Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2017 Apr 19;12(4):e0170541. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170541

The evolution of cost-efficiency in neural networks during recovery from traumatic brain injury

Arnab Roy 1, Rachel A Bernier 1, Jianli Wang 2, Monica Benson 1, Jerry J French Jr 1, David C Good 3, Frank G Hillary 1,3,4,*
Editor: Emmanuel Andreas Stamatakis5
PMCID: PMC5396850  PMID: 28422992

Abstract

A somewhat perplexing finding in the systems neuroscience has been the observation that physical injury to neural systems may result in enhanced functional connectivity (i.e., hyperconnectivity) relative to the typical network response. The consequences of local or global enhancement of functional connectivity remain uncertain and this is particularly true for the overall metabolic cost of the network. We examine the hyperconnectivity hypothesis in a sample of 14 individuals with TBI with data collected at approximately 3, 6, and 12 months following moderate and severe TBI. As anticipated, individuals with TBI showed increased network strength and cost early after injury, but by one-year post injury hyperconnectivity was more circumscribed to frontal DMN and temporal-parietal attentional control regions. Cost in these subregions was a significant predictor of cognitive performance. Cost-efficiency analysis in the Power 264 data parcellation suggested that at 6 months post injury the network requires higher cost connections to achieve high efficiency as compared to the network 12 months post injury. These results demonstrate that networks self-organize to re-establish connectivity while balancing cost-efficiency trade-offs.

1. Introduction

Each year in the United States there are approximately 1.5 million to 2 million cases of traumatic brain injury (TBI) resulting from an external source and causing significant physical, cognitive, and psychosocial impairment [1]. To aid in understanding the behavioral deficits associated with TBI, over the last decade, blood oxygen level dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD-fMRI, or fMRI) has been widely used to study functional connectivity changes associated with injury. In this literature functional connectivity strength has commonly been defined as the degree of temporal-correlation in the fMRI signals recorded from two identified regions in the brain [2]. Brain connectivity studies have provided novel insights into the influence of TBI on neural networks during motor learning (see [3]), working memory and attentional control [46] and the relationship between distinct subnetworks and cognitive performance (e.g., default mode network, DMN and salience network, SN) [4,710]. Here our goal is to use BOLD-fMRI to advance this literature in three important ways. First we examine the phenomenon of functional hyperconnectivity (discussed below) with focus on the timing when it occurs during the first year of recovery after TBI, and the regions where it is most likely to be expressed. The second goal was to examine the inter-relationship between network cost and its achieved efficiency (or cost-efficiency relationship) during the recovery period. Finally, we examine hyperconnectivity in the context of cognitive performance. In doing so this study represents an opportunity to examine global/local neural network characteristics at three time points during the first year following moderate and severe TBI.

1.1 Functional hyperconnectivity

While much work using connectivity modeling in neurological disorders has focused on specific relationships between localized neuropathology and loss of functional connectivity (see [1113] and for review see [14]) one commonly observed response to injury and disease is enhanced functional connectivity (henceforth referred to as hyperconnectivity). For the purposes of this paper, we will define hyperconnectivity as the increase in the strength of functional connections in the brain after injury relative to a healthy control (HC) sample. Given that the pathophysiology observed in moderate and severe TBI results in disruption of neural signaling, functional hyperconnectivity is a counterintuitive neural network response. In order to understand the nature of hyperconnectivity after TBI, we aim to examine distributed networks and document the evolution of connectivity changes during recovery with specific interest in the relationship between hyperconnectivity after TBI and network cost that remains unexplored.

1.2 Network cost and efficiency

Human brain functioning is the result of a finely tuned orchestra of billions of neurons oscillating across different spatio-temporal scales to achieve communication (i.e., connectivity). The functional connections achieved during states of signal transfer in a healthy brain are embedded within networks tuned to achieve optimal balance between network cost and routing efficiency [1518]. We anticipate that the functional changes evident post injury and, in particular hyperconnectivity, may disrupt this optimal balance between the cost and efficiency. If it is the case that neurological disruption results in enhanced connectivity either in the magnitude or number of connections, even in select regions within the brain, then this should have important implications for overall network cost. The cost of the functional network can be defined using the total number or the weight of the connections in the functional network. This traditional way of defining network cost has one important drawback: it fails to consider the physical length of a functional-connection which has important implications for the latencies of neural transmission [1920] and for the metabolic cost of signaling [21].

To capture network cost after injury in the current study and its consequences for efficiency, we include several novel metrics: for cost we summarize a) whole-brain network cost, and b) ROI-cost (see Section 2.6.3) and for cost-efficiency, we examine changes in global efficiency as a function of changes in cost. The cost metric is based upon three important properties of the functional network: the number of functional connections, the strength of each functional connection, and the physical length of each connection and this can be computed locally and globally. The goal is to determine if the relationship between cost and efficiency guides the evolution of neural network functioning during recovery from TBI. With the combined examination of connectivity strength and cost during a critical early window after moderate and severe TBI, the current study will advance our understanding of early network plasticity and its consequences for behavioral recovery.

1.3 Study goals and hypotheses

Our primary objective is to understand the early network response during the first year of recovery from moderate to severe TBI with specific focus on hyperconnectivity and negotiation of network cost and efficiency. In doing so, this will be the first study in TBI to examine neural network changes at three time points during the first year after injury. The primary hypothesis is that hyperconnectivity, defined as an increase in the number or strength of network connections, will be most evident during the early recovery period after TBI. With notable exceptions (see [2223]) prior findings that have reported enhanced connectivity within the default mode network (DMN) and the executive control network (ECN) after moderate to severe TBI [3,67,10, 2425], we hypothesize that hub regions within the DMN and ECN will primarily contribute to hyperconnectivity. Finally, we anticipate that network cost will be greater as a result of hyperconnectivity, but that networks are obliged to equilibrate cost-efficiency demands during recovery, so systems should gravitate to reduce network cost. As a result, we hypothesize that at one year post injury the functional network will achieve higher efficiency at a lower cost compared to the network early during the recovery period.

2. Materials and methods

2.1 Subjects

Subjects included 14 individuals who sustained moderate to severe traumatic brain injury between the ages of 18 and 36 with education ranging from 12 to 18 years, and 12 healthy controls of comparable age and education (see Table 1). A Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) of 3–8 at time of injury was indicative of severe injury and a GCS of 9–12 was indicative of moderate injury [26]. Two subjects who originally received a GCS of 14 and one subject who received a GCS of 15 were also included because their medical records indicated positive imaging findings of brain damage and/or mental status change during their acute inpatient stay. Patients were excluded if there were other concomitant injuries (e.g. orthopedic injuries or injury to the spinal cord) that would cause difficulty for the patients to stay still in the MRI environment. All subjects with TBI completed three separate sessions approximately 3, 6, and 12 months following injury, referred to as Time-1, Time-2, and Time-3. In order to determine the natural variation in network response over two measurements separated by time, we also collected MRI data for healthy controls from two MRI sessions separated by approximately 3 months (see Table 1). Each testing session included both MRI data collection and cognitive testing. The goal in the current study is to extend hyperconnectivity hypotheses from our previous work to examine metrics of cost and efficiency using whole-brain graph theoretical analysis and to include an additional time-point during the first year after injury. To provide context with regard to other published work from our laboratory examining longitudinal recovery in TBI, the data used in this study maintain partial overlap with Venkatesan et al. (9/14 TBI subjects [10]). However, the analysis in Venkatesan et al., was a focused seed-based analysis of the PCC and hippocampus at two time points. The current data do not overlap with recent longitudinal work [8] which made use of task-based fMRI data also only at two time points post injury. Moreover, the current fMRI data sets are completely distinct from other prior work examining task-based connectivity [24, 25] and the samples in those studies maintain either no overlap [25] or minimal overlap (2/14 TBI subjects [24]) with the current sample.

Table 1. Subject demographics and TBI injury profiles.

a. Demographics Mean HC (sd) Mean TBI (sd)
Age 32.00 (11.5) 26.07 (6.5)
Education 12.92 (1.73) 13.42 (2.43)
Gender 6 M; 6 F 7 M; 7 F
GCS N/A 7.92 (4.9)
Mos. post Time-1 N/A 3.36 (0.74)
Mos. post Time-2 N/A 6.64 (1.45)
b. Mechanisms of injury No. TBI (%)
Motor Vehicle Accident 7 (50)
Fall 4 (29)
Struck by Vehicle 3 (21)
c. Initial clinical CT/MRI findings No. TBI (%)
Diffuse Axonal Injury 7 (50)
Total Hematoma and/or Hemorrhage * 13 (93)
HH Left Lateralized 5 (36)
HH Right Lateralized 3 (21)
HH Bilateral 4 (29)
HH Frontal Lobe 12 (86
HH Temporal Lobe 6 (43)
HH Parietal Lobe 4 (29)
HH Occipital Lobe 2 (14)

GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale; Mos., months; No. TBI, number of TBI subjects; HH, Hematoma and/or Hemorrhage.

* One of the thirteen subjects with reports of intracranial hematoma and/or hemorrhage did not have a documented location of this finding

All conducted research was approved by the institutional review board and the Pennsylvania State University Office of Research Protections. Due to the nature of the study sample, some individuals included in the study demonstrated some level of cognitive impairment. However, all individuals in the study retained the ability to sign medical documents and/or functioned independently, so consent was accepted and caregiver consent was included when available.

2.2 Behavioral data

Each subject was administered a neuropsychological battery of tests at Time-1, and all TBI subjects were administered the same battery during subsequent time points. The battery was designed to examine cognitive domains associated with deficits in TBI, focusing on the domains of attention and working memory [27]. In order for the TBI sample to be capable of reliably completing this battery at 3-months post injury, the battery was focused on those tests most sensitive to deficits following TBI, including processing speed and working memory. Tests administered included the Visual Search and Attention Test (VSAT), WAIS-III Digit Span (Forward, Digits-F and Backward, Digits-B), Trail Making Test B (TMT), and Stroop Color and Color-Word (Stroop). Because normative data were not available for all tests (e.g., digit span forward v. backward, WAIS-III) mean and standard deviation of the TBI subjects’ raw scores were used during analyses. Independent samples t-test comparing mean performance at Time-1 for TBI with mean performance at time for HCs showed no differences in performance between groups. (see Table 2).

Table 2. Neuropsychological performance of TBI group.

Stroop mean (sd) VSAT mean (sd)
Color Time Color Total Color-Word Time Color-word Total Total
Time-1 62.15 (10.56) 111.69 (0.63) 105.62 (23.01) 106.83 (10.15) 110.08 (21.89)
Time-2 67.58 (16.64) 111.75 (0.62) 109.92 (11.44) 96.25 (25.69) 121.77 (25.90)
Time-3 61.23 (9.88) 111.62 (0.87) 109.25 (11.68) 102.42 (19.85) 122.46 (19.03)
Digits: mean (sd) Trails: mean (sd)
Forward Back Total A B
Time-1 10.69 (2.02) 6.92 (2.02) 17.62 (3.40) 29.31 (11.32) 61.38 (16.91)
Time-2 10.67 (1.96) 7.92 (1.75) 18.69 (3.35) 25.08 (5.99) 49.31 (14.26)
Time-3 10.84 (1,59) 7.23 (1.79) 18.08 (2.69) 21.23 (4.21) 46.08 (10.17)

Stroop C, color trial; Stroop CW, color/word interference trial; VSAT, visual search and attention task.

2.3 Functional data

Subjects were scanned using a Philips Achieva 3T scanner in the Department of Radiology at Hershey Medical Center, Hershey, PA (TBI n = 1; HC n = 6) or one of two identical Siemens Magnetom Trio 3T scanners (one at the Social, Life, and Engineering Sciences Imaging Center at the Pennsylvania State University in University Park, PA (TBI n = 1, HC = 4); the other at the Department of Radiology at Hershey Medical Center in Hershey, PA, (TBI n = 12; HC n = 2). TBI cases and HC cases were divided between the scanners and for repeated scanning, all subjects were scanned on the same machine across time points. Prior to scanning subjects were made aware of the importance of minimizing motion within the scanner. High resolution T1-weighted anatomical images of the brain were acquired with isotropic spatial resolution of 1.0 mm. Echo planar imaging (EPI) was used to examine the blood oxygen level dependent response for functioning imaging. Imaging parameters for EPI were 2000 ms/30 ms/90° (repetition time/echo time/flip angle), 240 × 240 mm2 field of view, and 80 × 80 acquisition matrix with 4 mm-thick axial slices parallel to the bicommissural line. This study demanded data collection over seven years using more than 1 MRI scanner, so formal analysis of differences in the temporal signal-to-noise-ratio (tSNR) was conducted to examine the possible contribution of voxelwise differences in SNR on the findings (see Supplemental Analysis of Scanner tSNR below).

2.4 fMRI data preprocessing

Resting data were collected over the course of a 5-minute period, resulting in 150 volumes of data. The first five volumes were removed from analyses to control for signal instability, resulting in a time series of 145 volumes. All preprocessing steps were conducted using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/). For all volumes bad slices were first repaired using art-slice procedure, which is part of ArtRepair toolbox [28]. The volumes were then slice-timing corrected and realigned. Spike artifacts were eliminated using despike filter available in ArtRepair toolbox. For each subject their high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical image was co-registered to the mean functional image using SPM8. The co-registered anatomical image was then segmented using SPM8, which produced a normalized gray matter image in the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) brain space. The functional images were then normalized to the MNI space. The normalized functional volumes and the gray matter image were resliced to a spatial resolution of 3 mm isotropic. The equivalence of voxel size allowed one to one mapping of voxels between normalized functional and anatomical volumes, and provided a mechanism to extract gray matter signal (see the following section). To reduce the effect of ringing artifact and to improve signal to noise ratio [29], a 6-mm isotropic smoothing filter was applied to the normalized functional data. The cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and the white matter nuisance signals were regressed out, and a bandpass filter of 0.01 Hz to 0.12 Hz was applied using CONN toolbox. Finally, all functional data were movement corrected using ArtRepair. The movement parameters have been summarized in Table 3.

Table 3. Motion correction summary.

Mean PVR (sd) Mean SD of data (sd) Mean CT (sd) Mean CT, % of mean (sd)
TBI Time-1 3.30 (6.49) 1.59 (0.58) 6.23 (2.48) 1.30 (0.01)
TBI Time-2 3.17 (6.87) 1.68 (1.02) 6.45 (3.12) 1.30 (0.02)
TBI Time-3 3.05 (4.72) 1.67 (0.61) 5.82 (1.89) 1.30 (0.01)
HC Time-1 2.47 (2.70) 1.76 (0.88) 6.30 (1.91) 1.30 (0.01)
HC Time-2 0.86 (2.14) 1.67 (0.57) 6.10 (1.67) 1.30 (0.01)

PVR, percent of volumes repaired; SD, standard deviation; CT, current threshold.

2.4.1 Head motion correction

The removal of nuisance signal due to physiology and head motion is a developing issue receiving significant recent attention in the resting MRI literature [30,31]. To correct motion, raw data were examined for motion- related slice and volume signal changes using ArtRepair [28] and standard 6-parameter realignment to serve as null regressors. Using this pipeline reveals that in our TBI Time-1 group, where motion is typically the greatest concern, we can resolve motion problems in ~89% of the scans using standard ArtRepair guidelines (25% volume correction; exclusion of 7 data sets of 63 total (21 subjects originally enrolled, 3 time points each). Power and colleagues (2012) demonstrate that global signal regression is a powerful method for reducing the effects of motion. Given concerns published elsewhere [32,33] we did not employ that approach. Results of motion correction are available in Table 3.

2.5 Volumetric values

To help constrain the interpretation of neural network connectivity we integrate information about brain volume change over time. The relative volumes of white matter, and gray matter relative to the intracranial volume were calculated for each subject at each time point using the VBM library in SPM8. To detect if there was significant brain atrophy after the injury, the volumes of gray and white matters of later time points were compared with Time 1 (Paired t-test, with multiple comparison correction for the TBI group) (See Table 4).

Table 4. VBM analysis: Volume ratio.

Mean GM/T (sd) Mean WM/T (sd)
TBI Time-1 0.471 (0.020) 0.370 (0.014)*
TBI Time-2 0.472 (0.017) 0.364 (0.017)
TBI Time-3 0.481 (0.013) 0.357 (0.023)*
HC Time-1 0.485 (0.025) 0.354 (0.017)
HC Time-2 0.483 (0.026) 0.353 (0.017)

GM/T, gray matter volume / total volume; WM/T, white matter volume / total volume.

* p = 0.024 calculated by pairwise t-tests

2.5.1 Lesion volumetrics

TBI is highly heterogeneous with respect to injury severity and pathophysiology. To address this issue and determine the influence of global pathology on brain networks, we created a 3-dimensional (3D) lesion model for each subject using 3D Slicer [34] that utilizes information from multiple MR sequences, such as SWI, FLAIR, and T1 MPRAGE. In the first step of the lesion modelling process, a neuroradiologist manually located and labelled all the hemorrhage and edema lesions in the three image modalities from each subject. During the lesion modelling process, first the multimodality MR images (SWI, FLAIR, and T1 MPRAGE) were co-registered, and then the lesions were segmented semi-automatically with the Atlas Based Classification (ABC) toolkit based on the labelling in the previous step. Finally, a 3D rendering of all lesions was generated and verified by the same neuroradiologist. As the goal here was to examine the overall effect of lesions on the global network metrics, we merged both lesion types (edema and hemorrhage) and lesion volume was calculated as the ratio of lesion volume to gray matter volume.

2.6 Analytic approach and pipeline

To test our hypotheses (See section 1.2), a functional connectivity network for each subject at each time-point was developed, and a subject-specific connectivity profile was created using the functional network. The connectivity profile included metrics that summarize total functional connections, network strength, network cost, and network efficiency.

In this article we performed two types of analyses: global and local analysis. For both these analyses the following computational steps were implemented: 1. A subject-specific binary gray matter mask at each time point was developed; 2. ROIs were defined using Power’s 264 anatomical coordinates and for each subject at each time point using the mean signal of each ROI to evaluate functional connectivity. 3. Using graph theoretical procedures a connectivity profile was developed for each subject at each time point.

In global analysis, we focused on examining the whole-brain connectivity changes during recovery period and to determine if these changes predict behavior. Finally, we also performed lesion analysis to determine the influence of focal pathology on global network metrics. In order to guarantee that our approach to brain parcellation was not a primary determinant of the regional and global network effects, we conducted a second brain parcellation using spatially constrained independent components analysis (sICA) method (see [35]) followed by a graph theoretical analysis for comparison (see Supplementary Analyses below).

In local analysis, we focus on finding regions (i.e. subsystems) in the brain that are consistently hyper- or hypo-connected at all 3 time points and also examine their cost. Further analysis using these hyper- hypo-connected subsystems are then conducted for only the TBI group. The analytic pipeline along with the preprocessing steps are illustrated in Fig 1. These steps are discussed in a greater detail in the following sub-sections.

Fig 1. fMRI data preprocessing steps and the analytic pipeline.

Fig 1

2.6.1. Subject-specific binary gray matter mask

A specific binary mask for each subject at each time point was created to better capture the change in the volumetric values across time. The segmentation procedure in SPM8 assigns a probabilistic value to each voxel of the gray matter segmented file (i.e. the probability that the voxel belongs to the gray matter). Therefore, to develop a binary gray matter mask, we had to use a threshold. We used those voxels with probability for gray matter greater than 0.6 as suggested by Zalesky and colleagues [36]. For each subject their binary gray matter mask was then overlaid on the normalized functional images to extract the BOLD signal from the gray matter voxels.

2.6.2. Developing subject-specific functional connectivity network

We used Power's 264 anatomical coordinates [37] to segment the brain into 264 regions of interests (ROIs). ROIs were defined as 15 mm spheres centered at these coordinates. Power's 264 anatomical coordinates represent putative functional areas of the brain, and therefore, this way of defining ROIs provides a mechanism to locate specific subnetworks as well as anatomic landmarks in the brain that are most affected during recovery from TBI.

For each subject the average signal time course of the gray matter voxels in each ROI was measured, and Pearson's correlation between the signal time courses of all possible pairs of ROIs was conducted. The correlations that fail the false discovery rate (FDR) at a p-threshold of 0.05 were discarded. The remaining set of correlation values thus represent a subject specific weighted functional connectivity network.

2.6.3 Subject-specific connectivity profile

The following network metrics were evaluated: 1) Total functional connections, 2) Network strength, 3) Local efficiency: clustering coefficient, 4) Global efficiency: average shortest path length, 5) Network cost.

The total functional connectivity in the brain for each subject at each time point was evaluated by counting the total number of statistically significant (FDR criterion) functional edges. This metric was used to determine the relative network connectivity.

Network strength was defined as the total sum of the absolute values of the network weights. The correlation coefficient between each pair of ROIs represents the weight of the connection. The rationale behind using the absolute value was to be able to examine the magnitude of the synchronization between ROIs. If the magnitude of s synchronization is high, it may be indicative of high metabolic consumption [38]. Additionally, we defined ROI-strength as the total strength of all functional edges that are incident upon a ROI. This allowed us to isolate brain subsystems that showed hyperconnectivity.

In order to examine local communication efficiency within the network, we calculated local clustering coefficient. Local clustering coefficient of vertex i is defined as:

Ci=|{ejk:vj,vkNi,ejkE}|ki(ki1) (1)

where ki is total number of neighbors (i.e. neighboring nodes) of node i, Ni represents the set of all nodes that are neighbors of node i, vj and vk are vertices in the neighborhood of i; E is the set of all edges in the network, and ejk represents the connections that exist between the neighbors of node i. A high clustering coefficient would mean that the neighbors of node i are well connected, hence, suggesting a high local efficiency around node i.

In order to examine global network efficiency, shortest path length, d(i,j), is computed between all pairs of nodes i and j. The global efficiency is then defined as follows:

E(G)=2n(n1)i<jG1d(i,j) (2)

A measure of network cost was derived based on the assumption that the synchronization between long distance ROIs must be more metabolically expensive than the short distance ROIs. We defined the cost of each functional edge as the product of the Euclidean distance between the ROI-pair and the absolute weight of the connection. The overall network cost was then derived by totaling the cost of all edges that are part of the network. This definition of the network cost encapsulates 3 different concepts into one: level of synchronization between all pair of ROIs (network strength), number of functional edges, and physical distance of the functional edges.

Additionally, we defined ROI-cost as the total cost of all functional edges that are incident upon a ROI. This allowed us to isolate brain subsystems that make more expensive connections in an injured brain comparing to the HCs.

2.6.4 Global and local analysis

Once the subject specific connectivity profiles are developed, we perform the following global and local analyses to test the study hypotheses (see Section 1.3). First, a global analysis was performed comparing network strength between the TBI and HC samples; the comparison is performed at all three time points (TBI Time vs. avg HC). To determine if differences in strength predicts cost, we also conducted a between group comparison of global network cost. We then examine how the cost-efficiency relationships change during the recovery period. This is accomplished by performing simulations to test network efficiency for different cost values at all three time points. We then examined the relationship between global network metrics and cognitive functioning. Finally, we examine the relationship between brain lesions for all TBI cases at Time-1 network connectivity and behavior.

For local analysis, we examined the subregions that contribute to hyperconnectivity, we mapped the Power-ROIs to 20 functionally defined subsystems. These 20 sub-networks were based upon prior findings in the TBI the literature and focused on the brain’s most robust sub-networks. While one goal of the study was to focus on within TBI group change during the first year, we first isolate the most high-strength and high-cost sub-regions in TBIs comparing to the HCs. For these isolated subsystems, we calculate the connection cost of ROIs that are part of these subsystems, and then analyze connection metrics (connection count, strength, and physical distance) that govern the change in their cost across time. Finally, we examine the relationship between the overall cost of these subsystems and behavioral performances.

2.6.5 Supplementary analyses

Implementing an identical pre-processing pipeline as above, we substituted a spatially constrained ICA as the brain parcellation method to determine if brain parcellation was an important determinant of the network findings. There are, of course, important limitations to these types of comparisons (see [39]) so the goal was not to achieve identical results when comparing networks of distinct sizes. Instead the primary goal was to determine if the hubs driving cost-efficiency tradeoffs were commensurate. We chose ICA as a second approach for two reasons. First, in contrast to Power-264 based parcellation scheme where the representative signal for a ROI is developed by averaging over all voxels with in the ROI, in ICA, the representative signal is established using a weighted average; the ICA procedure estimates a weight for each voxel using an adaptive algorithm. This provides us an opportunity to examine if the parcellation scheme and also the mechanism by which ROI representative signals are developed affects the results. Second, ICA is a well-established approach receiving widespread application across a number of clinical samples (for review see [4041]). While details for this analysis are consistent with [8], in brief, the procedure included the Group ICA of fMRI Toolbox (GIFT). To achieve a detailed component structure and because a higher ordered dataset was desirable for graph theoretical analyses, we chose a relatively high model order ICA (100 components) for all analyses [35, 4244]. Preprocessing included movement correction, coregistration, and normalization, but consistent with [35] we did not remove signal from cerebrospinal fluid and white matter and did not conduct volumewise correction prior to the ICA. Subject-specific data reduction principal components analysis retained 120 principal components and group data retained 100 principal components followed by a component sort based upon dynamic range and low/high frequency ratio using 0.01 and 0.1 Hz as guidelines (consistent with [35]). The time series from retained components were submitted to an adjacency matrix and connections were FDR corrected resulting in an edge list that was submitted to graph theoretical analysis.

3. Results

3.1 Global analysis

3.1.1 Examining hyperconnectivity

To examine the hyperconnectivity hypothesis, we used analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with MRI scanner as a covariate in order to compare the TBI distributions for network strength at each time-point to the mean HC distribution of network strength; the mean HC distribution was established by averaging the 2 time-point HC distributions. As expected, effects at the global level were subtle; heightened connectivity at a global scale for network strength (p = 0.049; ηp2 = 0.16) and number of links (p = 0.046; ηp2 = 0.16) was observable only at time-2 (p = 0.049; ηp2 = 0.16).

We also performed a similar analysis for the global network metrics, including clustering coefficient and mean path length, to determine how network efficiency may be affected due to TBI; the analysis revealed subtle effects for path length and clustering coefficient at Time-2 (see Table 5). A more nuanced analysis of cost and efficiency is performed in the following sub-section (see Section 3.1.4).

Table 5. Summary of graph network metrics (mean; sd).
Avg Short PL-U Avg Local CC-U Avg Local CC-W Network Strength Average Number Links
TBI Time-1 1.70 (0.07) 0.40 (0.08) 0.42 (0.08) 3363.56 (953.84) 10504.64 (2528.39)
TBI Time-2 1.69 (0.05)* 0.41 (0.05)* 0.43 (0.05)* 3456.77 (682.07)* 10822 (1886.06)*
TBI Time-3 1.71 (0.04) 0.39 (0.04) 0.41 (0.04) 3171.87 (509.80) 9988.85 (1418.27)
HC Average 1.71 (0.02) 0.38 (0.02) 0.40 (0.02) 3093.64 (284.43) 9784.29 (819.68)

Avg Short PL-U: Average shortest path length (unweighted); Avg Local CC-U: Average local clustering coefficient (unweighted); Avg Local CC-W: Average local clustering coefficient (weighted); AFC.

* p < 0.05 calculated by ANCOVA, with MRI scanner as a covariate.

3.1.2 Lesion analysis

The results of the 3D rendering procedure produced identifiable lesions in 13/14 cases (Fig 2). In the case showing no conspicuous hemorrhage or contusion, we anticipate that the injury was milder and/or diffuse. Results of the lesion analysis of the 13 cases with identifiable trauma lesions is illustrated in Fig 2. To examine the influence of total lesion, we collapsed for hemorrhage and contusion and calculated a percentage of lesion volume to total gray matter volume at the individual subject level. Unsurprisingly, the results do not show a significant relationship between lesion status and cognitive functioning based upon composite scores (r-values ranging from r = -.02 to -0.22). This finding is consistent with a TBI literature revealing inconsistent relationships between structural brain changes and cognitive outcome after TBI [4548]. Proportion of lesion volume also did not predict unweighted path length in the network (T1 r-value = 0.01, T2 r = 0.20, and T3 r = 0.01) and did not predict global network cost (T1 r = 0.05; T2 r = 0.24; T3 r = 0.09). Overall, the 3D lesion rendering provided some context for these injuries, but was not significantly predictive of patient outcome or the network response.

Fig 2. Results of 3D lesion modeling for TBI subjects at Time 1 (approximately 3-months post injury).

Fig 2

Total network cost and the lesion volume are not significantly correlated at Time 1, Time 2, or Time 3.

3.1.3 Examining the cost-efficiency relationship

We aimed to examine cost-efficiency relationship over time with focus on Time-2 and Time-3 as high efficiency/high strength was observed at Time-2 but not at Time-3 (based upon Table 5). High “efficiency” at Time-2 is a secondary effect of its cost: increasing the number of links results in lower path length, but the question remains whether Time-2 maintains superior cost-efficiency organization in the absence of these additional links. The approach taken was to randomly delete edges to test the cost-efficiency of the network as it becomes increasingly sparse. As an analogue, investigators have used similar approaches to simulate random attacks on edges to determine how cost-efficiency behaves [13, 49]. The steps involved in the simulation were the following:

  • Step-1. For a given time point, develop a group averaged weighted undirected functional connectivity network (N_avg) using the weighted functional connectivity networks (see Section 2.6.2 for details) of the 14 TBI subjects. Please note, in a subject specific weighted functional connectivity network, weight of each edge represents absolute correlation coefficient value. Now make an inverse network (N_avg_inverse) by subtracting the weight of each edge from 1.01. That is, if the average weight of an edge is 1, then we make it 0.01 so that it represents a short distance (but not a 0 distance) between the two ROIs; edges that are disconnected in N_avg also remain disconnected in N_avg_inverse, and are represented with a weight of 0 in both the networks.

  • Step-2. Define an integer variable X and initialize it to 1.

  • Step-3. Randomly delete X% edges from N_avg_inverse to produce a new network configuration.

  • Step-4. Evaluate the overall network cost and global efficiency of this new network and store it in a file.

  • Step-5. Replace back the deleted edges in the network, N_avg_inverse.

  • Step-6. Repeat step-3 to step-5 50 times.

  • Step-7. If X is less than 99

         Increment X by 1 and go to step-3.

       Else

         Stop.

The output for the above procedure, at each time point is a single file (each file has 99x50 = 4950 rows) consisting of 2 columns: cost and efficiency. The cost and the efficiency values stored in these 3 files are then plotted as a graph (see Fig 3). We also ran the above simulation for healthy control subjects, to examine if the cost-efficiency relationship at Time-3 was similar to that of the healthy controls. Our results show, under random edge-attack, Time-3 is more robust and will maintain high efficiency for a much greater range of network densities (see cost of 50000 to 200000) than other time points. Also, the HC data are also plotted to provide context; Time-3 cost-efficiency relationship is more similar to the control subjects compared to this relationship at Time 2.

Fig 3. Illustrates cost-efficiency relationship of group-averaged TBI network at all 3 time points (see Section 3.1.4 for details).

Fig 3

For cost bands 50000–100000, 100000–150000, and 150000–20000, there was a significant difference between the Time-2 and Time-3 efficiency values based on independent sample t-test. The Bonferroni corrected p-values and Cohen's-d for all cost bands are as follows, 0–50000 (p-value = 0.61, cohen's-d = 0.04), 50000–100000 (p-value << 0.05, cohen's-d = 0.30), 100000–150000 (p-value << 0.05, cohen's-d = 0.54), 150000–200000 (p-value << 0.05, cohen's-d = 0.70), and 200000–250000 (p-value = 1, cohen's-d = 0.06).

3.2 Local analysis

3.2.1 Finding hyper/hypoconnected subsystems

In order to isolate the most consistently observed regions showing hyper- and hypoconnectivity over the recovery period, we compared the TBI sample with the HC sample for network strength and cost as an initial “filter”. Global analysis demonstrated the most significant elevations in network strength and cost at Time-2, with no significant differences between the TBI and HC groups at Time points 1 and 3. Therefore, to reduce the number of analyses and to test specific hypotheses regarding hyperconnectivity in the brain’s subnetworks we grouped the 264 Power ROIs into 20 brain subsystems and compared Time-2 data (where hyperconnectivity was evident in global analysis; Table 5) to the HC data to determine regions of disproportionately greater connectivity. These 20 subnetworks derived from the Power 264 ROIs were based upon the brain’s most robust subnetworks and commonly sites of hyperconnectivity based upon the TBI literature (see S1 Table). For each brain subsystem, to examine hyperconnectivity we developed TBI and HC distributions of ROI-strengths (see section 2.6.3) using all ROIs that are part of the chosen subsystem. Then we performed independent sample t-tests comparing TBI and HC distributions. Thus, in all we performed 20 analyses for 20 collapsed brain systems for Time-2 data to direct further local analysis. The p-values were then corrected for multiple comparisons using FDR correction and in order to maintain sensitivity to medium effect sizes, we included result with a Cohen’s d > 0.70. If the mean of the TBI distribution was higher than the HCs it was labeled hyperconnected, and if it was less than the HCs then it was labeled hypo-connected. The results are summarized in Table 6 and the hyper- and hypo-connected sub-systems are shown in Fig 4.

Table 6. Hyperconnected and hypoconnected sub-systems for Time 2 TBI compared to HCs.
Sub-region description Power Reg P-value a Hyper/Hypo Cohens-d
Left Frontal DMN * 13 0.049 + 1.29
Right Frontal DMN 10 0.051 + 1.14
Left Cingulate/Parietal Memory Retrieval* 2 0.179 - -0.77
Right Frontal Task Control 9 0.051 + 1.11
Right Parietal Task Control 4 0.176 + 0.81
Right Ventral Temporal/Parietal Attention* 5 0.176 + 0.80

Power Reg, number of power regions in the sub-system; Hypo (-), hypoconnectivity; Hyper (+), hyperconnectivity; DMN, default mode network.

a p values were False Discovery Rate (FDR) corrected.

Significantly hyperconnected at Time 1.

* Significantly hyper/hypo connected at Time 3.

Fig 4. Hyper- and hypo-connected sub-systems in the brain found using local analysis (see Section 3.2.1 for details).

Fig 4

To examine cost, we performed an identical analysis as above for examining subsystem strength using findings from global Time-2 analysis to direct us to specific subsystems for within-group analysis. The results are summarized in Table 7. Based on the above strength and cost analyses for Time-2 data, several consistent regions demonstrated significantly higher strength and cost (Tables 6 and 7), so these regions served as the filter for examining Time-1-3 local effects.

Table 7. High cost brain sub-systems for Time 2 TBI compared to HC sample.
Sub-region description Power Reg P-value a Hyper/Hypo Cohens-d
Left Cingulate/Temporal DMN 20 0.208 + 0.74
Left Frontal DMN * 13 0.037 + 1.34
Right Frontal DMN * 10 0.070 + 1.08
Right Frontal Task Control 9 0.070 + 1.06
Right Parietal Task Control 4 0.13 + 0.90
Right Ventral Temporal/Parietal Attention 5 0.208 + 0.71
Right Dorsal Temporal/Parietal Dorsal Attention 5 0.208 + 0.77

Power Reg, Number of power regions in the sub-system; Hypo (-), hypoconnectivity; Hyper (+), hyperconnectivity; DMN, default mode network.

a p values were false discovery rate (FDR) corrected.

* Time 1 also significantly elevated for cost in these regions; No Time 3 also significantly elevated for cost in these regions.

Based upon the Time-2 filter, six regions were identified as high strength and seven regions as high cost for further analysis (Tables 6 and 7). To determine if these regions also showed significant increases at Time points 1 and 3, separate strength and cost analyses were conducted by comparing Time-1 and Time-3 data to the HC sample using an analysis of covariance with MRI scanner as a covariate in order to determine the influence of MRI scanner on results. Based upon a minimum of ηp2 = 0.06 (small to medium effects size), at Time-1, three areas showed relatively greater strength in connections for the left DMN [F(1) = 2.4; p = 0.134; ηp2 = 0.095], right DMN [F(1) = 2.6; p = 0.12; ηp2 = 0.10], and the cingulo-parietal memory retrieval network [F(1) = 1.91; p = 0.179; ηp2 = 0.07]. Time-1 also showed elevations in two subregions for cost: left DMN [F(1) = 2.3; p = 0.139; ηp2 = 0.09] and right DMN [F(1) = 2.4; p = 0.13; ηp2 = 0.10]. Similarly, for strength findings Time-1, the subregions Time-3 included hyperconnectivity for right frontal DMN and the right ventral parietal attention network and hypoconnectivity for the left cingulo-parietal memory retrieval network [left DMN: F(1) = 1.85; = 0.186; ηp2 = 0.07; cingulo-parietal memory retrieval: F(1) = 5.7; p = 0.025, ηp2 = 0.20; right ventral parietal attention: F(1) = 4.46; p = 0.046) ηp2 = 0.16]. A primary difference at Time-3 compared to Time-1 and Time-2 was that no hyperconnected regions showed significantly elevated cost—even in left DMN and parietal attention networks. In a final analysis, we aimed to determine the difference in cost from Time-2 to Time-3 in these hyperconnected regions (below).

3.2.2 Local cost x time analysis

Analyses comparing regions showing high connectivity and cost at Time-2 and Time-3 revealed hyperconnectivity in two subregions: the left frontal DMN and the ventral parietal attention network, but in the absence of increased cost at Time-3. To understand why these regions showed increased connectivity at both time points but heightened cost only at Time-2, we examined the factors that govern the cost (i.e. link strength, link distance, and link count). In this analysis, we first created a 2-dimensional (2-D) histogram of edge counts at each time point as follows (see Fig 5). For a given time point and for a given subsystem, for all connections that are incident upon the ROIs in the subsystem, the connection length and the connection strength of all TBI subjects were stored in a 2 column table. This table was then converted to a 2-D histogram where the color of each bin (or square box in the 2-D histogram) represents the number of connections that fall in that bin. These histogram plots were then overlaid with contour lines representing cost. A given contour line (e.g. contour line of cost = 80) represents all possible values of connection distance and strength that can produce the same cost value (distance*strength). For example, if the connection length is 40 mm and strength is 0.6 for a given connection, then the cost is 24, which is same as if the length was 60 mm and the strength was 0.4. Hence, both these points will fall on the same contour line, i.e. contour line of cost = 80. To compare Time-2 and Time-3 data, we then subtracted Time-3 histogram from Time-2 histogram to create a difference histogram. As shown in Fig 5, for Left Frontal DMN, Time-2 networks consisted of a greater number (pink regions) of medium/long length and medium/weak strength connections than Time-3 networks. A similar effect was also seen for Right Ventral Temporal/Parietal Attention regions; this effect appears to account for the cost differences in networks between Time-2 and Time-3 for these nodes.

Fig 5. 2-D histograms represent the distribution of connections that are incident upon ROIs within left frontal DMN (top) and Right Ventral Temporal/Parietal Attention (bottom) as a function of connection length and connection strength.

Fig 5

A contour line (e.g. contour line of cost = 80) represents all possible values of connection distance and connection strength that can produce the same cost value (distance*strength).

3.2.3 Behavioral analysis

To test whether the most consistently costly nodes within the network during recovery predict performance, we used bivariate Pearson correlations comparing cost and cognitive performance. To reduce the number of comparisons, we focused analyses on Time-2 and Time-3 connectivity data compared with three neuropsychological measures sensitive to deficits in TBI in the areas of executive control/switching (Trail Making Test), working memory (Digit Span Backward), and processing speed (VSAT total score). At 6 months post injury (Time-2), cost in the temporal-parietal attentional control region was significantly associated with performance on Digit Span backward (r = 0.777, p = 0.02); cost in this region was correlated with Trail Making Test (r = 0.462, p = 0.11), but showed no relationship with VSAT total (r = -0.052, p = 0.87). By contrasts, cost within the left frontal DMN showed little relationship with performance on Digits Backward (r = 0.033, p = 0.91) and Trail Making Test (r = 0.184, p = 0.55), but showed modest association with performance on the VSAT (r = -0.492, p = 0.09).

At 12 months post injury (Time-3), cost within the temporal-parietal attentional control region showed a modest association with performance on Digit Span Backward (r = 0.427, p = 0.15), and had little relationship with performance on Trail Making Test (r = -0.33, p = 0.92) and VSAT (r = 0.098, p = 0.75). Cost within the left frontal DMN showed the strongest association the Digit Span Backward (r = 0.664, p = 0.01), followed by performance on Trail Making Test (r = 0.410, p = 0.16) and the VSAT (r = -0.336, p = 0.26).

Overall, these data indicate that as cost increases, speed and executive control are reduced. Cost was positively associated with working memory performance, so there appears to be a trade-off between on-task processing efficiency and untimed working memory functioning. Indeed, in the TBI sample, working memory was negatively correlated with efficiency speeded performance.

3.2.4: Supplementary brain parcellation using ICA

To examine the reliability of the findings in the global analysis, we introduced an identical connectivity analysis, changing only the method of brain parcellation. The initial findings using a network composed of Power 264 regions demonstrates high cost early after injury that partially resolves toward 1-year post injury. These findings are generally confirmed in the Supplementary analysis with one important difference: the timing of this resolution. The Supplementary analysis using ICA, high cost networks are more pronounced at Time 1 and this diminishes at Time 2 and Time 3 (see Tables A-C in S1 File). We offer an explanation for the differences in the timing of cost-efficiency resolution between the two approaches below.

Local analysis was highly consistent between the two separate analyses. The most highly connected node at all three time points is the anterior default mode network (medial left and right prefrontal cortex; see Figs A-C in S1 File) which overlaps with the primary finding from the Power 264 analysis in Table 7. Therefore, the locations for the expression of high cost (i.e., hubs) remain the same irrespective of analysis, but the global findings (e.g., network cost) are dependent upon the parcellation approach.

3.2.5: Supplementary analysis: Influence of MRI scanner on connectivity results

In order to examine potential differences in tSNR between scanners, tSNR maps were generated for each visit of each subject based on the contrast between the mean signal of and the standard deviation of the EPI time-series for each voxel (after detrending with a second-order polynomial) (see Figs D-G in S1 File). For group analyses, the tSNR maps were normalized to the standard MNI brain space using the same normalization parameters for the fMRI data. The cross-system comparisons were conducted using one-way ANOVA with multiple comparisons. The longitudinal stability of tSNR for each MRI system was analyzed using the Sandwich Estimator for Neuroimaging Longitudinal Data (warwick.ac.uk/tenichols/SwE). There were no differences in tSNR in the same scanner across time, which is vital given the goals of this paper to examine the evolution of cost-efficiency during recovery. Between-scanner analyses revealed differences among the three scanners primarily in the mid-posterior cortical area of the inferior frontal lobe and the inferior cortical area of the temporal lobe. There are two important reasons why we do not anticipate that these differences between scanners influence the current findings. First, the higher tSNR occurs in the two scanners (Siemens HMC and Philips, Figs D-G in S1 File) where 8/12 HCs (and 21/26 total) were scanned, making tSNR as the determinant for hyperconnectivity in the TBI sample less intuitive. Second, there are few instances where tSNR overlaps spatially with the hubs that were seen as drivers of connectivity differences (e.g., insula; Figs F,G in S1 File). In summary, analysis of voxelwise tSNR revealed no observable influence of time on MRI signal which is vital to the central analyses presented in this paper. Additional, with regard to the few between-scanner findings observed, we anticipate that differences in scanner tSNR are not the source of the findings given the direction of these differences and the lack of spatial overlap with primary findings. We present these data in the Supplementary materials and make all data are publicly available.

4. Discussion

The current study used a graph theoretical analysis of whole-brain resting fMRI data and structural MRI to examine the changes in the brain connectivity during the first year following moderate and severe TBI. In doing so, this represents the first study to examine the evolution of network cost-efficiency trade-offs during recovery from neurological insult. Using a network of 264 functionally defined regions, hyperconnectivity was observable across distinct regions (e.g., hubs, peripheral nodes) and connection types (long-distance, short-distance connections) but, in the current data, increased connectivity peaked at about 6-months post injury (Time-2) before equilibrating in most subjects resulting in residual hyperconnectivity in only a few subnetworks by Time-3.

Hyperconnectivity at Time-3 did not result in significantly elevated cost and we interpret this to indicate that hyperconnectivity may be necessary to accommodate network disruption early after injury, but networks do not optimally sustain this early response. The relationship between early hyperconnectivity and later cost-efficiency trade-offs is elaborated upon in the following. Of note, there were important differences in network metrics based upon the parcellation approach used (Power 264 v ICA), but for reasons elaborated upon below, in this Discussion we focus our attention on the result of the analysis using the Power 264 parcellation approach.

4.1 Hyperconnectivity as a response to injury

The primary hypothesis that physical neurological disruption results in functional hyperconnectivity was supported; individuals with TBI showed increased network strength which peaked at around 6 months post injury (Time-2). Targeted analyses of the brain’s subnetworks revealed that the left frontal DMN and temporal-parietal attentional control regions were consistently hyperconnected at Time-1 and Time-2 and residual hyperconnectivity remained at Time-3, supporting hypothesis-2 that enhanced connectivity would be more evident in the “rich-club” regions (i.e. the most highly connected cortical hubs). Moreover, connectivity in frontal DMN and attentional control networks were both positively correlated with simple working memory rehearsal functioning at Time-3, but predicted reduced speed/efficiency, so the relationship between network cost and cognitive performance may not be entirely straight-forward.

Overall, the Time-3 findings presented here are largely consistent with the cross-sectional research examining TBI years post moderate and severe injury, showing residual hyperconnectivity in select subnetworks (see [37]). Given the significance of the injuries in this population, we endeavored to determine the influence of local pathology on large-scale networks using 3D lesion modeling. However, global metrics such as network strength and link count were not correlated with lesion percentage (total gray/lesion volume) and even exploratory analysis of the three regions significant connectivity differences at both Time-2 and Time-3 (e.g., left frontal DMN, cingulo-parietal memory network, parietal attention network) maintained only very small relationships to brain lesion percentage, with the highest at Time-3 ranging from r-values of 0.20 to 0.25.

In sum, hyperconnectivity was observed with significant elevations in connectivity at Time-2 and residual hyperconnectivity in network hubs at Time-3. The finding that Time-3 showed no regions of elevated cost paralleling the effects observed in Time-2 was an intriguing finding and became a focus of cost-efficiency analysis.

4.2 Balancing network cost-efficiency in the injured brain

The normally developed human cortex is organized so that local regions are more highly connected [13,17, 4950] and connection density falls off as an inverse square function [51] which has been tied to optimal wiring to balancing cost-efficiency and may have important implications for how networks might adapt to injury. We aimed to examine the relationship between elevated connectivity and network cost after injury with particular interest in determining if hyperconnectivity occurred equally across distinct connection distances. To examine the relationship between hyperconnectivity and connection distance, we introduced novel metrics to estimate the cost of each functional connection by considering their length and the strength.

Examination of the regional differences in network cost revealed that bilateral frontal DMN maintained elevated cost at Time-1 and Time-2 (see Table 7). By contrast, the increased network cost observed at the first two time points diminished by Time-3 so that while subtle effects of hyperconnectivity were apparent at Time-3 it did not result in elevated network cost. In Fig 5, we examined the factors that govern the cost (i.e. link strength, link distance, and link count); a contour line represents all possible values of connection distance and strength that can produce the same cost value (distance*strength). These data reveal that the elevation in Time-2 cost is most likely attributable to a higher number of medium-range connections (as opposed to long-distance connections as anticipated). This finding is important as it could provide heuristics for understanding the edges most likely to be exploited during the expression of network plasticity and is likely related to the higher probability for observing hyperconnectivity in centrally located medial structures.

In a global analysis designed to examine cost-efficiency over time, simulations revealed that cost-efficiency was lowest at Time-2 (even less efficient than Time-1) and was optimized by Time-3 resulting in residual relative hyperconnectivity but diminished overall cost (Fig 3). We interpret this finding to indicate that Time-2 cost is not sustainable and the system adapts to a less costly, but efficient network. Several imaging studies have confirmed that the functional connectivity network of a healthy brain is configured to produce high efficiency (i.e. efficiency of information exchange) at a low connection cost [5152]; i.e., healthy brains have been shown to exhibit some small-world-like characteristics. It is reasonable to assume that maintaining such high cost networks is accompanied by unwanted metabolic demand resulting in a network configuration that achieves higher efficiency at Time-3.

We observed elevated connectivity in the same sites at all three time points. Enhancing connectivity via hubs during recovery is consistent with the principle of preferential attachment [53,54], where the most highly connected nodes are the sites for integration of additional connections. These effects have been observed in animal models of neurodevelopment where the “rich get richer” during the formation of neural networks [55]. Hyperconnectivity via hubs after injury achieves two important goals. First, it may provide the opportunity to re-establish connectivity while maintaining an efficient network architecture. Second, centralized hub nodes are the most metabolically efficient regions in the brain [56]. Therefore, enhancing the connections in rich-club nodes permit re-establishing network functioning during critical recovery windows while balancing essential cost/efficiency trade-offs.

4.3 Influence of brain parcellation approach on findings

In a Supplementary Analysis, using a nearly identical processing stream up to the brain parcellation where we employed a spatially constrained ICA for comparison with the Power 264 functional atlas. Comparison of these two sets of results revealed nearly identical spatial overlap for regions with the highest cost in the TBI sample (e.g., medial frontal cortex, ECN). However, at the global level, there was partial resolution of hyperconnectivity by Time 2 as opposed to Time 3 observed in the Power 264 analysis. We anticipate that Time 2 hyperconnectivity was less evident using ICA because the most highly connected areas contributing to these effects using a Power 264 parcellation were collapsed into fewer nodes. Support for this interpretation is evident when inspecting the regional overlap between the two analyses. Unlike Time 1 and Time 3, for Time 2 all of the 10 most highly connected regions in the Power 264 analyses were contained within parts of the DMN and ECN. Mirroring this finding in Power 264, the three highest cost regions in the ICA at Time 2 included these same networks (see Figs A-C in S1 File). Thus network cost at Time 2 was differentially driven by nodes within the DMN and ECN, which were under-sampled during ICA. To underscore this point, right and left frontal DMN were the two most highly connected regions at Time 2 comprising 23 distinct Power “DMN” nodes in the prefrontal cortex (see Table 6). By contrast, of the 46 total components used for the network analysis after the ICA sort, only 3 frontal DMN regions were retained for network analysis thus greatly reducing the number of possible connections and the associated network cost observed in the DMN at Time 2. We therefore attribute the differences in Time 2 global results to the detailed sampling represented in the Power 264 compared to the ICA. Given the spatial overlap of the local findings and the more detailed spatial sampling provided by the Power 264 within identical regions, we anticipate that our focus on the Power 264 data offers a nuanced network analysis of cost-efficiency.

4.3 The implications of hyperconnectivity and increased network cost

There is a 150-year history in the neurological sciences of “lesion mapping” with the goal of pairing specific injury sites to functional loss [57] and in TBI this has been most recently observed in the resting state functional connectivity literature by examining specific connection loss in association with identified white matter disruption (see [4,6]). Given this context, the somewhat paradoxical, but consistent, finding that functional hyperconnectivity can occur in conjunction with loss of structural integrity of the brain must be integrated into models of functional plasticity. Hyperconnectivity after TBI appears to occur independent of the form or severity of pathological insult and this is particularly true when pathology does not target network hubs; in fact there are common profiles of hyperconnectivity across distinct clinical samples [58]. This conceptualization is consistent with a recent review of the influence of lesions on neural networks [59] and, in the current study global connectivity metrics such as network strength and path length maintained very little relationship to overall lesion load using 3D lesion mapping (see Fig 1) and residual hyperconnectivity was observable in centralized hubs (e.g., left frontal DMN) at Time-3 even in the context of global white matter volume loss from Time-1 to Time-3 (measured via VBM, see Table 4). Therefore, we argue that in order to understand system-level plasticity after neurological disruption, it is essential to understand the active network response to injury as opposed to focusing on the primary consequences of injury, which has been the longstanding emphasis in lesion mapping.

The mechanisms responsible for hyperconnectivity after TBI remain unclear, but there is increasing evidence that injury results in enhanced network connectivity in subnetworks of the brain (see [58] review) and we aimed to quantify how this brain response might influence network cost during the first year after injury. Establishing the timeline for the expression of hyperconnectivity and increased network cost may have important implications for brain health over the life span. For example, given the growing evidence in both animal [6062] and human [6365] research that local hypermetabolism may be a predictor of pathological protein deposition (e.g., beta amyloid), determining the permanence of the expression of high cost hyperconnectivity after injury may hold important implications for pathological aging and long-term outcome after TBI [6668].

It should be made clear that our definition of cost is based upon a measure that integrates the number, strength, and Euclidean (physical) distance for connections of each node. Euclidean distance represents a linear physical distance that does not conform to the natural shape of the white matter which is an important simplification for interhemispheric or u-shaped fibers. Moreover, signaling time is not constant with axonal diameter playing an important role in propagation times [69]. So while a measure of physical distance may not be sensitive to subtle cost dimensions in processing time and metabolism, we anticipate that the cost metric we use here is a good starting-point for a within-subject cost-efficiency analysis where these factors remain constant over time.

4.4. Study conclusions and limitations

The current approach provides an important opportunity to examine whole-brain connectivity over multiple observation points during a critical window of recovery. The current sample is unique to the TBI functional brain imaging literature with respect to injury severity and time line for assessment of network functioning. Moreover, TBI samples have commonly been heterogeneous with respect to age, ranging from late teens to mid-50s [47, 9, 10, 24,70] and time-post-injury often ranges from months to years even within the same sample [4,7,9,70]. Both of these factors have influence on the observed network response and have therefore been addressed with this relatively homogeneous sample with respect to age. Also, most studies to date have focused on a limited number of regions-of-interest (ROIs) [35,910,70], or relied-upon seed-based (10) or averaging signal across large ROIs determined via anatomical atlases [25] leaving unknown the effects of TBI on more finely parcellated networks. Even so, this study is not without limitations requiring discussion.

First, much work remains to be done in order to determine those factors that define the balance of hyper/hypo connectivity in distributed networks, the primary neural substrates involved and their role in cognitive performance, and the temporal dynamics of these subnetworks when examined over longer windows of time (e.g. establishing the stationarity of connections). If windows of network plasticity can be established, it may serve as the foundation for more focused research on: 1) healthy and pathological mechanisms of network change and 2) stage-specific interventions during periods of both recovery and decline.

Because this study required data collection over a long window of time (~7 years) and at separate sites, it required the inclusion of multiple MRI scanners during data acquisition. Between-site data collection of MRI data remains an important methodological challenge for longitudinal work and multi-center studies [7172] and we made efforts to guarantee the comparability of the signal-noise level across systems and the stability of each system over the study period with routine quality assurance scanning and analysis. In all formal between-group analysis of the data we included “MRI scanner” as a covariate to determine the contribution of scanner. Moreover, we conducted whole-brain voxelwise analysis of the tSNR revealing no differences in SNR in the same scanner over time which is vital to the within-subject hypotheses presented here. Between-scanner analysis revealed only subtle differences between the Philips and Siemens machines with each showing greater SNR in some locations (see Figs D,F in S1 File). More significant differences were evident when comparing the two Siemens scanners (see Fig E in S1 File), but for reasons discussed above, it is not clear how these regional affects would account for the current findings given the lack of spatial overlap. We do anticipate that scanner affects make it more difficult to observe consistent results between-groups and over time so we may lack sensitivity to very subtle effects during network analysis so the data collection across scanners remains a limitation of this study. The resting fMRI data included ample signal for examining connectivity (145 volumes), but remains only a brief representation of functioning in a dynamic system. Also, there are difficulties endemic to TBI when engaging in long, continuous scanning (e.g., fatigue, head movement), but longer time series provide the opportunity for examining the reliability of the results. While this is the largest sample of its kind in a study of recovery in moderate to severe TBI, the sample here remains small by behavioral study standards and a larger sample would permit more nuanced investigation of the relationship between distinct injury and clinical profiles and brain connectivity.

Finally, to test the reliability of these findings, we provide examination of distinct data parcellation techniques (e.g., Power 264 v. ICA) revealing an influence of parcellation approach on global metrics, but not in local findings. The influence of brain parcellation on network metrics is not surprising given the differences in the number of nodes (264 vs. 46) and the nature of the signal (i.e., averaged signal over a 15 mm cube of voxels vs. statistically derived “component”). The juxtaposition of the data resulting from these approaches is confirmatory regarding the specific regions showing the greatest contribution to elevated network cost after injury, but are a reminder that global metrics are node and degree dependent (see [66]).

Supporting information

S1 Table. List of 20 brain sub-systems.

(DOCX)

S1 File. Tables A-C: Network hubs for ICA-based network analysis for Times 1–3.

Regions highly overlapping between time points and with the primary Power-264 parcellation analysis. Figs A-C: Global and local cost findings for ICA-derived network. Legend: IC = independent component from GIFT, PFC = prefrontal cortex, DMN = default mode network, ECN = executive control network, HF = high frequency, LF = low frequency. Note: “network cost” is for all edges to all nodes surviving correct p<0.05, FDR correction. Figs D-G: All analyses corrected Using FWE at p<0.05. MRI machines are: 1) Philips Achieva 3T MRI, 2) Siemens Magentom Trio 3T at Hershey Medical Center, and 3) Siemens Magnetom Trio 3T, University Park. Scanners 1 and 2 showed the greatest SNR. Regions of increased tSNR between HMC Siemen’s and Philips Machines.

(PPTX)

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, NIHUL Tr000127 through the Clinical Translational Science Institute, Hershey Medical Center and the Social Sciences Research Institute at the Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA-16801.

Data Availability

Data are available from Openfmri at: ds000200.

Funding Statement

This work was funded in part by the New Jersey Commission for Brain Injury Research (09.001.BIR1) and the Social Sciences Research Institute at Pennsylvania State University in University Park, Pennsylvania. The research was also supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, NIHUL Tr000127, through Penn State University's Clinical Translational Science Institute. Work was supported by the Social Life and Engineering Sciences Imaging Center. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. CDC—Statistics—Traumatic Brain Injury—Injury Center [Internet]. USA.gov. 2013. http://www.cdc.gov/traumaticbraininjury/statistics.html
  • 2.Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. Functional connectivity: the principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab [Internet]. 1993;13(1):5–14. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8417010 10.1038/jcbfm.1993.4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Caeyenberghs K, Leemans A, Heitger MH, Leunissen I, Dhollander T, Sunaert S, et al. Graph analysis of functional brain networks for cognitive control of action in traumatic brain injury. Brain [Internet]. 2012;135(Pt 4):1293–307. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22427332 10.1093/brain/aws048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bonnelle V, Leech R, Kinnunen KM, Ham TE, Beckmann CF, De Boissezon X, et al. Default mode network connectivity predicts sustained attention deficits after traumatic brain injury. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2011;31(38):13442–51. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21940437 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1163-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hillary FG, Medaglia JD, Gates K, Molenaar PC, Slocomb J, Peechatka A, et al. Examining working memory task acquisition in a disrupted neural network. Brain. 2011;134(5):1555–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Palacios EM, Sala-Llonch R, Junque C, Roig T, Tormos JM, Bargallo N, et al. White matter integrity related to functional working memory networks in traumatic brain injury. Neurology. 2012;78(12):852–60. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31824c465a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sharp DJ, Beckmann CF, Greenwood R, Kinnunen KM, Bonnelle V, De Boissezon X, et al. Default mode network functional and structural connectivity after traumatic brain injury. Brain [Internet]. 2011;134(Pt 8):2233–47. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21841202 10.1093/brain/awr175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hillary FG, Rajtmajer SM, Roman CA, Medaglia JD, Slocomb-Dluzen JE, Calhoun VD, et al. The Rich Get Richer: Brain Injury Elicits Hyperconnectivity in Core Subnetworks. PLoS One [Internet]. 2014;9(8):e104021 Available from: http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-84905970674&partnerID=tZOtx3y1 10.1371/journal.pone.0104021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jilka SR, Scott G, Ham T, Pickering A, Bonnelle V, Braga RM, et al. Damage to the Salience Network and Interactions with the Default Mode Network. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2014. August 13;34 (33): 10798–807. Available from: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/34/33/10798.abstract 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0518-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Venkatesan UM, Dennis N a, Hillary FG. Chronology and Chronicity of Altered Resting-State Functional Connectivity after Traumatic Brain Injury. J Neurotrauma [Internet]. 2014;13:1–13. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24955788 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M, Kirsch A, et al. Cingulate-precuneus interactions: a new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry [Internet]. 2008;63(3):332–7. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=2745053&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Skudlarski P, Jagannathan K, Anderson K, Stevens MC, Calhoun VD, Skudlarska B a., et al. Brain Connectivity Is Not Only Lower but Different in Schizophrenia: A Combined Anatomical and Functional Approach. Biol Psychiatry 2010;68(1):61–9. Available from: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Aerts H, Fias W, Caeyenberghs K, Marinazzo D. Brain networks under attack: robustness properties and the impact of lesions. Brain. 2016. December;139(Pt 12):3063–3083. Review. 10.1093/brain/aww194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Catani M, ffytche DH. The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. Brain. 2005. October;128(Pt 10):2224–39. Review. 10.1093/brain/awh622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bullmore E, Sporns O. The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci [Internet]. 2012;13(5):336–49. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22498897 10.1038/nrn3214 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Achard S, Bullmore E. Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS Comput Biol [Internet]. 2007;3(2):e17 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17274684 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0030017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bassett D. S. et al. Cognitive fitness of cost-efficient brain functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 106, 11747–52 (2009). 10.1073/pnas.0903641106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kaiser M. & Hilgetag C. C. Nonoptimal component placement, but short processing paths, due to long-distance projections in neural systems. PLoS Comput Biol 2, e95–e95 (2006). 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Giessing C, Thiel CM, Alexander-Bloch AF, Patel AX, Bullmore ET. Human brain functional network changes associated with enhanced and impaired attentional task performance. J Neurosci. 2013. April 3;33(14):5903–14. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4854-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hahn A, Kranz GS, Sladky R, Ganger S, Windischberger C, Kasper S, et al. Individual Diversity of Functional Brain Network Economy. Brain Connect [Internet]. 2014;XX(Xx):1–10. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25411715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Aiello GL, Bach-y-Rita P. The cost of an action potential. J Neurosci Methods. 2000;103(2):145–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Marquez de la Plata C, Garces J, Kojori ES, Grinnan J, Spence J, Mccoll R, et al. Deficits in Functional Connectivity of Hippocampal and Frontal Lobe Circuits after Traumatic Axonal Injury. Arch Neurol. 2011;68(1):74–84. 10.1001/archneurol.2010.342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tarapore PE, Findlay AM, Lahue SC, Lee H, Honma SM, Mizuiri D, Luks TL, Manley GT, Nagarajan SS, Mukherjee P. Resting state Magnetoencephalography functional connectivity in traumatic brain injury. J Neurosurg. 2013. June;118(6):1306–16. 10.3171/2013.3.JNS12398 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hillary FG, Slocomb J, Hills EC, Fitzpatrick NM, Medaglia JD, Wang J, et al. Changes in resting connectivity during recovery from severe traumatic brain injury. Int J Psychophysiol [Internet]. 2011;82(1):115–23. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21473890 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nakamura T, Hillary FG, Biswal BB. Resting network plasticity following brain injury [Internet]. PLoS ONE. 2009. [cited 2015 Sep 6]. Available from: http://content.ebscohost.com.ezaccess.libraries.psu.edu/ContentServer.asp?T=P&P=AN&K=58081285&S=R&D=a9h&EbscoContent=dGJyMNHr7ESeprQ40dvuOLCmr02ep7VSsai4TbKWxWXS&ContentCustomer=dGJyMPGssU2wrbNOuePfgeyx44Dt6fIA [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet. 1974;2(7872):81–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Millis SR, Rosenthal M, Novack TA, Sherer M, Nick TG, Kreutzer JS, et al. Long-term neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2001;16(4):343–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Mazaika PK, Hoeft F, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Methods and Software for fMRI Analysis for Clinical Subjects. Hum Brain Mapp [Internet]. 2009;77309 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=pubmed&cmd=Retrieve&dopt=AbstractPlus&list_uids=3352639585966316575related:H1xvvfD3hi4J\npapers://3fee8da1-24f9-490b-9ed9-3b7bcb767493/Paper/p500 [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lindquist MA, Wager TD. Spatial Smoothing in fMRI Using Prolate Spheroidal Wave Functions. Hum Brain Mapp [Internet]. 2008;29(11):1276–87. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17979120 10.1002/hbm.20475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage [Internet]. 2014;84:320–41. Available from: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Power JD, Barnes K a., Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2142–54. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker D a, Jones TB, Bandettini P a. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage [Internet]. 2009;44(3):893–905. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18976716 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Murphy K, Chen G, Jo HJ, Martin A, et al. Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect [Internet]. 2012;2(1):25–32. Available from: http://online.liebertpub.com/doi/full/10.1089/brain.2012.0080 10.1089/brain.2012.0080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Irimia A, Goh SY, Torgerson CM, Vespa P, Van Horn JD. Structural and connectomic neuroimaging for the personalized study of longitudinal alterations in cortical shape, thickness and connectivity after traumatic brain injury. J Neurosurg Sci. 2014. September;58(3):129–44. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Allen EA, Erhardt E, Wei Y, Eichele T, Calhoun VD (2012) Capturing intersubject variability with group independent component analysis of fMRI data: a simulation study. NeuroImage, vol. 59, pp. 4141–4159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Zalesky A, Fornito A, Egan GF, Pantelis C, Bullmore ET. The relationship between regional and inter-regional functional connectivity deficits in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(11):2535–49. 10.1002/hbm.21379 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron [Internet]. 2011;72(4):665–78. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3222858&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nugent AC, Martinez A, D’Alfonso A, Zarate CA, Theodore WH. The relationship between glucose metabolism, resting-state fMRI BOLD signal, and GABAA-binding potential: a preliminary study in healthy subjects and those with temporal lobe epilepsy 7. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2015;35(1559–7016 (Electronic)):583–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.van Wijk BC, Stam CJ, Daffertshofer A. Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One. 2010. October 28;5(10):e13701 10.1371/journal.pone.0013701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ (2001) A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 14, 140–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Calhoun VD, Adali T (2012) Multisubject independent component analysis of fMRI: a decade of intrinsic networks, default mode, and neurodiagnostic discovery. IEEE Rev Biomed Eng. 5, 60–73. 10.1109/RBME.2012.2211076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Abou-Elseoud A, Starck T, Remes J, Nikkinen J, Tervonen O, Kiviniemi V. (2010) The effect of model order selection in group PICA. Hum Brain Mapp. 31, 1207–16. 10.1002/hbm.20929 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Allen EA, Erhardt EB, Damaraju E, Gruner W, Segall JM, Silva RF, et al. (2011) A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Front Syst Neurosci. 5, 2 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kiviniemi V, Starck T, Remes J, Long X, Nikkinen J, Haapea M, et al. (2009) Functional segmentation of the brain cortex using high model order group PICA. Hum Brain Mapp. 30, 3865–86. 10.1002/hbm.20813 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gale S.D., Burr R.B., Bigler E.D., Blatter D., 1993. Fornix degeneration and memory in traumatic brain injury. Brain Research Bulletin 32 (4), 345–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Serra-Grabulosa J.M., Junque C., Verger K., Salgado-Pineda P., Maneru C., Mercader J.M., 2005. Cerebral correlates of declarative memory dysfunctions in early traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 76 (1), 129–131 (January) 10.1136/jnnp.2004.027631 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Tomaiuolo F., Carlesimo G.A., Di Paola M., Fera F, Bonanni R, Formisano R, et al. , 2004. Gross morphology and morphometric sequelae in the hippocampus, fornix, and corpus callosum of patients with severe non-missile traumatic brain injury without macroscopically detectable lesions: a T1 weighted MRI study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 75 (9), 1314–1322 (September). 10.1136/jnnp.2003.017046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Yount R, Raschke KA, Biru M, Tate D, Miller MJ, Abildskov T, et al. , 2002. Traumatic brain injury and atrophy of the cingulate gyrus. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 14 (4), 416–423 (Fall). 10.1176/jnp.14.4.416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Alexander-Bloch AF, Vertes PE, Stidd R, Lalonde F, Clasen L, Rapoport J, et al. The anatomical distance of functional connections predicts brain network topology in health and schizophrenia. Cereb. Cortex 23 January 2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Horvát S, Gămănuț R, Ercsey-Ravasz M, Magrou L, Gămănuț B, Van Essen DC, Burkhalter A, Knoblauch K, Toroczkai Z, Kennedy H. Spatial Embedding and Wiring Cost Constrain the Functional Layout of the Cortical Network of Rodents and Primates. PLoS Biol. 2016. July 21;14(7):e1002512 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Salvador R, Suckling J, Coleman MR, Pickard JD, Menon D, Bullmore E. Neurophysiological architecture of functional magnetic resonance images of human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(9):1332–42. 10.1093/cercor/bhi016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chen B. L., Hall D. H., & Chklovskii D. B. (2006). Wiring optimization can relate neuronal structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(12), 4723–4728. 10.1073/pnas.0506806103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Albert R, Barabasi A-L. Topology of evolving networks: local events and universality. Phys Rev Lett [Internet]. 2000;85(24):5234–7. Available from: http://arxiv.org/abs/cond-mat/0005085 10.1103/PhysRevLett.85.5234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Barabási A-L, Albert R. Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science (80-). 1999;286:509–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Schroeter MS, Charlesworth P, Kitzbichler MG, Paulsen O, Bullmore ET. Emergence of rich-club topology and coordinated dynamics in development of hippocampal functional networks in vitro. J Neurosci [Internet]. 2015;35(14):5459–70. Available from: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/35/14/5459.full.pdf 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4259-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Tomasi D, Wang G-J, Volkow ND. Energetic cost of brain functional connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci [Internet]. 2013;110(33):13642–7. Available from: http://www.pnas.org/content/110/33/13642 10.1073/pnas.1303346110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Carrera E, Tononi G. Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain. 2014. September;137(Pt 9):2408–22. 10.1093/brain/awu101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hillary FG, Roman CA, Venkatesan U, Rajtmajer SM, Bajo R, Castellanos ND. Hyperconnectivity is a fundamental response to neurological disruption. Neuropsychology. 2015. January;29(1):59–75. 10.1037/neu0000110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Aerts H, Fias W, Caeyenberghs K, Marinazzo D. Brain networks under attack: robustness properties and the impact of lesions. Brain. 2016. August 6. pii: aww194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Busche M.A., Chen X, Henning HA, Reichwald J, Staufenbiel M, Sakmann B, et al. , Critical role of soluble amyloid-beta for early hippocampal hyperactivity in a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2012. 109(22): p. 8740–5. 10.1073/pnas.1206171109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Cirrito J.R., Yamada KA, Finn MB, Sloviter RS, Bales KR, May PC, et al. , Synaptic activity regulates interstitial fluid amyloid-beta levels in vivo. Neuron, 2005. 48(6): p. 913–22. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Bero AW, Bauer AQ, Stewart FR, White BR, Cirrito JR, Raichle ME, et al. Bidirectional relationship between functional connectivity and amyloidb deposition in mouse brain. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 4334–40. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5845-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Vlassenko AG, Vaishnavi SN, Couture L, Sacco D, Shannon BJ, Mach RH, et al. Spatial correlation between brain aerobic glycolysis and amyloid-β (Aβ) deposition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010. October 12;107(41):17763–7. 10.1073/pnas.1010461107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.de Haan W., Mott K., van Straaten E. C., Scheltens P. & Stam C. J. Activity dependent degeneration explains hub vulnerability in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput Biol 8, e1002582 (2012). 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Myers N, Pasquini L, Göttler J, Grimmer T, Koch K, Ortner M, et al. Within-patient correspondence of amyloid-β and intrinsic network connectivity in Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2014. July;137(Pt 7):2052–64. 10.1093/brain/awu103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chen XH, Johnson VE, Uryu K, Trojanowski JQ, Smith DH. A lack of amyloid plaques despite persistent accumulation of amyloid in axons of long-term survivors of traumatic brain injury. Brain Pathol. 2009;19(2):214–23. 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2008.00176.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Smith DH, Chen X-H, Iwata A, Graham DI. Amyloid beta accumulation in axons after traumatic brain injury in humans. J Neurosurg. 2003;98(5):1072–7. 10.3171/jns.2003.98.5.1072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Uryu K, Chen XH, Martinez D, Browne KD, Johnson VE, Graham DI, et al. Multiple proteins implicated in neurodegenerative diseases accumulate in axons after brain trauma in humans. Exp Neurol. 2007;208(2):185–92. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.06.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Wang SH, Shultz JR, Burish MJ, Harrison KH, Hof PR, Towns LC, et al. Functional trade-offs in white matter axonal scaling. J Neurosci. 2008;28(15):4047–4056. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5559-05.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Pandit AS, Expert P, Lambiotte R, Bonnelle V, Leech R, Turkheimer FE, et al. Traumatic brain injury impairs small-world topology. Neurology. 2013;80(20):1826–33. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182929f38 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Marchitelli R, Minati L, Marizzoni M, Bosch B, Bartrés-Faz D, Müller BW, et al. Test-retest reliability of the default mode network in a multi-centric fMRI study of healthy elderly: Effects of data-driven physiological noise correction techniques. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016. June;37(6):2114–32. 10.1002/hbm.23157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Bernal-Casas D, Balaguer-Ballester E, Gerchen MF, Iglesias S, Walter H, Heinz A, et al. Multi-site reproducibility of prefrontal-hippocampal connectivity estimates by stochastic DCM. Neuroimage. 2013. November 15;82:555–63. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. List of 20 brain sub-systems.

(DOCX)

S1 File. Tables A-C: Network hubs for ICA-based network analysis for Times 1–3.

Regions highly overlapping between time points and with the primary Power-264 parcellation analysis. Figs A-C: Global and local cost findings for ICA-derived network. Legend: IC = independent component from GIFT, PFC = prefrontal cortex, DMN = default mode network, ECN = executive control network, HF = high frequency, LF = low frequency. Note: “network cost” is for all edges to all nodes surviving correct p<0.05, FDR correction. Figs D-G: All analyses corrected Using FWE at p<0.05. MRI machines are: 1) Philips Achieva 3T MRI, 2) Siemens Magentom Trio 3T at Hershey Medical Center, and 3) Siemens Magnetom Trio 3T, University Park. Scanners 1 and 2 showed the greatest SNR. Regions of increased tSNR between HMC Siemen’s and Philips Machines.

(PPTX)

Data Availability Statement

Data are available from Openfmri at: ds000200.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES