Abstract
Pesticides, including neonicotinoids, typically target pest insects by being neurotoxic. Inadvertent exposure to foraging insect pollinators is usually sub-lethal, but may affect cognition. One cognitive trait, spatial working memory, may be important in avoiding previously-visited flowers and other spatial tasks such as navigation. To test this, we investigated the effect of acute thiamethoxam exposure on spatial working memory in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris, using an adaptation of the radial-arm maze (RAM). We first demonstrated that bumblebees use spatial working memory to solve the RAM by showing that untreated bees performed significantly better than would be expected if choices were random or governed by stereotyped visitation rules. We then exposed bees to either a high sub-lethal positive control thiamethoxam dose (2.5 ng−1 bee), or one of two low doses (0.377 or 0.091 ng−1) based on estimated field-realistic exposure. The high dose caused bees to make more and earlier spatial memory errors and take longer to complete the task than unexposed bees. For the low doses, the negative effects were smaller but statistically significant, and dependent on bee size. The spatial working memory impairment shown here has the potential to harm bees exposed to thiamethoxam, through possible impacts on foraging efficiency or homing.
Animal pollinators are key ecosystem service providers across most terrestrial landscapes. Ecologically, they pollinate around 87.5% of flowering plants worldwide1, and economically are responsible for approximately 9.5% of the value of global food production2. Insects represent the vast majority of animal pollinators3, with bees providing a large contribution to this pollination service3,4. It is therefore of concern that bees are considered to be under threat4,5, and understanding the stressors affecting bees and the mechanisms by which they impair bee behaviour and health is of ecological and economic importance6.
The application of pesticides in the environment, primarily for agriculture and horticulture, has been implicated as a principal threat to pollinating insects7. Whilst pesticides are used to target pest insect species, they may inadvertently harm visiting insect pollinators if applied to a flowering crop. For instance, neonicotinoids are a widely used class of agricultural insecticide8 that act systemically, allowing the pesticide to be transported to all parts of the plant including the nectar and pollen of flowers9,10. Insect pollinators foraging on these flowers can thus be exposed to neonicotinoid residues11. Although concentrations of neonicotinoids in the nectar and pollen of treated plants are unlikely to be high enough to be lethal to bees12, growing evidence is emerging for sub-lethal effects on bee behaviour and physiology13,14,15,16, which may lead to subsequent colony-level impacts17,18,19.
Neonicotinoids act as agonists of insect nicotinic acetylcholine receptors20, disrupting neural transmission and cognitive processes14,15. Correspondingly, olfactory learning and memory have been shown to be impaired by sub-lethal neonicotinoid exposure in honeybees21,22 and bumblebees23. However, the effects of pesticides on spatial learning and memory have been highlighted as an evidence gap, and standard assays to assess effects on behavioural processes such as spatial memory have been called for24. Spatial memory is extremely important to central place foragers, such as bees, both for navigation when returning to the nest after foraging trips25 and for increasing foraging efficiency26. Reduced homing ability of honeybees following acute sub-lethal doses of neonicotinoids has been demonstrated27,28,29, with evidence suggesting that impairment of spatial reference memory (e.g. long-term retention of landmarks30) could be responsible29. Spatial working memory, on the other hand, involves maintaining short-term task-relevant information31, such as remembering and avoiding flowers that have already been depleted in a foraging patch32,33, an efficient foraging strategy often employed by bees and other nectarivores34,35. Here, we investigate the effect of pesticide exposure on spatial working memory in a key pollinator, Bombus terrestris L. (Fig. 1a).
The radial-arm maze (RAM) was originally designed for use in rodents by Olton & Samuelson36 and is a well-established method for studying pharmacological effects on spatial working memory37. The task requires animals to identify and remember previously visited reward locations in order to avoid revisits37, making it particularly relevant to the foraging ecology of nectarivores35. Variations on the RAM have frequently been used in nectarivorous birds e.g.33; however, its potential in bees has been explored to only a limited extent in basic memory research32,38,39. Here we adapt this tool for application to bumblebee foraging, and apply it to study the impact of a stressor on insect cognition.
The aim of this experiment was to test the acute effects of thiamethoxam at concentrations that could be encountered in the field on spatial working memory in the bumblebee B. terrestris. Thiamethoxam is one of three neonicotinoid pesticides currently subject to an EU restriction regarding use on bee-attractive crops, for which a review is imminent40, and was the most widely used on flowering crops in the UK prior to the restriction8. We focus on the consequences of acute doses because these may be particularly severe in the context of spatial memory, where reduced foraging efficiency or failure of foragers to return to the nest could have immediate knock-on effects at the colony level17,19. We first validated our protocol by confirming that bumblebees use spatial memory to complete the RAM task efficiently (which requires bees to visit the eight presented flowers, minimizing revisits, and return to the nest), comparing initial performance of bees prior to pesticide treatment to simulated chance performance. We then tested for an effect of pesticide on RAM performance at a high dose (2.5 ng−1 bee) as a positive control, and two different low doses (LD.091: 0.091 ng−1 bee and LD.377: 0.377 ng−1 bee) based on estimates of field-realistic exposure.
Results
Protocol validation: comparison to chance and stereotypical simulations
Bees could complete a RAM by choosing locations to visit randomly, by using stereotyped movement rules (e.g. “always move clockwise”, “preferentially move upwards”), or by using spatial working memory. Analysis of the final training bout (i.e. prior to pesticide exposure) showed that bees displayed a moderate tendency to travel to the nearest neighbouring flower (“contiguity preference”), and in addition, transition frequency between neighbouring flower pairs was negatively correlated with angle from vertical between flowers (Spearman’s rho = −0.87) implying an additional preference to fly vertically upwards (see Supplementary Fig. S1). However, comparison of observed performance in this trial confirmed that individuals additionally use spatial working memory to complete the task. Observed performance (mean correct choices in the first eight visits, correct choices before first revisit and total revisits) on the final training bout for all bees was compared to simulated performance derived from a Monte Carlo simulation of the task run with 1,000,000 iterations. Bees performed better than both random transition (C) and chance plus stereotypical behaviour (C + S) on all three measures, as simulated means did not fall within the 95% CIs of the observed data (Fig. 2).
Performance on the RAM following pesticide exposure
Sixty one bees were trained on the RAM, after which each was assigned to one of four pesticide doses: 0 ng (n = 16, “Control”), 0.091 ng (n = 14, “LD.091”), 0.377 ng (n = 16, “LD.377”) or 2.5 ng (n = 15, “ High”) and subjected to a final testing bout 45 minutes after exposure to test the effect of pesticide on RAM performance. Four measures of RAM performance were assessed: i) total revisits to previously visited flowers37, representing the number of spatial working memory errors made, ii) correct choices before first revisit41, iii) correct choices in the first eight visits36 and iv) time per visit as a measure of decision-making time.
Total revisits
The most parsimonious model for total revisits contained treatment, size, and their interaction term (Table 1a). In other words, pesticide treatment had a strong effect on the number of revisits bees made to flowers they had already depleted (Fig. 3a), but this depended upon the size of the bee (Fig. 4, Table 2a). To obtain individual effect size estimates for each dose that took into account bee size, we thus split the dataset into two groups: ‘smaller bees’ with a thorax width of ≤5.46 mm (n = 31; mean (range) = 5.21 (4.51–5.46)) and ‘large bees’ >5.46 mm (n = 30; mean (range) = 5.68 (5.48–6.07)), and repeated the analysis separately on the subset data. For the smaller bees there was no effect of treatment on total revisits (ΔAIC between treatment only model and basic model: −0.9). However, for large bees, there was a strong effect of treatment on total revisits (ΔAIC to basic model: 7.36). Both the High and LD.377 groups significantly differed from the Control (High: estimate = 1.032, 95% CIs = [0.517–1.547]; LD.377: estimate = 0.727 [0.147–1.307], indicating that pesticide exposure influenced bee performance even at field-realistic levels.
Table 1. Tables of candidate models (a) using negative binomial GLMMs to investigate the effect of pesticide treatment and bee size on total revisits and (b) using Cox proportional hazards models to investigate the effect of pesticide treatment and bee size on choices before first revisit.
Model | AIC | ΔAIC | wi |
---|---|---|---|
a | Total revisits |
||
Treatment * size | 378.9 | 0 | 0.527 |
Treatment | 381 | 2.18 | 0.177 |
Basic | 381.2 | 2.36 | 0.162 |
Treatment + size | 382.9 | 4.03 | 0.07 |
Size | 383.1 | 4.22 | 0.064 |
b | Choices before first revisit | ||
Treatment | 384.7 | 0 | 0.41 |
Basic | 385.5 | 0.79 | 0.276 |
Treatment + size | 386.5 | 1.81 | 0.166 |
Size | 387.2 | 2.51 | 0.117 |
Treatment * size | 389.8 | 5.11 | 0.032 |
In all cases, the basic model included the constant and the residual variance, with all other models containing the basic model plus the indicated covariates. Models are presented in order of ΔAIC from the best model alongside their respective Akaike weights (wi). The best sets of models which were averaged to obtain model averaged estimates (models <2 ΔAIC from the model with the lowest AIC) are highlighted in bold.
Table 2. Coefficients and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the optimal model or model sets to predict a) total revisits (treatment*size model with colony as a random effect) and b) correct choices before first revisit (model-averaged coefficients for treatment only, treatment + size and basic models.
Parameters | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
a | Total revisits |
||||
Estimate | Std. Error | 95% CIs |
|||
Lower | Upper | ||||
(Intercept) | 6.384 | 3.769 | −1.003 | 13.771 | |
Treatment (High) | −11.010 | 4.471 | −19.773 | −2.247 | |
Treatment (LD.377) | −2.796 | 4.164 | −10.957 | 5.365 | |
Treatment (LD.091) | −2.534 | 4.698 | −11.742 | 6.674 | |
Size | −0.813 | 0.684 | −2.154 | 0.528 | |
Size:Treatment (High) | 2.084 | 0.810 | 0.496 | 3.672 | |
Size:Treatment (LD.377) | 0.566 | 0.762 | −0.928 | 2.060 | |
Size:Treatment (LD.091) | 0.536 | 0.854 | −1.138 | 2.210 | |
b | Correct choices before first revisit | ||||
Parameters | Regression coefficient (b) | Std. Error (SE(b)) | Hazard Ratio (eb) | 95% CIs on Hazard Ratio | |
Lower | Upper | ||||
Treatment (High) | 0.8295 | 0.3937 | 2.292 | 1.060 | 4.959 |
Treatment (LD.377) | 0.3116 | 0.3663 | 1.366 | 0.666 | 2.800 |
Treatment (LD.091) | 0.8353 | 0.3811 | 2.306 | 1.092 | 4.866 |
Size | 0.1964 | 0.452 | 1.217 | 0.502 | 2.952 |
Parameters highlighted in bold are considered important to the model based on 95% CIs.
Correct choices before first revisit
The optimal Cox proportional hazards model included treatment as the only independent variable (Table 1b), implying that pesticide treatment lowered the number of choices bees made before revisiting a drained flower (Fig. 5). The high treatment level had a high hazard ratio (HR; a measure of effect size) with 95% CIs that do not include one (model averaged HR = 2.292 [1.060–4.959]), as did the LD.091 group (model averaged HR = 2.306 [1.092–4.866]), indicating a significant negative effect on the number of correct choices made before the first revisit (Table 2b). We found no evidence of an effect in the LD.377 group (model averaged HR = 1.366[0.666–2.800]). Note that we also found no evidence of a size-dependent effect (ΔAIC to model with interaction term = 5.11), so smaller and large bees were considered within the same analysis.
Time per visit and correct choices in the first eight visits
There was a trend towards fewer correct choices in the first eight visits in bees treated with the high pesticide dose (Fig. 3b). However, there was no significant effect of treatment on correct choices in the first eight visits as model selection found the basic model to be most parsimonious (Supplementary Table S1a). We also found no effect of treatment on time per visit (task duration divided by total revisits; Supplementary Table S1b), although size had a negative effect (estimate = −0.335 [−0.536–−0.135]).
Discussion
Here, we have adapted the RAM to test the effect of a stressor on bumblebee spatial working memory for the first time. We first demonstrated that untreated bees performed better than chance simulations and better than would be expected based on stereotyped movement rules, implying that tested bees were indeed employing spatial working memory. Following this, we tested bees treated with a high dose of the neonicotinoid thiamethoxam as a positive control and observed negative effects on bumblebee spatial working memory. We then investigated how bees performed on the RAM after being treated with two different low doses based on estimated field-realistic exposure and we found small but detectable size-dependent effects on spatial working memory.
Acute exposure to the high, positive control dose of thiamethoxam (2.5 ng−1 bee) negatively affected two of the RAM performance measures assayed. Bees in this treatment group made fewer correct choices before the first revisit compared to the control, suggesting they were less able to remember which flowers they had already depleted. This may represent a simple reduction in spatial memory use or an accompanied greater reliance on a stereotypical-movement-based strategy, which would be consistent with the finding that pesticide-treated bees made their first revisit earlier in their visit sequence. This could be tested in future experiments by interrupting visits (analogous to confinement in the rodent RAM37) to disrupt stereotypical transition sequences42.
Depending on body size, the high pesticide treatment also increased the number of revisits to flowers that had already been depleted during the task. For bees in this treatment group, size had a positive relationship with total revisits, with large bees making up to five times more revisits than smaller bees. This suggests larger bees experienced a stronger effect from the pesticide at the high dose, possibly as a result of absorbing more pesticide in the 45 minute holding period, as larger bees empty their crops (i.e. transfer nectar to the midgut) at a faster rate43. While this is the first demonstration of pesticide-induced spatial working memory impairment in bees, these findings supplement previous laboratory research that found negative effects of neonicotinoids on olfactory learning and memory in honeybees21 and bumblebees23. A plausible mechanism for the cognitive impairments observed in our and other studies is the mode of action of neonicotinoids, which have been shown to disrupt the neurophysiological properties of Kenyon cells in the mushroom bodies of bee brains. These are involved in complex cognitive functions including associative learning and spatial orientation14,15.
We found no effect of treatment, however, on time per visit, suggesting that decision-making time was not affected by pesticide. Correct choices in the first eight visits was also not significantly affected by pesticide treatment; this measure is sensitive only to errors made early in the choice sequence suggesting that the working memory impairment experienced by treated bees primarily took the form of repeated errors later in the trial in the course of finding the last “missed” flowers37.
While it is of biological interest to understand the mechanisms by which sub-lethal doses of pesticides affect bee behaviour, it is crucial to investigate whether these effects are likely to be seen at levels encountered in the field in order to inform policy decisions44,45. We found evidence that spatial working memory was impaired in some measures of RAM performance at both low doses based on field-realistic exposure (LDs) tested. The lower LD.091 treatment resulted in bees making fewer flower choices before they made their first mistake, while the higher LD.377 treatment increased total revisits (spatial memory errors) in large but not smaller bees. Bumblebees exhibit intra-colony size variation46. This experiment used bees at the larger end of the spectrum and only the largest of these were affected, suggesting that in nature only a limited proportion of a colony may be vulnerable to some of the spatial memory impacts seen here. Bees in the size cohort that was found to be strongly affected by pesticide (thorax width >5.46 mm) make up on average 22% of a colony’s foragers, but contribute more to the foraging effort, bringing in 29% of daily forage47 (D. Goulson, unpublished data) making spatial working memory in this section of a colony’s workforce likely to disproportionately affect colony function. In addition, the positive relationship between size and crop-emptying rate43 may mean smaller bees are also affected after a longer amount of time post-exposure than our experiment allowed for.
Could such memory impairment to a cohort of workers have an impact on colony fitness? Spatial working memory is important for foraging efficiency in flower-visiting animals32,35; in bumblebees, increased re-visitation rates result in lower rates of nectar or pollen intake48. Previous research has demonstrated reduced foraging efficiency in terms of the quantity and frequency of pollen collected by pesticide-exposed bumblebees13,17,49,50, which could be explained by the spatial working memory impairment demonstrated in this study. Less efficient foraging by workers may have knock-on effects at the colony level17, where reduced daily nectar and pollen supply could restrict colony growth and even result in colony failure19. Indeed, a recent study found that bumblebee colonies placed in neonicotinoid-treated oilseed rape fields gained less weight than control colonies, which the authors suggest was due in part to reduced foraging efficiency51.
While the RAM apparatus used here simulates most closely within-patch foraging decisions, spatial working memory is also involved in components of bee navigation including vector memory or path integration52, impairment of which could lead to reduced homing ability and consequently loss of foragers from the colony27. Our finding that acute effects can be seen after just 45 minutes indicates forager loss is a realistic possibility, as homing failure may occur while a forager is still in the field following ingestion of contaminated nectar11. Acute neonicotinoid exposure has been reported to cause homing failure in honeybee foragers27,28,29, (but see Stanley et al.50) but whether this is a consequence of spatial working memory impairment is not yet clear. Longer-term spatial reference memory is also used in homing30, and a study by Fischer, et al.29 using honeybees found pesticide-induced disruption of this component of navigation. Our demonstration of impaired spatial working memory suggests that the shorter term, vector component of navigation could also be affected by exposure to neonicotinoids.
We have demonstrated that spatial working memory can be impaired just 45 minutes after a single dose of thiamethoxam. In real agricultural environments, however, bees are likely to be exposed to pesticides chronically over several weeks, typically during the bloom period of mass-flowering crops such as oilseed rape13,51. Evidence suggests that sub-lethal impacts on individuals23 and colonies19 may be more severe following chronic, compared to acute, exposure to pesticides. However, a response following acute exposure does not necessarily predict the presence or magnitude of chronic effects, due to mediating factors such as the rate at which bees clear pesticide from the body53. As such, future research should investigate whether the acute effects found here may be magnified with chronic exposure13. Alone or combined with other known sub-lethal effects54, spatial working memory impairment has the potential to harm bees that are exposed to thiamethoxam. With review of the scientific evidence for the EU neonicotinoid restriction imminent40, it is important that policy makers consider all potential impacts of sub-lethal effects on bee populations.
Materials and Methods
Bees
We obtained seven commercial B. t. audax colonies from Koppert Ltd, Haverhill, UK. Each colony was transferred to a bipartite wooden nest box on arrival, which was connected to a foraging arena (0.5 m × 0.5 m × 0.5 m) by a transparent Perspex tube containing four sliding ‘trap-doors’ to control the flow of bees entering and returning from the arena. When not being trained or tested, bees were allowed unrestricted access to the arena and provided with 43% (w/w) sucrose solution (see Supplementary Methods) ad libitum from two gravity feeders located on the back wall of the arenas in the centre of the artificial flower array (see later section for details on the array). Thirty grams of pollen (in the form of honeybee collected pellets, supplied by Koppert Ltd, Haverhill, UK) were provided every 2–3 days directly into the nest box. Thorax width (between tegulas (wing joints)) of all tested bees was measured following freezing as the mean of three digital calliper measurements (accuracy = 0.01 mm).
Radial-Arm Maze Apparatus
A typical RAM consists of eight arms, or reward locations; an animal must remember which arm(s) it has already visited to avoid revisiting a depleted location36. Adaptation of the RAM for flying animals (e.g. birds) typically applies an “open field” approach where all reward locations can be viewed from the centre of the array but no enclosed arms or central chamber are present33,55,56. In bumblebees, foraging within a patch typically involves flying between flowers or inflorescences, so a RAM apparatus that requires bees to fly represents a more ecologically relevant foraging decision than one where bees walk through maze arms. To adapt the RAM for bumblebees we built a circular array of eight artificial flowers (hereafter ‘flowers’) on a vertical board that constituted the back wall of the experimental arena. Flowers consisted of three square plastic chips glued together, which could be inserted through slots in the board (Fig. 1b.) The blue chip on the arena side of the board acted as a landing platform from which a bee could obtain a reward (a 10 μl droplet of 43% (w/w) sucrose solution) by inserting its proboscis through a 4 mm hole above the slot to reach the chip on the other side. Flowers were immediately replaced with clean ones after a bee had fed (while feeding on the subsequent flower), to remove information about visitation from scent marks57,58.
Wooden baffles (dimensions: 10 × 15 × 0.3 cm) were positioned between each flower so that bees had to fly to the centre of the arena to see the entire maze, as in the rodent RAM36, minimising the use of stereotypical movement rules such as travelling from one flower to the nearest neighbouring flower and preventing bees from seeing any other flowers when on a particular flower, meaning that each represented an independent reward location (see Supplementary Methods).
Group Training
Prior to testing, an hour-long group training period was allowed each day to train bees to use the artificial flowers and to identify motivated foragers (Fig. 6). Feeders were removed from the arena and the flowers were loaded with sucrose solution, which was continuously refilled during the group training session. Bees were allowed free access to the arena and those that spontaneously learnt to feed from the flowers during this period were tagged with a unique numbered disc (Opalith Plättchen, Christian Graze KG, Germany) and returned to the colony. If a feeding bee had already been marked from a previous training session its identity was recorded. After one hour all bees in the arena were returned to the nest box.
Individual Training
The RAM task required bees to visit all eight flowers and return to the nest. The first bee to arrive at the tunnel entrance that had been marked or recorded feeding in that morning’s group training session was chosen as a test subject, and allowed to enter the arena alone and feed from the flowers which had been reloaded with sucrose solution; this time flowers were not refilled after visitation and were instead replaced with clean, empty flowers to remove scent information. Visits to flowers were recorded as either ‘correct’ (flowers that had not yet been visited) or ‘revisits’ (to flowers that had been depleted). ‘Land’, where a bee landed on the platform but did not feed, and ‘approach’, where a bee came within 1 cm of the platform or hole, was also recorded. Flower visits were recorded as soon as the bee inserted its proboscis through the hole. After visiting the flowers the bee was permitted to return to the colony to empty its crop (see Supplementary Methods). The duration of the period in the arena, defined as a foraging bout, was recorded. Bees were given ten training bouts on the maze before pesticide exposure (data from a pilot study showed that the majority of bees reach asymptotic performance on the maze after ten bouts; see Supplementary Material).
Pesticide Exposure and Testing
Acute thiamethoxam doses were calculated to simulate the total pesticide consumed by a bee foraging for one hour on oilseed rape nectar contaminated with thiamethoxam at two concentrations over a range that can be found in the field: 2.4ppb, based on residues found in B. terrestris nectar pots59 and oilseed rape nectar in honeybee crops60 and 10ppb, based on residues in nectar of treated plants12,61. This equated to 0.091 ng of active ingredient per bee at a 2.4ppb concentration and 0.377 ng at 10ppb (for dose calculations see Supplementary Methods). An additional high dose of 2.5 ng per bee (half the acute oral toxicity LD50 for honeybees45) was used to test for effects at non-field-realistic levels, as a positive control.
After ten training bouts each bee was assigned to one of the four pesticide exposures: 0 ng (n = 16, “Control”), 0.091 ng (n = 14, “LD.091”), 0.377 ng (n = 16, “LD.377”) or 2.5 ng (n = 15, “High”). The bee was intercepted on its way to the arena and fed 18.85 μl of sucrose solution containing the relevant dose of pesticide (see Supplementary Methods), after which it was returned to the tunnel where it was held for 45 minutes after exposure to maximise pesticide absorption27 but avoid a decline in motivation. This represents a conservative estimate of the effects as the full dose of pesticide may take longer than this to be absorbed43 or there may be a time lag between absorption and effects being seen62, as thiamethoxam causes functional responses only after metabolism to clothianidin63. Following this the bee was allowed back into the arena for the final testing bout, which followed the same procedure as the training bouts.
Simulation of chance performance
To confirm that bees use spatial working memory to complete the RAM, observed performance (total revisits, choices before the first revisit and correct choices in the first eight visits) on the final training bout for all bees was first compared to chance performance derived from a Monte Carlo simulation of the task run with 1,000,000 iterations (see Supplementary Fig. S3). The chance simulation assumed all flowers were visited with equal probability, so if observed error rate was no less than that obtained from simulated chance performance it would indicate that bees visited flowers at random without using spatial memory.
Animals may also employ stereotypical movement rules, such as travelling to the nearest neighbouring flower, which can increase RAM performance without the use of spatial memory37. The RAM simulates within-patch foraging decisions, a context in which bees are known to employ heuristic movement rules in addition to spatial memory64. To account for this, we ran a second Monte Carlo simulation in which simulated bees did not move between flowers at random, but chose their next target flower according to a transition probability distribution (see Supplementary Fig. S1) obtained from pooled choices made during all final training bouts by actual bees in the experiment38. For example, a simulated bee visiting flower 1 would have a 19% chance of visiting flower 2 next, because the real bees moved to flower 2 on 19% of occasions, but importantly, whether the target flower had already been visited or not did not influence the simulated bees’ decisions. If observed performance was no different to this simulation, it could be concluded that performance was only explained by stereotypical behaviour. On the other hand, if observed performance was better than both the chance and stereotypical simulations it could be assumed spatial memory was being employed for the task in addition to stereotypical behaviour.
Statistical analysis
For each of the four assessed measures of RAM performance, a set of candidate models were fitted. We used an “all-subset” approach, with the full model containing bee size (continuous) and treatment (categorical), plus their interaction, as fixed effects. The comparison set included all subsets of this model, including the basic model containing only the constant and residual variance. Each candidate model also contained “colony” as a random effect. We selected the model or set of models with the lowest Akaike Information Criteria (AIC) as the best fitting model(s)65. Where several models were within two AIC units of the best model, model averaging was carried out to obtain parameter estimates derived from the best set of models including the basic model if applicable66. Final models were validated graphically to assess fit and check that assumptions had been met67.
For total revisits, a negative binomial GLMM (to account for over-dispersion) with log link and colony as a random effect was used. Correct choices in the first eight visits was analysed by considering each choice in the first eight visits as a binary response (success or failure) and using a binomial GLMM with logit link and bee nested within colony as random effects. Time per visit (log-transformed task duration divided by total visits) was analysed using linear mixed models with colony as a random effect. Correct choices before first revisit was subjected to a survival analysis using non-parametric Cox proportional hazards models. All analyses were conducted in R version 3.2.168 (for packages see Supplementary Methods).
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Samuelson, E. E. W. et al. Effect of acute pesticide exposure on bee spatial working memory using an analogue of the radial-arm maze. Sci. Rep. 6, 38957; doi: 10.1038/srep38957 (2016).
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Kate Vaughan-Williams for help with data collection; Dara Stanley, Gemma Baron, Callum Martin, Mark Brown and Aaishah Manan for advice and technical assistance and Dave Goulson for providing worker size distribution data. E.L.’s contribution was partly funded by a Leverhulme Trust Early Career Fellowship. This study was funded by a London BBSRC DTP studentship to E.E.W.S. R.J.G.’s research is supported by the Grand Challenges in Ecosystems and the Environment Initiative at Silwood Park.
Footnotes
Author Contributions E.E.W.S. and E.L. conceived the initial idea and designed the experiments; R.J.G. provided feedback on the experimental design; E.E.W.S. performed the experiment; Z.P.C.-W. and E.E.W.S. designed and performed the spatial simulation; E.E.W.S. and E.L. performed the statistical analyses; E.E.W.S. wrote the manuscript draft and E.L., R.J.G. and E.E.W.S. provided the final edit.
References
- Ollerton J., Winfree R. & Tarrant S. How many flowering plants are pollinated by animals? Oikos 120, 321–326 (2011). [Google Scholar]
- Gallai N., Salles J.-M., Settele J. & Vaissière B. E. Economic valuation of the vulnerability of world agriculture confronted with pollinator decline. Ecological Economics 68, 810–821 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- Calderone N. W. Insect Pollinated Crops, Insect Pollinators and US Agriculture: Trend Analysis of Aggregate Data for the Period 1992–2009. PLoS ONE 7, e37235, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037235 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potts S. G. et al. Global pollinator declines: trends, impacts and drivers. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 25, 345–353 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biesmeijer J. et al. Parallel declines in pollinators and insect-pollinated plants in Britain and the Netherlands. Science 313, 351–354 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gill R. J. et al. Protecting an ecosystem service: approaches to understanding and mitigating threats to wild insect pollinators. Advances in Ecological Research (2016). [Google Scholar]
- Goulson D. Review: An overview of the environmental risks posed by neonicotinoid insecticides. Journal of Applied Ecology 50, 977–987 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- Garthwaite D. et al. Pesticide Usage Survey Report 250: Arable crops in the United Kingdom. (FERA, York, 2012). [Google Scholar]
- Elbert A., Haas M., Springer B., Thielert W. & Nauen R. Applied aspects of neonicotinoid uses in crop protection. Pest Management Science 64, 1099–1105 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Sluijs J. P. et al. Neonicotinoids, bee disorders and the sustainability of pollinator services. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 5, 293–305 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- Rortais A., Arnold G., Halm M.-P. & Touffet-Briens F. Modes of honeybees exposure to systemic insecticides: estimated amounts of contaminated pollen and nectar consumed by different categories of bees. Apidologie 36, 71–83 (2005). [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Bayo F. & Goka K. Pesticide Residues and Bees – A Risk Assessment. PLoS ONE 9, e94482, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094482 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gill R. J. & Raine N. E. Chronic impairment of bumblebee natural foraging behaviour induced by sublethal pesticide exposure. Functional Ecology 28, 1459–1471 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- Palmer M. J. et al. Cholinergic pesticides cause mushroom body neuronal inactivation in honeybees. Nat Commun 4, 1634, doi: 10.1038/ncomms2648 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffat C. et al. Chronic exposure to neonicotinoids increases neuronal vulnerability to mitochondrial dysfunction in the bumblebee (Bombus terrestris). The FASEB Journal 29, 2112–2119 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Sluijs J. P. et al. Conclusions of the Worldwide Integrated Assessment on the risks of neonicotinoids and fipronil to biodiversity and ecosystem functioning. Environ Sci Pollut Res 22, 148–154, doi: 10.1007/s11356-014-3229-5 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gill R. J., Ramos-Rodriguez O. & Raine N. E. Combined pesticide exposure severely affects individual-and colony-level traits in bees. Nature 491, 105–108 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehorn P. R., O’Connor S., Wackers F. L. & Goulson D. Neonicotinoid pesticide reduces bumble bee colony growth and queen production. Science 336, 351–352 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryden J., Gill R. J., Mitton R. A., Raine N. E. & Jansen V. A. Chronic sublethal stress causes bee colony failure. Ecology Letters 16, 1463–1469 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomizawa M. & Casida J. E. Neonicotinoid insecticide toxicology: mechanisms of selective action. Annual Review of Pharmacology andToxicology 45, 247–268 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson S. M. & Wright G. A. Exposure to multiple cholinergic pesticides impairs olfactory learning and memory in honeybees. The Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 1799–1807 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han P., Niu C.-Y., Lei C.-L., Cui J.-J. & Desneux N. Use of an innovative T-tube maze assay and the proboscis extension response assay to assess sublethal effects of GM products and pesticides on learning capacity of the honey bee Apis mellifera L. Ecotoxicology 19, 1612–1619 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley D. A., Smith K. E. & Raine N. E. Bumblebee learning and memory is impaired by chronic exposure to a neonicotinoid pesticide. Scientific Reports 5 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Godfray H. C. J. et al. In Proc. R. Soc. B. 20151821 (The Royal Society).
- Collett M., Chittka L. & Collett T. S. Spatial memory in insect navigation. Current Biology 23, R789–R800 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lihoreau M., Chittka L. & Raine N. E. Travel optimization by foraging bumblebees through readjustments of traplines after discovery of new feeding locations. The American Naturalist 176, 744–757 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry M. et al. A common pesticide decreases foraging success and survival in honey bees. Science 336, 348–350 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bortolotti L. et al. Effects of sub-lethal imidacloprid doses on the homing rate and foraging activity of honey bees. Bulletin of Insectology 56, 63–68 (2003). [Google Scholar]
- Fischer J. et al. Neonicotinoids Interfere with Specific Components of Navigation in Honeybees. PloS ONE 9, e91364 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collett T. S. & Collett M. Memory use in insect visual navigation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 542–552 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolata W. & Kolata S. A model of working memory capacity in the radial-arm maze task. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 53, 242–252 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- Brown M. F. & Demas G. E. Evidence for spatial working memory in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Journal of Comparative Psychology 108, 344–352 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Healy S. & Hurly T. Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus): a field test. Animal Learning & Behavior 23, 63–68 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- Demas G. E. & Brown M. F. Honey bees are predisposed to win-shift but can learn to win-stay. Animal Behaviour 50, 1041–1045 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- Sulikowski D. & Burke D. Reward type influences performance and search structure of an omnivorous bird in an open-field maze. Behavioural processes 83, 31–35 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olton D. S. & Samuelson R. J. Remembrance of places passed: Spatial memory in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2, 97 (1976). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foreman N. & Ermakova I. In A Handbook of Spatial Research Paradigms and Methodologies (eds Foreman N. & Gillett R.) (Psychology Press, 1998). [Google Scholar]
- Brown M. F., Moore J. A., Brown C. H. & Langheld K. D. The existence and extent of spatial working memory ability in honeybees. Animal Learning & Behavior 25, 473–484 (1997). [Google Scholar]
- Burmeister S., Couvillon P. & Bitterman M. Performance of honeybees in analogues of the rodent radial maze. Animal Learning & Behavior 23, 369–375 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- EC. Commission implementing regulation (EU) No. 485/2013 of 24 May 2013 amending Implementing Regulation EU) No. 540/2011, as regards the conditions of approval of the active substances clothianidin, thiamethoxam and imidacloprid, and prohibiting the use and sale of seeds treated with plant protection products containing those active substances. Official Journal of the European Union 139, 12–26 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- Levin E. D., Castonguay M. & Ellison G. D. Effects of the nicotinic receptor blocker mecamylamine on radial-arm maze performance in rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology 48, 206–212 (1987). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sulikowski D. & Burke D. Movement and memory: different cognitive strategies are used to search for resources with different natural distributions. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65, 621–631 (2011). [Google Scholar]
- Fournier A., Rollin O., Le Féon V., Decourtye A. & Henry M. Crop-Emptying Rate and the Design of Pesticide Risk Assessment Schemes in the Honey Bee and Wild Bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Journal of Economic Entomology 107, 38–46 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carreck N. L. & Ratnieks F. L. The dose makes the poison: have “field realistic” rates of exposure of bees to neonicotinoid insecticides been overestimated in laboratory studies? Journal of Apicultural Research 53, 607–614 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- EFSA. Statement on the findings in recent studies investigating sub-lethal effects in bees of some neonicotinoids in consideration of the uses currently authorised in Europe. EFSA Journal 10, 27, doi: 10.2903/j.efsa.2012.2752 (2012). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goulson D. Bumblebees: their behaviour and ecology. (OUP, 2003). [Google Scholar]
- Goulson D. et al. Can alloethism in workers of the bumblebee, Bombus terrestris, be explained in terms of foraging efficiency? Animal Behaviour 64, 123–130 (2002). [Google Scholar]
- Cartar R. V. & Real L. A. Habitat structure and animal movement: the behaviour of bumble bees in uniform and random spatial resource distributions. Oecologia 112, 430–434 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feltham H., Park K. & Goulson D. Field realistic doses of pesticide imidacloprid reduce bumblebee pollen foraging efficiency. Ecotoxicology 23, 317–323 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley D. A., Russell A. L., Morrison S. J., Rogers C. & Raine N. E. Investigating the impacts of field‐realistic exposure to a neonicotinoid pesticide on bumblebee foraging, homing ability and colony growth. Journal of Applied Ecology 53, 1440–1449 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rundlöf M. et al. Seed coating with a neonicotinoid insecticide negatively affects wild bees. Nature 521, 77–80 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R. et al. The knowledge base of bee navigation. The Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 141–146 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cresswell J. E., Robert F. X. L., Florance H. & Smirnoff N. Clearance of ingested neonicotinoid pesticide (imidacloprid) in honey bees (Apis mellifera) and bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). Pest Management Science 70, 332–337 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desneux N., Decourtye A. & Delpuech J.-M. The sublethal effects of pesticides on beneficial arthropods. Annual Review of Entomology 52, 81–106 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilton S. C. & Krebs J. K. Spatial memory of four species of Parus: performance in an open-field analogue of a radial maze. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42, 345–368 (1990). [Google Scholar]
- Balda R. P. & Kamil A. The spatial memory of Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) in an analogue of the radial arm maze. Animal Learning & Behavior 16, 116–122 (1988). [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M. & Núñez J. A. Honeybees mark with scent and reject recently visited flowers. Oecologia 89, 113–117 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stout J. C., Goulson D. & Allen J. A. Repellent scent-marking of flowers by a guild of foraging bumblebees (Bombus spp.). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 43, 317–326 (1998). [Google Scholar]
- Thompson H. et al. Effects of neonicotinoid seed treatments on bumble bee colonies under field conditions. 76 (FERA, York, 2013). [Google Scholar]
- Pilling E., Campbell P., Coulson M., Ruddle N. & Tornier I. A Four-Year Field Program Investigating Long-Term Effects of Repeated Exposure of Honey Bee Colonies to Flowering Crops Treated with Thiamethoxam. PLoS ONE 8, e77193, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077193 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pohorecka K. et al. Residues of neonicotinoid insecticides in bee collected plant materials from oilseed rape crops and their effect on bee colonies. Journal of Apicultural Science 56, 115–134 (2012). [Google Scholar]
- Suchail S., Guez D. & Belzunces L. P. Discrepancy between acute and chronic toxicity induced by imidacloprid and its metabolites in Apis mellifera. Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry 20, 2482–2486 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nauen R., Ebbinghaus-Kintscher U., Salgado V. L. & Kaussmann M. Thiamethoxam is a neonicotinoid precursor converted to clothianidin in insects and plants. Pesticide Biochemistry and Physiology 76, 55–69 (2003). [Google Scholar]
- Burns J. G. & Thomson J. D. A test of spatial memory and movement patterns of bumblebees at multiple spatial and temporal scales. Behavioral Ecology 17, 48–55 (2006). [Google Scholar]
- Johnson J. B. & Omland K. S. Model selection in ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19, 101–108 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Symonds M. R. & Moussalli A. A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioural ecology using Akaike’s information criterion. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65, 13–21 (2011). [Google Scholar]
- Zuur A. F., Hilbe J. & Ieno E. N. A Beginner’s Guide to GLM and GLMM with R: A Frequentist and Bayesian Perspective for Ecologists. (Highland Statistics, 2013). [Google Scholar]
- R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 2015).
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.