Abstract
High-flying insect migrants have been shown to display sophisticated flight orientations that can, for example, maximize distance travelled by exploiting tailwinds, and reduce drift from seasonally optimal directions. Here, we provide a comprehensive overview of the theoretical and empirical evidence for the mechanisms underlying the selection and maintenance of the observed flight headings, and the detection of wind direction and speed, for insects flying hundreds of metres above the ground. Different mechanisms may be used—visual perception of the apparent ground movement or mechanosensory cues maintained by intrinsic features of the wind—depending on circumstances (e.g. day or night migrations). In addition to putative turbulence-induced velocity, acceleration and temperature cues, we present a new mathematical analysis which shows that ‘jerks’ (the time-derivative of accelerations) can provide indicators of wind direction at altitude. The adaptive benefits of the different orientation strategies are briefly discussed, and we place these new findings for insects within a wider context by comparisons with the latest research on other flying and swimming organisms.
This article is part of the themed issue ‘Moving in a moving medium: new perspectives on flight’.
Keywords: flight orientation, flow sensing, optomotor responses, turbulence directionality cues, migration strategies
1. Introduction
The sampling of insects migrating high in the air started as early as the 1920s and 1930s [1], but the realization that these insects could exhibit sophisticated ‘in-flight’ behaviour had to wait until the application of radar to entomology in the late 1960s [2,3]. It then became evident that nocturnal migrants, cruising at altitudes of several hundred metres above the ground, frequently shared well-defined heading directions that might persist for an hour or so and, in the larger species at least, cause an individual's trajectory to differ significantly from that of the wind ([3], and references therein). Frequency distributions of headings recorded over a short period (approx. tens of minutes) could be remarkably tight with circular standard deviations (s) approximately 15° (e.g. [4]) although s values of approximately 30° might be more typical. An individual's alignment is also stable over short timescales (a few seconds)—there is no evidence of rapid yawing around the mean direction [3, p. 246].
The migrants are generally too far apart for orientation to be maintained by visual reference to each other [5] and, in any case, mutual references of this sort would show signs of drift in mean headings over the very large areas (hundreds or even thousands of square kilometres) over which orientation patterns have been observed [6–9]. This implies that orientation cues are uniformly present over similar areas, and so the phenomenon is not generally a response to local ground features. (There is very little evidence that high-flying migrants react to ‘leading lines’ on the ground [3]—there is one report of large migrating insects occasionally changing their flightpaths so that they became channelled along the course of a large river [10], and smallish day-flying insects have been found to react to coastlines in some circumstances [11]). Despite the normally broad-scale nature of the flight orientations, there are some examples of bimodal heading distributions, i.e. where different species take up different orientations in response to the same aerial environment [4,5].
For some time, the function of this ‘common orientation’ phenomenon among high-flying migrants was unclear—did it materially improve a migrant's ability to reach an ecologically appropriate destination, or did it have another function (e.g. to reduce dispersal in a migrating population [12] or improve flight stabilization [3, p. 243])? Recent radar studies have demonstrated, however, that the flight orientations (along with flight-altitude selection) optimize displacement in seasonally favourable directions in some UK Lepidoptera such as Autographa gamma (the silver Y moth) [13–16].
The early radar studies established that orientation direction is generally related to wind direction. For example, on some occasions, the mean heading closely followed the downwind direction despite veering of the wind with altitude [2,17]. In other cases, a (relatively large) off-wind orientation angle to the wind was maintained after a substantial shift in the wind direction [4,18]. There was also evidence that insects take account of wind velocity by flying preferentially at altitudes with fast-moving and stable wind streams (see references in [19]). In some of these cases, the insects appear to be reacting to a wind-related feature [19], rather than a proxy cue for wind speed (such as local maxima in air temperature at the top of an inversion). In an interesting, although unusual, group of cases from the Middle Niger area in Mali, West Africa, night-flying insects (probably acridoid grasshoppers) were observed to head towards, and move in, a preferred geographical direction (approx. 30°–40°) in light winds (approx. 2–4 m s−1) from varying directions but with a distinct upwind component [4]. Although this indicates that the insects involved were using some sort of compass sense, they must still have perceived that the wind at high altitude was light enough for them to achieve this movement, because backwards drift was not observed.
Despite many informative case studies, investigations with the early scanning radars were constrained by their labour-intensive operation and data-extraction methodologies. Recently, very long runs (approx. 10 years) of data from continuously operating entomological radars have been analysed including, for the first time, extensive records from day-flying migrants [20]. These analyses revealed that, where migrants were numerous enough to form analysable events, wind-related orientations were extremely common, almost ubiquitous, in medium-sized (approx. 10–70 mg) insects flying in the day as well as at night.
The question thus arises as to how the high-altitude wind-mediated headings are selected and maintained and, especially, what sensory modalities are being used by the migrants. Here we review the evidence for the candidate mechanisms, postulate a new ‘turbulent jerks’ mechanism and consider how the various types of observed orientation might form part of an adaptive migration strategy. Finally, we discuss parallels and dissimilarities of the insect orientation cues to those used by other flying and swimming taxa, with special reference to the utility of turbulence in providing cues for different types of organisms, ranging from jellyfish to birds.
2. Mechanisms for the selection and maintenance of wind-related orientation
Orientation with reference to the concurrent wind velocity seemingly relies on either visual responses to the apparent movement of ground images over the ommatidia of the insect's eye, and/or is sensed through turbulent velocity or temperature structures in the atmosphere [4,5,19,21,22]. These mechanisms are now discussed in turn.
(a). Visual perception of relative ground movement
Insects rely heavily on the optomotor response and other visually mediated behaviours for flight stabilization and manoeuvres near the ground (e.g. [23,24]). For example, locusts take off more or less into the wind, and then climb until they are about to be blown backwards. This course is not tolerated and so the insect turns down- or crosswind. However, if an individual continues to climb, and the wind speed increases with height, there will come a point where a ‘preferred retinal velocity’ may be exceeded, which might provoke descent and landing (e.g. [25]). This apparent difficulty was rationalized by postulating a ‘maximum compensatory height’ (m.c.h.) above which the ‘grain size’ of the ground pattern will no longer be optically resolvable and/or the speed of image movement will be too slow to evoke a response [25]. Above the m.c.h., therefore, an optomotor response would no longer operate and the migrant would be free to be carried by the wind (with no wind-related orientation). There are occasional examples where this appears to occur, even in broad daylight, e.g. in the painted lady butterfly, Vanessa cardui, migrating up to at least 300 m above the ground where ‘many appeared to be drifting … as though allowing themselves to be carried NW by the wind. Some of these drifters were spinning slowly (like drifting leaves) with no attempt to maintain a constant orientation’ [26].
The ‘maximum compensatory height’ concept would, nonetheless, seem to require modification in view of the regular occurrence of narrow distributions of wind-related headings at high altitude, both during the day and at night. It would appear that, as a migrant insect continues to climb, the optomotor reactions shown near the surface must become untenable or are deliberately overridden. If migrants are indeed monitoring drift by the apparent movement of ground features, they must be replacing a particular ‘grain’ size in the ground pattern with another (coarser) one as they ascend, so that pattern elements continue to be resolvable.
Unless there are considerable wind speed increases with altitude, the higher the insect flies the slower will be the angular velocity of ground features passing beneath it; thus a deterioration in the precision of downwind orientation with altitude would be expected if vision is the primary modality for wind-related orientation. In fact, the contrary seems to be the case—the angular dispersion of headings observed by radar tends to decrease with altitude even when there is little change of wind speed with height [5,17,20,21]—a result more in keeping with a turbulence cue (see fig. 1 in [21]) rather than a visual one. However, in some cases, the apparent improvement in orientation performance with altitude may be confounded by changes in species composition, so further investigation of this topic is required.
Although the angular velocities at which ground features appear to move would be very slow at high altitudes, the possibility that insects can sense wind drift to some extent by visual reference to the ground during the day or in moonlight seems, on the face of it, quite plausible. More problematic is whether the mechanism would function at altitudes up to 1 km or more on dark nights, as radar observers agree that common orientation occurs under all illuminance levels [2,4,5]. It should be noted that examples of orientation have been observed in remote locations (e.g. in Mali and Niger) where there were few artificial light sources on the ground that might, potentially, have facilitated visual perception of drift [2,4,17].
Some insects are known to have extremely sensitive night vision [27–29]; for example, the halictid sweat bee, Megalopta genalis, can see landmarks well enough to navigate through the understory of a tropical forest at night when illumination levels from the background foliage were as low as 2 × 10−5 cd m−2 (10–20 times dimmer than starlight illumination) [30]. Moths, in particular, are noted for their ability to see extremely faint visual cues, and possess scotopic colour vision (e.g. [31]). Astonishing as these feats are, insects foraging near the surface are able to use angular rates of background image motion which are much higher than those available to high-flying migrants (and in some cases, the forager may hover and use temporal and spatial photon summation to improve sensitivity in very dim light). As far as insects flying at high altitudes are concerned, a key question might be: can they respond to transverse angular velocities over the ground within the range of about ±0.3–0.1° s−1 when terrain luminance was as low as 2 × 10−8 lamberts (6.37 × 10−5 cd m−2) and where the viewing platform (the insect's eye) will presumably be subject to some atmospheric turbulence [32]. The tentative conclusion in that case [32] was that the observed degree of close orientation to the wind did not seem possible in starlight illumination levels, although crude differentiation between, say, approximate crosswind and along-wind flight may have been achievable.
Another issue concerns predictions from optical orientation models which depend on the insect being able to maintain certain angular rates of lateral and/or longitudinal movement in relation to the ground using optomotor anemotaxis (for instance, to orientate with the flow the migrant could adjust its heading so that the apparent ground movement lateral to its body axis tends to zero). Such expectations do not seem to be met under some observed (particularly crosswind orientation) conditions. If, for example, insects were heading at an angle > 45° away from the downwind and showing a symmetrical frequency distribution of headings, then the resulting angular velocity distribution would be highly skewed (figure 1a). Consequently, if one postulates that the insects orientate so that the speed of apparent ground movement transverse to their body axes was not zero, but some preferred value, then the expected symmetric distribution of transverse angular rates about a preferred mean would result in a highly skewed heading distribution (figure 1b). However, this is not what is observed—headings are generally rather symmetrical about the mean (figure 3c) and not significantly different from a von Mises (circular normal) distribution [4].
Alternatively, one might postulate that the migrants maintain a ratio of lateral to apparent backward angular rates (i.e. a specific drift angle) assuming both rates were high enough to be perceived (cf. [33]). However, this would still not predict the observed symmetrical distributions for off-wind headings (although it would account for the observed altitude independence of both the standard deviation and the direction of off-wind headings) [4,5].
(b). Turbulence-induced cues
The alternative to a visual mechanism is that some intrinsic feature of the wind itself enables insect sensing of wind direction at high altitude. Such ideas have a long history (see references in [4]) but have been given a precise conceptualization only recently. There appear to be three putative mechanisms: (i) temperature ‘ramp-cliff’ structures [22]; (ii) a turbulent velocity mechanism [21,22] and (iii) a new theory (which we propound below) namely, a turbulent ‘jerks’ mechanism.
‘Ramp-cliff’ patterns are observed in a variety of turbulent shear flows in the atmosphere and are characterized by a gradual increase in temperature by as much as several degrees (the ramp) followed by a sharp decrease (the cliff) (or the order may be reversed in ‘cliff–ramp’ patterns). In each case, the cliffs form along the line of diverging flow between eddies. The suggestion was that a migrant aligned with the wind direction would detect near-periodic temperature fluctuations, but would feel more random fluctuations if flying crosswind [22]. There are no size restrictions for this theory, so the cues could, in principle, be used by migratory insects and by birds. The temperature ramps would be expected to show associations with gradients in either mean temperature or mean wind-speed shear, but an analysis of radar data from the UK produced no evidence that the degree of common orientation in nocturnally migrating insects was associated with such gradients [22].
Turning to how migrants might deduce the mean wind direction from turbulent velocity cues, the main concern has been the perception that small-scale eddies in the atmosphere are locally isotropic (i.e. invariant with respect to direction), even if these motions were derived from larger-scale anisotropic motions. Reynolds et al. [21,22] formulated fluid-dynamic models suggesting how small-scale turbulent velocity fluctuations and turbulent accelerations might nonetheless provide cues by which insects might align themselves approximately with the direction of the wind flow and, in fact, also account for wind-related layering [19,21,34]. A key prediction from these models was that insects using the wind-mediated cues will be somewhat ‘misled’ by the action of the Ekman spiral – the deflection of the mean wind direction owing to the Coriolis effect so that, in the Northern Hemisphere, surface winds blow to the left of winds aloft (and vice versa in the Southern Hemisphere). When the Ekman spiral is in full effect, insects responding to the turbulence cues should (theoretically) tend to head to the right of the mean wind line in the Northern Hemisphere (and to the left in the Southern Hemisphere). This prediction is best tested by the orientations of medium-sized insects, which might be expected to adopt a relatively unsophisticated strategy of heading downwind (see §4b below). Several extensive studies have now shown that nocturnal insects in this size category (body mass 10–70 mg) migrating over the UK routinely exhibit common orientation aligned close to the downwind direction, but typically offset to the right of the flow by an average of approximately 10–20° [20,21,34].
In contrast with a strategy of straightforward downwind orientation, some larger-sized Lepidoptera like A. gamma in the UK exhibit a complex strategy of ‘compass-biased downstream orientation’ (CBDO), one element of which involves deviating their heading a certain amount from the flow direction towards a seasonally preferred direction of movement (PDM) in order to (partially) correct for drift [15,35]. These migrants still need to detect the downwind direction, of course, but one would expect that the telltale presence of right offsets indicating the turbulence mechanism might be masked by the above-mentioned shift of headings towards the PDM. However, a careful analysis revealed that turbulence-induced offsets were still visible in the moth drift corrections, because offsets were considerably larger when the wind direction was to the left of the PDM (when turbulence-induced offsets and the drift corrections would both be on the right and thus additive), than when the flow was to the right of the PDM (when the two offsets would oppose each other) [36]. This finding did not apply to nocturnally migrating songbirds, indicating that these do not use turbulence to detect the flow, and presumably rely on visual assessment of drift to infer the flow direction [36].
As the Ekman spiral is typically not present in convective daytime atmospheres, the orientation of day-flying and crepuscular insects would not be expected to show significant directional bias in the flight heading offsets, and this is what was observed (i.e. headings were not systematically offset to either the left or the right of the flow) [20]. The mechanism by which these diurnal and crepuscular migrants identify the flow direction is currently unclear; it could be visual or turbulence-related, or a combination of both [20].
Further evidence in support of the turbulence mechanism could be obtained by observations in the Southern Hemisphere, testing for left-of-wind-line offsets, as predicted by the theory. However, the only systematic observations in the Southern Hemisphere were recorded by an insect-monitoring radar in inland New South Wales (Australia), and it was found that night-flying Australian plague locust (Chortoicetes terminifera) orientations were related to the wind but were shifted to the right of the downwind when locusts were moving northwards (the predominant situation in the prevailing winds from the east) but orientations were left-shifted when the insects were moving southwards [37]. While these results do not support the turbulence theory, they do not necessarily contravene it either, because strong-flying insects like C. terminifera may have complex orientations of the CBDO sort, and these orientations may well obscure any relatively subtle effects of the Coriolis response.
The sensory processes by which airborne insects actually detect small air flows remain to be elucidated. The turbulent velocity mechanisms postulated [21,22] will be quite weak, and so mechanoreceptors on the antennae (particularly those associated with Johnston's organ on the antennal pedicel [38,39]) or wind-sensitive setae on the head and prosternum [40] may have the advantage that they are positioned in front of the vortices produced by the flapping wings and so may be better at detecting small differential pressures on either side of the body. Recent findings show that the Johnston's organs are range-fractionated, i.e. they are capable of encoding antennal vibrations of low to high frequencies with exquisite sensitivity [38,41]. Although such high-frequency sensors may serve as turbulence sensors, the ‘jerks’ mechanism proposed in the next section does not demand sensitive mechanoreceptors specialized for detecting the weak airflows.
3. A new turbulence mechanism: anisotropic jerks
Our original theory [21,22] identified a putative mechanism by which migratory insects could determine the mean wind direction from cues provided by turbulent velocity fluctuations. This leaves open the question as to whether or not these turbulence cues would be obscured by disturbances of the surrounding airflow created by wing flapping, and leaves open the identification of the sensory organs. Preliminary numerical simulations of wing flapping suggest that such ‘flight noise’ amplifies rather than masks the turbulent cues (AM Reynolds 2016, unpublished) but these studies have not been verified either theoretically or experimentally. Similarly, some insects have body areas with abundant airflow-detecting mechanosensory hairs, some of extreme sensitivity (e.g. [42]) but these groups of sensors have various functions, and it is not clear if they could operate when migrants are in flight. Here we show that such open questions could be sidestepped, and confidence in the turbulence mechanisms bolstered, by showing that its predictions are not specific to the original modelling assumptions. We show that because migratory insects by virtue of their inertia necessarily lag behind the turbulent air currents, they experience ‘jerks’ (also known as ‘jolts’ and defined as the first derivative of acceleration); these are minimized when the insects are flying downwind, or to the right of the mean wind line when the Ekman spiral is present in the Northern Hemisphere. Jerks are rapidly changing accelerations that tend to produce a loss of flight control (and also whiplash effects), and so will be felt without the need for specialist sensory organs. The mechanism is much more robust in respect of cues being masked by flight noise because the wing-flapping process itself is likely to produce stable flight that would contrast with the turbulence-induced jerks by the wind.
(a). An illustrative example
Consider for illustrative purposes an inert body moving in a one-dimensional turbulent stream. The velocity of the body, v, can be related to that of the surrounding air stream, u, by Stokes law,
3.1 |
where τ is the body's characteristic aerodynamic response time. The accelerations, A, and velocities, u, of the surrounding air stream can, as shown in the electronic supplementary material, be modelled stochastically by
3.2 |
where and are the two timescales of turbulence, describing the largest scales of motion and the small (dissipative) scales of motion, σu is the standard deviation of the turbulent velocity fluctuations, C0 is Kolmogorov's constant, a universal constant of turbulence, is viscosity and ɛ is the rate of change of turbulent kinetic energy, The quantity dW is Gaussian noise with mean zero and variance dt. Equations (3.1) and (3.2) can be combined into a single set of equations for the jerks, J, (and accelerations A′) experienced by the body
3.3 |
This can be rewritten as
3.4 |
where
3.5 |
can be recognized as the conditional average jerk. This can be seen by carefully comparing equation (3.4) with equation (3.2), as both equations have the same form. But whereas the first term in equation (3.2) causes accelerations to be centred on zero, the first term in equation (3.4) causes jerks to the centred on , i.e. have conditional average given by . For larger migrants (with ), airborne in atmospheric turbulence (which has ), equation (3.5) reduces to and as a consequence the mean amplitude of the jerks experienced by the migrants is:
3.6 |
where σv is the standard deviation of the velocity of the body, which here is assumed to be approximately equal to σu. This assumption is justifiable when the body is responsive to most turbulent velocity fluctuations in the surrounding airstream, i.e. when τ < T. The mean amplitude of the jerks experienced by the migrants is thus dependent on the turbulent velocity fluctuations of the airstream, i.e. on σu. As a consequence, in three-dimensional turbulence, the average magnitudes of the jerks experienced along the body-line will be minimized when the migrant is flying downwind (or to the right of the mean wind line when the Ekman spiral is present), i.e. in the direction in which turbulent velocity fluctuations are largest.
Similarly, for small migrants () with but this orientation cue will be of little value because the jerks will inhibit flight control, and so inhibit the maintenance of their heading. In the original theory, the observed absence of common orientation in small insects was attributed to the absence of orientation cues.
These results are not specific to the modelling assumptions used above and can be deduced from general principles.
(b). Deriving the result from general principles
The distribution of jerks, accelerations and velocities experienced by a migrant can, in general, be modelled by
3.7 |
where the subscripts denote Cartesian coordinates. Equation (3.3) is the simplest example of this. The functions and a and b are determined by the Fokker–Planck equation
3.8 |
where P is the joint distribution of positions, velocity, accelerations and jerks. Equation (3.3) corresponds to the case when these velocities, accelerations and jerks are stationary, homogeneous and Gaussian. Here for simplicity (but without loss of generality), P is taken to be both stationary and homogeneous. Integrating equation (3.8) over all jerks and then over all accelerations then gives
3.9a |
and
3.9b |
which implies that the conditional average jerks are non-vanishing, whereas the mean accelerations do vanish. If example, P, is Gaussian then
3.10 |
here is the organism's acceleration variance (assumed to be isotropic and so like accelerations in the surrounding airstream) and denotes the inverse of the organism's velocity covariance matrix (which, like the airstreams velocity covariance matrix, will be decidedly anisotropic). Equation (3.10) is the three-dimensional analogue of equation (3.5). It is straightforward to show (see electronic supplementary material) that the average magnitude of the jerks is smallest along the mean wind line (or to right of the mean wind line in an Ekman spiral atmosphere in the Northern Hemisphere).
(c). Further remarks
The postulated effect requires three timescales, two of which are provided by the turbulence: the ‘integral’ timescale (which characterizes the larger, energy-containing motions) and the ‘dissipation’ timescale (which characterizes the smallest motions where turbulence is dissipated as heat). The third timescale (which allows for jerks) is provided by the insect and its aerodynamic response time. It is important to note that although airborne insects would experience jerks, they are a peculiarity of being carried along by a turbulent flow, and will not be evident in measurements made at a fixed location (e.g. by sonic anemometers). Likewise, the effect (like other turbulence cues) would be very difficult to investigate in the laboratory, which is why we advocate an indirect approach when looking for evidence such as a bias in headings of insects apparently attempting to orientate downwind (see above).
If turbulence cues are used to align an insect approximately with the direction of the wind flow, could the jerks be used to distinguish whether it was pointing upwind from pointing downwind? Examination of equation (3.10) reveals that jerks in the downwind direction tend to have an upward component, while those in the upwind direction tend to have a downwind component, so this coupling could, in principle, be used to distinguish the two directions.
Lastly, we speculate that jerks as orientation cues could ‘come for free’ evolutionarily, because being jerked about would amount to a loss of flight control, and so would be naturally avoided; a by-product of this would be downwind flight orientation. If it is indeed the case that migrants do not use visual cues for orientation under certain circumstances, then this mechanism provides a tentative explanation for how high-flying migrants may orient downwind. It also follows that insects would need to keep track of turbulence-induced jerks in time, in order to minimize them. This invokes the importance of a ‘working memory’ that would remember the average jerks sensed over a period of time, in order to elicit the appropriate orientation changes.
4. Migration strategies
The various orientation behaviours of both day-flying and nocturnal insect migrants flying within and outside their ‘flight boundary layer’ (the layer of the atmosphere within which the insects' self-powered flight speed exceeds the wind speed, allowing control of migration direction) were detailed in a recent review [43]. Assessing the evidence accumulated about high-altitude migration in insects, one can distinguish the following putative strategies (figure 2), which are based on certain of the categories identified by Chapman et al. [35] for movement in a fluid medium (air or water).
(a). Orientation in small insects: quasi-passive downwind transport
This category comprises organisms whose powers of self-propelled locomotion are either non-existent (e.g. aerially dispersing mites or spiders) or very weak (all small insects). Even so, small insects such as aphids can exert some control over when they take off or land, and on their height of flight, so their transport might be termed ‘quasi-passive’ [44]. The airspeeds of the insects involved will make a negligible contribution to their ground speed, and so one might assume that they would show no systematic orientation. This sometimes appears to be the case: radar observations of masses of nocturnally migrating small insects, such as the brown planthopper, Nilaparvata lugens (mass approximately 1–2 mg) [45] or rice leaf-roller moth, Cnaphalocrocis medinalis (approx. 8 mg) [46], showed no evidence of common orientation, indicating that orientation was random (or perhaps highly multimodal). However, it cannot be assumed that all small migrants' headings will be random because layers of unidentified insects predominantly in the 16–32 mg mass range, flying just after dawn, showed a significant degree of common orientation [47]. Layers of even smaller insects (aphids) flying at dawn also apparently showed orientation patterns [48–50] but the possibility that the pattern could have been due to small numbers of orientating large insects present within the layers was not completely excluded. The environmental cues used to achieve these orientations (which can sometimes be strongly crosswind), and their purpose, remain mysterious but as they are unable to influence the insect's speed and direction significantly, we assume that any adaptive benefit is not directly related to a ‘vector navigation’ strategy.
(b). Active downwind orientation
The most common wind-related orientation strategy in medium-sized radar-detectable insects is orientation in, or close to, the downwind direction (and here we include nocturnal migrants that show a slight offset due to them being ‘misled’ by the action of the Ekman spiral; see above). As winds at flight altitude are often approximately 8–20 ms−1 [3], the option of a track direction significantly different from the downwind direction is severely limited in these insects (with self-powered flight speeds of 1–2.5 ms−1), and may well be undesirable as the main benefit of windborne migration is using the power of the wind for long-range movement. Circumstances where simple downwind orientation may be adaptive include:
— migration in arid and semi-arid environments where persistent downwind movement is optimal because it takes insects (e.g. African armyworm moth, Spodoptera exempta) towards wind convergence zones where rain is likely to fall [51];
— movement in regions where seasonally prevailing winds happen to take insects in suitable directions (e.g. seasonal movements associated with the Intertropical Convergence Zone (ITCZ) in West Africa [3,51]);
— cases where favourable destination zones can be located in any direction from the source area (for example, nocturnal flight in the eastern spruce budworm moth, Choristoneura fumiferana, is consistently oriented downwind [52], probably because suitable (i.e. lightly or undefoliated) stands of host trees do not necessarily lie in any particular direction in the vast boreal forests of eastern North America).
It may be that the intrinsic rates of increase in some species are so high that they can ‘afford’ large migration losses, and a simple distance-maximizing strategy represents the best option.
(c). Compass-biased downstream orientation
The CBDO strategy, mentioned above, achieves a compromise between moving rapidly and moving in a PDM [35]. The strategy has been well studied in some larger migratory moths in the UK, particularly A. gamma [13,16,36,53,54], where it has the following characteristics:
— if the wind on the night in question is highly unfavourable for movement in the seasonally preferred direction the migration is suppressed, or limited to short flights only (see also [55]);
— if winds are broadly favourable, but the downstream direction is more than approximately 20° from the PDM, the moths deviate their heading so that it lies between downstream and the PDM, but they do not attempt full compensation for drift;
— if winds are highly favourable, and the downstream direction is within 20° of the PDM, the migrants do not make significant corrections for drift—in other words, they essentially orient downwind.
High-flying insects generally do not undertake complete compensation, even where this is possible, because the strategy becomes very energy-inefficient as the flow diverges from the PDM, and the migrant makes little progress towards its new habitat. We note that nocturnally migrating songbirds, capable of higher airspeeds than insects, were not always able to fully compensate for drift, even though they often flew at 90° to the wind direction [56], and partial compensation is the most common strategy in nocturnal songbird migrants [54,57–59]. In any case, full compensation is not usually necessary in insects because the migration goal (e.g. an overwintering area) would normally be a broad ecological zone, not a specific location.
Clearly, detailed analysis of this migration strategy depends on being able to identify a PDM, and studying A. gamma has the advantage that the species very largely quits the UK (and other parts of Northern Europe) in autumn and re-invades the following spring; so it can be plausibly assumed that the PDM is north in spring and south in autumn [15]. Alternatively, the PDM can be estimated from data by a regression method [60], which produces rather similar values (viz. 353° in spring and 210° in autumn) [54]. All in all, the migratory orientation of A. gamma seems remarkably effective, and the moths' utilization of strong winds blowing in favourable directions allows their ground speeds within a migratory bout to match and sometimes exceed those of songbirds, despite their self-powered airspeeds being only one-third or one-quarter that of songbirds [54,57]. In other cases, interpretation of observed orientation patterns is likely to be much more difficult because, inter alia, the place to which the migrant is travelling may well be unknown. Consequently, the observer cannot determine whether the migrant is steering a preordained track in spite of the wind, or whether the wind is drifting the migrant from a track it could have kept to under more favourable circumstances [1, p. 157].
(d). Full drift
In the case of A. gamma, described in the last section, orientation is usually fairly close to downwind, but this is not always the case for migrants observed in other situations [4,12]. As the headings become progressively more crosswind, the strategy can approach one of full drift where orientation remains approximately constant irrespective of the wind direction (but still with the proviso that oriented groups do not show general backwards drift). An example is provided by orientations of insects at Mara River (1°03′ S, 35°15′ E) in southwestern Kenya (figure 3), which probably included noctuids such as Spodoptera exempta. As a whole, the migrants showed a strong propensity to orientate northwards in a range of wind conditions (with downstream directions west through northeast). In some of these cases, the migrants were simply heading downstream (cf. figure 1a in [4]), but on other occasions (sometimes on the same night), they were experiencing strong sideways drift (figure 3c); for example, their orientation could be approximately 75° from the downwind direction in winds of approximately 10 m s−1, when the average insect airspeed was estimated to be 2.5 m s−1 [4].
Orientations of this sort can be difficult to interpret. Migratory flight in the strong, easterly winds that usually occurred during the first half of the night in Kenya results in rapid, downwind displacement to the west. In line with this, S. exempta outbreaks are known to first spread progressively westwards, but then populations are taken north, eventually into Ethiopia and Yemen [61]. This latter movement is associated with the northward movement of the ITCZ, and a seasonal wind change from northeasterly or easterly to southeasterly or southerly. As seasonally adaptive movements would occur anyway, just due to downwind displacement, how should the strong northwards orientations be construed? Are the migrants attempting to enhance movement in the ‘right’ seasonal direction, or is this over-interpretation of the orientation data in the light of what we know of ‘ultimate’ destinations?
(e). Orientation towards a fixed directional reference, with upwind displacement
In this case, heading is also in a constant compass direction, but unlike the S. exempta moths mentioned in the previous section—who seemed to ignore or be unaware of the severe drift that they were undergoing—migrants only show the present type of orientation when their airspeed is greater than the wind speed. In other words, certain large insects can detect that winds at high altitude are (rather unusually) weak enough for them to move in a fixed geographical direction with a distinct upwind component, and do so move—see the example of the northeastwards-moving grasshoppers in Mali (§1). Here the observations were all made at one site, and this could be a site-specific response, namely, an adaptive movement up the Middle Niger flood plains against the prevailing northeast trade winds [62]. Again, however, some caution is needed in the attribution of adaptiveness to directional orientations—we do not know how long the slow upwind movements were maintained, for example, and there may be other (more mechanistic) explanations. For example, day-flying desert locusts in lighter winds tended to head persistently into wind as a result of an optomotor response (e.g. [25]). Such upwind movements do not result in any notable displacements, however, and long-range locust displacements are downwind.
In the wandering glider dragonfly (Pantala flavescens) migrating at night over the Bohai Sea in eastern China during late summer, there were occasions when the wind was light, even at altitude, so that the migrants could orientate to the southwest and displace in approximately the same direction, regardless of how the wind direction changed [63]. (This species is, incidentally, known to compensate for wind drift, and to optimize flight speed in response to wind, when flying near the surface [64]). Displacement southwestward in late summer in China is evidently adaptive because it facilitates movement to the latitudes warm enough for the dragonflies to overwinter [63].
In butterflies there is abundant evidence that seasonal migrations take place in PDMs, particularly where movements are largely independent of the wind direction because the migrants are flying near the ground (within their ‘flight boundary layer’) using a solar-based compass [43,64,65]. Therefore, there seems every reason to expect that if conditions were suitable for movement in a fixed geographical direction at high altitudes it would occur; this behaviour is, after all, equivalent to the compass-mediated elements involved in partial compensation strategies (such as CBDO). The mechanism of the compass sense in night-fliers is unknown, but the most likely bases are, perhaps, the Earth's magnetic field [66] or a time-compensated celestial cue (such as the band of the Milky Way) [67,68].
5. Synthesis and inter-phylum comparisons
Radar-based investigations of insect orientation at high altitudes have combined case studies of instructive events with (more recently) extensive analyses of large datasets. Some remarkable phenomena have been revealed, and progress made in understanding the proximate (sensory) mechanisms influencing the observed orientations, and how these facilitate migration outcomes in some circumstances. Nonetheless, many uncertainties remain. One of the most problematic is the extent to which the insects use an apparently obvious cue: the visual perception of apparent ground movement. It seems difficult to believe that insects would not take advantage of this mechanism, particularly in conditions of relatively high illuminance (daylight or moonlight), given the superlative motion sensitivity of their visual systems. As mentioned above, however, there are certain night-time situations where a combination of very slow angular rates of background movement and very low reflectance values seem to militate against the use of the visual sense. Additionally, there are crosswind orientation scenarios where the skewness of the observed heading distributions do not accord with predictions of an optomotor-type response.
Considering the competing mechanism—various small-scale anisotropies in turbulent flow that provide cues as to the wind direction—some key predictions of this hypothesis have been met. In particular, our studies found the systematic bias in heading offsets expected when Ekman dynamics were likely to prevail (i.e. in a stably stratified nocturnal boundary layer) but not when such conditions were unlikely (i.e. in convective daytime conditions). It seems, therefore, that nocturnal insect migrants make considerable use of turbulence cues to align themselves with respect to the wind direction. The wind-related orientation mechanism employed by day-flying migrants is still unclear. We note that during the daytime in the UK, surface wind direction is a good predictor of direction at ‘cruising flight’ altitudes (G Hu, SJ Clark, JW Chapman 2016, unpublished data) so, in theory, migrants could detect the wind direction by optomotor means while near the surface, and compare this direction with, say, a time-compensated sun compass cue, and then decide whether or not to abort migration. If the wind direction was favourable they could continue to ascend, maintaining direction with respect to that compass cue and progress in a favourable direction even if they were no longer able to monitor ground image movement.
A more radical suggestion would be that high-flying insects do not make use of visual cues for wind-related orientation because their reactions to atmospheric turbulence, necessary for maintaining flight control, already provide a built-in mechanism for wind-finding (§3c). A more cautious position would be that the high-altitude wind-sensing, like most orientation behaviours, is likely to involve more than one sensory modality, and migrants integrate elements of both visual and mechanosensory reception, with one or other predominant depending on circumstances. We also need to bear in mind that some of the observed orientations (particularly in small insects) may have nothing to do with assisting directional movement.
Finally, we point out some parallels and dissimilarities in turbulence-sensing across various animal taxa in fluid media, as these may not be obvious at first sight. The mechanism proposed by Reynolds et al. [21] is a good approximation for smallish insects (with aerodynamic response times less than Lagrangian autocorrelation time of the turbulence) but as the insect size increases it becomes progressively untenable and probably should not be applied to insects with masses > 100 mg (i.e. with aerodynamic response times > 100 ms); it, therefore, would not apply to birds or bats. These size limitations do not apply to the variant of the turbulence theory proposed in Reynolds et al. [22], which suggests that larger aerial migrants might be able to use weak turbulent cues to orientate, particularly considering the recent identification of extremely sensitive wind-detecting hairs on bat wings [69]. Considering the new turbulent jerks theory—this would not apply to very small insects (less than or equal to 1 mg), which could not use the orientation cues because they cannot orient (maintain a constant heading) in the presence of turbulence. Large migrants, such as birds and bats, might not be able to detect the cues because their magnitude decreases as the size of the migrant increases, and so the jerks mechanism may not be feasible for these taxa.
Turning to orientation in marine flows, there is evidence that some pelagic animals, including fish [70], jellyfish [71] and juvenile turtles [72], may be able to orientate with respect to ocean currents, e.g. showing positive rheotaxis (facing into the current), where there are no obvious visual, tactile or hydrodynamical cues. The question is: are there any parallels between the putative turbulence mechanisms employed in wind-related orientations and turbulence mechanisms that might provide cues as to water current direction (taking into account that turbulent cues in marine flows will be weaker than in those in the atmosphere). Among the available directional cues are the current shears in the surface Ekman layer of the ocean due to wind stress, or detection of the movements of surface waves themselves. However, there are plenty of other (non-turbulence) possibilities for orientation in relation to the flow, e.g. large-scale odour plumes [73]. As many oceanic gyres are predictable, orientation could be achieved in an indirect way, e.g. by some map sense (using magnetic information [73,74]) and an evolved preferred direction. It seems likely that rheotactic orientation involves multisensory cueing.
6. Conclusion
Although there is much that we still do not understand, the identification and evaluation of putative mechanisms for directed responses to flow has recently developed in novel and sometimes surprising ways, as evidenced by the utilization of turbulence cues by high-flying insect migrants. The present addition of a ‘jerks’ model has augmented the robustness of the turbulence-sensing hypothesis. With the benefit of hindsight these mechanisms were hiding in plain view, but their identification nonetheless exemplifies the value of multidisciplinary approaches. In order to make real progress, however, the putative sensory mechanisms for detecting turbulent fluctuations, accelerations and ‘jerks’ need to be identified. Insects are known to detect air flow cues via the antennal Johnston's organs, or cephalic hair system, but it is not clear if the same system could enable the sampling of ‘jerks’. These are open questions, which we hope will be addressed in the next few years. Additionally, broad comparative studies across phyla moving through water and air may provide new insights into the generality of flow-detection mechanisms, as has been achieved by similar approaches for other kinds of movement phenomena [54,75,76].
Supplementary Material
Authors' contributions
J.W.C. conceived the review, A.M.R. conceived and developed the new turbulent ‘jerks’ hypothesis for insect orientation at altitude, and all authors contributed to writing the review.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
Rothamsted Research is a national institute of bioscience strategically funded by the UK Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC). The visiting scholarship of G.H. at Rothamsted was funded by Nanjing Agricultural University and the Priority Academic Programme Development of Jiangsu Higher Education Institutions. S.P.S. acknowledges the Ramanujan Fellowship from the Department of Science and Technology, India.
References
- 1.Johnson CG. 1969. Migration and dispersal of insects by flight. London, UK: Methuen. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schaefer GW. 1976. Radar observations of insect flight. In Insect flight (ed. Rainey RC.), pp. 157–197. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Drake VA, Reynolds DR. 2012. Radar entomology: observing insect flight and migration. Wallingford, UK: CABI. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Riley JR, Reynolds DR. 1986. Orientation at night by high-flying insects. In Insect flight: dispersal and migration (ed. Danthanarayana W.), pp. 71–87. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Riley JR. 1989. Orientation by high-flying insects at night: observations and theories. In Orientation and navigation—birds, humans and other animals. Papers presented at the Conf. of the Royal Institute of Navigation, Cardiff, 6–8 April 1989 (Paper no. 21). London, UK: The Royal Institute of Navigation.
- 6.Hobbs SE, Wolf WW. 1996. Developments in airborne entomological radar. J. Atmos. Ocean. Technol. 13, 58–61. ( 10.1175/1520-0426(1996)013%3C0058:DIAER%3E2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lang TJ, Rutledge SA, Stith JL. 2004. Observations of quasi-symmetric echo patterns in clear air with the CSU-CHILL polarimetric radar. J. Atmos. Ocean. Technol. 21, 1182–1189. ( 10.1175/1520-0426(2004)021%3C1182:OOQEPI%3E2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rennie SJ, Illingworth AJ, Dance SL, Ballard SP. 2010. The accuracy of Doppler radar wind retrievals using insects as targets. Meteorol. Appl. 17, 419–432. ( 10.1002/met.174) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rennie SJ. 2014. Common orientation and layering of migrating insects in southeastern Australia observed with a Doppler weather radar. Meteorol. Appl. 21, 218–229. ( 10.1002/met.1378) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Reynolds DR, Riley JR. 1979. Radar observations of concentrations of insects above a river in Mali, West Africa. Ecol. Entomol. 4, 161–174. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2311.1979.tb00571.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Russell RW, Wilson JW. 2001. Spatial dispersion of aerial plankton over east-central Florida: aeolian transport and coastline concentrations. Int. J. Remote Sens. 22, 2071–2082. ( 10.1080/01431160118367) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wolf WW, Westbrook JK, Raulston JR, Pair SD, Lingren PD. 1995. Radar observations of orientation of noctuids migrating from corn fields in the lower Rio Grande Valley. Southwest. Entomol. (Suppl 18), 45–61. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chapman JW, Reynolds DR, Mouritsen H, Hill JK, Riley JR, Sivell D, Smith AD, Woiwod IP. 2008. Wind selection and drift compensation optimize migratory pathways in a high-flying moth. Curr. Biol. 18, 514–518. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.080) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chapman JW, Reynolds DR, Hill JK, Sivell D, Smith AD, Woiwod IP. 2008. A seasonal switch in compass orientation in a high-flying migrant moth. Curr. Biol. 18, R908–R909. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chapman JW, Nesbit RL, Burgin LE, Reynolds DR, Smith AD, Middleton DR, Hill JK. 2010. Flight orientation behaviours promote optimal migration trajectories in high-flying insects. Science 327, 682–685. ( 10.1126/science.1182990) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chapman JW, Bell JR, Burgin LE, Reynolds DR, Pettersson LB, Hill JK, Bonsall MB, Thomas JA. 2012. Seasonal migration to high latitudes results in major reproductive benefits in an insect. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 14 924–14 929. ( 10.1073/pnas.1207255109) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Riley JR, Reynolds DR. 1983. A long-range migration of grasshoppers observed in the Sahelian zone of Mali by two radars. J. Anim. Ecol. 52, 167–183. ( 10.2307/4594) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Drake VA. 1983. Collective orientation by nocturnally migrating Australian plague locusts, Chortoicetes terminifera (Walker) (Orthoptera Acrididae): a radar study. Bull. Entomol. Res. 73, 679–692. ( 10.1017/S0007485300009287) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Reynolds AM, Reynolds DR, Riley JR. 2009. Does a ‘turbophoretic’ effect account for layer concentrations of insects migrating in the stable night-time atmosphere? J. R. Soc. Interface 6, 87–95. ( 10.1098/rsif.2008.0173) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hu G, Lim KS, Reynolds DR, Reynolds AM, Chapman JW. 2016. Wind-related orientation patterns in diurnal, crepuscular and nocturnal high-altitude insect migrants. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 32 ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00032) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Reynolds AM, Reynolds DR, Smith AD, Chapman JW. 2010. A single wind mediated mechanism explains high-altitude ‘non-goal oriented’ headings and layering of nocturnally-migrating insects. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 765–772. ( 10.1098/rspb.2009.1221) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Reynolds AM, Reynolds DR, Smith AD, Chapman JW. 2010. Orientation cues for high-flying nocturnal insect migrants: do turbulence-induced temperature and velocity fluctuations indicate the mean wind flow? PLoS ONE 5, e15758 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0015758) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Srinivasan MV, Poteser M, Kral K.. 1999. Motion detection in insect orientation and navigation. Vision Res. 39, 2749–2766. ( 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00002-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Srinivasan MV. 2011. Visual control of navigation in insects and its relevance for robotics. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 535–543. ( 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.020) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kennedy JS. 1951. The migration of the desert locust (Schistocerca gregaria). I. The behaviour of swarms. II. A theory of long-range migrations. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 235, 163–290. ( 10.1098/rstb.1951.0003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Giuliani D, Shields O. 1997. Migratory activity in Vanessa cardui (Nymphalidae) during 1992 in western North America, with special reference to eastern California. J. Lepid. Soc. 51, 256–263. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Warrant E, Dacke M.. 2011. Vision and visual navigation in nocturnal insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 56, 239–254. ( 10.1146/annurev-ento-120709-144852) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Warrant E. 2015. Visual tracking in the dead of night. Science 348, 1212–1213. ( 10.1126/science.aab2185) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sponberg S, Dyhr JP, Hall RW, Daniel TL. 2015. Luminance-dependent visual processing enables moth flight in low light. Science 348, 1245–1248. ( 10.1126/science.aaa3042) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Warrant EJ, Kelber A, Gislén A, Greiner B, Ribi W, Wcislo WT. 2004. Nocturnal vision and landmark orientation in a tropical halictid bee. Curr. Biol. 14, 1309–1318. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.057) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kelber A, Balkenius A, Warrant EJ. 2003. Colour vision in diurnal and nocturnal hawkmoths. Integr. Comp. Biol. 43, 571–579. ( 10.1093/icb/43.4.571) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Riley JR, Kreuger U, Addison CM, Gewecke M.. 1988. Visual detection of wind-drift by high-flying insects at night: a laboratory study. J. Comp. Physiol. A 169, 793–798. ( 10.1007/BF00610968) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Baker PS, Gewecke M, Cooter RJ. 1984. Flight orientation of swarming Locusta migratoria. Physiol. Entomol. 9, 247–252. ( 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1984.tb00706.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Aralimarad P, Reynolds AM, Lim KS, Reynolds DR, Chapman JW. 2011. Flight altitude selection increases orientation performance in high-flying nocturnal insect migrants. Anim. Behav. 82, 1221–1225. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.09.013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Chapman JW, Klaassen RHG, Drake VA, Fossette S, Hays GC, Metcalfe JD, Reynolds AM, Reynolds DR, Alerstam T. 2011. Animal orientation strategies for movement in flows. Curr. Biol. 21, R861–R870. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Chapman JW, Nilsson C, Lim KS, Bäckman J, Reynolds DR, Alerstam T, Reynolds AM. 2015. Detection of flow direction in high-flying insect and songbird migrants. Curr. Biol. 25, R733–R752. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.074) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wang HK. 2008. Evaluation of insect monitoring radar technology for monitoring locust migrations in inland eastern Australia. PhD thesis, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia.
- 38.Sane SP, Dieudonne A, Willis MA, Daniel TL. 2007. Antennal mechanosensors mediate flight control in moths. Science 315, 863–866. ( 10.1126/science.1133598) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sane SP, McHenry MJ. 2009. The biomechanics of sensory organs. Integr. Comp. Biol. 49, 8–23. ( 10.1093/icb/icp112) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Pflüger HJ, Tautz J.. 1982. Air movement sensitive hairs and interneurons in Locusta migratoria. J. Comp. Physiol. 145, 369–380. ( 10.1007/BF00619341) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dieudonné A, Daniel TL, Sane SP. 2014. Encoding properties of the mechanosensory neurons in the Johnston's organ of the hawk moth, Manduca sexta. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 3045–3056. ( 10.1242/jeb.101568) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Shimozawa T, Murakami J, Kumagai T. 2003. Cricket wind receptors: thermal noise for the highest sensitivity known. In Sensors and sensing in biology and engineering (eds Barth FG, Humphrey JAC, Secomb TW), pp. 145–159. Wien, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Chapman JW, Reynolds DR, Wilson K. 2015. Long-range seasonal migration in insects: mechanisms, evolutionary drivers and ecological consequences. Ecol. Lett. 18, 287–302. ( 10.1111/ele.12407) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Reynolds AM, Reynolds DR. 2009. Aphid aerial density profiles are consistent with turbulent advection amplifying flight behaviours: abandoning the epithet ‘passive’. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 137–143. ( 10.1098/rspb.2008.0880) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Riley JR, Cheng XN, Zhang XX, Reynolds DR, Xu GM, Smith AD, Cheng JY, Bao AD, Zhai BP. 1991. The long distance migration of Nilaparvata lugens (Stål) (Delphacidae) in China: radar observations of mass return flight in the autumn. Ecol. Entomol. 16, 471–489. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2311.1991.tb00240.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Riley JR, Reynolds DR, Smith AD, Edwards AS, Zhang X-X, Cheng X-N, Wang H-K, Cheng J-Y, Zhai B-P. 1995. Observations of the autumn migration of the rice leaf roller Cnaphalocrocis medinalis (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae) and other moths in eastern China. Bull. Entomol. Res. 85, 397–414. ( 10.1017/S0007485300036130) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Reynolds DR, Smith AD, Chapman JW. 2008. A radar study of emigratory flight and layer formation at dawn over southern Britain. Bull. Entomol. Res. 98, 35–52. ( 10.1017/S0007485307005470) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Drake VA. 1985. Solitary wave disturbances of the nocturnal boundary layer revealed by radar observations of migrating insects. Bound. Lay. Meteorol. 31, 269–286. ( 10.1007/BF00120896) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hendrie LK, Irwin ME, Liquido NJ, Ruesin WG, Mueller EA, Voegtlin DJ, Achtemeier GL, Steiner WM, Scott RW. 1985. Conceptual approach to modelling aphid migration. In The movement and dispersal of agriculturally important biotic agents (eds MacKenzie DR, Barfield CS, Kennedy GG, Berger RD, Taranto DJ), pp. 541–582. Baton Rouge, LA: Claitor's Publishing Division. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Farrow RA. 1986. Interaction between synoptic scale and boundary layer meteorology on microinsect migration. In Insect flight: dispersal and migration (ed. Danthanarayana W.), pp. 185–195. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pedgley DE, Reynolds DR, Tatchell GM. 1995. Long-range insect migration in relation to climate and weather: Africa and Europe. In Insect migration: tracking resources through space and time (eds Drake VA, Gatehouse AG), pp. 3–29. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Greenbank DO, Schaefer GW, Rainey RC. 1980. Spruce budworm (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) moth flight and dispersal: new understanding from canopy observations, radar, and aircraft. Mem. Entomol. Soc. Can. 110, 1–49. ( 10.4039/entm112110fv) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chapman JW, Lim KS, Reynolds DR. 2013. The significance of midsummer movements of Autographa gamma: Implications for a mechanistic understanding of orientation behavior in a migrant moth. Curr. Zool. 59, 360–370. ( 10.1093/czoolo/59.3.360) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Chapman JW, Nilsson C, Lim KS, Bäckman J, Reynolds DR, Alerstam T. 2016. Adaptive strategies in nocturnally migrating insects and songbirds: contrasting responses to wind. J. Anim. Ecol. 85, 115–124. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12420) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Feng H-Q, Wu X-F, Wu B, Wu K-M. 2009. Seasonal migration of Helicoverpa armigera (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) over the Bohai Sea. J. Econ. Entomol. 102, 95–104. ( 10.1603/029.102.0114) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Van Doren BM, Horton KG, Stepanian PM, Mizrahi DS, Farnsworth A. 2016. Wind drift explains the reoriented morning flights of songbirds. Behav. Ecol. 27, 1122–1131. ( 10.1093/beheco/arw021) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Alerstam T, Chapman JW, Bäckman J, Smith AD, Karlsson H, Nilsson C, Reynolds DR, Klaassen RHG, Hill JK. 2011. Convergent patterns of long-distance nocturnal migration in noctuid moths and passerine birds. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 3074–3080. ( 10.1098/rspb.2011.0058) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.McLaren JD, Shamoun-Baranes J, Bouten W.. 2012. Wind selectivity and partial compensation for wind drift among nocturnally migrating passerines. Behav. Ecol. 23, 1089–1101. ( 10.1093/beheco/ars078) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Horton KG, Van Doren BM, Stepanian PM, Hochachka WM, Farnsworth A, Kelly JF. 2016. Nocturnally migrating songbirds drift when they can and compensate when they must. Sci. Rep. 6, 21249 ( 10.1038/srep21249) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Green M, Alerstam T.. 2002. The problem of estimating wind drift in migrating birds. J. Theor. Biol. 218, 485–496. ( 10.1006/jtbi.2002.3094) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rose DJW, Dewhurst CF, Page WW. 2000. The African armyworm handbook: the status, biology, ecology, epidemiology and management of Spodoptera exempta (Lepidotera: Noctuidae), 2nd edn Chatham, UK: Natural Resources Institute. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Farrow RA. 1990. Flight and migration in acridoids. In Biology of grasshoppers. (eds Chapman RF, Joern A), pp. 227–314. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Feng H-Q, Wu K-M, Ni Y-X, Cheng D-F, Guo Y-Y. 2006. Nocturnal migration of dragonflies over the Bohai Sea in northern China. Ecol. Entomol. 31, 511–520. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2311.2006.00813.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Srygley R.B., Dudley R.. 2008. Optimal strategies for insects migrating in the flight boundary layer: mechanisms and consequences. Integr. Comp. Biol. 48, 119–133. ( 10.1093/icb/icn011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Mouritsen H, Frost BJ. 2002. Virtual migration in tethered flying monarch butterflies reveals their orientation mechanisms. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 10 162–10 166. ( 10.1073/pnas.152137299) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Guerra PA, Gegear RJ, Reppert SM. 2014. A magnetic compass aids monarch butterfly migration. Nat. Commun. 5: 4164 ( 10.1038/ncomms5164) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Dacke M, Baird E, Byrne M, Scholtz CH, Warrant EJ. 2013. Dung beetles use the Milky Way for orientation. Curr. Biol. 23, 298–300. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.034) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Warrant E, Dacke M.. 2016. Visual navigation in nocturnal insects. Physiology 31, 182–192. ( 10.1152/physiol.00046.2015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Sterbing-D'Angelo S, Chadha M, Chiu C, Falk B, Xian W, Barcelo J, Zook JM, Moss CF. 2011. Bat wing sensors support flight control. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 11 291–11 296. ( 10.1073/pnas.1018740108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Montgomery J, Carton G, Voigt R, Baker C, Diebel C. 2000. Sensory processing of water currents by fishes. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 355, 1325–1327. ( 10.1098/rstb.2000.0693) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Fossette S, Gleiss AC, Chalumeau J, Bastian T, Armstrong CD, Vandenabeele S, Karpytchev M, Hays GC. 2015. Current-oriented swimming by jellyfish and its role in bloom maintenance. Curr. Biol. 25, 342–347. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.050) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kobayashi DR, Farman R, Polovina JJ, Parker DM, Rice M, George H, Balazs GH. 2014. ‘Going with the flow’ or not: evidence of positive rheotaxis in oceanic juvenile loggerhead turtles (Caretta caretta) in the South Pacific Ocean using satellite tags and ocean circulation data. PLoS ONE 9, e103701 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0103701) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Endres CS, Putman NF, Ernst DA, Kurth JA, Lohmann CMF, Lohmann KJ. 2016. Multi-modal homing in sea turtles: modeling dual use of geomagnetic and chemical cues in island-finding. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 19 ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00019) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Putman NF, Endres CS, Lohmann CMF, Lohmann KJ. 2011. Longitude perception and bicoordinate magnetic maps in sea turtles. Curr. Biol. 21, 463–466. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.057) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Gleiss AC, et al. 2011. Convergent evolution in locomotory patterns of flying and swimming animals. Nat. Commun. 2, 352 ( 10.1038/ncomms1350) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hein AM, Hou C, Gillooly JF. 2012. Energetic and biomechanical constraints on animal migration distance. Ecol. Lett. 15: 104–110. ( 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01714.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.