga educator was blind to the hypothesis of the study. After 10 days of yoga intervention, all children were asked to complete the same questionnaires. The researchers explained about the study and gave stipulated instructions of the manuals to children for better understanding. Children were not given feedback as to their performance on designing experiments or any of the measures.

While data extraction, TMT scores were extracted based on wrong attempts, right attempts, and total attempts to see the error affect of the children. The extraction of TMT also included the completion time for the task, i.e., time duration in seconds. The SEQ-C was extracted based on the responses on three domains of self-efficacy, i.e., (i) social self-efficacy, which has to do with the perceived capability for peer relationship and assertiveness; (ii) academic self-efficacy, which is concerned with the perceived capability to manage one’s own learning behavior, to master academic subjects, and to fulfill academic expectations; and (iii) emotional self-efficacy, which pertains to the perceived capability of coping with negative emotions.

Data Analysis

The raw data were obtained from Trail Making Tasks A and B as well as SEQ and tabulated separately. The raw data were analyzed using Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS) Version 20. Data of different variables were tested with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for normality. Since, we had two Groups, i.e., yoga and control, and two states, i.e., day 1 and day 10, repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was carried out for each assessment. For Trail making tasks (A and B), RM-ANOVA was performed with one “Within-subjects” factor, i.e., state (day 1 and day 10), which in turn had four subdomains (wrong attempt, right attempt, total attempt, and time taken) and one “Between-subjects” factor, i.e., groups (yoga and control). Similarly, for self-efficacy, RM-ANOVA was performed with one “Within-subjects” factor, i.e., states (day 1 and day 10), which in turn had three subdomains (academic, social, and emotional) and one “Between-subjects” factor, i.e., groups (yoga and control). This was followed by a pairwise comparison between the mean values of day 1 and day 10 assessments with Bonferroni correction. The alpha level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

TMT

For Trail making task “A” (TMT-A), the repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant difference for Between-subjects factor, i.e., groups [F(1,418) = 12.38; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.029] as well as for Within-subjects factor, i.e., states [F(3,1254) = 2164.29; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.84] and for sub domains of TMT-A assessments, i.e., wrong attempt [F(1,418) = 29.45; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.06], right attempt [F(1,418) = 30.10; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.07], total attempt [F(1,418) = 0.55; p > 0.05; η2p = 0.001], and time taken [F(1,418) = 58.83; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.13]. The interaction between subdomains of TMT assessments × groups [F(3,1254) = 12.24; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.028] and State × Groups [F(1,418) = 61.24; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.13] were also significant.

After 10 days, in the Trail making task “A” (TMT-A), pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction showed a significant increase in the “right attempt” scores (p < 0.001) with the reduction in the “wrong attempt” scores (p < 0.001) in the yoga group, while there was no difference in the control group. Also, the “total numbers” attempted were significantly higher (p < 0.05) and the “time taken” was significantly lower (p < 0.001) in the yoga group compared with control group.

Similarly, for Trail making task-B (TMT-B), the repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant difference in “Between-subjects” factor, i.e., groups [F(1,418) = 12.38; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.029] as well as for “Within-subjects” factor, i.e., states [F(3,1254) = 2164.29; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.84], and for sub domain of TMT-B assessments, i.e., wrong attempt [F(1,418) = 28.91; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.06], right attempt [F(1,418) = 29.7; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.07], total attempt [F(1,418) = 1.01; p > 0.05; η2p = 0.002], and time taken [F(1,418) = 66.19; p = 0.001; η2p = 0.14]. The interaction between TMT-B assessments × groups [F(3,1254) = 12.24; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.028] and state × groups [F(1,418) = 61.24; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.13] was also significant.

Pairwise comparisons between mean values of the yoga group showed a significant increase in the scores of “right attempts” (p < 0.001) with the reduction of “wrong attempts” (p < 0.001). Additionally, yoga practices, improved “total attempts” (p < 0.001) with the reduction in the time taken (p < 0.001). On the other hand, there were no significant changes in the control group (p > 0.05) scores of day 1 to day 10. The group mean values, SD, median, and interquartile range of trail making task (A and B) of both the groups on day 1 and day 10 are given in Table 3.

Table 3.

The groups mean values ±SD of the trail making task (TMT) A and B for yoga and control groups in two states (pre and post).

Assessments Variables Groups Pre M ± SD (median) Post M ± SD (median) Pre Interquartile range Post Interquartile range % Change T p-Values
Trail making task A Wrong attempt Yoga 0.61 ± 1.26 (0) 0.05 ± 0.26 (0) 0 0 0.80 0.396 0.000
Control 0.14 ± 1.88 (0) 0.46 ± 1.01 (0) 0 0 0.65 0.000 0.000
Right attempt Yoga 0.39 ± 1.25 (25) 0.95 ± 0.26 (25) 0 0 0.30 −6.379 0.000
Control 0.87 ± 1.87 (25) 0.54 ± 1.01 (25) 0 0 0.93 −4.934 0.000
Total attempt Yoga 0.88 ± 0.76 (25) 0.00 ± 0.07 (25) 0 0 0.48 −2.239 0.026
Control 0.91 ± 0.60 (25) 0.95 ± 0.44 (25) 0 0 0.17 −0.736 0.463
Time duration (s) Yoga 0.92 ± 17.43 (32) 0.48 ± 9.88 (25) 0.5 0 0.28 0.128 0.000
Control 0.84 ± 26.99 (33) 0.23 ± 22.30 (33) 0 0 0.21 −1.441 0.151
Trail making task B Wrong attempt Yoga 0.20 ± 2.27 (0) 0.07 ± 0.31 (0) 2 0 0.17 0.329 0.000
Control 0.51 ± 0.94 (0) 0.36 ± 0.80 (0) 2 0 0.41 0.836 0.068
Right attempt Yoga 0.82 ± 2.26 (24) 0.94 ± 0.31 (24) 2 0 0.91 −7.265 0.000
Control 0.50 ± 0.93 (24) 0.63 ± 0.80 (24) 2 0 0.53 −1.731 0.085
Total attempt Yoga 0.73 ± 0.98 (24) 0.98 ± 0.19 (24) 0 0 0.05 −3.722 0.000
Control 0.93 ± 0.54 (24) 0.00 ± 0.07 (24) 0 0 0.28 −1.672 0.096
Time duration (s) Yoga 0.03 ± 28.38 (68) 0.98 ± 15.21 (47) 35.5 19 0.00 0.853 0.000
Control 0.23 ± 13.38 (58) 0.72 ± 11.87 (60) 30 31 0.17 0.118 0.002

Self-efficacy

For self-efficacy, the repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant difference “Between-subjects” factor, i.e., groups [F(1,418) = 18.94; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.043] and states [F(1,418) = 181.29; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.30]. The test of Within-subjects effect showed that there were significant interaction between states × groups [F(1,418) = 119.13; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.22] and subdomains of assessments × states [F(3,1254) = 107.02; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.20].

The pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction for the yoga group showed a significant increase in the scores of academic domains (p < 0.001), the social domain (p < 0.001), and emotional domain (p < 0.001), whereas there were no significant changes in the scores of the control group on day 1 to day 10. The group mean values, SD, median, and interquartile range of the SEQ of both the groups on day 1 and day 10 with percentage change (%) are given in Table 4.

Table 4.

Descriptive statistics groups mean values ±SD of the self-efficacy questionnaire for yoga and control groups in two states (pre and post).

Assessments Variables Groups Pre M ± SD (median) Post M ± SD (median) Pre Interquartile range Post Interquartile range % Change T p-Values
Self-efficacy 
questionnaire 
(SEQ) Academic domain Yoga 25.51 ± 7.12 (25) 29.71 ± 5.11 (30) 11 6 15.95 −7.108 0.000
Control 30.10 ± 6.44 (32) 30.46 ± 6.15 (32) 10 10 1.19 −1.754 0.081
Social domain Yoga 24.00 ± 5.94 (23) 28.86 ± 4.63 (29) 8 6 20.25 −10.495 0.000
Control 27.79 ± 5.48 (28) 28.36 ± 5.25 (29) 8 7 2.05 −2.947 0.004
Emotional domain Yoga 22.26 ± 5.65 (21) 27.98 ± 5.00 (28) 7.3 7 25.70 −13.173 0.000
Control 25.56 ± 5.20 (25) 26.19 ± 4.95 (26) 7.3 7 2.46 −3.318 0.001
Total score Yoga 71.96 ± 15.68 (70) 86.66 ± 11.10 (86) 21 13 20.43 −12.725 0.000
Control 83.49 ± 14.69 (84) 85.04 ± 14.01 (85) 22 21 1.86 −3.812 0.000

The partial correlation showed that there was a significant inverse relationship between total self-efficacy score and the time duration of the test in Trial A (r = −0.41; p = 0.04) and in Trial B (r = −0.21; p = 0.003) of yoga group, which suggest that with increasing self-efficacy, the duration of the test was reduced in trial A and trial B compared with the control group. This suggests that the self-efficacy was higher in yoga group with improved performance as compared with control group.

Discussion

The results of this study revealed that the school children who performed regular yoga practices showed higher self-efficacy and improved performance compared with control group who continued with their normal routine. It also showed a positive relationship between self-efficacy and performance after adjusting for age, gender, and education levels in yoga group.

Self-efficacy and executive functions, especially motor performance, play a major role in the scholastic behavior of an individual. Self-efficacy in an individual measures the confidence and ability to execute specific behavior. It suggests that high self-efficacy motivates proactive behavior (17). Hence, this study made an attempt to understand the changes in different dimensions of self-efficacy and executive functions (based on a trail making task).

Our results are consistent with the earlier studies that 10-day yoga practice improves visual perception (7) with significant decrease in the number of errors in repeated trials (10, 18). Previous studies demonstrated that the daily yoga intervention has a significant impact on key classroom behaviors (19, 20) and improvement in academic performance, especially reading, as well as social and peer interactions in most of the children (21, 22). Berger et al. demonstrated that yoga had sustained effects with improvements in behavior, especially in the child’s ability to pay attention in class, organizational skills with homework, and decreased impulsive behavior (23, 24). Recently, a long-term study showed a reduction in time taken to complete neurocognitive tasks after practicing vihangam yoga meditation in school children and also suggested that meditation improves mental functions, such as attention span (25), processing speed, attention alternation ability, and performance (26). This may be due to functional reorganization of brain activity patterns for focused attention and cognitive monitoring that takes place with mental practice, and this meditation-related changes are crucially associated to a functional reorganization of activity patterns in prefrontal cortex and in the insula (27). Yogic relaxation and meditation techniques have been shown to improve information processing speed (26, 28) in practitioners. In the context of attention, the TMT have been suggested to tap more complex attention or information processing, immediate memory and performance on recall capacity (29, 30) after yoga practice. Telles et al. studied 90 school children (45 in 2 groups) with ages ranging between 9 and 13 years and reported improvement in static motor performance (31) and a significant improvement in motor speed task on 53 adults and 152 children (10) after 10 days yoga intervention. A systematic review article mentioned that yoga and meditation intervention nurture mindfulness and may be a feasible and effective method of building resilience in childhood and adolescence (32). Fifty-seven healthy male adult volunteers showed significant improvements in psychomotor task performance (33 34), attention span, processing speed, attention alternation ability, and performance in interference tests (26) after yoga practices. Yoga may be an effective method to increase awareness and performance in cognitive, emotional, or social behavior (35). Our results are in concurrence with the above studies. All above mentioned studies have a common limitation, i.e., small sample size (2124), suggesting less statistical power, no proper controlled group, no controlled environment or noise (26), and a short duration of yoga practices (1–2 h/day) (2124). Whereas in this study, we had a large sample size (n = 210 in yoga and equal participants in controlled group) with controlled environment and comparably long duration of intervention as mentioned above.

The trail making task evaluates fine motor coordination, visual–motor integration, visual perception, and cognitive planning ability (36). Our study reflects the positive effects of yoga on academic performance, processing speed, and attention in school children. It was reported earlier that yoga practice improves ability of the individual to control visual distraction leading to increased ability for prepotent inhibition or orientational processing (orients to specific objects in the attentional field) (37) and reduces planning time and improves execution time (7). This effect could be a major factor for lowering time duration for matching alphabets with relevant numbers implying that automatic-response generation was lower in the yoga group compared with control group in this study. Electroencephalography (EEG) (38) and functional magnetic imaging (39) studies implicated frontal lobe activation during performance of TMT and while matching component of alternating letters of the alphabets and consecutive numbers activated the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, precentral gyrus, cingulate gyrus and medial frontal gyrus, and supplementary motor area, which are sensitive to executive functioning (40). These neuroimaging studies suggest that the improvement in the performance of TMT in this study may be due to the improvement of frontal lobe activation in school children following 10 days of intense yoga practice. Another study reported that a short-term yogic practice was associated with a physiological relaxation responses (41), leading to enhanced performance speed on color naming (42), decreased automatic responses (43, 44), and reaction time involving an attentional task (4548) suggest yoga may enhances parasympathetic dominance with improved frontal lobe activation.

Yoga brings about positive changes in behavior and mental health of school children (24, 49). The results of this study are in accordance with the earlier findings indicating a significant relationship of self-efficacy with academic achievements (50). Yoga practices in this study showed positive changes toward the three domains of self-efficacy, i.e., academic domain, social domain, emotional domain, and overall total domain. The social self-efficacy pertains to children’s capability to deal with social challenges, academic self-efficacy refers to children’s perceived capability to master academic affairs, and self-regulatory efficacy deals with children’s capability to resist peer pressure to engage in high risk activities (51). Mindfulness-based mental training study showed reduced level of stress and enhances mindfulness self-efficacy awareness and attention training, and positive states of mind (52). Similarly, this training also testified lowering of psychological distress, such as tension–anxiety, confusion–bewilderment, fatigue–inertia, and vigor–activity parameters in medical students (53). Further studies on yoga practices showed improvement in greater awareness of the feelings associated with stress, and it may enhance coping abilities possibly as part of the process of developing mindfulness or related to cognitive, emotional, or social development (23, 54, 55). This leads to academic performance, alertness, and academic excellence that are concerned with the quantity and quality of learning attained with yoga or mental training intervention. The academic performance and alertness in school children was high who practiced yoga and reported low stress in children (56). The regular practice of yoga can improve Sattva Guna (balanced personality trait) among students paving the way for their academic excellence. The academic excellence is essential to provide opportunities for students to work together to improve their understanding of concepts in their academic core that helps students to train problem solving and collaborative learning strategies (6). Another major component for higher academic performance is better planning ability with self-regulatory behavior (57). Studies suggest that yoga-based education program improve planning and execution skills in school boys (58). In addition to this, the beneficial effects of meditation on middle school children showed an increased state of restful alertness and greater capacity for self-reflection, self-control, and flexibility as well as improved academic performance that may facilitate the growth of social–emotional capacities necessary for regulating the emotional labiality and interpersonal stress of adolescence (59).

The findings of this study extended previous research on either self-efficacy or psychomotor performance in children, which were assessed simultaneously in this study. Yoga group children reported higher self-efficacy particularly in academic, social, and emotional domain with greater improvement in performance of psychomotor skills and executive functions, which require selective attention, concentration, and visual scanning abilities, and reduced the planning and execution time in TMT task.

Evidence-based previous scientific studies supporting the findings of the preset study on the effect of yogic practices on psychological, behavioral, and cognitive abilities in children are given in Figure 1. Yoga practices reduce visual, audio, and mental distractions, which helps in reducing stress and anxiety in children. Once mental distraction reduced, the mental abilities will be increased with the improvement in their behavioral skills.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Evidence-based beneficial effects of yoga in school children.

The findings of this study are interesting and straightforward, and the study has a major limitation in the selection of school children that was not random in the control group because of unavailability of non-yoga practitioner children in the yoga institute. There was also no follow-up, and so, it is not known whether improvements in test performance and self-efficacy are sustained. Despite of aforementioned limitations, the results of our study suggest that yoga-based intervention in school children can improve attention, motor function, and different domains of personality. Initial research on the usefulness of yoga for children and adolescents is promising, more systematic studies including long-term randomized controlled trials (RCTs) with follow-up and active control groups are needed.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fped.2016.00062

References

  • 1.Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev (1977) 84:191–215. 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bandura A, Cervone D. Differential engagement of self-reactive influences in cognitive motivation. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process (1986) 38:92–113. 10.1016/0749-5978(86)90028-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Caprara GV, Gerbino M, Paciello M, Di Giunta L, Pastorelli C. Counteracting depression and delinquency in late adolescence. Eur Psychol (2010) 15:34–48. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Thomasson P, Psouni E. Social anxiety and related social impairment are linked to self-efficacy and dysfunctional coping. Scand J Psychol (2010) 51:171–8. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00731.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Deshpande S, Nagendra HR, Nagarathna R. A randomized control trial of the effect of yoga on gunas (personality) and self esteem in normal healthy volunteers. Int J Yoga (2009) 2:13–21. 10.4103/0973-6131.43287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Tikhe SG, Nagendra HR, Tripathi N. Ancient science of yogic life for academic excellence in university students. Anc Sci Life (2012) 31:80–3. 10.4103/0257-7941.103179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Manjunath NK, Telles S. Improved performance in the Tower of London test following yoga. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (2001) 45:351–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Telles S, Hanumanthaiah B, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR. Improvement in static motor performance following yogic training of school children. Percept Mot Skills (1993) 76:1264–6. 10.2466/pms.1993.76.3c.1264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Naveen KV, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR, Telles S. Yoga breathing through a particular nostril increases spatial memory scores without lateralized effects. Psychol Rep (1997) 81:555–61. 10.2466/PR0.81.6.555-561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dash M, Telles S. Yoga training and motor speed based on a finger tapping task. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (1999) 43:458–62. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Telles S, Nagarathna R, Vani PR, Nagendra HR. A combination of focusing and defocusing through yoga reduces optical illusion more than focusing alone. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (1997) 41:179–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Telles S, Singh N, Balkrishna A. Finger dexterity and visual discrimination following two yoga breathing practices. Int J Yoga (2012) 5:37–41. 10.4103/0973-6131.91710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lawrence D, Paul C. The Complete Guide to Yoga for Fitness Professionals. London: Bloomsbury Publishing; (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills (1958) 8:271–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Muris P. A brief questionnaire for measuring self-efficacy in youths. J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2001) 23:145–9. 10.1023/A:1012783504852 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed New York: Lezak MD; (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Schunk DH. Self-efficacy and achievement behaviors. Educ Psychol Rev (1989) 1:173–208. 10.1007/BF01320134 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Telles S, Raghavendra BR, Naveen KV, Manjunath NK, Kumar S, Subramanya P. Changes in autonomic variables following two meditative states described in yoga texts. J Altern Complement Med (2013) 19:35–42. 10.1089/acm.2011.0282 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Koenig KP, Buckley-Reen A, Garg S. Efficacy of the get ready to learn yoga program among children with autism spectrum disorders: a pretest-posttest control group design. Am J Occup Ther (2012) 66:538–46. 10.5014/ajot.2012.004390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hagins M, Haden SC, Daly LA. A randomized controlled trial on the effects of yoga on stress reactivity in 6th grade students. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med (2013) 2013:607134. 10.1155/2013/607134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mehta S, Shah D, Shah K, Mehta S, Mehta N, Mehta V, et al. Peer-mediated multimodal intervention program for the treatment of children with ADHD in India: one-year followup. ISRN Pediatr (2012) 2012:419168. 10.5402/2012/419168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Streeter CC, Whitfield TH, Owen L, Rein T, Karri SK, Yakhkind A, et al. Effects of yoga versus walking on mood, anxiety, and brain GABA levels: a randomized controlled MRS study. J Altern Complement Med (2010) 16:1145–52. 10.1089/acm.2010.0007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.White LS. Reducing stress in school-age girls through mindful yoga. J Pediatr Health Care (2012) 26:45–56. 10.1016/j.pedhc.2011.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Berger DL, Silver EJ, Stein RE. Effects of yoga on inner-city children’s well-being: a pilot study. Altern Ther Health Med (2009) 15:36–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Booth JR, Burman DD, Meyer JR, Lei Z, Trommer BL, Davenport ND, et al. Neural development of selective attention and response inhibition. Neuroimage (2003) 20:737–51. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00404-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Prakash R, Dubey I, Abhishek P, Gupta SK, Rastogi P, Siddiqui SV. Long-term Vihangam Yoga meditation and scores on tests of attention. Percept Mot Skills (2010) 110:1139–48. 10.2466/pms.110.3c.1139-1148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Manna A, Raffone A, Perrucci MG, Nardo D, Ferretti A, Tartaro A, et al. Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation. Brain Res Bull (2010) 82:46–56. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Subramanya P, Telles S. Performance on psychomotor tasks following two yoga-based relaxation techniques. Percept Mot Skills (2009) 109:563–76. 10.2466/pms.109.2.563-576 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sharma VK, Das S, Mondal S, Goswami U, Gandhi A. Effect of Sahaj Yoga on neuro-cognitive functions in patients suffering from major depression. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (2006) 50:375–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chambers R, Lo BCY, Allen NB. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognit Ther Res (2007) 32:303–22. 10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Telles S, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR, Desiraju T. Physiological changes in sports teachers following 3 months of training in yoga. Indian J Med Sci (1993) 47:235–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Greenberg MT, Harris AR. Nurturing mindfulness in children and youth: current state of research. Child Dev Perspect (2012) 6:161–6. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00215.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Manjunath NK, Telles S. Factors influencing changes in tweezer dexterity scores following yoga training. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (1999) 43:225–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Naranjo JR, Schmidt S. Is it me or not me? Modulation of perceptual-motor awareness and visuomotor performance by mindfulness meditation. BMC Neurosci (2012) 13:88. 10.1186/1471-2202-13-88 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pradhan B, Nagendra HR. Normative data for the letter-cancellation task in school children. Int J Yoga (2008) 1:72–5. 10.4103/0973-6131.43544 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Halpern J, Cohen M, Kennedy G, Reece J, Cahan C, Baharav A. Yoga for improving sleep quality and quality of life for older adults. Altern Ther Health Med (2014) 20:37–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Chan D, Woollacott M. Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved? J Altern Complement Med (2007) 13:651–7. 10.1089/acm.2007.7022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Segalowitz SJ, Unsal A, Dywan J. CNV evidence for the distinctiveness of frontal and posterior neural processes in a traumatic brain-injured population. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (1992) 14:545–65. 10.1080/01688639208402844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Moll J, de Oliveira-Souza R, Moll FT, Bramati IE, Andreiuolo PA. The cerebral correlates of set-shifting: an fMRI study of the trail making test. Arq Neuropsiquiatr (2002) 60:900–5. 10.1590/S0004-282X2002000600002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Zakzanis KK, Mraz R, Graham SJ. An fMRI study of the trail making test. Neuropsychologia (2005) 43:1878–86. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Singh Y, Sharma R, Talwar A. Immediate and long-term effects of meditation on acute stress reactivity, cognitive functions, and intelligence. Altern Ther Health Med (2012) 18:46–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Foris D. The effect of meditation. J Undergrad Res (2005) 8:1–4. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bhargava R, Gogate MG, Mascarenhas JF. Autonomic responses to breath holding and its variations following pranayama. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (1988) 32:257–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Telles S, Raghavendra BR, Naveen KV, Manjunath NK, Kumar S, Subramanya P. Changes in autonomic variables following two meditative states described in yoga texts. J Altern Complement Med (2013) 19:35–42. 10.1089/acm.2011.0282 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Telles S, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR. Autonomic changes during “OM” meditation. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol (1995) 39:418–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Pradhan B, Nagendra H. Immediate effect of two yoga-based relaxation techniques on attention in children. Int J Yoga (2010) 3:67–9. 10.4103/0973-6131.72632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Luders E, Kurth F, Mayer EA, Toga AW, Narr KL, Gaser C. The unique brain anatomy of meditation practitioners: alterations in cortical gyrification. Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:34. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Telles S, Singh N, Bhardwaj AK, Kumar A, Balkrishna A. Effect of yoga or physical exercise on physical, cognitive and emotional measures in children: a randomized controlled trial. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health (2013) 7:37. 10.1186/1753-2000-7-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.White LS. Yoga for children. J Pediatr Nurs (2009) 35:277–83, 295. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tenaw Y. Relationship between self-efficacy, academic achievement and gender in analytical chemistry at Debre Markos College of teacher education. Afr J Chem Educ (2013) 3:3–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Muris P. Measure – the self-efficacy questionnaire for children. J Psychoeduc Assess (2007) 25:341–55. 10.1177/0734282907300636 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chang VY, Palesh O, Caldwell R, Glasgow N, Abramson M, Luskin F, et al. The effects of a mindfulness-based stress reduction program on stress, mindfulness self-efficacy, and positive states of mind. Stress Heal (2004) 20:141–7. 10.1002/smi.1011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Rosenzweig S, Reibel DK, Greeson JM, Brainard GC, Hojat M. Mindfulness-based stress reduction lowers psychological distress in medical students. Teach Learn Med (2003) 15:88–92. 10.1207/S15328015TLM1502_03 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Smith JA, Greer T, Sheets T, Watson S. Is there more to yoga than exercise? Altern Ther Health Med (2011) 17:22–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Melville GW, Chang D, Colagiuri B, Marshall PW, Cheema BS. Fifteen minutes of chair-based yoga postures or guided meditation performed in the office can elicit a relaxation response. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med (2012) 2012:501986. 10.1155/2012/501986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kauts A, Sharma N. Effect of yoga on academic performance in relation to stress. Int J Yoga (2009) 2:39–43. 10.4103/0973-6131.53860 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Tseng MH, Chow SM. Perceptual-motor function of school-age children with slow handwriting speed. Am J Occup Ther (2000) 54:83–8. 10.5014/ajot.54.1.83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rangan R, Nagendra HR, Bhat GR. Planning ability improves in a yogic education system compared to a modern. Int J Yoga (2008) 1:60–5. 10.4103/0973-6131.41033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rosaen C, Benn R. The experience of transcendental meditation in middle school students: a qualitative report. Explore (NY) (2006) 2:422–5. 10.1016/j.explore.2006.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Frontiers in Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES