Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Emotion. 2016 Jul 25;17(1):102–119. doi: 10.1037/emo0000171

Emotionally Negative Pictures Enhance Gist Memory

S H Bookbinder 1, C J Brainerd 1
PMCID: PMC5266749  NIHMSID: NIHMS793727  PMID: 27454002

Abstract

In prior work on how true and false memory are influenced by emotion, valence and arousal have often been conflated. Thus, it is difficult to say which specific effects are due to valence and which are due to arousal. In the present research, we used a picture-memory paradigm that allowed emotional valence to be manipulated with arousal held constant. Negatively-valenced pictures elevated both true and false memory, relative to positive and neutral pictures. Conjoint recognition modeling revealed that negative valence (a) reduced erroneous suppression of true memories and (b) increased the familiarity of the semantic content of both true and false memories. Overall, negative valence impaired the verbatim side of episodic memory but enhanced the gist side, and these effects persisted even after a week-long delay.

Keywords: emotional valence, fuzzy-trace theory, conjoint recognition


Personal experience and intuition tell us that our emotions affect the strength of our memories: It is typically easier to remember a birthday party or a funeral than what kind of sandwich we ate for lunch. For decades, researchers have studied memory for emotional content, and a particularly interesting line of work deals with the impact of emotion on false memory. As emotion affects memory for true events, so it may affect false memories for events that did not happen. The importance of understanding the connection between emotion and false memory becomes clear when one considers, for example, situations such as legal cases in which the to-be-remembered events are emotionally charged and memory is the only available record of events. Basing convictions on one or a few eyewitness reports is risky if they are apt to be distorted by emotion. However, conflicting data have been reported on that: Emotional content has sometimes increased false memory and sometimes decreased it. These differences could be due to the conflation of valence and arousal and the tendency to interpret results as valence effects even when arousal is not controlled. Consequently, in the present study, we investigated the effects of valence on false memory with arousal controlled.

Background

Researchers have found evidence of an emotional enhancement of memory for emotional content as compared to neutral content for stimuli that range from words to pictures to videos (e.g., Budson, Todman, Chong, Adams, Kensinger, Krangel, & Wright, 2006; Charles, Mather, & Carstensen, 2003; Maras, Gaigg, & Bowler, 2012). Further, in some of these studies emotion increased false memory compared to nonemotional stimuli. For example, Brueckner and Moritz (2009), as well as Gallo, Foster, and Johnson (2009), reported increased false memory for both positive and negative valence compared to neutral valence, using words and pictures, respectively. However, valence and arousal are often conflated in these studies. To illustrate, Budson et al. used versions of the Deese/Roediger/McDermott (DRM; Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995) word lists that were highly arousing as well as negative, relative to neutral, non-arousing DRM lists. Similarly, El Sharkawy, Groth, Vetter, Beraldi, and Fast (2008) used negative DRM lists without controlling arousal, finding that negative lists increased both true and false memory compared to neutral lists. A common result is enhanced memory for negative (and sometimes positive) arousing content compared to neutral, non-arousing content, which makes it difficult to determine whether the enhancement is a result of valence, arousal, or both. In order to measure actual valence effects, it is necessary to compare emotional stimuli to neutral stimuli with equivalent arousal levels.

Research on emotion-memory effects has been focused more on negative than positive valence and, indeed, positive valence has often been intentionally omitted. For example, Kensinger and colleagues (e.g., Kensinger & Corkin, 2004; Kensinger & Schacter, 2007) have explored negative emotion extensively by comparing negative, arousing stimuli to neutral stimuli, demonstrating greater true memory for negative, arousing items compared to neutral items. This difference has been attributed to arousal and is consistent with other research demonstrating enhanced memory for arousing content (e.g., Block, Greenberg, & Goodman, 2009). The focus on negative valence is motivated by the fact that in many high-stakes memory situations, such as legal cases and risky decision making, moods and to-be remembered information are negative and arousing. However, the lack of data on positive valence and the lack of control of arousal means that it is unclear whether memory enhancement is specific to negative content and whether it is due to arousal rather than valence.

The focus on negative valence applies to false memory studies as well. Neither Budson et al. nor El Sharkawy et al. included positive lists. Neither did two other similar studies by Pesta, Murphy, and Sanders (2001) and Gaigg and Bowler (2009), in which case false memory for negative, arousing lists was compared to false memory for neutral lists. Brainerd, Stein, Silveira, Rohenkohl, and Reyna (2008), however, controlled arousal while manipulating valence, and found that positive and negative valence affected memory differently. Using emotional DRM lists, they found that true and false memory were highest for negative words and lowest for positive words, suggesting that arousal was not the sole cause of emotion effects in prior false memory studies. In some similar research, Dehon, Laroi, and Van der Linden (2010) used positive, negative, and neutral word lists with equal arousal levels and found higher levels of false memory for positive and negative lists compared to neutral lists, although true memory was not affected by valence. Thus, there is evidence of pure valence effects on false memory for words.

Other researchers have manipulated valence and arousal factorially in false memory experiments. Van Damme and Smets (2014) reported dissociations between valence and arousal and between positive and negative valance. For instance, negative valence reduced true memory for peripheral details of pictures in the misinformation paradigm, but arousal increased true memory for central details. In addition, false memories of peripheral details were more common for negative information relative to positive information, regardless of arousal level. Similarly, Brainerd, Holliday, Reyna, Yang, and Toglia (2010) reported that the effects of valence but not arousal differed for true and false memory. Negative valence increased false memory and decreased true memory, relative to positive valence, but arousal increased both. These data suggest that when arousal is controlled or valence and arousal are manipulated factorially, pure valence effects on both true and false memory are obtained and that these effects may be different for positive versus negative valence.

In the work reviewed thus far, the dominant methodology was the DRM illusion, in which subjects study words that all relate to an unpresented critical word (e.g., the “sleep” list contains the words bed, rest, awake, etc., but not sleep) and then complete a recognition or recall test. The usual finding is high levels of false memory for critical words. Fuzzy-trace theory (FTT, Brainerd & Reyna, 1998) explains these false memories via two types of memory traces that are stored in parallel during list presentation. Verbatim traces capture the surface form of words, whereas gist traces capture broader, semantic content that includes connections among items with similar meanings. During the test phase, retrieval of gist traces supports false memories, but retrieval of verbatim traces suppresses them.

Activation monitoring theory (AMT; Roediger, Watson, McDermott, & Gallo, 2001) accounts for DRM false memory via automatic associative activation of unpresented critical words during list presentation. During the test phase, source confusion causes subjects to falsely recognize or recall the activated critical words. Another theory that is based on word associations, associative-activation theory (AAT; Howe, 2005; Howe, Wimmer, Gagnon, & Plumpton, 2009), proposes that activation of list words spreads to nearby concepts, which in turn activate other concepts, resulting in activation of critical words. A key difference between the gist versus word-association mechanisms for DRM false memory is that associative activations are known to be short-lived, typically lasting for a few seconds (Tse & Neely, 2005), whereas gist memories are known to be stable over weeks, months, and longer (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005). In DRM experiments, associative activation has typically been measured by incorporating implicit priming tasks, such as lexical decision, in the DRM paradigm. Such tasks reveal that associative priming effects in that paradigm are relatively short-lived (Tse & Neely, 2005, 2007), although longer lasting associative activation has been detected with other procedures, such as incubation in problem solving tasks (Sio & Ormerod, 2015). Thus, the gist mechanism expects that false memories for critical words will be long-lasting, spanning weeks, whereas the word-association mechanism predicts that they will be short-lived (Roediger et al., 2001). The former result has been consistently observed (e.g., Seamon, Luo, Kopecky, Price, Rothschild, Fung, & Schwartz, 2002; Toglia, Neuschatz, & Goodwin, 1999).

In another comparison of the two theoretical accounts, Dewhurst, Pursglove, and Lewis (2007) noted that the gist mechanism but not the word-association mechanism predicts that strengthening list themes without strengthening word associations, by presenting DRM lists in story contexts, should elevate false memory and elevate it especially in subjects with weaker meaning connection abilities. Their data confirmed both predictions. Some may interpret Dewhurst et al.’s findings as not ruling out the word-association mechanism on that ground that story contexts may somehow have unintentionally increased associative activation as well as strengthening list themes. Although that is possible, note that the gist mechanism clearly predicts Dewhurst’s results, whereas the word-association mechanism does not.

Returning to emotion and false memory, FTT has been applied to false memory for emotional words (Brainerd et al., 2008) and so have associative theories (Talmi & Moscovitch, 2004). However, questions about emotion and false memory extend to many types of to-be-remembered events other than word lists. It is not obvious how associative accounts could be applied outside the word list domain because the mechanism that generates false memories is concerned with pre-existing properties of words. FTT is readily applied in other domains, however, because its mechanism is not confined to properties of words; indeed, its mechanism was originally developed to explain false memory for more complex events, such as narratives and real-life experiences (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005).

This is an important consideration because it is clearly important to study emotion-false memory effects with materials other than DRM lists, and to at least determine whether the effects that are obtained with DRM lists hold up with other materials and paradigms. In the present research, we implemented a quite different procedure that involves false memory for realistic pictures, which was originally developed by Koutstaal and Schacter (1997). Pictures are presented that are all members of the same category (e.g., cows), and unpresented pictures from that category then serve as the critical distractors that measure false memory. This paradigm generates levels of false memory that are similar to the DRM paradigm, but false memories are rooted in categorical relations among real-world objects and events (as depicted in pictures) rather than word associations. We introduced an emotional valence manipulation in this paradigm in order to determine how false memories for such objects and events are affected by their valence.

Another important feature of the present research, relative to most prior work in this area, is that we measure valence effects at the level of specific retrieval processes as well as at the level of raw memory performance. Here, we implemented a procedure that Brainerd et al. (2008) used in their experiments, which is called conjoint recognition (Brainerd, Reyna, & Mojardin, 1999). In that procedure, the contributions of verbatim and gist retrieval to true and false memory are factored with the parameters of a mathematical model, which allows one to determine which type of retrieval is affected by valence. We briefly summarize conjoint recognition methodology before reporting our research.

Conjoint Recognition

In FTT, true memories are supported by both verbatim and gist traces, but the two types of traces have opposite effects on false memory. Many manipulations have been studied that should affect verbatim and gist retrieval differently (for a review, see Brainerd & Reyna, 2005), and such predictions can be measured with conjoint recognition methodology. The specific retrieval processes that are measured are noted and defined in Table 1. Conjoint recognition methodology has two key features. First, on memory tests, three different types of test questions are administered for all test items: Verbatim (V; I saw this item); gist (G; I did not see this item but I saw one with the same meaning); and verbatim + gist (VG; I either saw this item or one with the same meaning).1 Second, a multinomial model (see Appendix) is defined over this 3 × 3 data space that measures three distinct retrieval processes for false memory (see Brainerd, Reyna, Wright, & Mojardin, 2003): (a) recollection rejection, which supports correct rejection of related distractors by retrieving verbatim traces of their corresponding targets, (b) phantom recollection, which supports false acceptance of related distractors by retrieving gist traces that are so strong that they stimulate illusory vivid reinstatement of their “presentation” and (c) familiarity, which also supports false acceptance of related distractors by retrieving weaker gist traces. The model also measures three retrieval processes for true memory: (a) erroneous recollection rejection, which supports false rejection of targets via retrieval of verbatim traces of other related targets, (b) identity judgment, which supports correct acceptance of targets by retrieving their verbatim traces, and (c) familiarity, which supports correct acceptance of targets by retrieving their gist traces.

Table 1.

Parameters of the Conjoint Recognition Model

Parameter Definition
False Memory Processes
PD Phantom recollection (RD produces retrieval of gist trace of similar
target and false acceptance of RD)
RD Recollection rejection (RD produces retrieval of verbatim trace of
similar target and correct rejection of RD)
FD Similarity judgment (RD produces retrieval of gist trace of similar
target and acceptance of RD)
True Memory Processes
ET Erroneous recollection rejection (target produces retrieval of
verbatim trace for another target and incorrect rejection of target)
RT Identity judgment (target produces retrieval of verbatim trace and
correct acceptance of target)
FT Similarity judgment (target produces retrieval of gist trace of
similar target and acceptance of target)
Response Bias Processes
Β V URD produces false alarm in V condition
β G URD produces false alarm in G condition
β VG URD produces false alarm in VG condition

Note. RD = related distractor, TG = target, and URD = unrelated distractor.

In the research that we report, conjoint recognition methodology was combined with Koutstaal and Schacter’s (1997) false memory paradigm. We administered emotional pictures belonging to several categories and then estimated the processes in Table 1 in order to pinpoint the effects of emotional content on specific retrieval processes. To generate those data, the valence of the pictures (positive, negative, neutral) was factorially manipulated with arousal controlled.

Within this core design, we also investigated the effects of the number of exemplars that were presented per category, blocked versus random presentation of exemplars, amount of prior memory testing, and retention interval, on false memory. These four manipulations were included because they have been previously demonstrated to be effective at dissociating verbatim and gist processing. The number of exemplars is a gist repetition manipulation that strengthens memory for categorical relations but does not strengthen verbatim memory for individual exemplars because each exemplar is only presented once (Powell, Roberts, Ceci, & Hembrooke, 1999). Similarly, presenting all of the members of a category in a block, rather than interspersing exemplars from different categories, strengthens memory for categorical relations but does not strengthen verbatim memory for individual exemplars because each exemplar is only presented once (Payne, Elie, Blackwell, & Neuschatz, 1996). Finally, we investigated the effects of amount of prior testing and retention interval using two separate recognition tests in order to determine whether false memory can be elevated merely by testing the same item twice, and how false memories are affected over a week-long delay. These are matters of high relevance to forensic interviewing. Moreover, it is obviously important to understand how the effects of valence are modified by repeated testing and by forgetting.

Method

Subjects

Sixty-eight undergraduates (46 women and 22 men; mean age = 19.59 years) participated in exchange for course credit. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of two presentation order conditions (blocked or random).

Materials

Pilot test

Pilot testing was conducted in order to determine the valence and arousal levels of stimulus pictures, as well as their ability to induce false memory. Candidate pictures were taken from the International Affective Pictures System (IAPS; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 2008), the Geneva Affective Picture Database (GAPED; Dan-Glauser & Scherer, 2011), and stock photo websites, creating a large pool of pictures with categories that varied in valence and arousal. Mean valence scores for categories were computed based on the ratings of individual pictures. We used the valence and arousal norms that Lang, et al. and Dan-Glauser and Scherer reported for the IAPS and GAPED. For pictures from the photo website, we obtained valence and arousal ratings from 25 subjects using Bradley and Lang’s (1994) Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) rating method. Subjects viewed the images for 2 seconds and rated the degree to which each made them feel happy versus unhappy and calm versus excited. Each scale ranged from 1-9 with higher values representing more positive and more arousing feelings. Picture categories were selected to fall into three valence groups based on previously used valence categories for pictures using the SAM scale (e.g., Kensinger & Schacter, 2008; Mickley & Kensinger, 2009): positive (mean valence = 6.79), negative (mean valence = 2.09), and neutral (mean valence = 5.27). See Table 2 for valence and arousal means for each picture category. All three valence categories had mean arousal scores of less than 6.5 and did not differ significantly in mean arousal.

Table 2.

Valence and Arousal Means for Each Picture Category

Category Mean Valence (SD) Mean Arousal (SD)

Negative
Accidents 1.74 (1.14) 6.35 (1.7)
Funeral 1.50 (.79) 3.28 (1.88)
Garbage 2.27 (.99) 4.19 (1.64)
Human Concerns 1.53 (.91) 4.48 (2.14)
Medical 3.09 (1.53) 5.31 (1.54)
Spiders 2.40 (1.59) 6.15 (1.56)

Neutral
Abstract Figures 5.59 (1.65) 5.18 (1.81)
Couches 5.78 (1.77) 2.50 (1.25)
Men 5.12 (1.54) 3.87 (1.56)
Mushrooms 4.31 (2.10) 3.94 (1.66)
Mugs 5.06 (1.30) 3.39 (2.06)
Trees 5.78 (1.86) 3.28 (2.16)

Positive
Babies 7.41 (1.41) 5.55 (2.23)
Baseball 6.89 (1.51) 4.55 (1.98)
Clouds 7.00 (1.41) 1.61 (.78)
Holding Hands 7.00 (1.53) 4.26 (1.96)
Kittens 5.85 (2.22) 4.26 (2.12)
Mountains 6.56 (1.20) 4.61 (2.09)

The target and related distractor materials were 216 color pictures, which were members of 18 object, person, and scene categories (e.g., couches, babies, car accidents). There were 6 positive, 6 negative, and 6 neutral categories, each containing 12 items. There were also 18 unrelated pictures that were not members of any of the categories, 9 of which were used as unrelated targets and 9 of which were unrelated distractors included for use in bias correction. The distractors were evenly distributed among valences such that there were 3 unrelated distractors of each valence.

Within each picture category, 3 pictures were used as starting points to generate the remaining 9 pictures in the category by manipulating the picture’s hue, its mirror image, or both (see Table 3). These manipulations were chosen in order to create high levels of visual similarity as well as to maintain consistency in the way pictures of different valences were altered. Within each category there were 12 images comprised as follows: picture 1a with three other versions, 1b-1d; picture 2a with three other versions, 2b-2d; and picture 3a with three other versions, 3b-3d. Pictures 1, 2, and 3 were distinct from one another, whereas the other versions of each picture were all similar to it and to each other. As an example, the “baby” category contained pictures of three different babies. There were four versions of those babies, for a total of 12 baby pictures.

Table 3.

Examples of Experimental Stimuli

Babies (positive) Spiders (negative) Couches (neutral)
1a graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0005.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0006.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0007.jpg
1b graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0008.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0009.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0010.jpg
1c graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0011.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0012.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0013.jpg
1d graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0014.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0015.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0016.jpg
2a graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0017.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0018.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0019.jpg
2b graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0020.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0021.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0022.jpg
2c graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0023.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0024.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0025.jpg
2d graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0026.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0027.jpg graphic file with name nihms-793727-t0028.jpg

Note: Adapted from “The Geneva affective picture database (GAPED): A new 730-picture database focusing on valence and normative significance” by E. S. Dan-Glauser and K. R. Scherer, 2011, Behavioral Research Methods, 43, 468-477. Copyright 2011 by Springer.

Each category was assigned to one of two number of exemplar conditions such that 3-exemplar categories had 3 targets and 9 nonstudied related distractors, and 8-exemplar categories had 8 targets and 4 nonstudied related distractors. The number of exemplars manipulation was counterbalanced so that three of the 6 positive categories were 3-exemplar categories and 3 of the positive categories were 8-exemplar categories, etc. The 3-exemplar lists consisted of pictures one, 2, and 3 from a given category—that is, 3 distinct exemplars—whereas the 8-exemplar lists consisted of each of the 3 distinct pictures plus 1 or 2 of the other versions of each of those, randomly chosen (see Table 4 for a sample list). In other words, the number of exemplars manipulation was not purely numerical, but the two conditions also differed in terms of the strength of the gist of the list. The 3-exemplar lists would create a weaker gist, for babies in general, for example, whereas the 8-exemplars lists would create a stronger gist, and stronger gist for some babies in particular, by having more pictures in total and by repeating some of them either two or three times, counterbalanced across lists. From this pool of 216 (18 categories × 12 pictures per category), 108 categorized pictures were tested, as well as 18 unrelated pictures.

Table 4.

Sample study list

Item
Number
Name Valence
1 Buffer1 Neutral
2 Buffer2 Neutral
3 Buffer3 Neutral
4 Babies1c Positive
5 Babies2b Positive
6 Babies3a Positive
7 Funeral3b Negative
8 Funeral1a Negative
9 Funeral2c Negative
10 Funeral1c Negative
11 Funeral3a Negative
12 Funeral1d Negative
13 Funeral2b Negative
14 Funeral3d Negative
15 Unrelated1 Neutral
16 Abstract3a Neutral
17 Abstract2b Neutral
18 Abstract1c Neutral
19 Baseball2d Positive
20 Baseball1b Positive
21 Baseball3c Positive
22 Baseball2a Positive
23 Baseball3b Positive
24 Baseball1d Positive
25 Baseball3a Positive
26 Baseball2c Positive
27 Unrelated2 Neutral
28 Couch2d Neutral
29 Couch1a Neutral
30 Couch2c Neutral
31 Couch3b Neutral
32 Couch1c Neutral
33 Couch2a Neutral
34 Couch3c Neutral
35 Couch1b Neutral
36 Accidents3c Negative
37 Accidents2b Negative
38 Accidents1d Negative
39 Unrelated3 Positive
40 Men2a Neutral
41 Men1c Neutral
42 Men3d Neutral
43 Couples3b Positive
44 Couples1c Positive
45 Couples2a Positive
46 Couples3d Positive
47 Couples2b Positive
48 Couples1a Positive
49 Couples2c Positive
50 Couples3a Positive
51 Unrelated4 Positive
52 Garbage2b Negative
53 Garbage1a Negative
54 Garbage3c Negative
55 Mountain1a Positive
56 Mountain3b Positive
57 Mountain2c Positive
58 Mountain1d Positive
59 Mountain3a Positive
60 Mountain2d Positive
61 Mountain1c Positive
62 Mountain3d Positive
63 Unrelated5 Negative
64 Child1d Negative
65 Child2c Negative
66 Child3a Negative
67 Child1b Negative
68 Child3d Negative
69 Child2b Negative
70 Child1a Negative
71 Child3c Negative
72 Clouds1d Positive
73 Clouds2b Positive
74 Clouds3c Positive
75 Unrelated6 Positive
76 Medical3a Negative
77 Medical2d Negative
78 Medical1c Negative
79 Mug1b Neutral
80 Mug3d Neutral
81 Mug2c Neutral
82 Mug3b Neutral
83 Mug1a Neutral
84 Mug2d Neutral
85 Mug1c Neutral
86 Mug2b Neutral
87 Unrelated6 Negative
88 Cat3a Positive
89 Cat2c Positive
90 Cat1d Positive
91 Trees3c Neutral
92 Trees2d Neutral
93 Trees3a Neutral
94 Trees1b Neutral
95 Trees2c Neutral
96 Trees1d Neutral
97 Trees3b Neutral
98 Trees2a Neutral
99 Unrelated8 Negative
100 Mushroom2a Neutral
101 Mushroom1c Neutral
102 Mushroom3b Neutral
103 Unrelated9 Neutral
104 Spider3c Negative
105 Spider2a Negative
106 Spider1c Negative
107 Spider3b Negative
108 Spider1d Negative
109 Spider2c Negative
110 Spider3a Negative
111 Spider2b Negative
112 Buffer4 Neutral
113 Buffer5 Neutral
114 Buffer6 Neutral

Procedure

The experiment involved three phases over two sessions: (a) picture presentation, (b) immediate test, and (c) one-week delayed test. Phases (a) and (b) were completed during the first session and phase (c) was completed during the second session.

Picture presentation

Upon arrival at the laboratory, subjects were randomly assigned to one of the two presentation order conditions. Those in the blocked condition viewed all pictures from each category in sequence, with category order being randomized. Those in the random condition viewed pictures in a fully randomized order with no more than two pictures from the same category appearing in sequence. All subjects read instructions about the study task (to just look at the pictures), and then picture presentation began with 3 buffer pictures that did not belong to any of the categories, followed by the target pictures, and ended with 3 buffer pictures that did not belong to any of the categories (see Table 4). Each picture was presented for 1 second. Next, the subjects worked on math problems for 3 minutes.

Immediate test

Subjects read the test instructions for conjoint recognition (see Brainerd et al., 1999), again presented on the computer screen, and then proceeded with a self-paced conjoint recognition test. The instructions explained that subjects would view a series of test pictures, each of which might or might not have been presented earlier, and that each picture would be accompanied by one of three types of test questions, to which they should respond yes or no. The three conjoint recognition questions were explained in depth: if the question was “I saw this picture during the study phase” (V), subjects were to respond “yes” if the picture was presented and “no” otherwise. If the question was “this picture is new but similar to a picture from the study phase” (G), subjects were to respond “yes” if the picture was similar to one from the study phase, but not identical, and “no” otherwise. Finally, if the question was “I saw either this picture or a similar picture during the study phase,” (VG) they were to respond “yes” to any picture that was not an unrelated distractor. Subjects were instructed that the test would be self-paced, to click the mouse to progress from picture to picture, and to answer all items without skipping any, even if they were unsure of the answer. Subjects were shown example pictures for each type of test question, along with the correct responses, and were given the opportunity to ask questions before proceeding.

After reading the instructions, subjects completed the 63-item self-paced immediate recognition test. Half of the pictures were tested, with testing of the second half being delayed until the second session. This method of testing only half of the items and retesting them later along with untested items was chosen because it allows two effects to be measured (see Brainerd, Reyna, & Estrada, 2006): forgetting and prior testing. Forgetting can be measured by comparing responses to items on the immediate test with responses to untested items on the delayed test, whereas the effects of prior testing can be measured by comparing responses to tested and untested items on the delayed test. The immediate test was comprised of 30 targets, 28 related distractors, and 5 unrelated distractors, with the targets and distractors being drawn as equally as possible from each of the categories and from 3- and 8-exemplar lists. Further, the V, G, and VG test questions were distributed among categories, targets, and related distractors such that there were approximately 20 pictures per test question type, half of which were targets and half of which were related distractors, in addition to the 5 unrelated distractors (see Table 5).

Table 5.

Sample immediate test list

Item
Number
Name Valence Question
Type
Item
Type
1 Garbage1a Negative V T
2 Clouds2c Positive V RD
3 Unrelated9 Neutral VG URT
4 Accidents3c Negative VG T
5 Tree1b Neutral VG T
6 Mushroom2d Neutral G RD
7 Spider3b Negative V T
8 Babies3a Positive G T
9 Cat2a Positive VG RD
10 Unrelated6 Positive G URT
11 Mountains2c Positive V T
12 Couples1b Positive V RD
13 Baseball1b Positive VG T
14 Unrelated5 Negative V URT
15 Men1c Neutral G T
16 Abstract2b Neutral V T
17 Couch1c Neutral VG T
18 Accidents1d Negative V T
19 Mug3a Neutral G RD
20 Unrelated2 Neutral G URT
21 Couples3c Positive VG RD
22 Funeral2a Negative G RD
23 Baseball3a Positive V T
24 Child2b Negative V T
25 Child1c Negative VG RD
26 Couch2b Neutral G RD
27 Funeral1d Negative VG T
28 Cat1d Positive G T
29 Tree3d Neutral G RD
30 Medical3a Negative G T
31 Unrelated Positive V URD
32 Abstract1d Neutral VG RD
33 Men2a Neutral V T
34 Clouds1a Positive VG RD
35 Mountains1b Positive VG RD
36 Mug2b Neutral VG T
37 Garbage3c Negative VG T
38 Mushroom3b Neutral V T
39 Couples2a Positive V T
40 Babies2a Positive G RD
41 Unrelated Neutral V URD
42 Spider2c Negative VG T
43 Clouds3c Positive V T
44 Couch3c Neutral V T
45 Funeral3c Negative VG RD
46 Tree2b Neutral VG RD
47 Babies1d Positive VG RD
48 Garbage2d Negative V RD
49 Unrelated Negative G URD
50 Medical2a Negative V RD
51 Accidents2a Negative V RD
52 Mountains3c Positive G RD
53 Unrelated Negative VG URD
54 Child3b Negative V RD
55 Mug1a Neutral G T
56 Baseball2b Positive V RD
57 Abstract3c Neutral G RD
58 Cat3a Positive VG T
59 Mushroom1a Neutral V RD
60 Men3b Neutral G RD
61 Medical1c Negative V T
62 Spider1a Negative G RD
63 Unrelated Positive G URD

One-week test

After one week, subjects returned to the laboratory to complete the delayed recognition test. They read the same instructions as before and responded to a self-paced conjoint recognition test. On this test, all of the target, related distractor, and unrelated distractor pictures were tested, with half being tested for the first time and half being retested (see Table 6). Pictures that were tested for the second time were paired with the same test questions as on the immediate test. Upon completion of the conjoint recognition test, the subjects answered demographic questions and received a debriefing.

Table 6.

Sample delayed test list

Item
Number
Name Valence Question
Type
Item Type Tested?
1 Baby2b Positive VG T No
2 Mushroom2a Neutral G T No
3 Garbage1d Negative G RD No
4 Men1c Neutral G T Yes
5 Accidents1d Negative V T Yes
6 Spider3b Negative V T Yes
7 Cat1d Positive G T Yes
8 Unrelated1 Neutral V URD Yes
9 Baby3d Positive V RD No
10 Baseball1b Positive VG T Yes
11 Mug3a Neutral G RD Yes
12 Clouds3c Positive V T Yes
13 Abstract2a Neutral V RD No
14 Child2b Negative V T Yes
15 Tree1b Neutral VG T Yes
16 Mountain1c Positive VG T No
17 Spider3d Negative V RD No
18 Couch1c Neutral VG T Yes
19 Funeral1a Negative V T No
20 Unrelated2 Positive V URD Yes
21 Abstract1c Neutral VG T No
22 Accidents2b Negative G T No
23 Baseball1a Positive G RD No
24 Mug2b Neutral VG T Yes
25 Couch3d Neutral VG RD No
26 Cat1b Positive V RD No
27 Couples2a Positive V T Yes
28 Baseball3a Positive V T Yes
29 Couples1a Positive VG T No
30 Mushroom2d Neutral G RD Yes
31 Unrelated3 Neutral G URD No
32 Funeral1d Negative VG T Yes
33 Abstract3c Neutral G RD Yes
34 Couch1a Neutral G T No
35 Accidents3c Negative VG T Yes
36 Tree2c Neutral G T No
37 Garbage1a Negative V T Yes
38 Men1a Neutral V RD No
39 Baby2a Positive G RD Yes
40 Tree3c Neutral V T No
41 Unrelated4 Positive VG URT No
42 Mushroom3b Neutral V T Yes
43 Couples3a Positive G T No
44 Accidents3a Negative G RD No
45 Child3a Negative G T No
46 Baseball2a Positive G T No
47 Unrelated5 Negative VG URT No
48 Clouds3a Positive G RD No
49 Mountain1b Positive VG RD Yes
50 Funeral1c Negative G T No
51 Child1c Negative VG RD Yes
52 Mug1a Neutral G T Yes
53 Cat3a Positive VG T Yes
54 Couch3c Neutral V T Yes
55 Unrelated6 Neutral VG URD No
56 Medical3a Negative G T Yes
57 Garbage3a Negative VG RD No
58 Mushroom1d Neutral VG RD No
59 Abstract3a Neutral G T No
60 Unrelated7 Positive G URT Yes
61 Garbage2d Negative V RD Yes
62 Couples3c Positive G RD Yes
63 Men2a Neutral V T Yes
64 Men3d Neutral VG T No
65 Baseball2b Positive V RD Yes
66 Spider1a Negative G RD Yes
67 Clouds2b Positive VG T No
68 Funeral3c Negative VG RD Yes
69 Abstract2b Neutral V T Yes
70 Mountain2c Positive V T Yes
71 Baby1c Positive V T No
72 Mountain2d Positive G T No
73 Mug3c Neutral VG RD No
74 Mug1c Neutral V T No
75 Clouds1d Positive G T No
76 Child2a Negative G RD No
77 Baby1d Positive VG RD Yes
78 Funeral2a Negative G RD Yes
79 Mushroom1c Neutral VG T No
80 Clouds2c Positive V RD Yes
81 Spider2c Negative VG T Yes
82 Medical1c Negative V T Yes
83 Couch2b Neutral V RD No
84 Medical1a Negative VG RD No
85 Tree3d Neutral G RD Yes
86 Couples2d Positive VG RD No
87 Child3b Negative V RD Yes
88 Unrelated8 Negative V URT Yes
89 Cat3d Positive G RD No
90 Baby3a Positive G T Yes
91 Unrelated9 Negative G URT No
92 Mountain3c Positive G RD Yes
93 Abstract1d Neutral VG RD Yes
94 Baseball3d Positive VG RD No
95 Medical3b Negative G RD No
96 Medical2a Negative V RD Yes
97 Unrelated10 Neutral VG URT Yes
98 Garbage2b Negative G T No
99 Clouds1a Positive VG RD Yes
100 Unrelated11 Neutral G URT Yes
101 Unrelated12 Neutral V URT No
102 Couch2b Neutral G RD Yes
103 Child1a Negative VG T No
104 Unrelated13 Positive V URT No
105 Unrelated14 Negative V URD NO
106 Men3b Neutral G RD Yes
107 Unrelated15 Negative G URD Yes
108 Cat2c Positive V T No
109 Unrelated16 Positive VG URD No
110 Unrelated17 Negative VG URD Yes
111 Mushroom1a Neutral V RD Yes
112 Accidents1c Negative VG RD No
113 Cat2a Positive VG RD Yes
114 Couples1b Positive V RD Yes
115 Mountain3a Positive V RD No
116 Funeral2d Negative V RD No
117 Men2c Neutral VG RD No
118 Accidents2a Negative V RD Yes
119 Mug2a Neutral V RD No
120 Tree1a Neutral V RD No
121 Unrelated18 Positive G URD Yes
122 Medical2d Negative VG T No
123 Spider2a Negative G T No
124 Garbage3c Negative VG T Yes
125 Spider1b Negative VG RD No
126 Tree2b Neutral VG RD Yes

Negative: 42 V: 42 RD: 54 Untested: 63
Positive: 42 G: 42 T: 54 Tested 63
Neutral: 42 VG: 42 URD: 9

Results

Qualitative Patterns

Descriptive statistics

The mean acceptance proportions for targets, related distractors, and unrelated distractors are reported in Table 7. For the unrelated distractors on the delayed test (when all of the unrelated distractors were tested), acceptance rates were higher for positive and neutral items compared to negative items in the V condition, t(67) = 3.55 and t(67) = 4.49, both p-values < .05, but across test question there were no reliable valence differences, p-values > .05.

Table 7.

Mean Acceptance Proportions for Target, Related Distractors, and Unrelated Distractors by Valence, Test Question, and Test

V G VG
Targets
Immediate
Positive .79 .08 .94 .61
Negative .74 .19 .93 .62
Neutral .60 .10 .95 .55
.71 .12 .94

Delayed
Positive .63 .29 .79 .57
Negative .71 .23 .70 .55
Neutral .48 .33 .74 .52
.61 .29 .74

Related Distractors
Immediate
Positive .54 .35 .93 .61
Negative .50 .54 .97 .67
Neutral .26 .47 .84 .53
.43 .45 .92

Delayed
Positive .34 .46 .55 .45
Negative .39 .50 .72 .53
Neutral .24 .58 .72 .51
.32 .51 .66

Unrelated Distractors
Immediate
Pooled
Across
Valence
.02 .02 .17

Delayed
Positive .25 .25 .07 .19

Negative .04 .28 .28 .20

Neutral .32 .22 .17 .29

.21 .26 .17

True memory analyses of variance (ANOVAs)

To conduct ANOVAs, the acceptance proportions for targets and related distractors for the V, G, and VG test questions were bias-corrected using the two-high threshold method (Snodgrass & Corwin, 1988). Two mixed model ANOVAs were computed with presentation order as the between subjects factor and bias corrected target acceptance rates on the immediate and delayed tests as the dependent variables. The ANOVA for the immediate test was 3 (test question: V, G, VG) × 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (number of exemplars: 3, 8) × 2 (presentation order: blocked, random), and the ANOVA for the delayed test was 3 (test question: V, G, VG) × 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (number of exemplars: 3, 8) × 2 (presentation order: blocked, random) × 2 (prior testing: tested, not tested). Finally, a third ANOVA was conducted in order to examine the effects of forgetting over the one-week retention interval with 3 (test question: V, G, VG) × 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (number of exemplars: 3, 8) × 2 (presentation order: blocked, random) × 2 (retention interval: immediate, 1 week). The items in the one-week condition were only those that had not been tested on the immediate test so that the comparison could be made between immediate and delayed testing without an effect of repeated testing.

Immediate test

The main effect of test question on target acceptance was significant, F(2,128) = 153.09, p < .001. Planned comparisons revealed that the target acceptance rate was higher in the VG condition than in the V condition, which was higher than the G condition (see Table 7). (Remember that it is correct to accept targets in the VG and V conditions but not in the G condition.) There was also a main effect of valence on the target acceptance rate, F(2,128) = 3.18, p < .05. Planned comparisons revealed that the acceptance rate was higher for negative and positive pictures than neutral ones (see Table 7). Neither presentation order, F(1,64) = .46, n.s., nor number of exemplars, F(1,64) = .03, n.s., affected target acceptance.

There was a test question × valence × number of exemplars interaction, F(4,256) = 20.33, p < .001. As is shown in Figure 1, the number of exemplars did not affect target acceptance in the G or VG conditions, but increasing the number of exemplars reduced target acceptance in the V condition for positive and neutral items while increasing target acceptance in the V condition for negative items. In other words, presenting more pictures within a category increased verbatim memory for negative items but reduced it for positive and neutral items.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Effect of test question, valence, and number of exemplars on target acceptance rate on the immediate test.

Delayed test

The main effect of test question on target acceptance on the delayed test was significant, F(2,128) = 86.74, p < .001. Planned comparisons revealed that the target acceptance rate was higher in the VG condition than in the V condition, which was higher than the G condition, as on the immediate test. There was also a main effect of valence on the target acceptance rate, F(2,128) = 3.34, p < .05. Planned comparisons revealed that the acceptance rate was higher for negative and positive pictures than neutral ones, as on the immediate test. Neither presentation order, F(1,64) = .08, n.s., nor number of exemplars, F(1,64) = 1.15, n.s., affected target acceptance. There was also a significant effect of prior testing on the delayed test, F(1,64) = 30.96, p < .001, with target acceptance being higher for items that had been previously tested (M = .65) than those that had not been previously tested (M = .45).

There was a test question × valence × number of exemplars × prior testing interaction, F(4,256) = 19.41, p < .001 (see Table 8 and Figure 2). Prior testing increased the target acceptance rate in the V condition for all valences except for positive 3-exemplar items. Prior testing also increased target acceptance in the G condition for positive 8-exemplar items and negative 3-exemplar items only, and did not affect neutral items in that condition at all. More generally, prior testing seemed to increase both verbatim retrieval and gist retrieval, but the latter effect was only for emotional items.

Table 8.

Mean Acceptance Proportions for Delayed Test Targets and Related Distractors by Valence, Test Question, Number of Exemplars, and Prior Testing

V G VG

Tested Untested Tested Untested Tested Untested Tested Untested
Targets
3 Exemplars
Positive .65 .63 .37 .21 .91 .67 .64 .50
Negative .82 .55 .41 .15 .77 .66 .67 .36
Neutral .77 .34 .23 .22 .70 .65 .57 .41
.75 .51 .34 .19 .79 .66

8 Exemplars
Positive .73 .51 .56 .04 .88 .70 .72 .42
Negative .82 .66 .23 .45 .77 .59 .61 .57
Neutral .60 .21 .47 .40 .94 .68 .67 .43
.72 .46 .42 .30 .86 .66

Related Distractors
3 Exemplars
Positive .14 .18 .60 .41 .46 .65 .40 .41
Negative .62 .10 .45 .42 .80 .68 .62 .40
Neutral .14 .21 .88 .25 .91 .45 .65 .30
.30 .17 .65 .36 .72 .59

8 Exemplars
Positive .67 .38 .43 .40 .41 .67 .50 .48
Negative .33 .50 .69 .42 .74 .66 .59 .53
Neutral .38 .21 .88 .32 .86 .66 .70 .39
.46 .36 .67 .38 .67 .66
Figure 2.

Figure 2

Effect of test question, valence, number of exemplars, prior testing, and retention interval on target acceptance.

Retention interval

The effect of retention interval was significant, F(1,64) = 5.72, p < .05, with higher target acceptance for untested items on the immediate test (M = .64) than untested items on the delayed test (M = .43). The test question × valence × number of exemplars × retention interval interaction was significant, F(4,256) = 10.35, p < .001 (see Table 9 and Figure 2). Target acceptance in the V condition was lower on the delayed test for positive and neutral items and negative 8-exemplar items. Target acceptance in the G condition, however, was higher on the delayed test for positive 3-exemplar items, negative 8-exemplar items, and neutral items. In brief, forgetting reduced verbatim memory, particularly for emotional items, while increasing gist memory for neutral items and some emotional items. This pattern of decreasing verbatim retrieval coupled with gist stability or increases is consistent with prior research on forgetting and false memory (e.g., Howe, Candel, Otgaar, Malone, & Wimmer, 2010; Lampinen, Copeland, & Neuschatz, 2001).

Table 9.

Mean Acceptance Proportions for Targets and Related Distractors by Valence, Test Question, Number of Exemplars, and Retention Interval

V G VG

Immediate Delayed-
Untested
Immediate Delayed-
Untested
Immediate Delayed-
Untested
Immediate Delayed-
Untested
Targets
3 Exemplars
Positive .87 .63 .12 .21 .93 .67 .64 .50
Negative .56 .55 .19 .15 .92 .66 .56 .36
Neutral .67 .34 .10 .22 .96 .65 .58 .41
.70 .51 .13 .19 .93 .66

8 Exemplars
Positive .71 .51 .05 .04 .96 .70 .57 .42
Negative .91 .66 .19 .45 .94 .59 .68 .46
Neutral .52 .21 .10 .40 .95 .68 .52 .43
.72 .46 .11 .30 .95 .66

Related Distractors
3 Exemplars
Positive .32 .18 .45 .41 .90 .46 .56 .41
Negative .18 .10 .53 .42 .99 .80 .57 .40
Neutral .26 .21 .47 .25 .72 .91 .48 .30
.26 .17 .49 .36 .87 .72

8 Exemplars
Positive .76 .38 .24 .40 .97 .67 .65 .48
Negative .81 .50 .54 .42 .96 .66 .77 .53
Neutral .26 .21 .47 .32 .97 .66 .57 .39
.61 .36 .42 .38 .96 .66

To sum up the true memory ANOVAs, target acceptance was higher for emotional than neutral pictures on both the immediate and the delayed tests. Prior testing increased both verbatim and gist memory, whereas forgetting reduced verbatim memory for emotional items while increasing gist memory.

False memory ANOVAs

Another pair of ANOVAs was computed with presentation order as the between subjects factor and bias corrected related distractor acceptance rates on the immediate and delayed tests as the dependent variables. The ANOVA for the immediate test was 3 (test question: V, G, VG) × 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (number of exemplars: 3, 8) × 2 (presentation order: blocked, random), and the ANOVA for the delayed test was 3 (test question: V, G, VG) × 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (number of exemplars: 3, 8) × 2 (presentation order: blocked, random) × 2 (prior testing: tested, not tested). Finally, a third ANOVA was conducted in order to examine the effects of forgetting over the one-week retention interval, which was 3 (test question: V, G, VG) × 3 (valence: positive, negative, neutral) × 2 (number of exemplars: 3, 8) × 2 (presentation order: blocked, random) × 2 (retention interval: immediate, 1 week).

Immediate test

The main effect of test question on related distractor acceptance was reliable, F(2,128) = 60.06, p < .001. Planned comparisons revealed that the related distractor acceptance rate was higher in the VG condition than the V and G conditions (see Table 7). (Remember that it is correct to accept related distractors in the VG and G conditions but not in the V condition. This lack of difference between the V and G conditions reveals that this was a strong false memory illusion.) There was also a main effect of valence on the related distractor acceptance rate, F(2,128) = 16.01, p < .001. Planned comparisons revealed that the acceptance rate was higher for negative than positive pictures and higher for positive pictures than neutral ones (see Table 7). This is slightly different from the pattern for targets, where the acceptance rates for negative and positive items did not differ. Presentation order did not affect related distractor acceptance, F(1,64) = .17, n.s., but the number of exemplars did, F(1,64) = 60.20, p < .001. The effect of number of exemplars was that the related distractor acceptance rate was higher for 8-exemplar lists (M = .66) than 3-exemplar lists (M = .54). Recall that this effect was not present for targets.

There was a test question × valence × number of exemplars interaction, F(4,256) = 31.32, p < .001. As shown in Figure 3, increasing the number of presented exemplars increased positive and negative related distractor acceptance in the V condition and reduced positive related distractor acceptance in the G condition. This effect is parallel to the effect on targets, wherein increasing the number of exemplars increased acceptance of negative pictures in the G condition and reduced acceptance of positive pictures in the V condition. For related distractors, as well, the number of exemplars increased verbatim memory for emotional items but reduced gist memory for positive items.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Effect of test question, valence, and number of exemplars on related distractor acceptance rate on the immediate test.

Delayed test

The main effect of test question on related distractor acceptance on the delayed test was significant, F(2,128) = 41.44, p < .001. Planned comparisons revealed that the related distractor acceptance rate was higher in the VG condition than in the G condition, which was higher than the V condition. There was also a main effect of valence on the related distractor acceptance rate, F(2,128) = 11.37, p < .05. Planned comparisons revealed that the acceptance rate was higher for negative and neutral pictures than positive ones. Recall here that target acceptance rates were higher for positive and negative pictures compared to neutral pictures on the delayed test.

Presentation order did not affect the related distractor acceptance rate, F(1,64) = .56, n.s., but the number of exemplars did, F(1,64) = 36.28, p < .001, with related distractor acceptance being higher for 8-exemplar lists (M = .53) than 3-exemplar lists (M = .46), as on the immediate test. There was also a significant effect of prior testing on the delayed test, F(1,64) = 22.68, p < .001, with related distractor acceptance being higher for items that had been previously tested (M = .58) than those that had not been previously tested (M = .42), similar to the effect on targets.

There was a test question × valence × number of exemplars × prior testing interaction, F(4,256) = 34.42, p < .001 (see Table 8 and Figure 4). Prior testing increased related distractor acceptance in the G condition for positive 3-exemplar items, negative 8-exemplar items, and neutral items, and increased acceptance in the V condition for negative 3-exemplar items and positive 8-exemplar items. However, it reduced related distractor acceptance in the V condition for negative 8-exemplar items. In other words, prior testing increased gist retrieval for positive related distractors when few exemplars were presented and negative related distractors when many were presented, and increased verbatim retrieval for positive items when many were presented and negative items when few were presented.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Effect of test question, valence, number of exemplars, prior testing, and retention interval on related distractor acceptance.

Retention interval

The effect of retention interval was not significant, F(1,64) = 1.45, n.s., but the test question × valence × number of exemplars × retention interval interaction was , F(4,256) = 41.56, p < .001 (see Table 9 and Figure 4). Related distractor acceptance was reduced over the delay in the V condition for positive items and negative 8-exemplar items, but did not affect neutral items. The retention interval decreased related distractor acceptance of neutral items in the G condition but did not significantly affect it for negative items. Thus, over time verbatim memory declined, particularly for emotional items, and gist memory declined for neutral items while not affecting negative items.

To sum up the false memory results, emotional pictures caused more false memories (false alarms to V test questions) on the immediate test but only negative pictures caused more false memories on the delayed test. Increasing the number of exemplars increased false memories on both tests, likely driven by stronger gist memories. Prior testing increased related distractor acceptance overall, which was presumably also due to stronger gist memories.

Summary of qualitative patterns for valence

Emotional content appeared to increase both verbatim and gist memory for pictures, with the gist effect being more pronounced for negative content and the verbatim effect being more pronounced for positive content. Prior testing increased memory for both targets and related distractors by increasing both verbatim and gist memory, whereas the retention interval reduced verbatim memory, particularly for emotional items. These effects were modified slightly by the number of presented exemplars, with more exemplars tending to increase gist memory but not verbatim memory.

Modeling Results

Next, we consider why these emotional content effects occurred, at the level of specific retrieval processes. To pinpoint the particular retrieval processes that were involved, we applied the conjoint recognition model to the data. First, we consider whether the model fit the data, and then we move on to parametric analyses that identify the process loci of emotional content effects.

Fit

The model’s fit was evaluated in the usual way (see Brainerd et al., 2014) by computing likelihood ratio tests. Those tests generate G2 statistics that are asymptotically distributed as Χ2 with one degree of freedom for each condition. Thus, the critical value for rejection of the null hypothesis that the model fits the data of any condition is 3.84. As can be seen in Table 10, the G2 values for all of the conditions of the present experiment were below this critical value. Therefore, the conjoint recognition model provided a statistically acceptable account of all of the data.

Table 10.

Parameter Estimates of the Conjoint Recognition Model by Valence, Test, and Prior Testing

Model Parameters
ET RT FT PD RD FD β V β G β VG G2
Immediate Test
Positive .69 .73 .47 .58 .29 .73 .02 .18 .01 2.24
Negative .44 .58 .83 .55 .51 .87 .02 .18 .01 1.34
Neutral .90 .67 0 .41 .57 .45 .02 .18 .01 1.62
.70 .68 .61 .46 .41 .74 .02 .18 .01 1.33

Delayed Test
T U T U T U T U T U T U T U T U T U

Positive .57 .86 .51 .01 .83 .67 .44 0 .36 .24 .79 .57 .02 .04 .26 .22 .51 .04 1.52 1.04
Negative 0 .03 .49 .25 .75 .84 .49 .19 .45 .35 .75 .65 .02 .04 .26 .22 .51 .04 1.97 .78
Neutral .59 .63 .39 .27 .82 0 .27 0 .19 .60 .46 .79 .02 .04 .26 .22 .51 .04 .88 2.01
.32 .37 .43 .23 .76 .67 .44 .12 .35 .42 .60 .61 .02 .04 .26 .22 .51 .04 1.32 1.07
Positive .39 .26 .90 .39 .12 .60 .03 .25 .20 .94
Negative 0 .30 .83 .26 .36 .66 .03 .25 .20 1.01
Neutral .46 .31 .46 .62 .53 .26 .03 .25 .20 1.48
.30 .30 .75 .32 .39 .57 .03 .25 .20 .65

Note. T = tested and U = untested.

Parameter analyses

Estimates of the model’s parameters are presented in Table 10, and Table 11 contains the results of parameter significance tests among valence and testing conditions as well as omnibus tests for between-condition differences in parameter values. (The standard statistical procedure with models of this sort is, first, to compute omnibus tests that establish that at least some of the parameters differ reliably among conditions and then to parameterwise tests that identify the specific parameters that differ among conditions; see Brainerd, Howe, & Desrochers, 1982.) Remember that each of the model’s parameters measures a specific verbatim or gist retrieval process.

Table 11.

G2 Values for Differences Between Parameters of the Conjoint Recognition Model Based on Valence, Test, Prior Testing, and Retention Interval

ET RT FT PD RD FD β V β G β VG Omnibus
Test G2
Value
Immediate
Positive vs.
Negative
1.62 3.10 2.95 .06 17.54* 1.91 -- -- -- 26.63**
Positive vs.
Neutral
1.86 1.66 1.17 2.15 13.49
*
6.15* -- -- -- 51.38**
Negative vs.
Neutral
8.80* 2.37 5.53* 1.37 1.39 7.02* -- -- -- 30.49**

Delayed
Positive vs.
Negative
47.86* .11 4.78* 3.25 3.97* 2.00 -- -- -- 65.46**
Positive vs.
Neutral
.74 .01 79.42* 35.53* 33.41* 15.18* -- -- -- 130.35**
Negative vs.
Neutral
41.54* .13 72.77* 18.91* 16.19* 21.32* -- -- -- 185.25**

Tested vs.
Untested
.44 23.55
*
3.36 14.85* 2.96 .13 .78 .48 61.33* 245.42**
Immediate
vs. Untested
13.72* 118.3
1*
.17 28.38* .01 9.73* .73 .01 1.36 232.33**
*

Exceeds critical value for significance at an alpha level of .05.

**

Exceeds critical value of 12.59 for χ2 test with 6 degrees of freedom.

Immediate test

Turning first to true memory, negative valence affected both verbatim and gist memory for targets. The familiarity parameter, which measures the retrieval of gist traces by target cues, was higher for negative pictures compared to neutral pictures. Another parameter that measures verbatim retrieval of other targets by target cues, erroneous recollection rejection, was reduced for negative pictures. Thus, the overall picture of the effects of emotional content on retrieval processes for target cues was that (a) negative valence decreased incorrect verbatim retrieval (erroneous recollection rejection), and (b) it also increased gist retrieval (familiarity).

For false memory, the overall effects of emotional content were to increase gist retrieval and decrease verbatim retrieval, which can be seen in values of the familiarity and recollection rejection parameters for related distractors. Recollection rejection, which suppresses false memory by retrieving verbatim traces of targets, was lower for positive pictures on the immediate test. Familiarity, which supports false memory by retrieving weaker gist traces, was higher for negative pictures compared to neutral pictures.

Delayed test

First, for true memory, emotional pictures again increased familiarity compared to neutral pictures and negative pictures reduced erroneous recollection rejection compared to neutral pictures, demonstrating that the immediate effects on verbatim and gist retrieval were still present one week later.

For false memory, the results paralleled those of the immediate test with the additional effect of emotional content reducing the stronger form of gist retrieval, as measured by the phantom recollection parameter. More specifically, positive and negative pictures reduced recollection rejection, increased familiarity, and reduced phantom recollection compared to neutral pictures. Emotional pictures reduced verbatim memory and the stronger form of gist memory while increasing the weaker form of gist memory.

Prior testing also affected both the true and false memory parameters. It enhanced verbatim memory for targets while enhancing gist memory for related distractors and reducing verbatim memory for targets when related distractors were retrieval cues, thereby producing net increases in both true and false memory. For true memory, the familiarity and identity parameters were larger for items on the delayed test that had been previously tested. For false memory, the phantom recollection parameter was larger while the recollection rejection parameter was smaller for previously tested items.

Finally, forgetting, the comparison between the immediate test and items on the delayed test that had not been previously tested, also affected true and false memory parameters. The identity, erroneous recollection, phantom recollection, and related distractor familiarity parameters were all higher on the immediate test compared to the delayed test, suggesting that the forgetting taking place over the week-long delay reduced both verbatim and gist retrieval but especially verbatim retrieval.

These data indicate that the true memory advantage for negative pictures was due to enhanced gist memory retrieval and reduced erroneous verbatim retrieval for negatively valenced targets. However, the increased levels of false memory for negative pictures were due to reduced verbatim retrieval coupled with increased gist retrieval by related distractors. In the latter connection, negative valence increased one form of gist retrieval (familiarity) and actually decreased another (phantom recollection), at least on the delayed test, but the first effect was far larger, producing net increases in false memory for negative pictures. With respect to verbatim memory, negative valence also increased false memory by reducing subject’s ability to use verbatim traces of targets to reject related distractors.

Comparisons with Brainerd et al. (2008)

It is informative to compare the present parameter results with those of a study in which the conjoint recognition model was applied to positive, negative, and neutral DRM lists (Brainerd et al., 2008). Brainerd et al. found higher levels of false memory for negative than positive or neutral lists because negative lists increased the familiarity parameter and decreased the recollection rejection parameter; that is, negative lists increased gist retrieval and decreased verbatim retrieval. Negative valence affected true memory by increasing the familiarity parameter and decreasing the identity parameter; that is, by increasing gist retrieval and decreasing verbatim retrieval for targets. In the present study, using a very different procedure, we found similar valence effects at the level of retrieval processes: Negative pictures increased gist retrieval for both targets and related distractors, and they decreased verbatim retrieval for related distractors. Unlike Brainerd et al., however, negative valence did not decrease the identity parameter. Thus, the effects in the present study were completely parallel to Brainerd et al.’s for DRM at the level of gist retrieval but not entirely parallel are the level of verbatim retrieval.

Discussion

The aim of this experiment was to investigate the connections between emotional valence, false memory, and true memory in a novel paradigm using a model-based approach that measures valence effects at the level of specific retrieval processes. We manipulated the valence of pictures while controlling their arousal levels in order to avoid the trend in prior studies of conflating valence and arousal. By applying the conjoint recognition model to the data, we were able to measure underlying verbatim and gist retrieval processes. At a general level, a positive outcome of our experiment is that the results were consistent with prior findings that were generated by quite different materials (DRM word lists) in the sense that negative emotional content increased false memory. Thus, this distortive effect of negative valence seems to have considerable generality because it holds across very different memory materials and procedures.

Our results are less consistent with DRM prior findings showing that negative emotion can reduce true memory (e.g., Howe, 2007) or that it enhances true memory only if it is also arousing (e.g., Kensinger & Corkin, 2003). In the present experiment, true memory was enhanced by negative pictures when arousal was controlled at moderate levels. This experiment thus provides evidence that emotional valence can enhance true as well as false memory and that high levels of arousal are not required for either effect to occur. Unlike negative emotional effects on false memory, which appear to be consistent across paradigms, the effect of negative valence on true memory appears to be sensitive to the target materials. Some prior research with pictures is consistent with the negative advantage that we detected for true memory (Choi, Kensinger, & Rajaram, 2013; Gallo et al., 2009), but without evidence showing that negative pictures simultaneously inflate false memory. The present study appears to be the first to show that negative, nonarousing pictures of realistic objects and events have both of these effects.

A primary goal of this research was to identify the specific retrieval processes that control valence-memory effects. Based on estimates of the conjoint recognition model’s parameters, it appears that valence affects the retrieval of both verbatim and gist traces, although the verbatim effect depends on the retrieval cue (target versus related distractor). The gist effect was simple: Emotional pictures increased the familiarity parameters, relative to neutral pictures, which increased both true and false memory. The verbatim effect was more complex: Emotional pictures suppressed the recollection rejection parameter for related distractors, thereby increasing false memory, but did not affect the identity parameter, which contributes to increasing true memory; instead they suppressed the erroneous recollection parameter, thereby increasing true memory. Overall, emotionally valenced materials increased true and false memory because they (a) drove gist retrieval up for targets and related distractors and (b) drove verbatim retrieval down for related distractors. As previously noted, this pattern of parameter differences is somewhat unlike that of previous emotional word experiments (Brainerd et al., 2008), which found that negative valence increased gist retrieval for both targets and related distractors and suppressed verbatim retrieval for both. It therefore appears that although the gist effects of valence are robust in the face of procedural variations, its verbatim effects may be sensitive to changes in target materials.

Two other manipulations, presentation order and the number of category exemplars, provided further evidence for effects that change as a function of the nature of the target materials. Concerning presentation order, a good deal of prior DRM research has shown that blocking items that share meaning increases false memory, relative to random presentation, by selectively strengthening gist memory (e.g., Dewhurst et al., 2009; McDermott, 1996; Toglia Neuschatz, & Goodwin, 1999). With realistic pictures, however, neither false nor true memory were affected by this manipulation. This may be due to an important difference in the level of relatedness of our pictorial materials versus DRM word lists. The words on DRM lists are semantically but not physically related (e.g., “pillow” and “dream” are both share “sleep” meaning but they neither look nor sound alike). In contrast, our emotional pictures were similar in both meaning and visual appearance (e.g., all pictures of trees looked more similar to each other than to other pictures). In other words, the fact that our target materials were related was more obvious than it is with word lists, making the materials less sensitive to manipulations that encourage subjects to process relations among items.

Turning to the number of exemplars, remember that this manipulation varied the number of exemplars of each category that were presented, which has been found to affect the strength of gist for categorical relations in prior research. We found that presenting more exemplars of a category increased false memory for related distractors but did not affect true memory for targets. The modeling analyses revealed that the familiarity parameter was larger for categories for which larger numbers of exemplars were presented, implicating the gist retrieval in the false memory enhancement for 8-exemplar categories. These findings are inconsistent with prior work on DRM false memory (e.g., Robinson & Roediger, 1997; Swannell & Dewhurst, 2013), which showed that increasing the length of word lists increases false memory and reduces true memory. However, the present data are consistent with research on categorized word lists, which showed that increasing list length increases false memory without affecting true memory (Dennis & Chapman, 2010). This difference suggests that although associatively-related items and categorized items have similar gist effects (increasing false memory), their verbatim effects are different.

Looking at those results in relation to valence, increasing the number of exemplars increased gist memory for negative items, but reduced gist memory and increased verbatim memory for positive items. This pattern highlights the different roles that gist and verbatim memory play for different types of valence, which is apparently more gist-based for negative valence and more verbatim-based for positive valence, a general pattern than has also been noted by others in word list experiments (Brainerd et al., 2008; Gomes, Brainerd, & Stein, 2013). This pattern is also consistent with previous research demonstrating that whereas negative information promotes false memory, positive information may actually protect against it (e.g., Porter, Spencer, & Birt, 2003; Porter, ten Brinke, Riley, & Baker, 2014).

Finally, we were interested in the extent to which these effects persist over time and whether the retrieval processes that control them change over time, as well as how prior testing modulates those effects. The effect of valence on both true and false memory declined slightly over time but remained reliable. It is well established that verbatim memory declines more rapidly than gist memory and that it contributes more to true than to false memory (Brainerd & Reyna, 2005). Thus, when there are comparable declines in a manipulation’s effects on true and false memory, the implication is that it is chiefly a gist effect. This was the pattern in our experiment, and that agrees with the modeling finding that the most consistent valence effect was on the familiarity parameters.

Prior testing increased verbatim and gist retrieval for targets, as well as increased gist memory for related distractors. That is consistent with previous demonstrations that prior testing elevates false memory on delayed tests (Brainerd et al., 2006). Taken together, the decline in verbatim retrieval over time and the increase in gist retrieval for related distractors after prior testing provides a simple explanation for the known tendency of repeated questioning to falsely alter the contents of memory (e.g., Ceci & Bruck, 1995).

At the broadest level, our results provide further evidence challenging the notion that arousal is the key component of emotional content that produces emotional enhancement of memory. It appears that valence is able to affect both true and false memory when arousal is controlled at moderate levels. Another finding of broad significance is the apparent generality of negative valence’s ability to distort episodic memory. We now know that this pattern holds for pictures of realistic objects and events as well as for word lists, and that in both spheres, the effect is tied to the tendency of negative valence to strengthen memory for the gist of experience. Potentially, this pattern has wide applicability in high-stakes situations in the real world where negative emotion figures prominently. Eyewitness memory in legal cases is a classic example. The determinative evidence in criminal proceedings comes overwhelmingly from the memory reports of witnesses, even in capital trials (Brainerd, 2013), and those reports revolve around events that are fraught with negative emotion. When our findings are added to earlier work, it seems that such circumstances increase the risk of memory reports of events that did not happen—as when a witness incorrectly reports that a robbery suspect was carrying a knife or made threatening statements. In contrast to most memory experiments, witnesses’ reports of events usually occur after substantial delays. Hence, it is particularly important that the distortive effects of negative valence remained stable over the delay, and indeed, those effects have sometimes been found to increase over a delay (e.g., Howe et al., 2010).

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 1RC1AG036915 to the second author.

Appendix

Table A1.

Expressions for Acceptance of Related and Unrelated Distractor Test questions in the Model in the Three Conjoint Recognition Conditions

Acceptance probability by
condition
Expression

pDV (1 - RD)PD + (1 – RD)(1 - PD)FD + (1 – RD)(1 - PD)(1 - FD) βV
pDG RD + (1 – RD)(1 - PD)FD + (1 – RD)(1 - PD)(1 - FD) βG
pDVG RD + (1 – RD)PD + (1 – RD)(1 – PD)FD + (1 – RD)(1 - PD)(1 - FD) βVG
pTV RT + (1 - RT)ET + (1 - RT)(1 - ET)FT + (1 - RT)(1 - ET)(1 - FT) βV
pTG (1 - RT)ET + (1 - RT)(1 – ET)FT + (1 - RT)(1 – ET)(1 - FT) βG
pTVG RT + (1 - RT)ET + (1 - RT)(1 - ET)FT + (1 - RT)(1 - ET)(1 - FT) βVG
pUDV βV
pUDG βG
pUDVG βVG

Note. V = accept only targets, G = accept only related distractors, VG = accept both targets and related distractors, D = related distractor, T = target, and UD = unrelated distractor.

Footnotes

1

There are two versions of this first feature: In the multiple choice version (Stahl & Klauer, 2008), each test item is accompanied by a multiple-choice question that contains all three options, one of which subjects must pick; in the yes/no version, each test item is paired with one of the three test questions and subjects must pick yes or no (Brainerd et al., 2003). We implemented the second version because it provides more independent response probabilities and thus delivers identifiable parameters for all of the retrieval processes in Table 1, which the multiple-choice procedure does not do.

References

  1. Block SD, Greenberg SN, Goodman GS. Remembrance of eyewitness testimony: Effects of emotional content, self-relevance, and emotional tone. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2009;39:2859–2878. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 1994;25:49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Brainerd CJ. Murder must memorise. Memory. 2013;21:547–555. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.791322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brainerd CJ, Gomes CFA, Moran R. The two recollections. Psychological Review. 2014;121:563–599. doi: 10.1037/a0037668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brainerd CJ, Holliday RE, Reyna VF, Yang Y, Toglia MP. Developmental reversals in false memory: Effects of emotional valence and arousal. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2010;107:137–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brainerd CJ, Howe ML, Desrochers A. The general theory of two-stage learning: A mathematical review with illustrations from memory development. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;91:634–665. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brainerd CJ, Reyna V. Fuzzy-trace theory and children's false memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1998;71:81–129. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF. The science of false memory. Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  9. Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF, Estrada S. Recollection rejection of false narrative statements. Memory. 2006;14:672–691. doi: 10.1080/09658210600648449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF, Mojardin AH. Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review. 1999;106:160–179. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.1.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF, Wright R, Mojardin AH. Recollection rejection: false-memory editing in children and adults. Psychological Review. 2003;110:762–784. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brainerd CJ, Stein LM, Silveira RA, Rohenkohl G, Reyna VF. How does negative emotion cause false memories? Psychological Science. 2008;19:919–925. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02177.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brueckner K, Moritz S. Emotional valence and semantic relatedness differentially influence false recognition in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease, and healthy elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2009;15:268–276. doi: 10.1017/S135561770909047X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Budson AE, Todman RW, Chong H, Adams EH, Kensinger EA, Krangel TS, Wright CI. False recognition of emotional word lists in aging and Alzheimer disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2006;19:71–78. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000213905.49525.d0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ceci SJ, Bruck M. Jeopardy in the courtroom. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  16. Charles S, Mather M, Carstensen L. Aging and emotional memory: The forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General. 2003;132:310–324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Choi HY, Kensinger EA, Rajaram S. Emotional content enhances true but not false memory for categorized stimuli. Memory & Cognition. 2013;41:403–415. doi: 10.3758/s13421-012-0269-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dan-Glauser ES, Scherer KR. The Geneva affective picture database (GAPED): A new 730-picture database focusing on valence and normative significance. Behavioral Research Methods. 2011;43:468–477. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0064-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Deese J. On the prediction of occurrence of certain verbal intrusions in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1959;58:17–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dehon H, Larøi F, Van der Linden M. Affective valence influences participant's susceptibility to false memories and illusory recollection. Emotion. 2010;10:627–639. doi: 10.1037/a0019595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dennis S, Chapman A. The inverse list length effect: A challenge for pure exemplar models of recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language. 2010;63:416–424. [Google Scholar]
  22. Dewhurst SA, Pursglove RC, Lewis C. Story contexts increase susceptibility to the DRM illusion in 5-year-olds. Developmental Science. 2007;10:374–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00592.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dewhurst SA, Bould E, Knott LM, Thorley C. The roles of encoding and retrieval processes in associative and categorical memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language. 2009;60:154–164. [Google Scholar]
  24. El Sharkawy JE, Groth K, Vetter C, Beraldi A, Fast K. False memories of emotional and neutral words. Behavioural Neurology. 2008;19:7–11. doi: 10.1155/2008/587239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gaigg SB, Bowler DM. Illusory memories of emotionally charged words in autism spectrum disorder: further evidence for atypical emotion processing outside the social domain. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39:1031–1038. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0710-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gallo DA, Foster KT, Johnson EL. Elevated false recollection of emotional pictures in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging. 2009;24:981–988. doi: 10.1037/a0017545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gomes CF, Brainerd CJ, Stein LM. Effects of emotional valence and arousal on recollective and nonrecollective recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2013;39:663–677. doi: 10.1037/a0028578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Howe ML. Children (but not adults) can inhibit false memories. Psychological Science. 2005;16:927–931. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Howe ML. Children's emotional false memories. Psychological Science. 2007;18:856–860. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01991.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Howe ML, Candel I, Otgaar H, Malone C, Wimmer MC. Valence and the development of immediate and long-term false memory illusions. Memory. 2010;18:58–75. doi: 10.1080/09658210903476514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Howe ML, Wimmer MC, Gagnon N, Plumpton S. An associative-activation theory of children’s and adults’ memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language. 2009;60:229–251. [Google Scholar]
  32. Kensinger EA, Corkin S. Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory & Cognition. 2003;31:1169–1180. doi: 10.3758/bf03195800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kensinger EA, Corkin S. Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101:3310–3315. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0306408101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. Remembering the specific visual details of presented objects: Neuroimaging evidence for effects of emotion. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2951–2962. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. Neural processes supporting young and older adults' emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:1161–1173. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Koutstaal W, Schacter D. Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. Journal of Memory and Language. 1997;37:555–583. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lampinen JM, Copeland SM, Neuschatz JS. Recollections of things schematic: Room schemas revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27:1211–1222. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.27.5.1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical report A-8) National Institute of Mental Health Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention; Bethesda, MD: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  39. Maras KL, Gaigg SB, Bowler DM. Memory for emotionally arousing events over time in autism spectrum disorder. Emotion. 2012;12:1118–1128. doi: 10.1037/a0026679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McDermott KB. The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996;35:212–230. [Google Scholar]
  41. Mickley KR, Kensinger EA. Phenomenological characteristics of emotional memories in younger and older adults. Memory. 2009;17:528–543. doi: 10.1080/09658210902939363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Payne DG, Elie CJ, Blackwell JM, Neuschatz JS. Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996;35:261–285. [Google Scholar]
  43. Pesta BJ, Murphy MD, Sanders RE. Are emotionally charged lures immune to false memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27:328–338. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.2.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Porter S, Spencer L, Birt AR. Blinded by emotion? Effect of the emotionality of a scene on susceptibility to false memories. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 2003;35:165–175. [Google Scholar]
  45. Porter S, Ten Brinke L, Riley SN, Baker A. Prime time news: The influence of primed positive and negative emotion on susceptibility to false memories. Cognition and Emotion. 2014;28:1422–1434. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.887000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Powell MB, Roberts KP, Ceci SJ, Hembrooke H. The effects of repeated exposure on children’s suggestibility. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:1462–1477. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.6.1462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Robinson KJ, Roediger HL. Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science. 1997;8:231–237. [Google Scholar]
  48. Roediger H, McDermott K. Creating false memories - remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition. 1995;21:803–814. [Google Scholar]
  49. Roediger HL, III, Watson JM, McDermott KB, Gallo DA. Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2001;8:385–405. doi: 10.3758/bf03196177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Seamon JG, Luo CR, Kopecky JJ, Price CA, Rothschild L, Fung NS, Schwartz MA. Are false memories more difficult to forget than accurate memories? The effect of retention interval on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition. 2002;30:1054–1064. doi: 10.3758/bf03194323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sio UN, Ormerod TC. Incubation and cueing effects in problem-solving: Set aside the difficult problems but focus on the easy ones. Thinking & Reasoning. 2015;21:113–129. [Google Scholar]
  52. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1988;117:34–50. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.117.1.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Stahl C, Klauer KC. A simplified conjoint recognition paradigm for the measurement of verbatim and gist memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2008;34:570–586. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Swannell ER, Dewhurst SA. Effect of presentation format and list length on children’s false memories. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2013;14:332–342. [Google Scholar]
  55. Talmi D, Moscovitch M. Can semantic relatedness explain the enhancement of memory for emotional words? Memory & Cognition. 2004;32:742–751. doi: 10.3758/bf03195864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Toglia MP, Neuschatz JS, Goodwin KA. Recall accuracy and illusory memories: When more is less. Memory. 1999;7:233–256. doi: 10.1080/741944069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Tse CS, Neely JH. Assessing activation without source monitoring in the DRM false memory paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language. 2005;53:532–550. [Google Scholar]
  58. Tse CS, Neely JH. Semantic and repetition priming effects for Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) critical items and associates produced by DRM and unrelated study lists. Memory & Cognition. 2007;35:1047–1066. doi: 10.3758/bf03193477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Van Damme I, Smets K. The power of emotion versus the power of suggestion: Memory for emotional events in the misinformation paradigm. Emotion. 2014;14:310–320. doi: 10.1037/a0034629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES