Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2017 Apr 5;12(4):e0173776. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0173776

A neural mass model of cross frequency coupling

Mojtaba Chehelcheraghi 1, Cees van Leeuwen 1,2, Erik Steur 1,¤, Chie Nakatani 1,*
Editor: Gennady Cymbalyuk3
PMCID: PMC5381784  PMID: 28380064

Abstract

Electrophysiological signals of cortical activity show a range of possible frequency and amplitude modulations, both within and across regions, collectively known as cross-frequency coupling. To investigate whether these modulations could be considered as manifestations of the same underlying mechanism, we developed a neural mass model. The model provides five out of the theoretically proposed six different coupling types. Within model components, slow and fast activity engage in phase-frequency coupling in conditions of low ambient noise level and with high noise level engage in phase-amplitude coupling. Between model components, these couplings can be coordinated via slow activity, giving rise to more complex modulations. The model, thus, provides a coherent account of cross-frequency coupling, both within and between components, with which regional and cross-regional frequency and amplitude modulations could be addressed.

Introduction

Electrophysiological activity of interconnected neurons encompasses multiple oscillatory components [1,2]; these are subject to modulation of frequency [35] and amplitude [6,7]. Modulation may have various utilities [8,9], such as sequence encoding [10,11], rectification of local neural activity [12,13] and long-rage information transfer [14,15]. The mechanisms behind these effects, however, are still not well-understood. In theory, they could all be manifestations of a single underlying principle. To contemplate this possibility, we propose a model that describes frequency and amplitude modulations as systematic relationships between slow and fast oscillatory components of neural population activity.

Systematic relationships between oscillatory components are collectively known as cross-frequency coupling (CFC). Jensen & Colgin [2] listed six types of CFC of interest to electrophysiology: phase-phase, phase-frequency, phase-amplitude, frequency-frequency, amplitude-amplitude and amplitude-frequency couplings (PPC, PFC, PAC, FFC, AAC, and AFC, respectively). Empirical studies have mostly observed PPC [1620], PAC [2123] and occasionally AAC [2426]. To our knowledge, PFC, AFC and FFC have not been empirically observed so far. Shortcomings of current CFC detection methods may, in part, be responsible for this. In particular, PFC may have been misidentified as PAC [9,27]. In anticipation of further progress in measurement, it is desirable to have a model predicting the conditions under which these modulations could arise, as part of an encompassing account of CFC.

We propose a neural mass model (NMM) for CFC. The neural mass approach collectively describes neuronal activity by representing it at the level of neural populations. NMMs generate oscillatory activities through the interaction of neuronal populations within and across regions. NMMs have been successfully used to describe various mesoscopic brain activities [28] such as ongoing alpha, beta, gamma, delta, and theta band activity [2931], the sleep spindle/k-complex [32,33], as well as evoked activity [34,35].

Recently we introduced a NMM that manifests PAC [36]. The model consists of four neural populations: pyramidal neurons, excitatory interneurons, slow and fast inhibitory interneurons. A noise source, which represents un-modeled activity of neighboring brain regions, serves as external input. The noise drives both the pyramidal neuron and the fast inhibitory interneuron populations. Like its predecessor NMMs, the four-populations model generates slow activity comparable to alpha, theta, and delta band oscillations through interactions between pyramidal neuron and slow-inhibitory interneuron populations. Unique to our NMM, the fast inhibitory interneuron population has a dynamic self-feedback mechanism, which plays a crucial role in generating fast oscillatory activity.

Within this mechanism, the mean level of noise input acts as a bifurcation parameter for the fast inhibitory interneuron population. For zero noise level, no fast oscillations arise. As the noise level is increased, fast activity emerges in the model’s output, due to the self-feedback of the fast-inhibitory interneuron population. The fast activity is comparable to interneuron gamma (ING), which has been observed in neocortex and allocortex, e.g., in visual cortices and hippocampus [3739]. In Chehelcheraghi et al. [36], we demonstrated that the mean level of input noise is a critical factor for the modulation of fast activity (see also [40]). From a certain nonzero noise level, fast oscillations occur and above a critical level, the model produces a PAC signal: modulation of fast activity amplitude by the phase of the slow rhythm. Here we will first show that noise below that level leads to PFC: modulation of fast activity frequency by the phase of the slow rhythm. Next, we will demonstrate that besides PAC and PFC, other types of CFCs, namely FFC, AAC and AFC can all be modeled based on the four-populations model, depending solely on the choice of mean input noise level.

The four-populations model is considered to represent a cortical patch between a few millimeters and a centimeter in diameter, in which oscillations in various frequencies can be observed [41]. Thus, we considered the four-populations model to represent a local region. Empirical studies reports CFC between local regions. For instance, AAC was observed across brain regions such as superior temporal and fusiform gyri [42]. We therefore connected two identical four-populations models symmetrically via populations of pyramidal neurons. These between-region populations relay slow oscillatory activity from one region to the other, and allow the slow oscillations within both components to become synchronized. Synchronized slow activity thus serves as a common modulator for the fast activities within both regions. The between-region pyramidal populations could, in principle, be replaced by or extended with an arbitrary oscillator (e.g., a thalamic pacemaker [43]). While allowing such possible extensions, the current study used pyramidal units which we consider as the generic between-region connectivity.

Although both PFC and PAC can arise within a single component, here we will operate from the onset with a two components of the four-populations model, to show the role of synchronization of the slow activity between the components in modeling cross-regional PAC and PFC. In the first simulation, the two components of the four-populations model were used to show PFC across the regions. Similarly, cross-regional PAC was shown in the second simulation. In both simulations, the second component has zero mean noise level, thereby preventing any fast oscillations to emerge within the second component. Thus, cross regional PAC and PFC arise via synchronization with the slow oscillations that connect the two components. In the third, fourth, and fifth simulations, FFC, AAC and AFC were generated, respectively, each as a function of the levels of nonzero mean noise input to both components. Based on the results, we propose the present model as an inclusive theoretical framework for the different CFC phenomena.

Methods

Overview

As illustrated in Fig 1, the present model consists of two identical components labelled as Node 1 and Node 2. Each component is a four-populations NMM node which simulates electrophysiological activity [36]. The four populations represent, respectively, pyramidal neuron, excitatory interneuron, slow inhibitory interneuron, and fast inhibitory interneuron of the cortex. In each node, interaction between the pyramidal neurons and slow inhibitory interneurons generates a steady-state activity, which is considered as its main (intrinsic) oscillation (e.g. alpha band activity in visual cortex in [33]. Unique to our four-populations model, the fast-inhibitory interneuron population has dynamic self-feedback. The feedback enables the fast inhibitory interneurons to generate oscillations in the gamma range. This fast activity can occur simultaneously with the slow activity, which is a prerequisite for CFC in the model. The two four-populations nodes are mutually connected via two between-node pyramidal neuron populations.

Fig 1. Schematic diagram of the proposed model.

Fig 1

Each node is comprised of four neural population units: pyramidal neurons, excitatory interneurons, slow inhibtory interneurons and fast ihbitory internneurons. Each node is identical to the NMM in [36]. The nodes are mutually connected by the dynamics of the pyramidal neurons. Two independent Gaussian noise sources drive the NMM nodes.

The emergence and stability of fast activity in each node depends on two parameters, P1 and P2, which represent the mean levels of noise input to the nodes [36]. Small values deactivate the fast oscillations (damping). Values above the damping regime determine the type of modulation between the slow and fast oscillation. For parameter values below a critical threshold, resonance occurs, allowing frequency modulation, namely, PFC, AFC, and FFC; above the threshold the fast oscillations reach a steady-state, in which the frequency is invariant, and amplitude modulation results, allowing for PAC and AAC. For an analytical account of this mechanism, see [44]). We systematically vary the values of P1 and P2 in order to demonstrate that PFC, PAC, FFC, AAC and AFC appear as a function of the parameters P1 and P2, without changing model structure or connectivity.

Model

Each of the four neural population units of a node, pyramidal neurons, excitatory interneurons, slow inhibitory interneurons, and fast inhibitory interneurons, has two variables: mean firing rate fr(t) which is a function of mean membrane potential vm(t), and mean post synaptic potential v(t). The mean membrane potential is obtained from the weighted summation of inputs. The mean membrane potential is converted to the mean firing rate via a sigmoid function:

fr(t)=Sig(vm(t))=υmax1+exp(r(vm(t)vθ))

Here vmax is the maximum firing rate of the population of neurons, vθ is the value of the potential for which a 50% mean firing rate is achieved, and r is the slope of the sigmoid at vθ; The parameter vθ will serve as mean firing threshold. The sigmoid function is parametrized equally for all populations in the model. A second-order differential equation called the dendritic transfer function relates the mean firing rate, fr(t), of a population to the un-weighted mean post synaptic potential, v(t), of that population via the dendritic transfer function:

H(S)=GωS2+2ωS+ω2,

where G and ω characterize the mean strength and speed, respectively. Different population units are distinguished by their time constant T=1ω [sec] and the low frequency gain of the dendritic transfer function Gω [mV.sec]. The dendritic transfer function of each population unit is denoted as Hlx(S), in which l ∈ {p: pyramidal neuron population, q: excitatory interneuron population, f: fast inhibitory interneuron population, s: slow inhibitory interneuron population} and x ∈ {1,2} is the node number, e.g. Hs2(S) denotes the dendritic transfer function of the slow inhibitory interneuron population in the second node.

Four interconnected population units constitute a single node. Inputs to each population unit are the weighted outputs from the other units, reflecting the mean excitatory or inhibitory post synaptic potentials of an excitatory or inhibitory population (EPSP or IPSP). The input synaptic potentials are weighted by constant connectivity strengths which are called synaptic gains. A synaptic gain between populations is represented by Cαβ where α, β ∈ {p: pyramidal neuron population, q: excitatory interneuron population, f: fast inhibitory interneuron population, s: slow inhibitory interneuron population}. Here α and β refer to target and source population, respectively; e.g. the constant Cps is the gain of the synaptic connection from the slow inhibitory population to the pyramidal neuron population.

The pyramidal neuron population unit excites all three interneuron population units within the node and in turn receives input from them. The slow inhibitory interneuron population unit inhibits the fast inhibitory interneuron population unit. The fast inhibitory interneuron population has an extra state variable, vff(t), for the dynamic self-inhibition. The dynamics of the self-feedback in the fast inhibitory population is of first order and serves as a low pass filter with cut-off frequency fc=1(2πτf).

As shown in Fig 1, Nodes 1 and 2 have identical structure. The two nodes are mutually connected via the pyramidal populations. The synaptic gain of Node 1 to Node 2 is represented by K21, while that in the inverse direction is given as K12. The dendritic transfer functions of pyramidal neurons between the nodes are labeled as Hp21(S) and Hp12(S). For each node, independent Gaussian noise excites both the fast inhibitory interneuron and pyramidal neuron units. The noise represents the mean firing rate of unmodeled external neural populations. The noise is passed through the dendritic transfer function of pyramidal neurons between the nodes, i.e., Hp21(S) and Hp12(S), in order to convert it to mean post synaptic potentials. The potentials are weighted by factors Kp and Kf in order to excite the pyramidal neurons and fast inhibitory interneurons, respectively. For each node, the mean membrane potential of the pyramidal population is the main output. The pyramidal population unit has been the main output (and input) unit in previous cortical neural mass models [29,35]. The mean membrane potential of the neurons represents population activity of the node; thus, this output is comparable with macroscopic electrophysiological brain signals, such as, LFP, ECoG, and EEG. The dynamics of the full model is described as follows.

Equations for Node 1

Pyramidal neuron population:

d2vp1dt2=Gp1ωp1Sig(vmp1(t))2ωp1dvp1dtωp12vp1(t)
vmp1(t)=Cpqvq1(t)Cpsvs1(t)Cpfvf1(t)+K12vp12(t)+Kp1vn1(t)

Excitatory interneuron population:

d2vq1dt2=Gq1ωq1Sig(vmq1(t))2ωq1dvq1dtωq12vq1(t)
vmq1(t)=Cqpvp1(t)

Slow inhibitory interneuron population:

d2vs1dt2=Gs1ωs1Sig(vms1(t))2ωs1dvs1dtωs12vs1(t)
vms1(t)=Cspvp1(t)

Fast inhibitory interneuron population:

d2vf1dt2=Gf1ωf1Sig(vmf1(t))2ωf1dvf1dtωf12vf1(t)
τf1dvff1(t)dt=vff1(t)+vf1(t)
vmf1(t)=Cfpvp1(t)Cfs(t)vs1(t)Cffvff1(t)+Kf1vn1(t)

External inputs:

d2vp12dt2=Gp12ωp12Sig(vmp2(t))2ωp12dvp12dtωp122vp12(t)(From Node2)
d2vn1dt2=Gp12ωp12n1(t)2ωp12dvn1dtωp122vn1(t)(Filtered noise)
n1(t)=N(P1,σ12)(White noise)

Equations for Node 2

Pyramidal neuron population:

d2vp2dt2=Gp2ωp2Sig(vmp2(t))2ωp2dvp2dtωp22vp2(t)
vmp2(t)=Cpqvq2(t)Cpsvs2(t)Cpfvf2(t)+K21vp21(t)+Kp2vn2(t)

Excitatory interneuron population:

d2vq2dt2= Gq2ωq2Sig(vmq2(t)) 2ωq2dvq2dt ωq22vq2(t)
vmq2(t)=Cqpvp2(t)

Slow inhibitory interneuron population:

d2vs2dt2=Gs2ωs2Sig(vms2(t))2ωs2dvs2dtωs22vs2(t)
vms2(t)=Cspvp2(t)

Fast inhibitory interneuron population:

d2vf2dt2=Gf2ωf2Sig(vmf2(t))2ωf2dvf2dtωf22vf2(t)
τf2dvff2(t)dt=-vff2(t)+vf2(t)
vmf2(t)=Cfpvp2(t)Cfs(t)vs2(t)Cffvff2(t)+Kf2vn2(t)

External inputs:

d2vp21dt2=Gp21ωp21Sig(vmp1(t))2ωp21dvp21dtωp212vp21(t)(From Node 1)
d2vn2dt2=Gp21ωp21n2(t)2ωp21dvn2dtωp212vn2(t)(Filtered noise)
n2(t)=N(P2,σ22)(White noise)

Parameters

Table 1 shows the parameter values that are common to all current CFC simulations. The two nodes are identically parametrized, except for the time constant of the dynamic self-feedbacks in the fast inhibitory interneurons, τf1 and τf2. As we will explain further in the subsection Choice of Slow and Fast Oscillation Frequencies, these parameters were fixed at different values for the sake of obtaining FFC, AAC and AFC. The remaining parameter values are those suggested by [33] and [30], same as in our previous study [36]. Jansen and Rit [33] assumed that synaptic connections are active in equal proportions between interacting units, and proposed to the fix ratios of synaptic gains. The set of parameters was modified by Wendling et al., [30] to model fast inhibitory interneurons. The modified set of parameters has been the basis set for fast oscillations in many neural mass modeling studies, including the current one.

Table 1. Model parameters: Fixed values.
Parameter Interpretation Notation Value
Synaptic gain from excitatory interneurons to pyramidal neurons Cqp1, Cqp2 135
Synaptic gain from pyramidal neurons to excitatory interneurons Cpq1, Cpq2 108
Synaptic gain from slow inhibitory interneurons to pyramidal neurons Csp1, Csp2 33.75
Synaptic gain from pyramidal neurons to slow inhibitory interneurons Cps1, Cps2 33.75
Synaptic gain from fast inhibitory interneurons to pyramidal neurons Cfp1, Cfp2 40.5
Synaptic gain from pyramidal neurons to fast inhibitory interneurons Cpf1, Cpf2 27
Synaptic gain from fast inhibitory interneurons to slow inhibitory interneurons Cfs1, Cfs2 10.8
Synaptic gain of fast inhibitory interneurons self-feedback Cff1, Cff2 135
Noise excitation weight for pyramidal neurons Kp1, Kp2 40
Noise excitation weight for fast inhibitory interneurons Kf1, Kf2 108
Average time constant of pyramidal neurons membrane potential, the inverse divided by 2π is equivalent to low cut-off frequency [sec] ωp1-1, ωp2-1 10−1
Average time constant of between node pyramidal neurons membrane potential, the inverse divided by 2π is equivalent to low cut-off frequency [sec] ωp21-1, ωp12-1 100−1
Average time constant of excitatory interneurons membrane potential, the inverse divided by 2π is equivalent to low cut-off frequency [sec] ωq1-1, ωq2-1 100−1
Average time constant of slow inhibitory interneurons membrane potential, the inverse divided by 2π is equivalent to low cut-off frequency [sec] ωs1-1, ωs2-1 50−1
Average time constant of fast inhibitory interneurons membrane potential, the inverse divided by 2π is equivalent to low cut-off frequency [sec] ωf1-1, ωf2-1 200−1
Average dendritic gains of pyramidal neurons [mV] Gp1, Gp2 0.32
Average dendritic gains of between node pyramidal neurons [mV] Gp21, Gp12 3.2
Average dendritic gains of excitatory interneurons [mV] Gq1, Gq2 3.2
Average dendritic gains of slow inhibitory interneurons [mV] Gs1, Gs2 22
Average dendritic gains of fast inhibitory interneurons [mV] Gf1, Gf2 50
Expected spiking threshold voltage [mV] vθ 5
Half-maximum firing rate [Hz] υ0 2.5
Variance of membrane potential over individual neurons in the population [mV-1] R 1.12
Variance of white noise [Hz2] σ21, σ22 0.5
Between node synaptic gain K21, K12 40
Average time constant of self-synaptic decay in fast inhibitory interneurons [sec] τf1 0.01
τf2 0.005

Noise sources and fast oscillations

Table 2 shows how the values of the main parameters P1 and P2 were systematically varied in order to enable different types of CFCs. Two independent noise sources, N(P1,σ12) and N(P2,σ22), excite both the pyramidal neurons and the fast inhibitory interneurons of Nodes 1 and 2, respectively. The noise sources represent the unmodeled mean firing rates, which originate from various cortical and subcortical regions. As long as the mean noise level P is too small, fast oscillations fail to arise due to damping. Higher levels of noise trigger fast oscillations, for which P serves as a bifurcation parameter. We will refer to the bifurcation value as limit-cycle threshold. When a P parameter value is below the limit-cycle threshold, the fast inhibitory population with feedback serves as a resonance filter. Such a filter is selective to a specific frequency in the input and the output contains the resonance frequency component. In this regime, the frequency of the fast oscillation is sensitive to the slow oscillations in the modulating input. Hence, the resonance is suitable as a frequency modulation mechanism. For PFC and FFC simulations, the parameter was set in the resonance range. When P value is above the threshold, a limit cycle emerges corresponding to the fast oscillation. In this type of oscillation, the amplitude of the fast oscillation is sensitive to modulatory input. Therefore, the limit cycle oscillation is a suitable mechanism for amplitude modulation. For PAC and AAC simulations, thus, the P parameters are set to obtain a limit-cycle osillation. For AFC, P values were set to obtain a limit cycle in one node and resonance oscillations in the other.

Table 2. P parameters for each CFC simulation.
Interpretation Notation cross-node PFC cross-node PAC FFC AAC AFC
Mean Input Noise Level P1 4.5 7 4.5 7 7
P2 0 0 4.5 7 4.5

Note: the value 0 deactivates fast oscillation; 4.5 induces a resonance regime and 7 induces a stable limit cycle corresponding to fast oscillations.

Slow oscillations, generation and synchronization

As in previous NMM studies [29,45], the model generates slow oscillations through the interactions between the pyramidal and slow inhibitory interneuron populations within each node. When nodes are connected, the slow oscillations synchronize, desynchronize, or show intermittent synchronization behavior, depending on the coupling strengths K12 and K21 between them [46]. Synchronization will not arise for small values of K12 and K21. We used K12 = K21 = 40 throughout this study, which assures that the slow oscillations are synchronized.

Choice of slow and fast oscillation frequencies

For frequency modulation, intuitively, the condition ffast >> fslow should be met where ffast is the frequency of the fast oscillation and fslow is that of the slow one. We set the slow oscillation in the delta band and the fast oscillation in the gamma band, the slowest and the fastest commonly observed in EEG. To generate slow oscillation in the delta band (~3Hz), the time constants of the pyramidal neurons in both nodes were set to ωp1=ωp2=10rads. This value differs by a factor 10 from that used in previous studies to obtain alpha oscillations [29,30,36]. In order to keep the stationary conditions of the model unaffected, the low frequency gain (Gω) was kept in the same proportion, meaning G was changed from 3.2 (for alpha) to 0.32 (for delta). The frequency of the fast oscillations is tuned through the time constant of the dynamic feedback, τf1 and τf2. These were set to 0.5 ms and 1ms, respectively, resulting in 51Hz and 42Hz gamma activity in the first and the second node, respectively. These frequencies were chosen to be different in order to show that for FFC, AAC, and AFC in general the fast oscillations do not need to have the same frequencies. For amplitude modulation, the condition should be met that ffastfslow>2 [27]. The fast and slow frequencies chosen above meet these conditions.

Evaluation of Cross-Frequency Coupling (CFC)

To assess the CFC generated by our model, the output of each node was decomposed into fast-oscillation (FO) and slow-oscillation (SO) signals, respectively, by high-pass or low-pass filtering at a 15Hz cut-off frequency (fc). For filtering we used the eeglab13.4.4b Matlab toolbox [47]. FO and SO signals were always normalized to zero mean and unit variance. To evaluate frequency modulation, the average number of zero crossings of the FO signal was calculated for each positive and negative phase of the SO signal in the same node. The average number of zero crossings was obtained by calculating the zero-crossing rate according to the following equation:

zcr=12Tt=0T×fsI{FO(t)×FO(t+1)<0},

in which T is the time interval of SO in negative (from sine(-π) to sine(0)) or positive (from sine(0) to sine(π)) phases and fs is the sampling frequency. The indicator function I{A} is 1 if its argument is true and 0 otherwise. The average over T does not reflect small fluctuations, which do not cross zero. As a result, the number zcr tends to be lower than the actual frequency, i.e., the average numer of zero crossing is not identical to the average frequency, but may serve as an index is of it. The power spectral density (PSD) of the output signal was calculated using the [48] method. A Hanning time window of length equal to 16348 data point was moved stepwise with 25% overlap. Amplitude modulation was evaluated by visual inspection, comparing the envelope of the FO signals with the SO signal.

Simulation specifications

The model was implemented and ran in Matlab/Simulink 8.0 (R2012b), using a 4th order Runge—Kutta method with fixed time step of 0.0001 seconds. The number of iterations was chosen sufficiently large to assure stationarity of the dynamics. Transient dynamics were omitted from data analyses and graphical presentations of the results. The model is available at Zenodo website [49].

Results

Phase-frequency coupling

For P1 a value of 4.5, lower than the limit cycle threshold, was selected (Table 2), which assures that the node operates as a resonance filter circuit in the gamma band. As shown in Fig 2, for every positive phase of the slow oscillation in Node 1, the average zero crossings of fast oscillation in Node 1 was increased and for every negative phase it was decreased. Amplitude of the fast oscillation was slightly modulated as well, but far less pronounced than frequency. This illustrates that in the resonance regime, the fast oscillation is more sensitive to frequency than to amplitude modulation. The value of P2 was set to 0 in order to deactivate the fast oscillation in the second node. The PFC arises within Node 1. The slow oscillation in the first node was synchronized with that in the second node (Fig 2). As a consequence, the frequency of the fast oscillation in Node 1 also correlates with the phase of the slow oscillations in Node 2, resulting in cross-node PFC. Note that cross-node PFC was generated only through the settings of mean noise input levels, P1 and P2, while connection strengths remained fixed. The cross-node PFC arises through cross-node phase-phase coupling of the slow oscillations, combined with local phase-frequency coupling. Although the fast oscillation in Node 2 appears to be oscillating at around 18Hz, this is due to the cut-off frequency of the filters. As we will report in the Power spectrum densities (PSDs) of Model output, the inhibitory interneurons in Node 2 do not produce any fast oscillations.

Fig 2. Phase-Frequency Coupling (PFC) simulation.

Fig 2

Time domain signals are shown for visual inspection of each CFC. FO1, SO1, FO2 and SO2 are the filtered output of the Node 1 and Node 2 respectively. The frequency of FO1 varies between positive (yellow stripes) and negative (green stripes) phases of SO1. SO1 and SO2 are synchronized (i.e., cross-node PPC). Integers below the FO time waves are average numbers of FO zero crossings, which is used as an index of average frequency of each phase. Local PFC between FO1 and SO1 and cross-node PFC between FO1 and SO2 are observed.

Phase-amplitude coupling

The mechanism for implementing cross-node PAC is similar to above, except that in the first node, the value of parameter P1 was set to 7, which is higher than the limit cycle threshold which triggers a steady-state gamma band oscillation. P2 again was set to 0, in order to deactivate the fast oscillation in the second node (Table 2). The amplitude of the fast oscillation in Node 1 was modulated by the slow activity via the coupling between the slow and the fast inhibtory interneurons. Through the synchronization of the slow oscillations across the nodes, the slow oscillation in Node 2 also correlates with the amplitude changes of the fast oscillation in Node 1, resulting in cross-node PAC (Fig 3).

Fig 3. Phase-Amplitude Coupling (PAC) simulation.

Fig 3

The instantaneous amplitude of FO1 correlates with the synchronized SO1 and SO2. Local PAC between FO1 and SO1 and cross-node PAC between FO1 and SO2 are observable. Integers below the FO time waves indicate average number of zero crossings of FOs.

Frequency-frequency coupling

With FFC, two distinct fast oscillations change their frequencies over time. Thus, two frequency-modulated signals are required. To enable this, P1 and P2 were both set to 4.5, i.e. in the resonance regime. The resulting FFC is shown in Fig 4. Fast oscillations in both nodes show increase and decrease in average number of zero crossings within, respectively, the negative and positive phases of the slow oscillations. The synchronized slow oscillations modulate the frequency of the fast oscillations. Thus, the FFC is the result of two local PFCs of which the slow signals are synchronized between nodes.

Fig 4. Frequency-Frequency Coupling (FFC) simulation.

Fig 4

The frequency of FO1 and FO2 varies for the synchronized SO1 and SO2. Integers below the FO time waves are the average number of zero crossings, which is an index of average frequency of each phase. The numbers change between positive and negative phases of synchronized SO1 and SO2. Local PFC between FO1 and SO1, and between FO2 and SO2 are also observed.

Amplitude-amplitude coupling

AAC refers to the correlation between two amplitude signals from two distinct fast oscillations. Therefore, AAC requires a pair of amplitude-modulated signals. To achieve this, P1 and P2 were both set to 7, well above the limit cycle threshold. Time constants for the fast interneuron populations self-feedback were set to obtain a fast oscillation of about 51Hz in Node 1, and of about 42Hz in Node 2. As shown in Fig 5, the amplitudes of the fast oscillations were modulated by synchronization between the slow oscillations. As a result, the amplitude of the fast oscillation in Node 1 correlated with that of the fast oscillation in Node 2. AAC is thus the product of two local PACs of which the slow signals are synchronized between nodes.

Fig 5. Amplitude-Amplitude Coupling (AAC) simulation.

Fig 5

The instantaneous amplitude of FO1 and FO2 correlate with the synchronized SO1 and SO2. Integers below the FO time waves indicate average number of zero crossings of FOs.

Amplitude-frequency coupling

AFC is the most complex type of CFC; amplitude of one fast oscillation correlates with the fluctuations in the frequency of the other. To obtain AFC, PAC is generated in Node 1, i.e., the P1 is set to 7, well above the limit-cycle threshold value, and P2 is set to 4.5, in the resonance regime. PAC within Node 1 and PFC within Node 2 were combined via the cross-node phase synchronization. The synchronized slow oscillations between nodes simultaneously modulate the amplitude of the fast oscillation in Node 1 and the frequency of the fast oscillation in Node 2, resulting in AFC between the fast oscillations of Nodes 1 and 2 (Fig 6).

Fig 6. Amplitude-Frequency Coupling (AFC) simulation.

Fig 6

The instantaneous amplitude of FO1 correlates with the synchronized SO1 and SO2. The frequency of FO2 is about constant while the frequency of FO1 varies for the synchronized SO1 and SO2. See average number of zero crossings in FOs, indicated as integers below the FO time waves.

Power Spectrum Densities (PSDs) of model output

Fig 7 shows PSDs of outputs from Nodes 1 and 2 in each CFC simulation. Peaks appear in the delta (< 4Hz) and gamma (>30Hz) bands, which indicates stationary oscillatory activity. In case of frequency modulation (Panels a and c), the gamma activity shows wider peaks than in case of amplitude modulation (Panels b and d). In Panel e, where both modulations occur, a narrow (Node 1) and a wide (Node 2) bands can be observed in the gamma activity. In the PFC and PAC results (Figs 2 and 3), the fast oscillation in Node 2 appeared to have oscillatory activity around 18Hz. However, no peak at 18Hz exists in Panels a and b. Thus, the apparent ‘18Hz oscillation’ is shown to be an artifact of filtering. (Note that numbers of zero crossings of fast oscillations in Figs 26 are slightly lower than that of their actual frequency due to the estimation method of the zero crossing)

Fig 7. The comparison of PSDs in different forms of CFC.

Fig 7

PFC: Phase-Frequency Coupling, PAC: Phase-Amplitude Coupling, FFC: Frequency-Frequency Coupling, AAC: Amplitude-Amplitude Coupling, AFC: Amplitude-Frequency Coupling.

Discussion

As suggested by Jensen and Colgin [2], coupling between slow and fast neural activity could take one of six forms: PFC, PAC, FFC, AAC, AFC or PPC. The first five of these were obtained in a model consisting of two mutually connected four-populations NMM nodes. For PAC or PPC, models have previously been developed with greater neurological detail than the present one [5052]. Instead, we aimed to understand the systematic relationship among the cross-frequency couplings (CFCs). Neural mass models afford this based on their relative simplicity while preserving sufficient neural plausibility for simulating population level electrophysiological signals [30,33,45]. The neural mass modeling approach has been used for both quantitative estimation of the system parameters in cortical and subcortical regions [43,5355] and qualitative simulation for neural systems behavior [31,5658]. Here, all results are qualitative, as they serve the purpose of introducing a plausible common mechanism for the emergence of different types of CFC.

Each node of the model represents a patch of cortical tissue, anywhere between a few millimeters and a centimeter in diameter, in which oscillations in various frequencies can be observed [41]. Whereas nodes were connected symmetrically to represent interconnected brain electrical activity sources within a cortical region, asymmetric inter-node connectivity is more likely to represent hierarchical structures, such as top-down/bottom-up connectivity in visual cortex [59]. Addressing this issue will require further elaboration of the inter-node connectivity of the model.

To generate different types of CFC, the model structure and connectivity were not changed, rather, the level of input noise of the nodes (parameters P1 and P2) was varied. The noise level determines the stability of the fast activity: when P is in the resonance regime, local PFC occurs, while the parameter is set above the limit-cycle threshold, local PAC emerges. The results showed that PFC and PAC are two phases of a CFC which are determined by the stability of the fast inhibitory activity. The modeled fast activity represents interneuron gamma (ING). Thus, the simulation results predict that, as the mean noise input level to a cortical region changes, stability of ING will change, and CFC will change in appearance between frequency and amplitude modulation.

The slow activity of the nodes was synchronized via the inter-node connection. The local fast activities are modulated via the synchronized slow activity. Our results show how local ING can be modulated across brain regions via a common intrinsic slow activity, e.g., alpha band activity in visual cortices. Cross-regional PAC was reported even between bilateral motor cortices in monkeys [60]. For the inter-hemispheric PAC, other regions, such as, the subthalamic nucleus, could also contribute as a pacemaker. A pacemaker region could replace or complement the between-node pyramidal population [43,61]. This would not affect to the framework of cross-regional PAC/PFC generation, as long as the slow activities of the individual nodes remain synchronized in the same frequency band. Across brain regions of which the main frequencies differ, e.g., alpha rhythms in visual cortices and theta in hippocampus, m:n synchrony (where m≠n) is needed for modulation of local fast activities. Neural mass modeling studies have been addressing the issue of cross-frequency phase-phase coupling (PPC), in which coupling strength between the nodes is the major parameter of interest [46]. The current study, however, does not address the issue since our main parameters were the mean noise input levels.

The model also successfully implemented AAC, FFC and AFC. In all simulations, amplitude and/or frequency of fast activity was modulated by synchronized slow activity. The assumption of a common modulator is in accordance with empirical studies. For example, AAC measurement provided evidence that two fast activities were modulated by a common slow activity in LFP signals [2426]. To our knowledge, PFC, FFC and AFC have not yet been reported in brain signals. The model predicts FFC if slow activity in two regions is phase synchronized while stability of ING in both is intermediate, i.e., in the resonance regime. Likewise, AFC is expected when the slow activity is synchronized while ING is stable in one region but intermediately stable in the other.

Our PFC, FFC and AFC results provide a possible explanation why frequency modulations are scarcely reported. Frequency modulation occurred when the model was in the resonance regime, where amplitude modulation also occurrs to some extent. This suggests that in the brain signal, PFC and PAC may co-exist. Since PFC measures are still under-developed compared to PAC measures [6,6265], PFC may fail to be observed in a noisy signal such as EEG, while the weaker PAC is detected.

The frequency modulation could be more than a mere transient state to amplitude modulation. Empirical studies have shown that fast inhibitory interneurons fire at a specific phase of the gamma band oscillation [37,66]. The gamma frequency, in this context, is synonymous to the average number of spiking. As the gamma frequency increases/decreases according to the slow phases, the inhibitory population changes in excitability. In the current model, the range between the maximum and minimum frequencies is larger in the frequency modulation than in the amplitude modulation. Such a wide range of excitability is beneficial, for example, for implementing a gating function for information transfer.

In our model, frequency was determined by the fixed time constants of the neural populations (see also [67]). Frequency modulation was realized by through the effect of mean input noise level on the gain of the sigmoid block. The product of the gain and the time constant determines the resonance frequency. In principle, the time constants could also be considered as function of noise level [40]. This, however, would not change the principle behind the current approach.

We conclude that two fundamental forms of CFCs in cortex are PFC and PAC. They emerge as modulations of ING. The couplings can either be kept local or spread across regions via PPC, depending on the synchronization of slow intrinsic oscillation between these regions.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by an Odysseus grant from the Flemish organizations for science (FWO) awarded to Cees van Leeuwen.

Data Availability

The current work does not have any human recording data. Simulink/MATLAB code for the model is available at: https://zenodo.org/record/248829. Default parameters are set for amplitude-frequency coupling simulation. To obtain simulated data for other types of couplings, parameters P1 and P2 needs to be changed as listed in Table 2 of the manuscript (or as mentioned in the comments of Main.m file). The rest of the parameters are common for all types of couplings (See Table 1).

Funding Statement

This research is founded by an Odysseus grant from the Flemish organizations for science (FWO) awarded to Cees van Leeuwen.

References

  • 1.Buzsáki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal Oscillations in Cortical Networks. Science. 2004;304: 1926–1929. 10.1126/science.1099745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Jensen O, Colgin LL. Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11: 267–269. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bragin A, Jandó G, Nádasdy Z, Hetke J, Wise K, Buzsáki G. Gamma (40–100 Hz) oscillation in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 1995;15: 47–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Atallah BV, Scanziani M. Instantaneous Modulation of Gamma Oscillation Frequency by Balancing Excitation with Inhibition. Neuron. 2009;62: 566–577. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Womelsdorf T, Schoffelen J-M, Oostenveld R, Singer W, Desimone R, Engel AK, et al. Modulation of Neuronal Interactions Through Neuronal Synchronization. Science. 2007;316: 1609–1612. 10.1126/science.1139597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, et al. High Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Theta Oscillations in Human Neocortex. Science. 2006;313: 1626–1628. 10.1126/science.1128115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Osipova D, Hermes D, Jensen O. Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Posterior Alpha Activity. PLOS ONE. 2008;3: e3990 10.1371/journal.pone.0003990 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Canolty RT, Knight RT. The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14: 506–515. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hyafil A, Giraud A-L, Fontolan L, Gutkin B. Neural Cross-Frequency Coupling: Connecting Architectures, Mechanisms, and Functions. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38: 725–740. 10.1016/j.tins.2015.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lisman JE, Idiart MA. Storage of 7 +/- 2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science. 1995;267: 1512–1515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jensen O, Lisman JE. Hippocampal sequence-encoding driven by a cortical multi-item working memory buffer. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28: 67–72. 10.1016/j.tins.2004.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Schroeder CE, Lakatos P, Kajikawa Y, Partan S, Puce A. Neuronal oscillations and visual amplification of speech. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12: 106–113. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lakatos P, Musacchia G, O’Connel MN, Falchier AY, Javitt DC, Schroeder CE. The Spectrotemporal Filter Mechanism of Auditory Selective Attention. Neuron. 2013;77: 750–761. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9: 474–480. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, et al. Frequency of gamma oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature. 2009;462: 353–357. 10.1038/nature08573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Koch C, Davis JL. Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain. MIT press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Varela F, Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2: 229–239. 10.1038/35067550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Melloni L, Molina C, Pena M, Torres D, Singer W, Rodriguez E. Synchronization of Neural Activity across Cortical Areas Correlates with Conscious Perception. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 2858–2865. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4623-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fell J, Axmacher N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12: 105–118. 10.1038/nrn2979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Belluscio MA, Mizuseki K, Schmidt R, Kempter R, Buzsáki G. Cross-Frequency Phase—Phase Coupling between Theta and Gamma Oscillations in the Hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2012;32: 423–435. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4122-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cohen MX, Elger CE, Fell J. Oscillatory Activity and Phase—Amplitude Coupling in the Human Medial Frontal Cortex during Decision Making. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;21: 390–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schutter DJLG, Knyazev GG. Cross-frequency coupling of brain oscillations in studying motivation and emotion. Motiv Emot. 2012;36: 46–54. 10.1007/s11031-011-9237-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.de Hemptinne C, Ryapolova-Webb ES, Air EL, Garcia PA, Miller KJ, Ojemann JG, et al. Exaggerated phase-amplitude coupling in the primary motor cortex in Parkinson disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110: 4780–4785. 10.1073/pnas.1214546110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bekisz M, Wróbel A. Coupling of beta and gamma activity in corticothalamic system of cats attending to visual stimuli. Neuroreport. 1999;10: 3589–3594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bruns A, Eckhorn R, Jokeit H, Ebner A. Amplitude envelope correlation detects coupling among incoherent brain signals. Neuroreport. 2000;11: 1509–1514. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Jackson J, Goutagny R, Williams S. Fast and Slow Gamma Rhythms Are Intrinsically and Independently Generated in the Subiculum. J Neurosci. 2011;31: 12104–12117. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1370-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Aru J, Aru J, Priesemann V, Wibral M, Lana L, Pipa G, et al. Untangling cross-frequency coupling in neuroscience. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015;31: 51–61. 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Deco G, Jirsa VK, Robinson PA, Breakspear M, Friston K. The Dynamic Brain: From Spiking Neurons to Neural Masses and Cortical Fields. PLoS Comput Biol. 2008;4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jansen BH, Zouridakis G, Brandt ME. A neurophysiologically-based mathematical model of flash visual evoked potentials. Biol Cybern. 1993;68: 275–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wendling F, Bartolomei F, Bellanger JJ, Chauvel P. Epileptic fast activity can be explained by a model of impaired GABAergic dendritic inhibition. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;15: 1499–1508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.David O, Friston KJ. A neural mass model for MEG/EEG: coupling and neuronal dynamics. NeuroImage. 2003;20: 1743–1755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Goodfellow M, Schindler K, Baier G. Intermittent spike—wave dynamics in a heterogeneous, spatially extended neural mass model. NeuroImage. 2011;55: 920–932. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jansen BH, Rit VG. Electroencephalogram and visual evoked potential generation in a mathematical model of coupled cortical columns. Biol Cybern. 1995;73: 357–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.David O, Harrison L, Friston KJ. Modelling event-related responses in the brain. NeuroImage. 2005;25: 756–770. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.David O, Kiebel SJ, Harrison LM, Mattout J, Kilner JM, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modeling of evoked responses in EEG and MEG. NeuroImage. 2006;30: 1255–1272. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Chehelcheraghi M, Nakatani C, Steur E, van Leeuwen C. A neural mass model of phase—amplitude coupling. Biol Cybern. 2016; 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wang X-J, Buzsáki G. Gamma Oscillation by Synaptic Inhibition in a Hippocampal Interneuronal Network Model. J Neurosci. 1996;16: 6402–6413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bartos M, Vida I, Frotscher M, Meyer A, Monyer H, Geiger JRP, et al. Fast synaptic inhibition promotes synchronized gamma oscillations in hippocampal interneuron networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002;99: 13222–13227. 10.1073/pnas.192233099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P. Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8: 45–56. 10.1038/nrn2044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lefebvre J, Hutt A, Knebel J-F, Whittingstall K, Murray MM. Stimulus Statistics Shape Oscillations in Nonlinear Recurrent Neural Networks. J Neurosci. 2015;35: 2895–2903. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3609-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Timofeev I, Grenier F, Bazhenov M, Sejnowski TJ, Steriade M. Origin of Slow Cortical Oscillations in Deafferented Cortical Slabs. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10: 1185–1199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Park H, Kang E, Kang H, Kim JS, Jensen O, Chung CK, et al. Cross-Frequency Power Correlations Reveal the Right Superior Temporal Gyrus as a Hub Region During Working Memory Maintenance. Brain Connect. 2011;1: 460–472. 10.1089/brain.2011.0046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bhattacharya BS, Coyle D, Maguire LP. A thalamo-cortico-thalamic neural mass model to study alpha rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease. Neural Netw Off J Int Neural Netw Soc. 2011;24: 631–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Chehelcheraghi M, Nakatani C, van Leeuwen C. Analysis of an Interneuron Gamma Mechanism for Cross-Frequency Coupling. Math Model Nat Phenom. in press; [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wilson HR, Cowan JD. Excitatory and Inhibitory Interactions in Localized Populations of Model Neurons. Biophys J. 1972;12: 1–24. 10.1016/S0006-3495(72)86068-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Huang G, Zhang D, Meng J, Zhu X. Interactions between two neural populations: A mechanism of chaos and oscillation in neural mass model. Neurocomputing. 2011;74: 1026–1034. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Brunner C, Delorme A, Makeig S. Eeglab—an Open Source Matlab Toolbox for Electrophysiological Research. Biomed Eng Biomed Tech. 2013; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Welch PD. The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: A method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms. IEEE Trans Audio Electroacoustics. 1967;15: 70–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Chehelcheraghi M. Matlab/Simulink implemenetation of a Neural Mass Model for Simulating Cross Frequency Coupling. Zenodo. 2017; [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kopell N, Ermentrout GB, Whittington MA, Traub RD. Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2000;97: 1867–1872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Neymotin SA, Lazarewicz MT, Sherif M, Contreras D, Finkel LH, Lytton WW. Ketamine disrupts θ modulation of γ in a computer model of hippocampus. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2011;31: 11733–11743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Neymotin SA, Hilscher MM, Moulin TC, Skolnick Y, Lazarewicz MT, Lytton WW. Ih tunes theta/gamma oscillations and cross-frequency coupling in an in silico CA3 model. PloS One. 2013;8: e76285 10.1371/journal.pone.0076285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Cona F, Zavaglia M, Massimini M, Rosanova M, Ursino M. A neural mass model of interconnected regions simulates rhythm propagation observed via TMS-EEG. NeuroImage. 2011;57: 1045–1058. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cona F, Lacanna M, Ursino M. A thalamo-cortical neural mass model for the simulation of brain rhythms during sleep. J Comput Neurosci. 2014;37: 125–148. 10.1007/s10827-013-0493-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Moran R, Pinotsis DA, Friston K. Neural masses and fields in dynamic causal modeling. Front Comput Neurosci. 2013;7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ursino M, Cona F, Zavaglia M. The generation of rhythms within a cortical region: Analysis of a neural mass model. NeuroImage. 2010;52: 1080–1094. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Garnier A, Vidal A, Huneau C, Benali H. A Neural Mass Model with Direct and Indirect Excitatory Feedback Loops: Identification of Bifurcations and Temporal Dynamics. Neural Comput. 2014;27: 329–364. 10.1162/NECO_a_00696 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Youssofzadeh V, Prasad G, Wong-Lin K. On self-feedback connectivity in neural mass models applied to event-related potentials. NeuroImage. 2015;108: 364–376. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bastos AM, Usrey WM, Adams RA, Mangun GR, Fries P, Friston KJ. Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron. 2012;76: 695–711. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Canolty RT, Ganguly K, Kennerley SW, Cadieu CF, Koepsell K, Wallis JD, et al. Oscillatory phase coupling coordinates anatomically dispersed functional cell assemblies. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107: 17356–17361. 10.1073/pnas.1008306107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mina F, Benquet P, Pasnicu A, Biraben A, Wendling F. Modulation of epileptic activity by deep brain stimulation: a model-based study of frequency-dependent effects. Front Comput Neurosci. 2013;7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lakatos P, Shah AS, Knuth KH, Ulbert I, Karmos G, Schroeder CE. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94: 1904–1911. 10.1152/jn.00263.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Cohen MX. Assessing transient cross-frequency coupling in EEG data. J Neurosci Methods. 2008;168: 494–499. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Tort ABL, Komorowski R, Eichenbaum H, Kopell N. Measuring Phase-Amplitude Coupling Between Neuronal Oscillations of Different Frequencies. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104: 1195–1210. 10.1152/jn.00106.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Nakhnikian A, Ito S, Dwiel LL, Grasse LM, Rebec GV, Lauridsen LN, et al. A novel cross-frequency coupling detection method using the generalized Morse wavelets. J Neurosci Methods. 2016;269: 61–73. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291: 1560–1563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Marreiros AC, Daunizeau J, Kiebel SJ, Friston KJ. Population dynamics: variance and the sigmoid activation function. NeuroImage. 2008;42: 147–157. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The current work does not have any human recording data. Simulink/MATLAB code for the model is available at: https://zenodo.org/record/248829. Default parameters are set for amplitude-frequency coupling simulation. To obtain simulated data for other types of couplings, parameters P1 and P2 needs to be changed as listed in Table 2 of the manuscript (or as mentioned in the comments of Main.m file). The rest of the parameters are common for all types of couplings (See Table 1).


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES