Skip to main content
BMC Evolutionary Biology logoLink to BMC Evolutionary Biology
. 2017 Apr 17;17:101. doi: 10.1186/s12862-017-0944-9

Testosterone, territorial response, and song in seasonally breeding tropical and temperate stonechats

Beate Apfelbeck 1,2,✉,#, Kim G Mortega 1,3,#, Heiner Flinks 4, Juan Carlos Illera 5, Barbara Helm 1
PMCID: PMC5392926  PMID: 28412929

Abstract

Background

Testosterone facilitates physiological, morphological, and behavioral changes required for breeding in male vertebrates. However, testosterone concentrations and the link between its seasonal changes and those in reproductive behaviors vary greatly among species. To better understand the impact of tropical and temperate environments and life history factors on this variation, we have compared testosterone, territorial behavior and song performance across sequential stages of the breeding season in males of 16 closely related taxa of East African tropical and West European temperate stonechats (Saxicola spp), which all breed during a short breeding season, but differ in migratory behavior, seasonal territory-acquisition and pace of life.

Results

We found that generally, the profiles of testosterone and territorial behavior were similar across latitudes. African stonechats with a slow pace of life had equally high peak testosterone concentrations and responded as aggressively to an intruder as European stonechats with a fast pace of life. However, song performance at the beginning of the breeding season was lower in African than in European stonechats. The differences in song performance were not associated with variation in testosterone levels between tropical and temperate stonechats.

Conclusions

The results suggest a very similar role for testosterone as a mediator of high intensity territorial aggression during the fertile period of females in tropical and temperate stonechats, which all are highly seasonal, locally synchronous breeders. A potential explanation may be high risk of extra-pair copulations which has been associated with synchronous breeding. Interestingly, an association was not consistent for song performance. Our data suggest that song performance can be disassociated from peak testosterone levels depending on its role in breeding behavior. Despite similar testosterone levels, European males, which early in the breeding season acquire territories and mates, showed greater song performance than African stonechats, which maintain year-round territories and pair-bonds. Taken together, our study comparing related taxa of old world songbirds suggests that short breeding seasons may be a major selective force for high peak testosterone levels during breeding regardless of latitude and pace of life, but that particular behaviors, in our case song, can be uncoupled from peak testosterone levels.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s12862-017-0944-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Steroid hormones, Testosterone, Territorial behavior, Migratory behavior, Song, Tropical, Temperate, Songbirds, Breeding season length

Background

The sex steroid testosterone coordinates many reproductive traits in male vertebrates (reviewed by [1]). Testosterone has pleiotropic effects and enhances male fecundity by activating spermatogenesis, secondary sex characteristics and sexual and aggressive behaviors associated with breeding (reviewed by [1, 2]). These effects are supported by experimental studies and by coinciding seasonal profiles of testosterone with reproductive behaviors. For example, implants of testosterone can increase territorial aggression (e.g. [3, 4]) and song rate (e.g.[5, 6]), while implants of androgen receptor blockers can decrease territorial aggression (e.g. [710]) and the occurrence of dawn song [11]. Coinciding seasonal profiles of testosterone and behavior have been observed in detail in many temperate seasonally breeding birds. For example in temperate socially monogamous birds, males commonly engage strongest into territorial disputes (e.g. [1215]), sing most (e.g. [1621]) and have the highest testosterone levels at the beginning of the breeding season when territories are established and females are fertile (e.g. [2125]).

However, quantitative differences in peak testosterone levels and differences in the degree to which testosterone mediates reproductive behaviors have been observed [26]. For example, many tropical species with extended breeding seasons tend to have lower testosterone levels than high latitude species with short breeding seasons [2730]. Two main explanations have been suggested for what had seemed to be a general pattern of lower testosterone concentrations in tropical birds. Firstly, assuming generally longer breeding seasons in the tropics, lower testosterone levels may help to avoid extended costs associated with testosterone, such as potential impairment of the immune system, survival and male parental care [31, 32]. Alternatively or in addition, tropical species typically fall on the “slow” end of the pace of life syndrome [28, 33] by being longer-lived, showing reduced metabolic measures and lower fecundity, often paired with extended parental care. Thus, they may especially benefit from low testosterone concentrations throughout the breeding season, which would aid survival while imposing little cost given low fecundity.

More recently, however, several studies have shown that males of tropical species that breed in highly seasonal environments have indeed similar peak testosterone concentrations as temperate seasonal breeders ([3438]; reviewed by [29, 39]). It has, therefore, been suggested that seasonal peak testosterone levels are determined by the length of the breeding season and associated factors independently of breeding latitude [29]. Among the factors that are associated with different degrees in seasonality and that could influence peak testosterone levels is type of territoriality, depending on seasonal migration [39]. Permanent residents, which are more common in the tropics, often defend year-round territories and form long-term monogamous pair bonds regardless of the timing of breeding [40], whereas migratory species typically acquire territories and breeding mates after returning to the breeding grounds [41]. A second factor that has been associated with different degrees in seasonality and that could influence peak testosterone levels is risk of extra-pair copulations, which is thought to increase when breeding seasons are short and pairs are highly synchronous [40, 4244]. Thus, a perceived pattern of low peak testosterone in the tropics may have resulted from heavy emphasis on species breeding in the lowland forests of the Neotropics with low seasonality and extended breeding seasons. In these habitats, low risk of extra-pair copulations and typically continuous territory ownership could have specifically selected for low testosterone levels [40, 42]. Thus, based on hormone profiles, tropical seasonal breeders could be expected to show similar territorial behavior and song performance as temperate seasonal breeders.

In contrast, based on life-history patterns, it could still be expected that tropical and temperate seasonally breeding species differ in their testosterone profiles and in the behaviours that are commonly under testosterone control. Even in seasonal tropical biomes some environmental conditions are relatively stable year-round (e.g. only minor changes in temperature and day length [45, 46]). Such general tropical-temperate differences seem to have promoted a slow pace of life in the tropics with reduced fecundity and increased survival [33, 47], which for birds has been robustly established. For example, tropical birds, independent of biome, lay smaller clutches [48] and have lower basal metabolic rates [49], but live longer [50, 51] than temperate birds. Based on their low fecundity and greater longevity, tropical species generally are expected to invest less into mating than temperate species [28, 52]. Thus, while apparently overall differences in peak testosterone concentrations appear to depend on the length of the breeding season, pace-of-living could be associated with differences in the fine seasonal profile of testosterone levels, or in the association of testosterone with behaviours linked to specific aspects of breeding, such as territorial aggression and song. For example, it has been proposed that in birds with year-round territories, but short, synchronous breeding seasons, the major role of testosterone is to promote territorial aggression and mate-guarding during the fertile period of females to prevent extra-pair copulations [39].

In birds, song is a major component of territory establishment and mate attraction [5355] and different song characteristics matter in these different contexts [56, 57]. For example, high element rates have been described in courtship songs of several species and seem to be sexually attractive to females when tested under laboratory conditions (e.g. [58, 59]). Song rate has been shown to increase after the experimental removal of the mate [54] and in many species females prefer males that sing at high rates (e.g. [60]). Species that are supposed to be under greater pressure to establish a territory and attract a mate at the beginning of the breeding season are expected to sing more and more complex song than species breeding under more relaxed conditions [6163]. Therefore, non-withstanding the general similarity in testosterone profiles, song characteristics are expected to differ between high-paced temperate and slow-paced tropical species which have low fecundity and no need for annual acquisition of territories and mates. Indeed, comparative studies across latitudinal gradients find that migratory species, species with high annual fecundity and species breeding at higher latitudes sing longer songs, with more syllables and at a higher rate than non-migratory resident species, species with low fecundity and species breeding closer to the equator [6469]. Despite this body of literature, the implications of similar peak testosterone concentrations in tropical and temperate seasonal breeders for the variation of behaviors related to mating are still unclear. In addition to these research gaps, there is need for a broader coverage of global biomes and for insights from closely related species. Most of our present knowledge on testosterone, song and territorial behavior in tropical birds is biased towards species breeding in the lowland forests of the Neotropics with low seasonality and extended breeding seasons, which are often compared to different Nearctic species.

In this study, we therefore compare testosterone, song and territorial behavior during different breeding stages within a group of closely related old world songbirds. Stonechats (Saxicola spp) have a wide distribution range from tropical to temperate environments, but wherever they occur, they are seasonal breeders [70]. Many other traits differ between stonechats from different environments. For example, they display a full range of migratory habits from long-distance migrant to resident [70], and they are a classical example for differences in pace of life between tropical and temperate birds as highlighted in Ricklefs’ and Wikelski’s influential article in 2002 [33]. The behavioral endocrinology of temperate and tropical stonechats is also well studied. During the breeding season, territorial behavior in temperate stonechats has been shown to depend on testosterone [8, 71] and individual variation in testosterone concentrations during mating, but not during parenting, relates to variation in reproductive success [72]. However, temperate male stonechats do not show short-term increases in testosterone concentrations during simulated territorial intrusions despite an aggressive response towards the intruder [73]. Furthermore, territorial aggression outside the breeding season is independent of testosterone [8]. Thus, in temperate stonechats testosterone may mediate aggressive behavior particularly in a mating context and may promote aggressive mate-guarding during the fertile period of females. In tropical stonechats, Dittami and Gwinner [74] studied seasonal patterns in reproductive hormones, and found weak, annual patterns in testosterone and LH in Kenyan males [74]. They found indication of declines in both hormones during the breeding season, but had low power for fine-scale resolution of breeding seasonality. In a more detailed study, Goymann et al. [36] compared testosterone of tropical Kenyan male stonechats during the breeding season to those of temperate German stonechats [36]. They found that African stonechats showed a pronounced peak of testosterone during the nest-building and egg-laying stage. Compared to European stonechats, the study reported overall lower testosterone in African stonechats, except for the nest-building and egg-laying stages. Associations between the seasonal profile of testosterone and aggressive and vocal behaviors are currently not known for tropical stonechats.

Few studies so far have considered song within the comparative stonechat system. Earlier work in temperate stonechats showed that song plays an important role to attract a mate [12, 75] and song rate peaks at the beginning of the breeding season, just before egg-laying of the first brood [12]. Detailed seasonal song profiles are not available for tropical stonechats, however both tropical and temperate stonechats only sing during the breeding season [74, 76]. An earlier study on temperate stonechats has studied song performance and aggression in response to territorial intrusions using song playback and decoys [77], but did not relate these findings to hormone concentrations.

Building on the strength of the comparative stonechat system, we extend the earlier work to compare testosterone, territorial behavior and song across a wide range of tropical and temperate stonechat populations. The broad latitudinal comparison is intended to produce generalizable conclusions, while the combination with detailed territorial and song behavior is entirely novel. We compare populations of East African tropical and West European temperate male stonechats that differ in fecundity, territoriality and the stability of pair bonds. Specifically, our study aims

  • i)

    to test the generality of previously reported similar testosterone peaks using males of several populations, during sequential stages of the breeding cycle.

  • ii)

    to test whether seasonal changes in testosterone are associated with population-wide changes in territorial aggression and song performance. As male temperate, but not tropical, stonechats, have to establish a territory and attract a female at the beginning of the breeding season, we expect higher territorial aggression and higher song performance (song rate, element rate, peak frequency) in temperate than tropical stonechats at the beginning of the breeding season. We expect that a lack of differences would indicate that testosterone in tropical stonechats has other functions, such as mediating aggression in a mating context. If so, we predict that both testosterone and aggression should peak in both tropical and temperate stonechats during nest-building when females are fertile.

Methods

Study system

Species background

Stonechats are especially well suited to address the aims specified above, because we have extensive knowledge of the life history and endocrinology of tropical and temperate populations. Tropical stonechats follow a slower pace of life than temperate stonechats. They have lower fecundity, fewer annual clutches and a lower metabolic rate than their higher latitude relatives. Common garden experiments have shown that these differences are genetically fixed [7880]. Despite these and other differences in pace of life, stonechats are socially monogamous seasonal breeders with relatively short breeding seasons, and defend a breeding territory regardless of the latitude at which they breed [70]. Tropical stonechats are usually resident and stay with the same mate year round [74]. Migratory temperate stonechats establish separate territories in breeding and wintering areas and form seasonal pair bonds with different mates [76]. Resident temperate stonechats may show local movements during winter depending on environmental conditions [70]. In temperate stonechats, territorial disputes between males are most frequently observed at the beginning of the breeding season [12]. At this time, breeding territories are being established in temperate stonechats, whereas in tropical stonechats establishment of breeding territories occurs after the post-fledgling period [81]. As in other songbirds [82], copulation usually takes place 1–4 days before the first egg is laid [70].

Field work

We measured testosterone levels in several populations of tropical East African and temperate European stonechats during different stages of the breeding cycle and studied seasonal changes in territorial behavior and song in a subsample of these populations. During their respective breeding seasons, we studied closely related stonechat species in East Africa (Saxicola torquata axillaris, 10 populations, 173 individuals, latitudes 0° - 4°S, altitudinal range: 1376–2500 m asl, in the years 2012–2013) and in Europe (Saxicola torquata rubicola, 6 populations, 201 individuals, latitudes 37° - 51°N, altitudinal range: 15–1350 m asl, in the years 2009–2013). Both in tropical and temperate environments stonechats breed in open shrub and grassland habitats, sometimes close to human habitations. Two of the European populations (Germany: Düffel and Heubach) were sampled as part of a different study on the effects of simulated territorial intrusions on testosterone and behavior [73]. The East African populations are resident and breed at different times of the year corresponding to the two major rainy seasons: Southern populations breed from October to December (Tanzania: Arusha National Park, 3°S, Monduli, 3°S, Pare Mountains, 4°S, Usambara Mountains, 4°S), while Northern populations breed from March to July (Kenya: Kinangop, 0°N, Olkalou, 0°N, Mount Kenya, 0°N, Ngong Hills, 1°S, Kakamega, 0°N, Mataara, 0°S). European stonechats breed from March to July and differ in migratory disposition. Three of the sampled European populations were migratory (Western (Düffel and Heubach, 51°N) and Southern (Bavaria, 47°N) Germany), two were partial migrants (Central Spain, 40°N, Ireland, 51°N) and one was a resident population (Southern Spain, 37°N). Breeding stage was determined through careful observation of pairs before capture (e.g. singing activity, nest-building activity by females, feeding activity of nestlings, the presence of fledglings). In some cases, we were able to catch the female and breeding stage could be ascertained by the presence or absence of a brood patch. For located nests, we recorded the number of eggs or nestlings and checked the nest again after a few days. However, especially when females are egg-laying or incubating, it can be very challenging to determine the exact breeding stage (when unable to catch the female), thus for a number of pairs we were not able to ascertain this information. We determined the age of all individuals (yearling or adult (≥ 2years)) based on the wing molt pattern [83] and a picture of the wing was taken for later reference. All birds were bled, measured (body mass, tarsus and wing length), checked for molt, ringed with a uniquely numbered aluminum ring and a combination of three color plastic rings, and released back into their territories.

Capture methods

Males were caught between 7:00 h and 18:00 h with baited clap net traps and/or clap net traps combined with a mounted decoy and playback. In Europe, we used playbacks from British or Spanish stonechats downloaded from the British library or www.xeno-canto.org, respectively. In East Africa, we used playbacks recorded from the same population. As decoys we used stuffed males in full adult plumage that were protected by an inconspicuous cage. In Europe, we randomly assigned two decoys from Austria and in East Africa eight decoys from each of the two populations, Kinangop (Kenya) and Arusha (Tanzania). In stonechats, stimulation with a decoy and playback does not change circulating testosterone concentrations [72], and hence to analyze testosterone profiles data from passively and actively caught birds were combined.

Blood sampling and hormone analysis

Immediately upon capture (mean ± SD: 228 ± 191 s), a blood sample (~ 120 μl) was taken after venipuncture from the wing vein and collected into heparinized capillaries. Plasma was immediately separated by centrifugation with a Compur Minicentrifuge (Bayer Diagnostics) or a Spectrafuge Mini Laboratory Centrifuge (Labnet International, Inc.). The amount of plasma was measured with a Hamilton syringe and was stored in 500 μl pure ethanol [84]. After the end of each field season (≤ 4 months) samples were stored at −80 °C. Testosterone concentration was determined by direct radioimmunoassay (RIA, following [36] and [85]). Mean efficiency of the extraction with dichloromethane varied between 81% and 89%. Samples were measured in duplicates in five assays. Three of the assays contained both samples from African and European populations. Samples that had been obtained in Germany in 2009 and 2010 had been analyzed separately in the same laboratory using the same assays [73]. The intra-extraction coefficients of variation ranged between 2.8% and 11.8% (mean 5.6%). The inter-assay variation was 11.9%. The lower limit of detection of the assay was determined as the first value outside the 95% confidence intervals for the zero standard (Bmax) and was on average 0.35 ± 0.05 pg/tube. As the testosterone antibody (Esoterix Endocrinology) shows significant cross-reactions with 5a–dihydrotestosterone (44%) our measurements may include a minor fraction of this additional androgen.

Behavioral response to simulated territorial intrusions

For two migratory neighboring populations in Europe (Germany: Düffel and Heubach, n = 55) and four populations in Africa (Tanzania: Monduli, Kenya: Kinangop, Olkalou, Mount Kenya, n = 46) we performed simulated territorial intrusion (STI) experiments. We recorded the behavior of male stonechats towards an intruder during breeding, using latency to attack as the main metric, because previous experiments in stonechats have shown that other behavioral measures (e.g. response and approach latency) strongly correlate with the latency to attack the decoy [73]. To elicit a territorial response we placed a stuffed decoy into the center of a territory (determined by singing posts of the male or nest location) and played back song. For all tropical tests, we used exclusively decoys of African stonechats, and for all temperate tests, we used exclusively decoys of European stonechats, which all originated from a large pool of a captive breeding program at the German Max Planck Institute for Ornithology [86]. African stonechats were taken as nestlings from several study sites in the Kenyan Rift valley and Arusha National Park (Tanzania), while European stonechats were collected as nestlings from a population in Lower Austria. All birds were raised and bred at the Max Planck Institute. Specimens from the original and subsequent generations were kept in a freezer immediately after natural death. In 2011, Mortega chose those birds for preparation of African decoys which were in full adult plumage and physically best maintained while trying to cover the natural variation in their morphology with specific focus on breast patch patterns within each population. All decoy birds were made by one technical assistant of the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology. Decoys of European stonechats had been chosen and prepared at an earlier date using similar criteria, but had never been used in experiments before and were, thus, in very good condition. All decoys, African and European, were presented in a standardized posture, resembling a perching male stonechat. During experiments we prevented the damage of decoys by protecting them with an inconspicuous cage made of a wire frame and a mist net. The Kenyan taxidermic preparations were employed in STI experiments in the three Kenyan populations that breed from March to July (distance between populations ranged between 50 – 95 km) and the Tanzanian decoys were used in the Tanzanian population that breeds from October to December similar to the population where the decoy was from (distance between populations 40 km). For song playback, in Germany, we used eight different playbacks in random order (wav - files, British library). In East Africa, we used four to six different playbacks recorded from the same population and took care to avoid songs from neighboring males within hearing distance. Territory holders were observed until they attacked the decoy (min), but not longer than 60 min. Therefore, males that did not attack the decoy were assigned an attack latency of 60 min.

Song recording and analysis

We recorded 30–45 min of song from males of the same two European (8:00 h - 12:00 h, n = 30) and seven East African populations (5:20 h - 10:00 h, n = 32) using a Marantz PMD 661 solid state recorder (Osnabrück, Germany) and Sennheiser ME66/K6 directional microphones (Georgsmarienhütte, Germany). In general, 5–10 min of song were of sufficient quality to be analyzed. We analyzed songs (sampling frequency: 44.1 kHz; resolution: 16 bit) using the software Avisoft Sound Analysis Pro, v5.1.09 (Raimund Specht, Berlin, Germany). We considered five parameters to describe song output and structure in our study species [77]: song rate (average number of songs per minute over a five minute period), element rate (number of elements divided by song duration), as well as peak (frequency of the highest amplitude sound), minimum, and maximum frequencies. For a spectrogram of an exemplary song in European stonechats see [77]. We used the automatic parameter measurements setup to obtain the minimum and maximum frequency values measured at a standard decibel threshold (here −20 dB, total option) below the peak in the power spectra [87]. We compared song between African and European stonechats during the pre-nesting and nest-building stages.

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed with the program R (R version 3.2.2, [88]), and using the packages JAGS [89] and runjags [90]. We chose a Bayesian approach to draw inferences from general linear mixed models. We determined whether testosterone concentrations, latency to attack an intruder and measures of song activity and structure differed between stonechats breeding at different latitudes (tropical or temperate) and breeding stages (a factor with up to six levels: pre-nesting, nest-building, egg-laying, incubation, feeding of nestlings, and feeding of fledglings, see below) and the interaction between latitude and breeding stage. In all models, we included population identity as a random intercept. Model parameters were estimated as the mean of their posterior distributions, and the 2.5% and 97.5% credible intervals. We used minimally informative priors for both mean (dnorm (0, 10^−6)) and variance (dgamma (0.001, 0.001)) parameters and MCMC simulations were run in two parallel chains for each parameter. Covariate predictor variables (time of day, body mass) were centered to a mean of zero and were removed from the models when they did not explain a detectable amount of variation in the data (i.e. Bayesian credible intervals included zero). Predictor variables that were part of the experimental set-up/hypothesis were always retained in the models. MCMC simulations were checked for convergence of chains using trace plots and psrf values [91]. Effective sample sizes were > 15000 in all cases. Model residuals were graphically checked for violations of model assumptions (normality, heteroscedasticity, autocorrelations). Sample sizes for the different measures differed, as for some individuals we obtained data on testosterone, behavior and song, while for other individuals we were only able to obtain one of these measures.

Testosterone levels were natural log-transformed prior to analysis and are reported on a natural log scale unless stated otherwise. In a first step, we analyzed all testosterone samples (n = 357) including also individuals without exact breeding stage information to determine the effect of latitude on testosterone levels. We categorized breeding phase as early, medium or late depending on sampling date and the exact breeding stage of some individuals and a less exact estimate in others. To test whether the relationship between breeding phase and testosterone differs between tropical and temperate latitudes, we included an interaction between latitude and breeding phase in the model. Furthermore, we controlled for capture method (playback yes/no), time of day, body mass, and age. In a second step, we reduced the data set to include only individuals with known breeding stage (n = 299). Because European stonechats have a longer breeding season, we only used data from the first brood to match the typical single-broodedness of African stonechats. As time of day at capture, capture method and body mass had no detectable influence on testosterone in the larger data-set (see below) we did not include them in this model.

Attack latency, song rate and frequency parameters were natural log-transformed prior to analysis. Breeding stage was categorized as a factor with four levels (pre-nesting, nest-building, egg-laying and incubation, feeding of nestlings and fledglings). We analyzed whether the latency to attack a simulated territorial intruder differed between males from Africa and Europe, considering breeding stage or the interaction of latitude and breeding stage. Lastly, we analyzed whether song rate, element rate and frequency parameters differed between African and European stonechats during combined pre-nesting and nest-building phases. As the models concerning behavioral data also included individuals that we were not able to catch, no other factors (e.g. age, body mass or capture method) were included in these models.

Results

Morphology

As expected from previous studies [92], stonechats from Africa were larger than stonechats from Europe (posterior means and credible intervals; tarsus length: Africa: 22.9 mm [22.8, 23.0], Europe: 22.4 mm [22.1, 22.6], difference: −0.5 mm [−0.7, −0.4]; wing length: Africa: 71.5 mm [71.3, 71.8], Europe: 68.2, [67.5, 68.9], difference: −3.4 mm [−3.8, −2.9]) and heavier (Africa: 16.2 g [16.1, 16.4], Europe: 14.5 g [14.1, 14.8], difference: −1.8 g [−2.0, −1.6]).

Testosterone levels

When all data were considered, testosterone concentrations were highest early in the breeding season and then declined gradually. This effect of breeding phase on testosterone concentrations was similar at different latitudes, and testosterone did not differ between tropical and temperate environments (Table 1, Fig. 1). Time of day (slope: −0.0002 [−0.0006, 0.0003]), capture method (difference playback no – yes: −0.2 [−0.5, 0.07]) and body mass (slope: 0.01 [−0.15, 0.17]) had no detectable influence on testosterone concentrations. Yearling males had lower testosterone levels than adult males (difference adult - yearling: -0.30 [-0.58, -0.02]).

Table 1.

Estimates of natural log-transformed testosterone concentrations of male stonechats in relation to latitude and breeding phase

Estimates and 95% credible intervals Estimates and 95% credible intervals Estimates and 95% credible intervals
Africa (intercept) Europe Difference Africa – Europe
Breeding phase
 Early (intercept) 7.18 [6.84, 7.51] 7.48 [6.74, 8.24] 0.31 [-0.12, 0.73]
 Medium 6.74 [6.11, 7.37] 7.05 [6.0, 8.1]
 Late 5.83 [5.05, 6.62] 6.13 [4.94, 7.35]

Columns show Bayesian estimates and credible intervals. When zero is not included within the credible intervals of the differences there is a detectable effect of this parameter on the dependent variable (printed in bold).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Testosterone concentrations (ng/ml) of African and European male stonechats in relation to breeding phase and population. Points and error bars (means and 95% credible intervals) represent African (white fills) and European (black fills) populations. Letters indicate the location of each population and full location names are displayed together with sample sizes in the legend

The model including only individuals for which exact breeding stage was known, revealed that at all latitudes testosterone concentrations were especially low during the nestling stages (Table 2, Fig. 2). They peaked during nest-building, but this peak appeared to be more pronounced in African than European stonechats (Table 2, Fig. 2). Within this data-set, testosterone concentrations did not differ between age classes (difference adult – yearling: −0.22 [−0.51, 0.07]).

Table 2.

Estimates of natural log-transformed testosterone concentrations of male stonechats in relation to latitude and detailed breeding stage

Estimates and 95% credible intervals Estimates and 95% credible intervals Estimates and 95% credible intervals
Breeding stage Africa (intercept) Europe Difference Africa – Europe
Pre-nesting (intercept) 6.96 [6.46, 7.46] 7.25 [6.15, 8.35] 0.29 [−0.31, 0.89]
Nest-building 8.36 [7.07, 9.64] 7.46 [5.59, 9.68] -0.9 [−1.84, 0.04]
Difference 1.39 [0.61, 2.18] 0.21 [−0.56, 1.33]
Egg-laying 6.33 [4.49, 8.13] 6.54 [3.26, 9.79] 0.21 [−1.23, 1.66]
Difference −0.66 [−1.97, 0.66] −0.71 [−2.89, 1.44]
Incubation 6.58 [5.44, 7.7] 6.83 [4.91, 8.74] 0.25 [−0.53, 1.04]
Difference −0.38 [−1.02, 0.25] −0.42 [−1.24, 0.39]
Nestlings 6.32 [5.16, 7.48] 6.26 [4.27, 8.26] -0.06 [−0.89, 0.78]
Difference 0.64 [−1.3, 0.02] −0.99 [−1.88, −0.09]
Fledglings 6.4 [5.23, 7.56] 6.55 [4.53, 8.56] 0.15 [−0.7, 1.00]
Difference −0.56 [−1.21, 0.1] −0.7 [−1.62, 0.21]

Columns show Bayesian estimates and credible intervals. Differences refer to differences from the pre-nesting intercept. When zero is not included within the credible intervals of the differences there is a detectable effect of this parameter on the dependent variable (printed in bold). Italic font indicates credible intervals that cross zero only marginally.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Testosterone concentrations (ng/ml) of African and European male stonechats during detailed breeding stages of the first brood. Especially in African stonechats, testosterone concentrations peaked during nest-building and were lowest when stonechats were feeding nestlings. Bars and error bars represent back-transformed posterior means and their 95% credible intervals. Dots represent data points of individuals from different populations. Populations were included as random intercepts in the models

Behavioral response to simulated territorial intrusions

Latency to attack a decoy did not depend on latitude (natural log-transformed difference Africa – Europe: 0.04 [−0.35, 0.44], Fig. 3), and changed similarly with breeding stage in tropical and temperate environments (intercept pre-nesting: 2.8 [2.4, 3.1], difference nest-building: −1.0 [−1.5, −0.4], incubation: 0.03 [−0.6, 0.5], feeding of nestlings and fledglings: −0.6 [−1.1, −0.1]). Males attacked the intruder faster during nest-building and parental stages than during pre-nesting (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Latency to attack (min) of African and European male stonechats in response to simulated territorial intrusions during different stages of the breeding season. Male stonechats attacked the decoy faster during nest-building and parental stages than during pre-nesting and incubation. Males that did not attack the decoy were assigned a latency of 60 min. Bars and error bars represent back-transformed posterior means and their 95% credible intervals. Dots represent data points of individuals from different populations

Song

Several song traits differed between African and European stonechats during the mating phase (pre-nesting and nest-building). European stonechats sang with a higher song rate (natural log-transformed difference Africa – Europe: 0.64 [0.27, 1.01]), element rate (0.66 [0.43, 0.9]), and peak frequency (0.13 [0.07, 0.19]) than African stonechats (Fig. 4). Minimum and maximum frequencies did not differ between African and European stonechats (natural log-transformed difference Africa – Europe: 0.15 [−0.12, 0.42]; 0.08 [−0.009, 0.17]).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Song traits of African and European stonechats with a) song rate, b) element rate per song, and c) peak frequency. European stonechats sang with a higher song rate, element rate, and peak frequency during the mating period than African stonechats. Filled points and error bars represent back-transformed posterior means and 95% credible intervals. Sample sizes are displayed below error bars and are similar for element rate and peak frequency. Open symbols represent data points of individuals from different populations

Discussion

This comparative study on stonechats revealed that despite their slow pace of life, but in accordance with their seasonal breeding behavior, males of several populations of tropical African stonechats expressed similar peak concentrations of testosterone as male temperate European stonechats. These findings are in accordance with results from meta-analyses that suggested that tropical and temperate seasonal breeders express similar peak testosterone concentrations and that the length of the breeding season may be a better predictor of maximum testosterone concentrations than latitude [29, 39]. Our findings also generally confirm earlier data on temperate and tropical stonechats, which demonstrated seasonal peaks during nest-building when females are fertile [36, 74]. In contrast to Goymann et al. [36], our more extensive study found no evidence for overall lower levels of testosterone in tropical compared to temperate stonechats.

In our study, seasonal changes in testosterone were similar in tropical and temperate stonechats and were associated with seasonal changes in territorial aggression: both temperate and tropical stonechats had the highest testosterone levels and were most aggressive during nest-building – the period of highest female fertility. In contrast, song performance at the beginning of the breeding season was lower in African than in European stonechats and differences in song were not associated with variation in testosterone levels between tropical and temperate stonechats. This indicates that breeding season length has a stronger influence on testosterone and the expression of aggressive behaviors than the type of territoriality or pace of life. Short seasonal breeding seasons usually lead to more synchronous breeding with more competition between males and potentially a higher risk of extra-pair copulations (breeding synchrony hypothesis, [93]). Synchrony is generally high in the stonechats we studied. For example, a previous study on African stonechats in Arusha National Park (which was also one of our study populations) found that stonechats bred during a short breeding season from mid-October to January and initiated laying within 2–4 weeks. Replacement clutches were initiated within 10 days after the loss of a nest [94]. The breeding season of European stonechats is a little bit longer, usually from mid-March/April to July [95]. Replacement of clutches occurs also within 10 days [96]. Thus, because stonechats breed during a short breeding season and replacement clutches occur in fast succession there are always many nests active at the same time. Therefore, extra-pair paternity may pose a high risk to males during the fertile period of their mates at all latitudes, and increased territorial aggression during this period may help to prevent extra-pair copulations. For example, in an experimental study in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis), testosterone treated males suffered fewer losses of paternity than control males [97]. Supporting the fitness relevance of testosterone during mating, earlier studies in European stonechats found that variation in testosterone concentrations during mating, but not during parental stages, was associated with reproductive success in temperate stonechats [72]. Similar to stonechats, testosterone concentrations also peaked during nest-building and mate guarding in temperate barn swallows (Hirundo rustica, [20]), blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus, [16]) and in tropical Seychelle warblers (Acrocephalus sechellensis, [98]). In stonechats and other species, territorial behavior outside the reproductive phase, does not appear to be mediated by testosterone. Both tropical and temperate stonechats also defend territories in a non-breeding context. However, antiandrogen blockers reduced territorial aggression in temperate stonechats during the breeding season, but had no influence on territorial behavior in a non-breeding context [8, 71]. A disconnection of territoriality from testosterone outside a breeding context was also reported in red-throated Ant-tanagers (Habia fuscicauda). In this neotropical species with high levels of extra-pair matings, territorial take-overs take mainly place outside the breeding season when testosterone levels are low and not during breeding when testosterone levels are high [34].

On a cautionary note, our conclusions on stonechats are based on simulated territorial intrusions with decoys and playback that originated from the overall region of a tested population, but not always from the tested population itself. Studies in other species found a low discrimination ability between local and non-local playback stimuli in males, but a high discrimination ability in females [99, 100]. However, male and female European stonechats have been shown to respond more strongly to playback and decoys from their own than from foreign populations [77]. In our study, behavioral experiments in European stonechats were conducted in Germany, but performed with playback from English populations and decoys from an Austrian population. Thus, we might have underestimated the aggressive response of temperate male stonechats towards simulated territorial intrusions, because using local playback and decoys may have elicited a stronger response. Nevertheless, 82% of all males attacked the decoy, which is higher than the probability of an aggressive response found in a previous study in the same populations where no song playback was used [24]. Furthermore, the latency to attack varied strongly with breeding stage and was lowest during nest-building, which all suggests that the birds responded appropriately. In contrast, in our African populations, we used local playback, but decoys also originated from other African populations. Hence, there was slightly greater local familiarity with the stimuli in African populations, but we may have also underestimated aggressive responses of African stonechats.

Nevertheless, the seasonal variation in attack latency seems to be robust and is very similar in tropical and temperate stonechats. Overall, we find it unlikely that the total lack of differences in aggression between the populations could have arisen from small differences in familiarity.

In contrast to the consistent association between seasonal aggression and seasonal testosterone in stonechats, in our study differences in seasonal changes in song behavior were not associated with those in seasonal changes in testosterone levels. At the beginning of the breeding season European stonechats sang with higher song and element rates and a higher peak frequency than African stonechats, despite similar testosterone levels. Thus, the main function of high levels of testosterone in stonechats and likely in many other tropical and temperate species seems facilitation of high intensity territorial aggression associated with the fertile period of the female. This conclusion is supported by other studies on temperate species that defend territories both in a breeding and a non-breeding context. These studies have shown that in these species song, territorial behavior and testosterone are also at least partially dissociated (e.g. [9, 23]).

Previous generalizations of low testosterone concentrations in tropical birds may have been biased by studies of neotropical or Australian species with extended breeding seasons [28, 101107]. Long breeding seasons can lead to low breeding synchrony [93] and low competition between pairs [105], potentially making high testosterone concentrations unnecessary. Studies comparing the variation in testosterone concentrations across temperate and tropical latitudes in closely related species or populations are rare. Apart from stonechats, studies have only been published on closely related taxa of the genus Zonotrichia that breed in the Neotropics and North America. The life history of neotropical high altitude rufous-collared sparrows (Z. capensis) is very similar to the life history of afrotropical stonechats: both are residents and breed seasonally at high altitudes and in open grassland habitats during a relatively short breeding season of 3–4 months [108]. Similar to tropical stonechats, equatorial rufous-collared sparrows have high peak testosterone levels that can be even higher than those of their temperate congeners [38]. However, testosterone implants or androgen blockers had no effect on territorial aggression during breeding [37, 109] – however, the exact breeding stage of individuals was not known. If in tropical seasonally breeding species the main function of testosterone is to support high intensity territorial aggression during female fertility, then testosterone or androgen blocker implants may only be effective during the fertile period of the female.

The differences in song performance which we report for tropical versus temperate stonechats may well be related to differences in territoriality and migratory behavior. Song characteristics play an important role during male-male interactions [56] and for mate attraction [57]. In European stonechats, as well as in many other species, song is under strong sexual selection [77] and may be used to communicate the males’ quality in a reproductive context [75]. At the beginning of the breeding season, populations with seasonal breeding territories, and in particular migratory species, are under temporarily more intense sexual selection than resident populations with year-round territories and may have to invest more into sexually selected traits such as song [61]. Specifically, a high song and element rate and a high peak frequency may accelerate the acquisition of a territory and a mate, and may promote early laying, which is crucial for breeding success in many species [110]. Thus, high performance in these traits at the beginning of the breeding season is presumably particularly important for the reproductive success of European stonechats, which during these stages acquire both, territories and mates. The overall greater song performance of European stonechats fits well with evidence that song performance is increased in response to selective pressures [69]. To the best of our knowledge, only one study so far has compared song structure between tropical and temperate populations within the same taxon: similar to stonechats, neotropical house wrens (Troglodytes aedon) sang fewer elements at a slower rate than temperate house wrens [68]. Nevertheless, although selective pressure might not be as high as in temperate migrants, tropical stonechats and several other seasonally breeding tropical species sing more during the breeding season than outside a breeding context [35, 37, 74, 111114]. To clarify the role of testosterone for song behaviour in stonechats, studies using androgen and estrogen receptor blockers are necessary [115].

In seasonally breeding species, regardless of latitude, short peaks of testosterone may help to avoid costs associated with high levels of this hormone, including potential impairment of the immune system, survival and male parental care [31, 32]. Several mechanisms have been suggested that may allow species to benefit from the positive effects of testosterone on reproductive performance, but avoid the associated costs [31]. As reported here, the peak in testosterone may be restricted to a short period of time (resistance hypothesis, [31]). Additionally, the brain may become insensitive to testosterone outside this brief period of sexual activity (insensitivity hypothesis, [31]). These two mechanisms need not be coupled. For example, in seasonally breeding tropical rufous-collared sparrows, which naturally sustain only a short peak in testosterone, most males stopped taking care of young after they had been implanted with testosterone pellets, indicating that they had not become insensitive to the hormone [116].

In some species with long breeding seasons, testosterone concentrations increase only during social interactions or periods of social instability (social modulation hypothesis, [31]). For example, in tropical spotted antbirds (Hylophylax n. naevioides) testosterone concentrations were very low during breeding, but rose transiently during prolonged simulated territorial intrusions [105]. Alternatively, tropical seasonal breeders, like the stonechats in this study, may especially benefit from brief periods of high testosterone levels and low testosterone concentrations throughout the rest of the breeding season. They could reap the benefits, such as prevention of extra-pair copulations, while not threatening the advantage of their slow, tropical pace of life [28], exemplified by greater longevity and extended parental care [81, 117, 118]. Whether tropical male stonechats are also additionally behaviorally insensitive to testosterone during parenting could be tested through testosterone implant studies.

Conclusions

This is the first study that assessed the role of testosterone, territorial behavior and song concomitantly in several closely related taxa and populations of a songbird breeding at different latitudes from Europe to the Afrotropics. In both European and African male stonechats, a strong territorial response during the fertile period of females coincided with high testosterone concentrations. However, European males sang with a higher song and element rate during the pre-nesting and nest-building phase than African males, despite similar testosterone levels. Thus, in male tropical stonechats seasonal breeding and short breeding seasons may have led to similar peak testosterone concentrations and territorial aggression as in temperate stonechats. However, year-round territories and stable pair-bonds have reduced the pressure of finding a new mate at the beginning of the breeding season in tropical stonechats, which is reflected in a lower song performance in African compared to European male stonechats. Future studies should determine the role of extra-pair paternity in temperate and tropical stonechats as a potential evolutionary driver of high testosterone peaks when females are fertile. Further, as the data collected in this study are correlational, experimental approaches using androgen and estrogen receptor blockers to determine causal relationships between testosterone and behavior are necessary. This kind of information is particularly rare in tropical species, and especially for studies that also investigate song structure. Taken together, our study suggests that breeding in seasonal environments during short breeding seasons is a major selective force for high peak testosterone levels of male birds independent of breeding latitude and variation in pace of life, and that particular behaviors, in our case song performance, can be uncoupled from peak testosterone levels.

Acknowledgments

We thank Wolfgang Goymann and Monika Trappschuh for their immunoassay expertise and for conducting the testosterone measurements. We want to thank John O′Halloran, Pat Smiddy, Guillermo López, Musa Makomba, Jack Kiiru, Isaac (Monduli), Julius (Pare) and Ann Gathitu for help with the fieldwork and two anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on previous versions of the manuscript. We also thank the National Museums of Kenya (NMK) and the Tanzania Wildlife Research Institute (TAWIRI) for providing support for fieldwork in Kenya and Tanzania.

Funding

This work was supported by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation (BA), the German Science foundation (DFG, research grant to BH grant HE3488/5–1) and the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology (testosterone assays). This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the Technical University of Munich (TUM) in the framework of the Open Access Publishing Program.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available as Additional file 1.

Authors' contributions

BA, KGM and BH conceived of and designed the project. BA, KGM, HF, JCI, JK collected the data. BA and KGM processed and analysed the data. BA wrote the manuscript with essential input from KGM and BH. BA designed the figures. All authors read, edited, and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All experimental procedures were approved by the governmental authorities of the respective countries (Germany: Regional Government of North Rhine Westphalia and Vogelwarte Helgoland License No. 0147; Ireland: License No. C092/2013; Kenya: National Commission for Science, Technology and Innovation (NACOSTI); Spain: Regional Governments of Junta de Castilla y León (EP-CYL-33-2013) and Andalucía (35–36/13); Tanzania: Tanzania Wildlife Research Institute (TAWIRI) and Tanzania Commission for Science and Technology (COSTECH)).

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Abbreviation

STI

Simulated territorial intrusion

Additional file

Additional file 1: Table S1. (99.2KB, xlsx)

Datasets. (XLSX 99 kb)

Footnotes

Beate Apfelbeck and Kim G. Mortega contributed equally to this work.

References

  • 1.Nelson RJ. An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology. Third Edition. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Adkins-Regan E. Hormones and animal social behavior. Hormones and animal social behavior. Princeton University Press: Princeton; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Silverin B. Effects of long-acting testosterone treatment on freeliving pied flycatchers, Ficedula hypoleuca, during the breeding period. Anim Behav. 1980;28:906–912. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80152-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ketterson ED, Nolan JV, Wolf L, Ziegenfus C. Testosterone and avian life histories: effects of experimentally elevated testosterone on behavior and correlates of fitness in the dark-eyed junco (Junco hyemalis) Am Nat. 1992;140:980–999. doi: 10.1086/285451. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.De Ridder E, Pinxten R, Eens M. Experimental evidence of a testosterone-induced shift from paternal to mating behaviour in a facultatively polygynous songbird. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2000;49:24–30. doi: 10.1007/s002650000266. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Boseret G, Carere C, Ball GF, Balthazart J. Social context affects testosterone-induced singing and the volume of song control nuclei in male canaries (Serinus canaria) J Neurobiol. 2006;66:1044–1060. doi: 10.1002/neu.20268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hau M, Wikelski M, Soma KK, Wingfield JC. Testosterone and year-round territorial aggression in a tropical bird. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2000;117:20–33. doi: 10.1006/gcen.1999.7390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Canoine V, Gwinner E. Seasonal differences in the hormonal control of territorial aggression in free-living European stonechats. Horm Behav. 2002;41:1–8. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schwabl H, Kriner E. Territorial aggression and song of male European robins (Erithacus rubecula) in autumn and spring: Effects of antiandrogen treatment. Horm Behav. 1991;25:180–194. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(91)90049-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sperry TS, Wacker DW, Wingfield JC. The role of androgen receptors in regulating territorial aggression in male song sparrows. Horm Behav. 2010;57:86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Van Duyse E, Pinxten R, Snoeijs T, Eens M. Simultaneous treatment with an aromatase inhibitor and an anti-androgen decreases the likelihood of dawn song in free-living male great tits. Parus major. Horm Behav. 2005;48:243–251. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Greig-Smith PW. Seasonal patterns of song production by male stonechats (Saxicola torquata) Ornis Scand. 1982;13:225–231. doi: 10.2307/3676303. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Stefanski RA. Utilization of the breeding territory in the black-capped chickadee. The Condor. 1967;69:259–267. doi: 10.2307/1366315. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bukacinska M, Bukacinski D. Seasonal and diurnal changes in aggression and territory size in the black-headed gull (Larus ridibundus) on islands in the middle reaches of the vistula river. Ethology. 1994;97:329–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1994.tb01051.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.McDonald PG. The function of vocalisations and aggressive behaviour used by male Rufous Whistlers,Pachycephala rufiventris. Emu. 2001;101:65–72. doi: 10.1071/MU00063. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Foerster K, Poesel A, Kunc H, Kempenaers B. The natural plasma testosterone profile of male blue tits during the breeding season and its relation to song output. J Avian Biol. 2002;33:269–275. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-048X.2002.330309.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bruni A, Foote JR. Dawn singing of Eastern phoebes varies with breeding stage and brood number. Wilson J Ornithol. 2014;126:500–507. doi: 10.1676/13-175.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mace R. The dawn chorus in the great tit, Parus major, is directly related to female fertility. Nature. 1987;330:745–746. doi: 10.1038/330745a0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Osiejuk TS, Kuczynski L. Song functions and territoriality in Eurasian treecreeper, Certhia familiaris, and short-toed treecreeper Certhia brachydactyla. Acta Ornithol (Wars) 2000;35:109–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Saino N, Moller AP. Testosterone correlates of mate guarding, singing and aggressive behaviour in male barn swallows, Hirundo rustica. Anim Behav. 1995;49:465–472. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1995.0060. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Van Duyse E, Pinxten R, Eens M. Seasonal fluctuations in plasma testosterone levels and diurnal song activity in free-living male great tits. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2003;134:1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0016-6480(03)00213-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Beletsky LD, Orians OH, Wingfield JC. Relationships of steroid hormones and polygyny to territorial status, breeding experience, and reproductive success in male red-winged blackbirds. The Auk. 1989;106:107–117. doi: 10.2307/4087763. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Apfelbeck B, Mortega K, Kiefer S, Kipper S, Vellema M, Villavicencio CP, Gahr M, Goymann W. Associated and disassociated patterns in hormones, song, behavior and brain receptor expression between life-cycle stages in male black redstarts,Phoenicurus ochruros. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2013;184:93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.11.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Schwabl H, Flinks H, Gwinner E. Testosterone, reproductive stage, and territorial behavior of male and female European stonechats Saxicola torquata. Horm Behav. 2005;47:503–512. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wingfield JC, Hahn TP. Testosterone and territorial behaviour in sedentary and migratory sparrows. Anim Behav. 1994;47:77–89. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hau M, Goymann W. Endocrine mechanisms, behavioral phenotypes and plasticity: known relationships and open questions. 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hau M, Gill SA, Goymann W. Tropical field endocrinology: ecology and evolution of testosterone concentrations in male birds. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2008;157:241–248. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hau M, Ricklefs RE, Wikelski M, Lee KA, Brawn JD. Corticosterone, testosterone and life-history strategies of birds. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010;277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 29.Goymann W, Moore Ignacio T, Scheuerlein A, Hirschenhauser K, Grafen A, Wingfield JC. Testosterone in tropical birds: effects of environmental and social factors. Am Nat. 2004;164:327–334. doi: 10.1086/422856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Garamszegi LZ, Hirschenhauser K, Bokony V, Eens M, Hurtrez-Boussès S, Møller AP, Oliveira RF, Wingfield JC. Latitudinal distribution, migration, and testosterone levels in birds. Am Nat. 2008;172:533–546. doi: 10.1086/590955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wingfield JC, Lynn SE, Soma KK. Avoiding the costs of testosterone: ecological bases of hormone-behavior interactions. Brain Behav Evol. 2001;57:239. doi: 10.1159/000047243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lynn SE. Endocrine and neuroendocrine regulation of fathering behavior in birds. Horm Behav. 2016;77:237–248. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ricklefs RE, Wikelski M. The physiology/life-history nexus. Trends Ecol Evol. 2002;17:462–468. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02578-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Chiver I, Stutchbury BJM, Morton ES. Seasonal variation in male testosterone levels in a tropical bird with year-round territoriality. J Field Ornithol. 2014;85:1–9. doi: 10.1111/jofo.12044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Addis EA, Busch DS, Clark AD, Wingfield JC. Seasonal and social modulation of testosterone in Costa Rican rufous-collared sparrows (Zonotrichia capensis costaricensis) Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2010;166:581–589. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Goymann W, Geue D, Schwabl I, Flinks H, Schmidl D, Schwabl H, Gwinner E. Testosterone and corticosterone during the breeding cycle of equatorial and European stonechats (Saxicola torquata axillaris and S. t. rubicola) Horm Behav. 2006;50:779–785. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Moore IT, Wada H, Perfito N, Busch DS, Hahn TP, Wingfield JC. Territoriality and testosterone in an equatorial population of rufous-collared sparrows, Zonotrichia capensis. Anim Behav. 2004;67:411–420. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Moore IT, Perfito N, Wada H, Sperry TS, Wingfield JC. Latitudinal variation in plasma testosterone levels in birds of the genus Zonotrichia. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2002;129:13–19. doi: 10.1016/S0016-6480(02)00563-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Goymann W, Landys MM. Testosterone and year-round territoriality in tropical and non-tropical songbirds. J Avian Biol. 2011;42:485–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2011.05464.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Stutchbury BJ and Morton ES. Behavioral ecology of tropical birds. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2001.
  • 41.Newton I. Migration ecology of birds. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2008.
  • 42.Stutchbury BJM, Morton ES. Recent advances in the behavioral ecology of tropical birds. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology. 2008;120:26–37. doi: 10.1676/07-018.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bonier F, Eikenaar C, Martin PR, Moore IT. Extrapair paternity rates vary with latitude and elevation in Emberizid sparrows. Am Nat. 2014;183:54–61. doi: 10.1086/674130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Eikenaar C, Bonier F, Martin PR, Moore IT. High rates of extra-pair paternity in two equatorial populations of rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis. J Avian Biol. 2013;44:600–602. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2013.00212.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goymann W, Helm B. Seasonality of life histories in tropical birds: Circannual rhythms and Zeitgeber., in Annual, lunar, and tidal clocks, Hideharu Numata and B. Helm, Editors. Tokyo: Springer; 2013;247–275.
  • 46.Olson DM, Dinerstein E, Wikramanayake ED, Burgess ND, Powell GVN, Underwood EC, D'Amico JA, Itoua I, Strand HE, Morrison JC, Loucks CJ, Allnutt TF, Ricketts TH, Kura Y, Lamoreux JF, Wettengel WW, Hedao P, Kassem KR. Terrestrial ecoregions of the world: a new map of life on earth: a new global map of terrestrial ecoregions provides an innovative tool for conserving biodiversity. Bioscience. 2001;51:933–938. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2001)051[0933:TEOTWA]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Promislow DEL, Harvey PH. Living fast and dying young: A comparative analysis of life-history variation among mammals. Journal of Zoology. 1990;220:417–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1990.tb04316.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Jetz W, Sekercioglu CH, Böhning-Gaese K. The worldwide variation in avian clutch size across species and space. PLoS Biol. 2008;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wiersma P, Muñoz-Garcia A, Walker A, Williams JB. Tropical birds have a slow pace of life. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2007;104:9340–9345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702212104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Møller AP. Senescence in relation to latitude and migration in birds. J Evol Biol. 2007;20:750–757. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01236.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Peach WJ, Hanmer DB, Oatley TB. Do southern African songbirds live longer than their European counterparts? Oikos. 2001;93:235–249. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2001.930207.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Spottiswoode C, Moller AP. Extrapair paternity, migration, and breeding synchrony in birds. Behav Ecol. 2004;15:41–57. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arg100. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kunc HP, Amrhein V, Naguib M. Seasonal variation in dawn song characteristics in the common nightingale. Anim Behav. 2005;70:1265–1271. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Otter K, Ratcliffe L. Changes in singing behaviour of male black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) following mate removal. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1993;33:409–414. doi: 10.1007/BF00170256. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Gil D, Gahr M. The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends Ecol Evol. 2002;17:133–141. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02410-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Searcy WA, Beecher MD. Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds. Anim Behav. 2009;78:1281–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.08.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Catchpole CK, Slater PJB. Bird Song. Biological Themes and Variations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Leitner S, Voigt C, Gahr M. Seasonal changes in the song pattern of the non-domesticated island canary (Serinus canaria), a field study. Behaviour. 2001;138:885–904. doi: 10.1163/156853901753172700. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Nolan PM, Hill GE. Female choice for song characteristics in the house finch. Anim Behav. 2004;67:403–410. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Alatalo RV, Glynn C, Lundberg A. Singing rate and female attraction in the pied flycatcher - an experiment. Anim Behav. 1990;39:601–603. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80430-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Collins SA, de Kort SR, Perez-Tris J, Luis TJ. Migration strategy and divergent sexual selection on bird song. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;276:585–590. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Botero CA, Boogert NJ, Vehrencamp SL, Lovette IJ. Climatic patterns predict the elaboration of song displays in mockingbirds. Curr Biol. 2009;19:1151–1155. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.04.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Catchpole CK. Sexual selection and the evolution of complex songs among European warblers of the genus Acrocephalus. Behaviour. 1980;74:149–166. doi: 10.1163/156853980X00366. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cardoso GC, Hu Y, Mota PG. Birdsong, sexual selection, and the flawed taxonomy of canaries, goldfinches and allies. Anim Behav. 2012;84:111–119. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Greig EI, Price JJ, Pruett-Jones S. Song evolution in Maluridae: influences of natural and sexual selection on acoustic structure. Emu. 2013;113:270–281. doi: 10.1071/MU12078. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Lawson AM, Weir JT. Latitudinal gradients in climatic-niche evolution accelerate trait evolution at high latitudes. Ecol Lett. 2014;17:1427–1436. doi: 10.1111/ele.12346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Weir JT, Wheatcroft D. A latitudinal gradient in rates of evolution of avian syllable diversity and song length. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;278:1713–1720. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kaluthota C, Brinkman BE, dos Santos EB, Rendall D. Transcontinental latitudinal variation in song performance and complexity in house wrens (Troglodytes aedon). Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 69.Read AF, Weary DM. The evolution of bird song: comparative analyses. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1992;338:165–187. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0137. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Urquhart ED. Stonechats. London: Christopher Helm; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Marasco V, Fusani L, Dessì-Fulgheri F, Canoine V. Non-migratory stonechats show seasonal changes in the hormonal regulation of non-seasonal territorial aggression. Horm Behav. 2011;60:414–419. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Apfelbeck B, Flinks H, Goymann W. Variation in circulating testosterone during mating predicts reproductive success in a wild songbird. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2016;4:107. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Apfelbeck B, Flinks H, Goymann W. Territorial aggression does not feed back on testosterone in a multiple-brooded songbird species with breeding and non-breeding season territoriality, the European stonechat. Horm Behav. 2017;87:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Dittami JP, Gwinner E. Annual cycles in the African stonechat Saxicola torquata axillaris and their relationship to environmental factors. Journal of Zoology. 1985;207:357–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1985.tb04937.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Greig-Smith PW. Song-rates and parental care by individual male stonechats (Saxicola torquata) Anim Behav. 1982;30:245–252. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80261-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Rödl T. The wintering of territorial stonechat pairs, Saxicola torquata, in Israel. Journal für Ornithologie. 1995;136:423–433. doi: 10.1007/BF01651589. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Mortega KG, Flinks H, Helm B. Behavioural response of a migratory songbird to geographic variation in song and morphology. Front Zool. 2014;11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 78.Gwinner E, König S, Haley CS. Genetic and environmental factors influencing clutch size in equatorial and temperate zone stonechats (Saxicola torquata axillaris and S. T. rubicola): an experimental study. The Auk. 1995;112:748–755. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Tieleman BI, Versteegh MA, Fries A, Helm B, Dingemanse NJ, Gibbs HL, Williams JB. Genetic modulation of energy metabolism in birds through mitochondrial function. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;276:1685–1693. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Wikelski M, Spinney L, Schelsky W, Scheuerlein A, Gwinner E. Slow pace of life in tropical sedentary birds: a common-garden experiment on four stonechat populations from different latitudes. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;270:2383–2388. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Koenig S, Gwinner E, Helm B. Programmed patterns of territorial aggression in first-year European and African stonechats (Saxicola torquata rubicola and S. t. axillaris) Ökologie der Vögel. 2002;24:579–594. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Birkhead TR, Møller AP. Why do male birds stop copulating while their partners are still fertile? Anim Behav. 1993;45:105–118. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Flinks H. Die Altersbestimmung des Schwarzkehlchens Saxicola torquata rubicola an Gefiedermerkmalen. Limicola. 1999;8:28–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Goymann W, Schwabl I, Trappschuh M, Hau M. Use of ethanol for preserving steroid and indoleamine hormones in bird plasma. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2007;150:191–195. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2006.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Apfelbeck B, Goymann W. Ignoring the challenge? Male black redstarts (Phoenicurus ochruros) do not increase testosterone levels during territorial conflicts but they do so in response to gonadotropin-releasing hormone. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;278:3233–3242. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.0098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Helm B. Geographically distinct reproductive schedules in a changing world: costly implications in captive Stonechats. Integr Comp Biol. 2009;49:563–579. doi: 10.1093/icb/icp037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Zollinger SA, Podos J, Nemeth E, Goller F, Brumm H. On the relationship between, and measurement of, amplitude and frequency in birdsong. Anim Behav. 2012;84:E1–E9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Team RC. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Plummer M. JAGS: a program for analysis of Bayesian graphical models using Gibbs sampling. In: Hornik K, Leisch F, Zeileis A, eds. Proceedings of the 3rd International Workshop on Distributed Statistical Computing. Vienna: Technische Universität Wien; 2003.
  • 90.Denwood MJ. runjags: An R package providing interface utilities, parallel computing methods and additional distributions for MCMC models in JAGS. Journal of Statistical Software. 2016;71:1–25. doi: 10.18637/jss.v071.i09. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Brooks SP, Gelman A. General methods for monitoring convergence of iterative simulations. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics. 1997;7:434–455. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Klaassen M. Moult and basal metabolic costs in males of two subspecies of stonechats: the European Saxicola torquata rubicula and the East African S. t. axillaris. Oecologia. 1995;104:424–432. doi: 10.1007/BF00341339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Stutchbury BJ, Morton ES. The effect of breeding synchrony on extra-pair mating systems in songbirds. Behaviour. 1995;132:675–690. doi: 10.1163/156853995X00081. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Scheuerlein A. Control of reproduction in a tropical bird, the stonechat (Saxicola torquata axillaris) Shaker Verlag: Aachen; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Flinks H, Helm B, Rothery P. Plasticity of moult and breeding schedules in migratory European Stonechats (Saxicola rubicola). Ibis. 2008;9999
  • 96.Raess M. Annual timing and life-history variation in free-living stonechats. LMU München: Munich; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Raouf SA, Parker PG, Ketterson ED, Nolan V, Ziegenfus C. Testosterone affects reproductive success by influencing extra-pair fertilizations in male dark-eyed juncos (Aves: Junco hyemalis) Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1997;264:1599–1603. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0223. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Van de Crommenacker J, Richardson DS, Groothuis TGG, Eising CM, Dekker AL, Komdeur J. Testosterone, cuckoldry risk and extrapair opportunities in the Seychelles warbler. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;271:1023–1031. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Danner JE, Danner RM, Bonier F, Martin PR, Small TW, Moore IT. Female, but not male, tropical sparrows respond more strongly to the local song dialect: implications for population divergence. Am Nat. 2011;178:53–63. doi: 10.1086/660283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Nelson DA, Soha JA. Male and female white-crowned sparrows respond differently to geographic variation in song. Behaviour. 2004;141:53–69. doi: 10.1163/156853904772746600. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Gill SA, Costa LM, Hau M. Males of a single-brooded tropical bird species do not show increases in testosterone during social challenges. Horm Behav. 2008;54:115–124. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Fedy BC, Stutchbury BJM, Vega Rivera JH. Testosterone does not increase in response to conspecific challenges in the white-bellied antbird (Myrmezica longipes), a resident tropical passerine. The Auk. 2006;123:61–66. doi: 10.1642/0004-8038(2006)123[0061:TDNIIR]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Levin RN, Wingfield JC. The hormonal control of territorial aggression in tropical birds. Ornis Scand. 1992;23:284–291. doi: 10.2307/3676651. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Peters A, Astheimer LB, Cockburn A. The annual testosterone profile in cooperatively breeding superb fairy-wrens, Malurus cyaneus, reflects their extreme infidelity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2001;50:519–527. doi: 10.1007/s002650100403. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Wikelski M, Hau M, Wingfield JC. Social instability increases plasma testosterone in a year-round territorial neotropical bird. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;266:551–556. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0671. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Wikelski M, Hau M, Douglas Robinson W, Wingfield JC. Reproductive seasonality of seven neotropical passerine species. The Condor. 2003;105:683–695. doi: 10.1650/7251. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Wiley CJ, Goldizen AW. Testosterone is correlated with courtship but not aggression in the tropical buff-banded rail, Gallirallus philippensis. Horm Behav. 2003;43:554–560. doi: 10.1016/S0018-506X(03)00066-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Moore IT, Bonier F, Wingfield JC. Reproductive asynchrony and population divergence between two tropical bird populations. Behav Ecol. 2005;16:755–762. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ari049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Moore IT, Walker BG, Wingfield JC. The effects of combined aromatase inhibitor and anti-androgen on male territorial aggression in a tropical population of rufous-collared sparrows, Zonotrichia capensis. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2004;135:223–229. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2003.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Velmala W, Helle S, Ahola MP, Klaassen M, Lehikoinen E, Rainio K, Sirkiä PM, Laaksonen T. Natural selection for earlier male arrival to breeding grounds through direct and indirect effects in a migratory songbird. Ecology and Evolution. 2015;5:1205–1213. doi: 10.1002/ece3.1423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Class AM, Moore IT. Seasonality of territorial aggression in male tropical resident rufous-collared sparrows (Zonotrichia capensis) Ornitol Neotrop. 2011;22:89–102. [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Chiver I, Stutchbury BJ, Morton ES. The function of seasonal song in a tropical resident species, the red-throated ant-tanager (Habia fuscicauda) J Ornithol. 2015;156:55–63. doi: 10.1007/s10336-014-1139-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Wikelski M, Hau M, Wingfield JC. Seasonality of reproduction in a neotropical rain forest bird. Ecology. 2000;81:2458–2472. doi: 10.1890/0012-9658(2000)081[2458:SORIAN]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Quispe R, Sèbe F, da Silva ML, Gahr M. Dawn-song onset coincides with increased HVC androgen receptor expression but is decoupled from high circulating testosterone in an equatorial songbird. Physiol Behav. 2016;156:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Apfelbeck B, Kiefer S, Mortega KG, Goymann W, Kipper S. Testosterone affects song modulation during simulated territorial intrusions in male black redstarts (Phoenicurus ochruros) PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e52009. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Lynn SE, Prince LE, Schook DM, Moore IT. Supplementary testosterone inhibits paternal care in a tropically breeding sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2009;82:699–708. doi: 10.1086/605915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Russell EM, Yom-Tov Y, Geffen E. Extended parental care and delayed dispersal: northern, tropical, and southern passerines compared. Behav Ecol. 2004;15:831–838. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arh088. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Gill SA, Haggerty TM. A comparison of life-history and parental care in temperate and tropical wrens. J Avian Biol. 2012;43:461–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2012.05637.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available as Additional file 1.


Articles from BMC Evolutionary Biology are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES