Summary
Accumulating evidence indicates that cerebellar long-term potentiation (LTP) is necessary for procedural learning. However, little is known about its underlying molecular mechanisms. Whereas AMPA receptor (AMPAR) subunit rules for synaptic plasticity have been extensively studied in relation to declarative learning, it is unclear whether these rules apply to cerebellum-dependent motor learning. Here we show that LTP at the parallel-fiber-to-Purkinje-cell synapse and adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex depend not on GluA1- but on GluA3-containing AMPARs. In contrast to the classic form of LTP implicated in declarative memory formation, this form of LTP does not require GluA1-AMPAR trafficking but rather requires changes in open-channel probability of GluA3-AMPARs mediated by cAMP signaling and activation of the protein directly activated by cAMP (Epac). We conclude that vestibulo-cerebellar motor learning is the first form of memory acquisition shown to depend on GluA3-dependent synaptic potentiation by increasing single-channel conductance.
Keywords: learning, cerebellum, Purkinje cell, synapse, LTP, AMPA receptor, GluA3, Epac
Highlights
-
•
Cerebellar learning depends on expression of GluA3, but not GluA1, in Purkinje cells
-
•
GluA3 is required to induce LTP, but not LTD, at PF-PC synapses
-
•
GluA3-dependent potentiation involves a cAMP-driven change in channel conductance
-
•
GluA3-mediated LTP and learning are induced via cAMP-mediated Epac activation
Gutierrez et al. show a novel form of synaptic plasticity that entails an increase in channel conductance of GluA3-containing AMPARs and that is required for vestibulo-cerebellar motor learning.
Introduction
Plasticity mediated by synaptic trafficking of α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic-acid-type glutamate receptors (AMPARs) plays an important role in the acquisition of declarative memories (Kessels and Malinow, 2009). Ionotropic AMPARs drive fast, excitatory neuronal activity and can consist of four different subunits named GluA1 through GluA4. In hippocampal pyramidal cells, most AMPARs are hetero-oligomers composed of either GluA1/GluA2 or GluA2/GluA3 subunits, and the subunit composition dictates which role AMPARs play in synaptic plasticity (Shi et al., 2001). In the hippocampus, cortex, and amygdala, both long-term potentiation (LTP) and learning depend on the trafficking of GluA1-containing AMPARs to synapses (Makino and Malinow, 2011, Nedelescu et al., 2010, Rumpel et al., 2005, Mitsushima et al., 2011), whereas GluA3-containing AMPARs contribute relatively little to synaptic currents, synaptic plasticity, or learning (Adamczyk et al., 2012, Meng et al., 2003, Humeau et al., 2007). To what extent GluA1 and GluA3 play a role in adaptive motor behavior remains to be established.
Here we sought to unravel the potential role of GluA1- and/or GluA3-containing AMPARs in cerebellar motor learning. Unlike the rich insight into the role of AMPARs in declarative memory formation in the hippocampus, relatively little is known about their role in procedural memory formation in the cerebellum. AMPAR plasticity occurs at parallel-fiber-to-Purkinje-cell (PF-PC) synapses, reflecting the expression of LTP or long-term depression (LTD) (Kakegawa and Yuzaki, 2005, Steinberg et al., 2006), but the full functional significance of this plasticity and the precise molecular pathways underlying it remain to be further elucidated (Gao et al., 2012). In addition, the roles of GluA1- and/or GluA3-containing AMPARs in the plasticity of Purkinje cells (PCs) have hardly been studied (Bats et al., 2013, Douyard et al., 2007, Kakegawa and Yuzaki, 2005).
We found that adaptation of compensatory eye movements, which is one of the most widely studied forms of cerebellar motor learning serving to stabilize gaze (Anzai et al., 2010, Nguyen-Vu et al., 2013, Schonewille et al., 2011), depends on GluA3-containing AMPARs, but not on GluA1-containing AMPARs. The GluA3-containing AMPARs in PCs are critical for the induction and expression of PF-PC LTP not by trafficking of receptors, but by a change in the conductance and open probability of the channel. This form of plasticity requires activation of Epac through an increase of cyclic AMP. Together, these findings not only show that GluA3 is crucial for cerebellar potentiation and learning, but also that its actions of plasticity are evoked through a novel mechanism.
Results
Cerebellar Motor Learning Depends on GluA3, but Not on GluA1
Unlike GluA2 global knockout (KO) mice, which suffer from severe motor performance deficits including ataxia (Gerlai et al., 1998, Jia et al., 1996), mice that lack AMPAR subunit GluA1 or GluA3 (GluA1-KO and GluA3-KO) displayed intact basic motor behavior (Figure S1). Indeed, they were able to stabilize the images on their retina and/or gaze with respect to a moving visual field (i.e., optokinetic reflex, or OKR; Figure S1A), with respect to their head movements (i.e., the vestibulo-ocular reflex in the dark, or VOR: Figure S1B), or with respect to a combination of both, as occurs in daily life (i.e., VOR in the light, or VORL; Figure S1C). None of the comparisons between GluA1-KO mice, GluA3-KO mice, and their control littermates showed a significant difference in any of these paradigms (for p values, see Figure S1 and corresponding legends). Far more challenging is the test for VOR phase-reversal adaptation, which involves cerebellum-dependent motor learning (Gutierrez-Castellanos et al., 2013). During this paradigm, mice learn to shift the phase of their VOR following sinusoidal visuovestibular mismatch stimulation, in which the visual stimulus moves in the same direction as the vestibular stimulus (i.e., in phase), yet at a greater amplitude (Figure 1A). After 5 days of visuovestibular training, wild-type (WT) mice moved their eyes during table stimulation in the dark in the same direction as the body, rather than the opposite direction as they used to do before the training (i.e., they normally show an innate contraversive compensation). More specifically, the mature WT mice learned to shift their VOR in the dark by 159° out of the perfect 180° after the training (Figure 1B). Likewise, when we subjected littermate GluA1-deficient mice to this phase-reversal adaptation paradigm, they reached final average phase shifts of 162° (GluA1-KO versus WT, p = 0.13; Figure 1B), indicating that GluA1-containing AMPARs are dispensable for VOR adaptation. In contrast, GluA3-deficient mice showed striking deficits in shifting the phase of their VOR in the dark; they showed a final phase shift of only 35° after five training sessions (GluA3-KO versus WT, p = 0.001; Figure 1B). When we looked not only at the oculomotor phase but also at the learning trajectory extent (as explained in Figure S2A), we observed that, although the initial performances of VOR catch-learning trials were not significantly different for any of the three groups tested (p = 0.3 and p = 0.11 for GluA1-KO and GluA3-KO, respectively), the final performances of GluA3-KO after 5 days of training were significantly different from those of both WT littermates (p = 0.01) and GluA1-KO mice (p = 0.01) (Figure 1C). Accordingly, the vector of total learning extent per mouse, which equals the distance between the initial (first recording, day 1) and the final VOR performance coordinate throughout the 5-day-spanning phase-reversal paradigm (Figure S2), was significantly smaller for GluA3-KO mice than it was for WT and GluA1-KO mice (p = 0.001 and p = 0.0002, respectively) (Figures 1D and 1E). Moreover, the consolidation rate of learning, which equals the ratio between the total learning extent and the ideal learning extent with no overnight memory loss between training days (Figure S2A), was also significantly lower in GluA3-KO than in both GluA1-KO and WT littermates (p = 0.0008 and p = 0.001, respectively) (Figure 1E). Importantly, all groups of mice performed equally well during the visually driven vestibular training trials over the 5 days of training (all p values > 0.05; Figure 1A), indicating that the learning deficits in GluA3-KO as measured in the dark during the catch trials did not directly result from a poor response to the visuovestibular training signal but rather from an impaired ability to maintain this learned vestibular response in the absence of the visual cue (Figure 1B).
In addition to phase modulation, we also investigated gain modulation of vestibulo-ocular movements after either in-phase (gain-down) or out-of-phase (gain-up) visuovestibular training paradigms that aim to reduce or increase the amplitude, respectively, of the eye-movement response to a constant vestibular input. GluA3-KO mice showed severe learning deficits in both the gain-down (p = 0.001 for final catch trials) and gain-up (p = 0.009 for final catch trials) paradigms, whereas GluA1-KO and WT mice again performed equally well in both training paradigms (p = 0.11 and p = 0.2 for gain-down and gain-up final catch trials, respectively) (Figures 1F and S1D). These experiments indicate that GluA3-containing AMPARs contribute to cerebellum-dependent motor learning.
GluA3 Is Required to Induce LTP, but Not LTD, at PF-PC Synapses
PCs form the sole output of the cerebellar cortex. It has previously been shown that synaptic plasticity at their parallel fiber afferents crucially contributes to motor learning (Schonewille et al., 2010). To investigate the contribution of GluA1- and GluA3-containing AMPARs to basal synaptic transmission, we recorded spontaneous miniature excitatory synaptic currents (mEPSCs) of PCs in cerebellar slices from 4–6 week old mice (Figure 2A), an age at which GluA3-KO mice showed motor learning deficits similar to those shown during adulthood (Figure S2D). The average amplitude and frequency of mEPSCs in GluA1-deficient PCs were not significantly different (p = 0.4 and p = 0.2, respectively) from those in WT PCs (Figure 2B). In PCs of GluA3-KO mice, the average amplitude (p = 0.0003) and frequency (p = 0.02) of mEPSCs were significantly lower than those in WT PCs. The low basal transmission in the GluA3-KO mice was neither reflected in structural changes at the level of spine densities of proximal or distal PC dendrites (p = 0.7 and p = 0.2 for proximal and distal, respectively) (Figure S3) nor compensated for by increased synaptic currents from kainate receptors (for details, see Figure S4 and corresponding legends). In PCs of mice that lack both GluA1 and GluA3, mEPSC events were virtually absent (Figure 2B), suggesting that the large majority of synaptic currents in PCs are derived from either GluA1- or GluA3-containing AMPARs.
A reduced basal transmission in GluA3-deficient PC synapses can either be a cause or a consequence of impaired synaptic plasticity. We therefore investigated both LTD and LTP at the PF-PC synapse using whole-cell recordings. LTD was induced either by pairing PF stimulation with a depolarizing voltage-clamp step, mimicking climbing fiber (CF) input (Linden, 2001; Figure 2C), or by pairing PF stimulation with CF stimulation (Schonewille et al., 2011; Figures S3C–S3E). The magnitudes of LTD in PCs of GluA1-KO and GluA3-KO mice were indistinguishable from those in the PCs of WT littermates with either induction protocol (with somatic depolarization for GluA1-KO versus WT, p = 0.4, and for GluA3-KO versus WT, p = 0.2; with direct CF stimulation for GluA1-KO versus WT, p = 0.9, and for GluA3-KO versus WT, p = 0.8). These data are in line with other studies showing that GluA2 is the key subunit for AMPAR internalization and therefore for LTD induction (Steinberg et al., 2006, Schonewille et al., 2011). Next, we induced LTP in PCs by 1 Hz tetanic stimulation of PF input alone (Lev-Ram et al., 2002) (Figure 2D). This stimulus protocol reliably produced significant LTP in both WT PCs (p = 0.0005) and GluA1-KO PCs (p = 0.0003) with a similar magnitude (p = 0.5) and without significant changes in paired-pulse facilitation (PPF) of the evoked EPSCs after LTP induction (p = 0.11 and p = 0.6 in GluA1-KO and WT PCs, respectively). In contrast, with the same stimulation protocol, LTP could not be induced in GluA3-KO PCs (p = 0.7) (Figure 2D). These experiments demonstrate that PF-PC LTP requires GluA3-containing AMPARs.
GluA3-Dependent Synaptic Potentiation Involves a cAMP-Driven Change in Channel Conductance
What is the molecular mechanism underlying GluA3-dependent LTP of PF-PC synapses? To test whether GluA3-dependent synaptic plasticity in PCs depends on cAMP signaling, we administered the adenylyl cyclase activator forskolin (FSK) to PCs of GluA3-KO brain slices and compared the effects to those in WT slices and GluA1-KO slices (Figure 3A). Whereas FSK produced, on average, a 2-fold potentiation in PF-evoked EPSCs in both WT and GluA1-KO PCs (230% ± 25% and 215% ± 35%, respectively), it failed to induce synaptic potentiation in PCs that lack GluA3 (95% ± 10%, p = 0.005, GluA3-KO versus WT). Importantly, AMPAR potentiation also occurred in WT PCs when local stimulation with 1 μM AMPA was used while blocking PFs with TTX (189% ± 17%, p = 0.001; Figure 3B), highlighting its postsynaptic nature (Chen and Regehr, 1997). These data indicate that GluA3-dependent synaptic potentiation at PF-PC synapses can occur upon a rise in the cellular level of cAMP.
We next examined whether cAMP-driven synaptic potentiation is a result of synaptic trafficking of GluA3-containing AMPARs. To assess whether FSK increases GluA3 levels on the cell surface of spines, we performed time-lapse two-photon imaging of PCs in cultured organotypic cerebellar slices infected with Sindbis virus to acutely express GluA3 subunits fused to superecliptic pHluorin (SEP). SEP is a pH-sensitive variant of GFP that shows a reduction in fluorescence upon rapid application of acidic (pH 5) ACSF (Figure S4F; Makino and Malinow, 2009). To test whether GluA3 trafficking can be detected with this method, we first triggered LTD chemically by adding the metabotropic mGluR1 receptor agonist DHPG to induce internalization of AMPARs (Linden, 2001). Indeed, application of DHPG to WT PCs expressing SEP-GluA3 produced a significant decrease in SEP fluorescence at spines (p < 0.0001; Figure 4A) and in synaptic strength (p = 0.004 for amplitude and p = 0.04 for frequency; Figure 4B), which is in line with the endocytosis of AMPARs that occurs during the expression of LTD at the PF-PC synapse (Wang and Linden, 2000). In contrast, washing in FSK failed to induce any change in SEP-GluA3 fluorescence at PC spines (0.03% ± 0.015% change, p = 0.4; Figure 4A), even though FSK significantly increased synaptic currents in the SEP-GluA3-expressing PCs (p = 0.04 for amplitude and p < 0.0001 for frequency; Figure 4B). These data suggest that the cAMP-driven synaptic potentiation does not require an insertion of GluA3-containing AMPARs at the surface of spines. To assess whether FSK promotes lateral mobility of GluA3 receptors instead of an increase in receptor externalization, we performed fluorescence recovery after photobleaching (FRAP) experiments of single spines of PCs expressing SEP-GluA3 (Figure 4C). After ∼80% photobleaching, the SEP signals recovered to ∼50%, suggesting that a proportion of SEP-GluA3 is immobilized at synapses (Makino and Malinow 2009). The SEP fluorescence intensity recovered at a similar pace in the presence or absence of FSK (p = 0.9), indicating that the lateral mobility of GluA3-containing AMPARs is not influenced by a rise in cAMP.
To assess whether GluA3 plasticity involves a change in channel properties, we resolved single-channel, AMPA-mediated currents by clamping a single AMPAR in cell-attached mode at the cell body of either GluA1-KO or GluA3-KO PCs with the recording pipette containing near-saturating concentrations of AMPA (Poon et al., 2010, Poon et al., 2011). GluA1-containing AMPARs at the surface of GluA3-KO PC cell bodies stochastically reached open states 1, 2, and 3 (indicating binding of 2, 3, and 4 glutamates per receptor complex, respectively) and displayed similar conductance levels and open-channel probability in the presence or absence of FSK application (Figure S5). In contrast, GluA3-containing AMPARs on cell bodies of GluA1-KO PCs produced the vast majority of their openings in the first and lower conductance state (O1) under basal conditions (Figures 5A and 5B), indicating that only two out of the four ligand-binding domains (LBDs) present in the AMPAR tetramer are activated by AMPA. After application of FSK, the behavior of GluA3-containing AMPARs changed strikingly and produced a significantly higher amount of openings in state O2 and O3—similar to GluA1-containing AMPARs (compare Figures 5A, 5B, 5E, and S5B). The average duration of the openings was unchanged (p = 0.4, p = 0.13, and p = 0.09 for O1, O2, and O3, respectively; Figure 5C), but an increase of the absolute frequency of the openings caused shortening of the closed-state dwell-time and thus a significant net increase in open probability (p < 0.0001; Figure 5E). Although FSK did not significantly change the conductance level of any of the open states (p = 0.7, p = 0.14, and p = 0.15 for O1, O2, and O3, respectively; Figure 5D), the higher relative fraction of events in the highly conductive open states O2 and O3 caused a significant increase (p < 0.0001) in the overall conductance of cAMP-stimulated GluA3 channels (Figures 5B and 5D). These experiments suggest that a rise in intracellular cAMP produces synaptic potentiation by increasing the open-channel probability of the GluA3 subunit, indicating a novel mechanistic model for GluA3-dependent synaptic plasticity.
GluA3-Mediated Plasticity Is Induced via cAMP-Mediated Epac Activation
To further elucidate the molecular mechanism underlying GluA3-dependent plasticity, we aimed to identify the intermediary factor that translates a rise in cAMP into synaptic potentiation of GluA3-containing AMPARs. Protein kinase A (PKA) is activated by a rise in cAMP and exerts cAMP-dependent synaptic effects (Lev-Ram et al., 2002, Sokolova et al., 2006). However, incubating WT PCs with PKA antagonist KT5720 or PKA antagonist H89 did not have a significant effect on synaptic potentiation induced by FSK (215% ± 20% with KT5720 and 235% ± 19% with H89; p = 0.7 and p = 0.9, respectively; Figure 6A), indicating that PKA is not involved in mediating GluA3 plasticity. We next assessed the involvement of Epac (exchange proteins directly activated by cAMP, a.k.a. Rap guanine-nucleotide-exchange factor) as an alternative cAMP-dependent pathway that can trigger synaptic changes (Gekel and Neher, 2008, Woolfrey et al., 2009). The blockade of Epac with its selective antagonist ESI-05 (Tsalkova et al., 2012) did not reduce basal transmission at PF-PC synapses (Figure 7H), but it effectively prevented the FSK-induced synaptic potentiation in WT PCs (p = 0.9 versus baseline and p < 0.0001 versus control condition without ESI-05) (Figure 6A). To assess whether Epac activation is not only necessary but also sufficient to cause GluA3-dependent synaptic potentiation, we investigated the impact of the selective Epac activator 8-CPT-2Me-cAMP (8CPT). Adding 20 μM 8CPT to the intracellular recording solution produced synaptic potentiation in WT PCs (185% ± 17%, p = 0.0004; Figure 6B), but not in GluA3-deficient PCs (100% ± 5%, p = 0.8; Figure 6B). In addition, the postsynaptic application of 8CPT increased the amplitude and frequency of PC mEPSCs (p = 0.0005 and p = 0.001, respectively; Figures 6C and 6D) and did not change the PPF ratio (104% ± 5%; Figure 6B). These experiments indicate that a rise in cAMP triggers synaptic potentiation through Epac-mediated activation of postsynaptic, GluA3-containing AMPARs. This Epac-driven activation of GluA3-containing AMPARs was not limited to AMPARs located at synapses. Outside-out patches excised from WT PC somata produced a peak current of approximately 10 pA in response to puffs of 100 μM AMPA (Figure S4E). When the Epac activator 8CPT was added to the internal solution of the patch pipette, the peak current obtained under the same conditions was increased 2.5-fold in the absence of a presynaptic component (25 ± 3 pA, p < 0.0001 versus control). This difference in peak current was largely maintained in the presence of AMPAR-desensitization blockers PEPA and cyclothiazide (45 ± 8 pA without 8CPT versus 97 ± 10 pA with 8CPT, p < 0.0001; Figure 6E), indicating that cAMP-driven GluA3 plasticity does not depend on a change in the desensitization properties of AMPAR channels. As expected from our single-channel results, nonstationary noise analysis of these nondesensitizing AMPAR responses showed a significant increase in conductance and open probability (Figures 6F and 6G). This analysis revealed how, in a mixed pool of GluA1- and GluA3-containing AMPARs, Epac-dependent GluA3 potentiation was translated into an increase in current amplitude without altering the dynamics of the response (Figure 6E), highlighting the consistent results with miniature and evoked EPSC recordings.
Epac Activation Is Required for LTP and Motor Learning
We next tested whether PF-PC LTP depends on Epac activation. Incubation of slices with Epac inhibitor ESI-05 significantly inhibited synaptic potentiation induced by tetanic PF stimulation (102% ± 13% versus 140% ± 8% in control conditions, p < 0.0001 after 15 min; Figure 7A). In addition, LTP was fully occluded when brain slices were preincubated with the membrane-permeable analog of the Epac activator (8pCPT) (GluA3-KO versus WT, p = 0.008; Figure 7B). Together, these data indicate that Epac2 activation is responsible for postsynaptic LTP at the PF-PC synapse through activation of GluA3-containing AMPARs.
To investigate the involvement of Epac activation in cerebellar synaptic plasticity in vivo, we performed phase-reversal adaptation in WT mice that received daily IP injections either with Epac antagonist ESI-05 (0.2–0.3 mL at 10 mg/kg) or with vehicle alone 30 min prior to the training protocol. Mice administered ESI-05 had unaffected basal-eye-reflex behavior, but performed significantly worse in the phase-reversal task than did vehicle-injected animals (Figure 7D). Although both groups eventually reached a reversal of the VOR phase (Veh, 157% ± 2%, and ESI-05, 148% ± 15%; Figure 7D), its magnitude was significantly lower in the ESI-05-injected mice than in vehicle controls (p = 0.01; Figures 7E–7G). This difference reached after training could not be explained by a poor basic response to the training stimuli (Figure 7C), but only by a significantly reduced learning extent (p = 0.01) and consolidation rate (p = 0.03). Importantly, systemic ESI-05 injections produced learning deficits without a change in basal synaptic transmission compared to vehicle-injected mice (p = 0.5 and p = 0.9 for mEPSC amplitude and frequency, respectively; Figure 7H), suggesting that absence of Epac-dependent synaptic potentiation without any change in basal transmission is sufficient to impair learning capabilities.
GluA3 Expression in PCs Is Required for VOR Learning
We showed that VOR learning depends on global expression of GluA3 and that LTP at PF-PC synapses requires GluA3 plasticity—but does VOR learning depend on GluA3 specifically in PCs? To address this question, we generated and tested a PC-specific GluA3-KO mouse (referred to as L7/GluA3-KO; Figure S6) by crossbreeding mice expressing Cre-recombinase under the PC-specific promoter L7-pcp2 with mice in which the GluA3 gene is flanked by loxP sites (Barski et al., 2000, Sanchis-Segura et al., 2006). After establishing the single-unit identity of floccular vertical-axis PCs in adult L7/GluA3-KO mice by demonstrating a CF pause in their simple spike activity as well as a preferred modulation tuning-curve during extracellular recordings in vivo (Figure 8A), we investigated the action-potential generation of their simple spike activity in the awake state. In the absence of visual stimulation, both the firing frequency and regularity (i.e., coefficient of variation of adjacent interspike intervals, or CV2) of the simple spike activity of the L7/GluA3-KO mice did not differ significantly from those of WTs (Figure S7A), which is consistent with the similar I-V relationships recorded in vitro for WT and GluA3-lacking PCs (Figure S7B). Next, we provided visual stimulation at the frequency that was used for the training paradigm (0.6 Hz). Again, the firing frequency and regularity (i.e., CV2) of the simple spike activity of the L7/GluA3-KO mice did not differ from those of WTs (p = 0.7 and p = 0.8, respectively; Figure 8A). Moreover, and most importantly, the amplitude of the simple spike modulation during visual stimulation did not differ (p = 0.8; Figure 8A), suggesting that the PF output is, in effect, sufficient to mediate the visual training signals in the L7/GluA3-KO mice despite the reduced PF-PC synaptic transmission (Figure S7C). Finally, the firing frequency and modulation amplitude of the complex spikes did not show any significant difference, either (p = 0.7 and p = 0.9, respectively; Figure 8A). Together, these data indicate that the in vivo excitability and spike generation of PCs are intact in L7/GluA3-KO mice.
We then tested 3- to 5-month-old L7/GluA3-KO and control littermates for their ability to adapt eye reflexes. The baseline OKR and VOR performances of these L7/GluA3-KO mice were indistinguishable from those of controls (Figure S1). Similarly to global GluA3-KOs, VOR motor learning was prominently affected in L7-GluA3-KO mice (Figures 8C–8G). Mice lacking GluA3 in PCs showed significant deficits throughout the phase-reversal paradigm (all p values < 0.01 after day 2) and had a significantly different learning extent at the end of the paradigm (p = 0.0005; Figures 8B–8F). Moreover, consolidation during the phase-reversal paradigm was significantly lower (p = 0.0006, Figure 8F). Gain modulation was also impaired, as shown by a significant difference between the final eye movement gain of L7/GluA3-KO mice and that of their control littermates after either gain-down or gain-up training sessions (p = 0.04 and p = 0.006 for gain-down and gain-up, respectively) (Figure 8G).
In contrast to the single L7/GluA3-KO as well as the single global GluA1-KO and GluA3-KO mice, mice that lacked both GluA1 and GluA3 receptor subunits specifically in PCs (L7-GluA1/GluA3-KO) showed significant aberrations in baseline eye movement performance (p = 0.0001 for OKR and p = 0.03 for VORD; Figures S1A and S1B). Together with the findings presented above (see also Figure 1), these data suggest that the presence of GluA3 in the GluA1-KO mouse can compensate for its lack of GluA1 during both baseline and learning behavior, but the presence of GluA1 in the global GluA3-KO mouse and single L7-GluA3-KO mouse is not sufficient to fully compensate for the lack of GluA3 during adaptation of compensatory eye movements. This highlights the putative impact of GluA3-dependent synaptic plasticity in PCs.
Discussion
It is widely believed that LTP- and LTD-type synaptic plasticity mechanisms act in concert to mediate several types of learning in brain regions such as the hippocampus, amygdala, and cerebral cortex (Malinow and Malenka, 2002, Makino and Malinow, 2011, Nabavi et al., 2014, Nedelescu et al., 2010, Rumpel et al., 2005, Takahashi et al., 2003). For cerebellar learning, LTD at the PF-PC synapse has historically been considered the dominant plasticity mechanism (Ito, 2002, Linden and Connor, 1995). Although the simple spike suppression observed at early stages of some forms of motor learning in vivo may suggest LTD occurrence (ten Brinke et al., 2015, Yang and Lisberger, 2014), an increasing number of studies suggest that LTD is not a strict requisite for motor learning (Hesslow et al., 2013, Schonewille et al., 2011). In the present study, we show that LTP at PF-PC synapses is a required mechanism for cerebellar motor learning. We show that LTP, but not LTD, at the PF-PC synapse requires plasticity of GluA3-containing AMPARs and that both the selective removal of GluA3 in PCs and the pharmacological blockade of the pathway leading to GluA3 plasticity in vivo severely impair the ability to adapt the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Combined, these findings provide the first correlative link between GluA3-dependent LTP and behavioral learning in general.
Previous studies proposed a role for cerebellar LTP in the context of bidirectional gain modulation (Boyden et al., 2006). This work suggested that gain-down modulation of eye movements might require PF-PC LTP and, conversely, that gain-up modulation would require LTD. According to this hypothesis, one would expect GluA3-KO mice to show impaired gain-down modulation with intact gain-up adaptation. However, our data show that the specific absence of GluA3 in PCs most prominently impairs gain-up and phase modulation, supporting an opposite kind of role for GluA3-dependent LTP in oculomotor learning. Whereas the role of GluA3 in PC plasticity and cerebellar motor learning is becoming more clear now, the role of GluA1 is still largely obscure. The presence of GluA1 in PCs was essential neither for the induction of LTD nor of LTP, and there were no overt signs of deficits in motor performance or motor learning in the GluA1-KO mice. Its possible role became only indirectly apparent, when we observed that, in contrast to the single GluA1-KO mice, the double GluA1/GluA3-KO mice virtually completely lacked glutamatergic currents in PCs, and the double L7-GluA1/GluA3-KO mice showed significant signs of ataxia and deficits in motor learning. Given that the single GluA3-KO mice did not show any sign of motor performance deficit, these findings indicate that GluA1-containing AMPARs in PCs do contribute to cerebellar motor performance but that their absence can be compensated for by GluA3-containing AMPARs.
The possible role of LTP at the PF-PC synapse in cerebellar motor learning has been suggested before by various other cell-specific mouse mutant studies (Andreescu et al., 2007, Schonewille et al., 2010, Peter et al., 2016). However, these studies tackled more upstream PC processes that involved the nuclear estrogen receptor, cytosolic protein phosphatase calcineurin, and subsynaptic protein shank2, and as a consequence they suffered from various side effects that prevented definitive conclusions (Gao et al., 2012). In the current study, in which we tackled PF-PC LTP more downstream by targeting GluA3-containing AMPARs at the level of the synapse itself, we did not find any evidence for structural changes or firing differences in PCs of awake behaving mice. We did find that the basal transmission was reduced in PCs lacking GluA3 (both in the global and the cell-specific KO mice), but this deficit was probably not the reason for the impairment of LTP or of motor learning because acutely inhibiting GluA3 plasticity through blockade of Epac prevented both LTP and motor learning without affecting basal transmission. We therefore propose that the reduced basal transmission in GluA3-KO mice is the consequence of a prolonged deficit in LTP.
GluA1-dependent synaptic plasticity is mediated by active trafficking (Makino and Malinow, 2011, Shi et al., 2001) and by changes in conductance and open probability at the single receptor level (Benke et al., 1998, Derkach et al., 1999). Here we present evidence that, at least at the short-term scale of tens of minutes, synaptic potentiation through activation of GluA3 plasticity does not involve trafficking but mainly involves a prominent increase in open-channel probability of GluA3-containing receptors, suggesting that in the case of GluA3, a change in receptor properties is the predominant mechanism for producing synaptic potentiation. These findings imply that PF-PC LTP is, mechanistically, not just the reverse of LTD at this synapse (Jörntell and Hansel, 2006). Linden and colleagues have shown that PF-PC LTD is largely expressed by endocytosis of GluA2-containing AMPARs (Linden, 2001, Schonewille et al., 2011) and thus mainly dependent on AMPAR trafficking. Given the current findings on GluA3-mediated LTP, it may be worthwhile to find out whether changes in AMPAR unitary conductance or glutamate affinity also play a minor role in early LTD expression at the PF-PC synapse, as interference with clathrin-mediated endocytosis did not produce a total attenuation of LTD expression (Wang and Linden, 2000).
Full genetic ablation of GluA2 subunits, in contrast to that of GluA3, produces an ataxic, hardly viable phenotype (Gerlai et al., 1998). Interestingly, the remaining mainly GluA1- and GluA3-containing AMPARs in these KO mice have an unusual subunit composition and are abnormally distributed at the synapse (Sans et al., 2003). In this respect, it should be noted that GluA3 is an obligatory heteromeric subunit: GluA3 homomers are energetically unfavorable (Rossmann et al., 2011) and form intracellular aggregates that do not reach the cellular surface efficiently (Coleman et al., 2016). Genetic mouse models in which GluA2 trafficking is blocked reveal an impairment in LTD induction at their PF-PC synapses, whereas LTP is normal (Schonewille et al., 2011, Steinberg et al., 2006), which is in line with our finding that LTP can be induced without trafficking of GluA2/GluA3-containing AMPARs. Differently from these GluA2 mutants, GluA3-KO mice prominently express surface GluA2-containing AMPARs (heteromerized with GluA1) but lack a cAMP-dependent synaptic LTP. These data highlight the differential roles of GluA2 and GluA3 in the structural dynamics and localization of AMPARs and the related forms of synaptic plasticity. In contrast to GluA3, GluA2 is unlikely to be directly involved in cAMP-dependent plasticity, since its expression coupled to GluA1 does not compensate for the absence of GluA3 subunits. We propose that GluA2 expression is a structural requisite for GluA3 plasticity, as it appears necessary for proper expression and location of GluA3-containing AMPARs. GluA1/GluA2 heteromers in PCs may then serve to maintain basal synaptic currents when cAMP levels are low.
The finding that an Epac-mediated change in single-channel conductance and open probability of GluA2/GluA3-containing AMPARs may underlie LTP at the PF-PC synapse raises the question of how this change in configuration comes about. Interestingly, the distribution of GluA3-containing AMPARs openings does not seem to respond to a stochastic probability distribution of four LBDs “catching” glutamate with equal probability. Instead, it is biased toward the lowest conductance-state opening, in which only two out of four LBDs bind glutamate. Since GluA3-containing AMPARs predominantly consist of two GluA3 and two GluA2 subunits, only the GluA2 LBDs may effectively bind glutamate under basal conditions. Our observation that enhancing cAMP levels exerts GluA3-containing receptors to produce higher conductance openings (resembling the behavior of GluA1-containing receptors) may suggest that Epac activation triggers a conformational change in the two GluA3 subunits present in each tetramer, such that they become responsive to glutamate binding at the LBD (Figure 5F; Sukumaran et al. 2011).
It is widely accepted that intracellular calcium signaling is a key mechanism for LTP induction in PCs (Coesmans et al., 2004, van Woerden et al., 2009). In the present study we show that postsynaptic LTP depends on cAMP-dependent activation of GluA3-containing receptors. How low calcium signals in PCs are transduced into activation of adenylyl cyclase to raise cAMP levels remains to be elucidated. Interestingly, it has been shown that the tetanic activity of PFs required for LTP induction produces local calcium increases dependent on low-threshold CaV3.1 T-type calcium channels (Hildebrand et al., 2009) and that global deletion or blockage of these channels prevents LTP induction and motor learning (Ly et al., 2013). In this respect, the calcium/calmodulin-dependent adenylyl cyclase Adcy1 (Masada et al., 2012) could be an interesting candidate to convert a local calcium signal into a rise in cAMP.
We have shown here that postsynaptic, GluA3-dependent synaptic potentiation depends on a rise in cAMP. Therefore, this study expands the repertoire of forms of PC plasticity already known to depend on cAMP, such as presynaptic plasticity (Chen and Regehr, 1997, Kaneko and Takahashi, 2004, Lev-Ram et al., 2002, Salin et al., 1996), intrinsic plasticity (Belmeguenai et al., 2010), or plasticity at inhibitory synapses (Mitoma and Konishi, 1996). Epac2 has recently been reported to also have a role in presynaptic plasticity, in that it may modify glutamate release probability (Gekel and Neher, 2008). This raises the interesting possibility that Epac2 and/or cAMP, in their presynaptic and postsynaptic domains, operate in a synergistic fashion to control synaptic plasticity (Le Guen and De Zeeuw, 2010, Wang et al., 2014). Likewise, the induction protocol of LTP produces an increase in intrinsic excitability in PCs via cAMP-mediated PKA modulation of SK potassium channels (Belmeguenai et al., 2010). Thus, since this change in intrinsic excitability occurs at least partly as a secondary process following tetanic PF stimulation, LTP at the PF-PC synapse may act as a feedforward amplifier of synaptic inputs to modulate firing rate in PCs via cAMP production. Finally, it should be noted that rebound potentiation at the molecular-layer interneuron-to-PC synapse, which occurs following PC depolarization, is also mediated by cAMP-mediated PKA modulation (Hirano and Kawaguchi, 2012). Together, these findings point toward a central role of cAMP following induction of PF-PC LTP in regulating multiple forms of plasticity with different identities and natures in a synergistic fashion (Gao et al., 2012).
Synapses are highly dynamic structures, and early removal of synaptic proteins can lead compensatory mechanisms to occur in order to overcome unbalanced synaptic function. However, no compensatory mechanism is able to overcome the declarative memory deficits observed in GluA1-KO mice (Feyder et al., 2007, Humeau et al., 2007). In contrast, GluA1-KO mice learned to adapt their vestibulo-ocular reflexes virtually identically to WT littermates. This finding suggests two possible scenarios: either PC synapses are capable of compensating for the absence of GluA1 through a mechanism that is not present in hippocampal pyramidal or amygdalar cells, or GluA1 is not involved in this form of learning at all. With the evidence presented here, neither of these possibilities can be unequivocally discarded. Yet these findings in GluA1-KO mice emphasize the insufficiency of compensation in GluA3-KO mice; the fact that their PCs could not compensate for the absence of GluA3 to overcome the lack of LTP and the learning deficits highlights the importance of GluA3 for PC synaptic plasticity and motor learning. Taken together, the picture emerges that the learning rules for AMPAR-mediated plasticity in PCs are inverted compared with those in the hippocampus: cerebellar LTP and learning do not require GluA1 but depend on the plasticity of GluA3-containing AMPARs.
Experimental Procedures
Below, the experimental methods are briefly summarized; for extended experimental procedures, which have all been done in a blinded fashion, see Supplemental Information.
Mice
GluA1-KO mice were generated by mating heterozygous c57bl6/129 mice (Kim et al., 2005), GluA3-KO mice were bred from c57bl6x129P2-Gria3tm1Dgen/Mmnc mutant ancestors (MMRRC), and PC-specific GluA3-KO mice were generated by crossing floxed GluA3 mice (Sanchis-Segura et al., 2006) with L7-Cre mice (Barski et al., 2000).
Eye Movement Recordings
Mice were prepared for chronic experiments (de Jeu and De Zeeuw, 2012). Eye orientation was measured using video pupil tracking (Pulnix TM-6710CL). Online image analysis was performed using custom-built software (National Instruments). Angular eye velocity was computed offline (Stahl et al., 2000). The horizontal VOR was characterized using sinusoidal rotation about the vertical axis and subsequently subjected to a VOR cancellation and reversal stimulus.
In Vitro Electrophysiology
Sagittal slices of the vermis were obtained in ice-cold “slicing” solution, and subsequently transferred to the same solution at 34°C. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed using an EPC-10 amplifier (HEKA, Lambrecht). PF-PC LTD was induced by pairing PF stimulation with somatic depolarization (Linden, 2001) or CF stimulation (Schonewille et al., 2011). PF-PC LTP was induced by PF stimulation alone (Schonewille et al., 2010). Cells with more than a 20% change in series resistance over time after plasticity induction were discarded for analysis (Figure S8). Single-channel activity was measured in cell-attached configuration. The driving potential, resulting from subtraction of the resting potential and clamped voltage, was used to calculate the receptor conductance. For the outside-out patches, pipettes with 4–6 MΩ resistance were used to establish Giga-seals. After breaking into whole cell mode, the pipette was retracted until both the cell and the outside-out patch were re-sealed. Spontaneous mEPSC and evoked EPSC recordings were analyzed with MiniAnalysis software (Synaptosoft) and ClampFit (Molecular Devices). The decay time constant for AMPA-evoked currents in outside-out patches in the presence of desensitization blockers was calculated by dividing the total charge transfer (in fC) by the peak amplitude (in pA). Nonstationary fluctuation analysis of outside-out patch traces was carried out according to Hartveit and Veruki (2007).
Drugs
For mEPSC recordings, TTX (Sigma) was added to the bath solution to only measure excitatory spontaneous release. For investigation of the cAMP-GluA3-dependent pathway, we used FSK (Sigma), H89 (Tocris), KT5720 (Sigma), ESI-05 (BioLog), and 8-CPT-2Me-cAMP (Tocris Bioscience). To obtain a monophasic time decay of the AMPA-evoked responses in outside-out patches, we added PEPA (Tocris bioscience) and cyclothiazide (Tocris bioscience).
In Vitro Two-Photon Imaging
Organotypic cerebellar slices were transfected with sindbis-virus-expressing rat GluA3(i) fused to the pH-sensitive version of GFP super-ecliptic pHluorin (SEP-GluA3). For imaging, slices were transferred from the incubation solution to the recording chamber containing ACSF. Three-dimensional images were collected, and optical sections were captured from transfected PC dendrites using ImageJ software (NIH). For single-spine bleaching in the FRAP experiments, a ROI was selected covering the surface of a single spine.
In Vivo Electrophysiology
Mice were prepared for chronic experiments (Schonewille et al., 2010). A recording chamber was constructed around a small craniotomy, and animals were habituated in the setup. Extracellular activities were recorded with glass micropipettes filled with 2M NaCl solution and advanced into the cerebellar cortex. Electrode signals were stored for offline analyses (Spike2, CED, and Cambridge, UK). PCs were identified by the occurrence of both simple spikes and complex spikes, and single-unit activity was confirmed by a brief pause in simple-spike firing following each complex spike. The whole-field visual stimulation was presented by rotating a cylindrical screen. Offline analysis was conducted in MATLAB (Mathworks).
Statistics
For statistical analysis, we used either MATLAB statistical toolbox (MathWorks) or GraphPad Prism 6.
Author Contributions
N.G.-C., C.M.D.S.-M., O.O., H.W.K., and C.I.D.Z. designed, performed, and analyzed the behavioral and histological experiments; N.G.-C., L.M.C.K., K.Z., C.B.C., M.C.R., H.W.K., and C.I.D.Z. designed, performed, and analyzed the electrophysiological and pharmacological experiments; R.S. generated conditional AMPAR-KO mice; and N.G.-C., C.B.C., H.W.K., and C.I.D.Z. wrote the manuscript. H.W.K. and C.I.D.Z. contributed equally to this work.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Dutch Organization for Medical Sciences (C.I.D.Z.), Life Sciences (C.I.D.Z.), and Social and Behavioral Sciences (C.I.D.Z.); by the ERC-adv, ERC-POC, CEREBNET, and C7 programs of the EU (C.I.D.Z.); and by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (H.W.K.).
Published: December 29, 2016
Footnotes
Supplemental Information includes Supplemental Experimental Procedures and eight figures and can be found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.11.046.
Supplemental Information
References
- Adamczyk A., Mejias R., Takamiya K., Yocum J., Krasnova I.N., Calderon J., Cadet J.L., Huganir R.L., Pletnikov M.V., Wang T. GluA3-deficiency in mice is associated with increased social and aggressive behavior and elevated dopamine in striatum. Behav. Brain Res. 2012;229:265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreescu C.E., Milojkovic B.A., Haasdijk E.D., Kramer P., De Jong F.H., Krust A., De Zeeuw C.I., De Jeu M.T. Estradiol improves cerebellar memory formation by activating estrogen receptor beta. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:10832–10839. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2588-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anzai M., Kitazawa H., Nagao S. Effects of reversible pharmacological shutdown of cerebellar flocculus on the memory of long-term horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex adaptation in monkeys. Neurosci. Res. 2010;68:191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.07.2038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barski J.J., Dethleffsen K., Meyer M. Cre recombinase expression in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Genesis. 2000;28:93–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bats C., Farrant M., Cull-Candy S.G. A role of TARPs in the expression and plasticity of calcium-permeable AMPARs: evidence from cerebellar neurons and glia. Neuropharmacology. 2013;74:76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belmeguenai A., Hosy E., Bengtsson F., Pedroarena C.M., Piochon C., Teuling E., He Q., Ohtsuki G., De Jeu M.T., Elgersma Y. Intrinsic plasticity complements long-term potentiation in parallel fiber input gain control in cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:13630–13643. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3226-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benke T.A., Lüthi A., Isaac J.T., Collingridge G.L. Modulation of AMPA receptor unitary conductance by synaptic activity. Nature. 1998;393:793–797. doi: 10.1038/31709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyden E.S., Katoh A., Pyle J.L., Chatila T.A., Tsien R.W., Raymond J.L. Selective engagement of plasticity mechanisms for motor memory storage. Neuron. 2006;51:823–834. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen C., Regehr W.G. The mechanism of cAMP-mediated enhancement at a cerebellar synapse. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:8687–8694. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-22-08687.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coesmans M., Weber J.T., De Zeeuw C.I., Hansel C. Bidirectional parallel fiber plasticity in the cerebellum under climbing fiber control. Neuron. 2004;44:691–700. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman S.K., Hou Y., Willibald M., Semenov A., Möykkynen T., Keinänen K. Aggregation limits surface expression of homomeric GluA3 receptors. J. Biol. Chem. 2016;291:8784–8794. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M115.689125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Jeu M., De Zeeuw C.I. Video-oculography in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 2012;65:e3971. doi: 10.3791/3971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derkach V., Barria A., Soderling T.R. Ca2+/calmodulin-kinase II enhances channel conductance of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate type glutamate receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:3269–3274. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.6.3269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douyard J., Shen L., Huganir R.L., Rubio M.E. Differential neuronal and glial expression of GluR1 AMPA receptor subunit and the scaffolding proteins SAP97 and 4.1N during rat cerebellar development. J. Comp. Neurol. 2007;502:141–156. doi: 10.1002/cne.21294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feyder M., Wiedholz L., Sprengel R., Holmes A. Impaired associative fear learning in mice with complete loss or haploinsufficiency of AMPA GluR1 receptors. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2007;1:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.004.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao Z., van Beugen B.J., De Zeeuw C.I. Distributed synergistic plasticity and cerebellar learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:619–635. doi: 10.1038/nrn3312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gekel I., Neher E. Application of an Epac activator enhances neurotransmitter release at excitatory central synapses. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7991–8002. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0268-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlai R., Henderson J.T., Roder J.C., Jia Z. Multiple behavioral anomalies in GluR2 mutant mice exhibiting enhanced LTP. Behav. Brain Res. 1998;95:37–45. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00002-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutierrez-Castellanos N., Winkelman B.H., Tolosa-Rodriguez L., De Gruijl J.R., De Zeeuw C.I. Impact of aging on long-term ocular reflex adaptation. Neurobiol. Aging. 2013;34:2784–2792. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartveit E., Veruki M.L. Studying properties of neurotransmitter receptors by non-stationary noise analysis of spontaneous postsynaptic currents and agonist-evoked responses in outside-out patches. Nat. Protoc. 2007;2:434–448. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesslow G., Jirenhed D.A., Rasmussen A., Johansson F. Classical conditioning of motor responses: what is the learning mechanism? Neural Netw. 2013;47:81–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2013.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hildebrand M.E., Isope P., Miyazaki T., Nakaya T., Garcia E., Feltz A., Schneider T., Hescheler J., Kano M., Sakimura K. Functional coupling between mGluR1 and Cav3.1 T-type calcium channels contributes to parallel fiber-induced fast calcium signaling within Purkinje cell dendritic spines. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:9668–9682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0362-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirano T., Kawaguchi S.Y. Regulation of inhibitory synaptic plasticity in a Purkinje neuron. Cerebellum. 2012;11:453–454. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0325-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humeau Y., Reisel D., Johnson A.W., Borchardt T., Jensen V., Gebhardt C., Bosch V., Gass P., Bannerman D.M., Good M.A. A pathway-specific function for different AMPA receptor subunits in amygdala long-term potentiation and fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:10947–10956. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2603-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. Historical review of the significance of the cerebellum and the role of Purkinje cells in motor learning. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 2002;978:273–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07574.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jia Z., Agopyan N., Miu P., Xiong Z., Henderson J., Gerlai R., Taverna F.A., Velumian A., MacDonald J., Carlen P. Enhanced LTP in mice deficient in the AMPA receptor GluR2. Neuron. 1996;17:945–956. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80225-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jörntell H., Hansel C. Synaptic memories upside down: bidirectional plasticity at cerebellar parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses. Neuron. 2006;52:227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kakegawa W., Yuzaki M. A mechanism underlying AMPA receptor trafficking during cerebellar long-term potentiation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:17846–17851. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508910102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaneko M., Takahashi T. Presynaptic mechanism underlying cAMP-dependent synaptic potentiation. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:5202–5208. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0999-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessels H.W., Malinow R. Synaptic AMPA receptor plasticity and behavior. Neuron. 2009;61:340–350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim C.H., Takamiya K., Petralia R.S., Sattler R., Yu S., Zhou W., Kalb R., Wenthold R., Huganir R. Persistent hippocampal CA1 LTP in mice lacking the C-terminal PDZ ligand of GluR1. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:985–987. doi: 10.1038/nn1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Guen M.C., De Zeeuw C.I. Presynaptic plasticity at cerebellar parallel fiber terminals. Funct. Neurol. 2010;25:141–151. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lev-Ram V., Wong S.T., Storm D.R., Tsien R.Y. A new form of cerebellar long-term potentiation is postsynaptic and depends on nitric oxide but not cAMP. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:8389–8393. doi: 10.1073/pnas.122206399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ly R., Bouvier G., Schonewille M., Arabo A., Rondi-Reig L., Léna C., Casado M., De Zeeuw C.I., Feltz A. T-type channel blockade impairs long-term potentiation at the parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapse and cerebellar learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:20302–20307. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311686110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linden D.J. The expression of cerebellar LTD in culture is not associated with changes in AMPA-receptor kinetics, agonist affinity, or unitary conductance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:14066–14071. doi: 10.1073/pnas.241384598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linden D.J., Connor J.A. Long-term synaptic depression. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1995;18:319–357. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makino H., Malinow R. AMPA receptor incorporation into synapses during LTP: the role of lateral movement and exocytosis. Neuron. 2009;64:381–390. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makino H., Malinow R. Compartmentalized versus global synaptic plasticity on dendrites controlled by experience. Neuron. 2011;72:1001–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malinow R., Malenka R.C. AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plasticity. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;25:103–126. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masada N., Schaks S., Jackson S.E., Sinz A., Cooper D.M. Distinct mechanisms of calmodulin binding and regulation of adenylyl cyclases 1 and 8. Biochemistry. 2012;51:7917–7929. doi: 10.1021/bi300646y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng Y., Zhang Y., Jia Z. Synaptic transmission and plasticity in the absence of AMPA glutamate receptor GluR2 and GluR3. Neuron. 2003;39:163–176. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00368-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitoma H., Konishi S. Long-lasting facilitation of inhibitory transmission by monoaminergic and cAMP-dependent mechanism in rat cerebellar GABAergic synapses. Neurosci. Lett. 1996;217:141–144. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitsushima D., Ishihara K., Sano A., Kessels H.W., Takahashi T. Contextual learning requires synaptic AMPA receptor delivery in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:12503–12508. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1104558108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nabavi S., Fox R., Proulx C.D., Lin J.Y., Tsien R.Y., Malinow R. Engineering a memory with LTD and LTP. Nature. 2014;511:348–352. doi: 10.1038/nature13294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nedelescu H., Kelso C.M., Lázaro-Muñoz G., Purpura M., Cain C.K., Ledoux J.E., Aoki C. Endogenous GluR1-containing AMPA receptors translocate to asymmetric synapses in the lateral amygdala during the early phase of fear memory formation: an electron microscopic immunocytochemical study. J. Comp. Neurol. 2010;518:4723–4739. doi: 10.1002/cne.22472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen-Vu T.D., Kimpo R.R., Rinaldi J.M., Kohli A., Zeng H., Deisseroth K., Raymond J.L. Cerebellar Purkinje cell activity drives motor learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:1734–1736. doi: 10.1038/nn.3576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peter S., Ten Brinke M.M., Stedehouder J., Reinelt C.M., Wu B., Zhou H., Zhou K., Boele H.J., Kushner S.A., Lee M.G. Dysfunctional cerebellar Purkinje cells contribute to autism-like behaviour in Shank2-deficient mice. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:12627. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poon K., Nowak L.M., Oswald R.E. Characterizing single-channel behavior of GluA3 receptors. Biophys. J. 2010;99:1437–1446. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2010.06.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poon K., Ahmed A.H., Nowak L.M., Oswald R.E. Mechanisms of modal activation of GluA3 receptors. Mol. Pharmacol. 2011;80:49–59. doi: 10.1124/mol.111.071688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossmann M., Sukumaran M., Penn A.C., Veprintsev D.B., Babu M.M., Greger I.H. Subunit-selective N-terminal domain associations organize the formation of AMPA receptor heteromers. EMBO J. 2011;30:959–971. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2011.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rumpel S., LeDoux J., Zador A., Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science. 2005;308:83–88. doi: 10.1126/science.1103944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salin P.A., Malenka R.C., Nicoll R.A. Cyclic AMP mediates a presynaptic form of LTP at cerebellar parallel fiber synapses. Neuron. 1996;16:797–803. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80099-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchis-Segura C., Borchardt T., Vengeliene V., Zghoul T., Bachteler D., Gass P., Sprengel R., Spanagel R. Involvement of the AMPA receptor GluR-C subunit in alcohol-seeking behavior and relapse. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:1231–1238. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4237-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sans N., Vissel B., Petralia R.S., Wang Y.X., Chang K., Royle G.A., Wang C.Y., O’Gorman S., Heinemann S.F., Wenthold R.J. Aberrant formation of glutamate receptor complexes in hippocampal neurons of mice lacking the GluR2 AMPA receptor subunit. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:9367–9373. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-28-09367.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonewille M., Belmeguenai A., Koekkoek S.K., Houtman S.H., Boele H.J., van Beugen B.J., Gao Z., Badura A., Ohtsuki G., Amerika W.E. Purkinje cell-specific knockout of the protein phosphatase PP2B impairs potentiation and cerebellar motor learning. Neuron. 2010;67:618–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schonewille M., Gao Z., Boele H.J., Veloz M.F., Amerika W.E., Simek A.A., De Jeu M.T., Steinberg J.P., Takamiya K., Hoebeek F.E. Reevaluating the role of LTD in cerebellar motor learning. Neuron. 2011;70:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shi S., Hayashi Y., Esteban J.A., Malinow R. Subunit-specific rules governing AMPA receptor trafficking to synapses in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Cell. 2001;105:331–343. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(01)00321-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokolova I.V., Lester H.A., Davidson N. Postsynaptic mechanisms are essential for forskolin-induced potentiation of synaptic transmission. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;95:2570–2579. doi: 10.1152/jn.00617.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stahl J.S., van Alphen A.M., De Zeeuw C.I. A comparison of video and magnetic search coil recordings of mouse eye movements. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2000;99:101–110. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(00)00218-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg J.P., Takamiya K., Shen Y., Xia J., Rubio M.E., Yu S., Jin W., Thomas G.M., Linden D.J., Huganir R.L. Targeted in vivo mutations of the AMPA receptor subunit GluR2 and its interacting protein PICK1 eliminate cerebellar long-term depression. Neuron. 2006;49:845–860. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sukumaran M., Rossmann M., Shrivastava I., Dutta A., Bahar I., Greger I.H. Dynamics and allosteric potential of the AMPA receptor N-terminal domain. EMBO J. 2011;30:972–982. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2011.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T., Svoboda K., Malinow R. Experience strengthening transmission by driving AMPA receptors into synapses. Science. 2003;299:1585–1588. doi: 10.1126/science.1079886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- ten Brinke M.M., Boele H.J., Spanke J.K., Potters J.W., Kornysheva K., Wulff P., IJpelaar A.C., Koekkoek S.K., De Zeeuw C.I. Evolving models of Pavlovian conditioning: cerebellar cortical dynamics in awake behaving mice. Cell Rep. 2015;13:1977–1988. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.10.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsalkova T., Mei F.C., Li S., Chepurny O.G., Leech C.A., Liu T., Holz G.G., Woods V.L., Jr., Cheng X. Isoform-specific antagonists of exchange proteins directly activated by cAMP. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:18613–18618. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210209109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Woerden G.M., Hoebeek F.E., Gao Z., Nagaraja R.Y., Hoogenraad C.C., Kushner S.A., Hansel C., De Zeeuw C.I., Elgersma Y. betaCaMKII controls the direction of plasticity at parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:823–825. doi: 10.1038/nn.2329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y.T., Linden D.J. Expression of cerebellar long-term depression requires postsynaptic clathrin-mediated endocytosis. Neuron. 2000;25:635–647. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81066-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang D.J., Su L.D., Wang Y.N., Yang D., Sun C.L., Zhou L., Wang X.X., Shen Y. Long-term potentiation at cerebellar parallel fiber-Purkinje cell synapses requires presynaptic and postsynaptic signaling cascades. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:2355–2364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4064-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolfrey K.M., Srivastava D.P., Photowala H., Yamashita M., Barbolina M.V., Cahill M.E., Xie Z., Jones K.A., Quilliam L.A., Prakriya M., Penzes P. Epac2 induces synapse remodeling and depression and its disease-associated forms alter spines. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1275–1284. doi: 10.1038/nn.2386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y., Lisberger S.G. Role of plasticity at different sites across the time course of cerebellar motor learning. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:7077–7090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0017-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.