Abstract
A state of pathological uncertainty about environmental regularities might represent a key step in the pathway to psychotic illness. Early psychosis can be investigated in healthy volunteers under ketamine, an NMDA receptor antagonist. Here, we explored the effects of ketamine on contingency learning using a placebo-controlled, double-blind, crossover design. During functional magnetic resonance imaging, participants performed an instrumental learning task, in which cue-outcome contingencies were probabilistic and reversed between blocks. Bayesian model comparison indicated that in such an unstable environment, reinforcement learning parameters are downregulated depending on confidence level, an adaptive mechanism that was specifically disrupted by ketamine administration. Drug effects were underpinned by altered neural activity in a fronto-parietal network, which reflected the confidence-based shift to exploitation of learned contingencies. Our findings suggest that an early characteristic of psychosis lies in a persistent doubt that undermines the stabilization of behavioral policy resulting in a failure to exploit regularities in the environment.
Introduction
One of the big challenges facing psychiatry is to develop an understanding of psychotic symptoms that goes beyond clinical description to uncover underlying computational and neurobiological mechanisms. A comprehensive account of the bizarre perceptions (hallucinations) and beliefs (delusions) that characterizes psychotic illness would require a mechanistic understanding of how the brain extracts and exploits regularities in the succession of events that occur in its environment. Reinforcement learning theory shows promise in this regard, by offering a framework within which we can consider causative disturbances at both the computational and neurobiological levels.1, 2, 3 Such perspectives might therefore give us the sort of mechanistic understanding that can ultimately shape diagnostic and therapeutic questions.
Insights derived from reinforcement learning models have already proven useful in developing theoretical accounts of how psychotic experiences may arise and how they may relate to disrupted brain processes. Previous empirical studies have focused on how prediction error signaling may be deranged in psychosis.4, 5, 6, 7, 8 Extending this several authors have suggested that the key deficit may reside not in prediction error per se, but rather in how prediction errors are used to update representations of the environment.9, 10 Of relevance, probabilistic learning tasks have been widely studied in schizophrenia (see refs. 11, 12, 13 for reviews), providing evidence for a complex pattern of deficit depending on the precise nature of the task (for example, complexity, occurrence and number of contingency reversals, explicit vs implicit learning) as well as of the profile of recruited patients (for example, predominantly positive vs negative symptoms, treated vs untreated patients). Interestingly, it has been proposed that the core impairment in schizophrenia might not affect learning ability per se, but rather the flexible control required to perform complex tasks and/or the capacity to optimize behavior in order to maintain a high level of performance.11 In line with such proposals, our hypothesis is that a key feature of early psychosis is a disruption in how confidence is updated and used to drive behavior in a dynamic environment.
In situations of low confidence (or elevated uncertainty), individuals may seek explanations, exploring various possibilities in an effort to identify regularities. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that in such situations, healthy subjects tend to perceive illusory patterns, creating regularities where there are none, and providing superstitious or conspiratorial explanations for ambiguous scenarios.14 These observations resemble the early features of psychosis, including sense of change and feeling of strangeness,15, 16, 17 search for explanation,18, 19 apophenia20 and jumping to conclusions.21, 22
Here, we sought to capture this transitory state in the context of an associative learning task implementing a dynamic environment. We predicted that, during learning of environmental contingencies, lack of confidence could lead to a reduced ability to stabilize an internal model of the world, with an ensuing, persistent sense of surprise. This would eventually result in sub-optimal behavior, characterized by an under-exploitation of true environmental regularities and an accompanying tendency to over-readily update in response to incidental violations of those regularities
Testing our predictions in a clinical setting is challenging given that, by the time psychosis is clearly identified, the expression of altered confidence may have been obfuscated by delusion formation and treatment effects. An established and fruitful solution is to use pharmacological models of early psychosis in healthy volunteers such as ketamine, a noncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist23, 24 that induces subtle dissociative symptoms,25 perceptual learning alterations and, critically, psychosis-like experiences (see26 for a review). Here, we examined placebo-controlled, within-subject effects of a single dose of ketamine.
The task was adapted from previous paradigms.27, 28 On each trial, participants made a decision in response to a visual cue. The two options were always betting £1 versus betting 10p. The two options thus differed in risk, defined as the variance of possible outcomes. This does not imply that probability of winning was known, since it had to be learned by trial and error. This probability was 80% given one (positive) cue and 20% for the other (negative) cue. The optimal policy was to select the risky option following the positive cue and the safer option following the negative cue. To introduce instability into the environment, contingencies were reversed three times, such that the positive cue became the negative one and vice-versa. This task is close to tasks previously used to examine model learning under volatility (as in Behrens et al.29), except that transitions in probabilistic contingencies were not smooth but rather abrupt, as we wanted subjects to experience large variations in confidence, from the beginning to the end of learning blocks.
The key challenge posed to participants by our task was to notice unexpected outcomes that signaled a change in contingencies while ignoring those related to the probabilistic nature of these contingencies. Ignoring probabilistic errors requires confidence in the estimates of experimental regularities. Thus, we hypothesized that ketamine would prevent subjects from ignoring probabilistic errors, leading to sub-optimal behavior at the end of learning blocks, where subjects under placebo would fully exploit the learned contingencies. We explored the neural underpinnings of this ketamine-induced dysfunction, with the prediction that activity in confidence-related brain areas would show altered dynamics during the course of learning. Neural responses were concurrently tracked using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), while subjects performed the probabilistic contingency learning task. Each participant underwent this procedure during both ketamine and placebo infusions.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Twenty-one healthy, right-handed volunteers (11 males), aged 25–37 years (mean 28.7, s.d. 3.2), were recruited from the local community by advertisement, and screened using an initial telephone interview and subsequent personal interview. Exclusion criteria were: personal/familial history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, MRI contra-indications, illicit substance use in the last 12 months or any lifetime substance misuse syndrome or alcoholism, history of cardiac illness or high blood pressure, weight >10% above ideal body mass index. The study was approved by the Cambridge Local Research Ethics Committee, Cambridge, England, and was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was given by all of the subjects.
Ketamine infusion
Racemic ketamine (2 mg ml−1) was administered intravenously by initial bolus and subsequent continuous target-controlled infusion using a computerized pump (Graseby 3500; Graseby Medical, Watford, UK) to achieve plasma concentrations of 100 ng ml−1 using the pharmacokinetic parameters of a three-compartment model.30 One blood sample was drawn prior to the fMRI scan. Blood sample was placed on ice, plasma obtained by centrifugation and plasma samples stored at −70 oC. Plasma ketamine concentration was measured by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry.
Experimental design
A double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized, within-subjects design was used (see Figure 1a). At each visit, after starting the infusion of saline or low-dose ketamine, subjects underwent a clinical rating of positive psychotic symptoms as assessed by the Rating Scale for Psychotic Symptoms.31 Seven key items on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale32 representing symptoms of the psychosis prodrome (somatic concerns, anxiety-depression, elevated mood, grandiosity, hallucination and unusual thought content) were also assessed. Dissociative symptoms were assessed by the Clinician Administered Dissociative States Scale.33 Subjects then performed the probabilistic learning task in the fMRI scanner. Subjects also performed two other cognitive tasks while in the fMRI scanner. These were perceptual tasks not related to the current task and will not be reported here. Resting state data were also acquired.34
Behavioral task
The task (see Figure 1) required participants, on each trial, to make a choice between a more and less risky option, indicating their choice by pressing a key or not. Risk taking was orthogonalized with respect to the motor dimension, so that pressing the key was assigned to the risky response only for half of participants and to the less risky response for the other half.
The risky (‘risk' being defined as the variance of the outcome) choice would lead to either the gain or the loss of £1, while the less risky option would lead to either the gain or loss of 10 pence. There were two contextual cues. One was associated with 80% chance of winning £1 (and a corresponding 20% chance of losing £1) following the risky choice and with 80% chance of winning 10 pence (and a 20% chance of losing 10 pence) following the less risky choice. For the other cue the contingencies were the opposite, that is, the risky choice would lead to an 80% chance of losing £1, while the less risky choice gave an 80% chance of losing 10 pence.
An unannounced contingency reversal occurred after each block of 60 trials (for a total of three reversals across the 240 trials). Reversal means that the positive cue (for which the risky choice was optimal) became the negative one and vice-versa. Therefore participants encountered the same contingency set only twice during the experiment.
Two abstract cues randomly taken among 24 letters from the Agathodaimon font were used. After fixation delay and cue display, the response interval was indicated on the computer screen by a question mark. The interval was fixed to 3 s and the response was taken at the end: this response was categorized as ‘risky'or ‘less risky' and was written on the screen as soon as the delay had elapsed. Monetary outcome was then displayed for 2 s. Participants were explicitly told that they would not receive the virtual money earned during the task. Instead, they were paid a fix amount that compensated for their time and their expenses associated with taking part in the study.
Before performing the task in the scanner, participants were familiarized with the task structure and with the notion that cue-outcome relationships were not necessarily constant. However, they were not warned that contingencies could be reversed.
Model-free behavioral analysis
The overall percentage of risky response and button presses were compared between sessions in order to assess drug effects on choice and motor impulsivity, respectively. To assess drug effects on learning, the percentage of optimal responses (risky choice for the positive cue, less risky choice for the negative cue) were collapsed across the two cues and averaged within six bins of 10 consecutive trials. These data were then submitted to repeated-measure analysis of variance with three experimental factors (bin*block*session) and subjects as random factor. Post-hoc comparisons were performed to characterize the learning deficit observed under ketamine.
Model-based behavioral analysis
The whole model space consisted of 27 models (see SOM): three variants of the reinforcement learning level without any confidence monitoring plus 24 variants of the hierarchical model (three reinforcement learning models × two ways to compute confidence × four ways to modulate low-level parameters) (see Figure 3 for a more detailed description of model space).
All models were inverted using a variational Bayes approach under the Laplace approximation,35, 36, 37 http://sites.google.com/site/jeandaunizeauswebsite/). This algorithm not only inverts nonlinear models but also estimates their evidence, which represents a trade-off between accuracy (goodness of fit) and complexity (degrees of freedom). The log-evidences estimated for each participant and model were submitted to a group-level random-effect analysis separately for placebo and ketamine sessions. To complete model selection, we also performed family analyses.37
fMRI data analysis
fMRI data were preprocessed and statistically analyzed using SPM5 toolbox (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) running on Matlab (Mathworks). T1-weighted structural images were coregistered with the mean functional image, segmented, and normalized to a standard T1 template and averaged across all subjects to allow group-level anatomical localization. The first five volumes of each session were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. Preprocessing consisted of spatial realignment, normalization using the same transformation as structural images, and spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel with a full-width at half-maximum of 8 mm.
We devised two general linear models (GLM) to account for individual time series. The first GLM included separate categorical regressors for cue and outcome onsets, respectively, modulated by the computational variables, βm and αm. As parametric modulators were applied to different categorical regressors, they were not orthogonalized to each other. Note, however, that their correlation was quite low (R2=0.1) In the second GLM, outcome onsets were modulated by two computational variables, outcome category (confirmatory vs contradictory) and αm, that were serially orthogonalized, following on SPM default procedure. This second GLM was exclusively used for the region of interest (ROI) analysis. These variables were computed using subject-specific free parameters of the best fitting computational model (see computational results) and were then z-scored. All regressors of interest were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. To correct for motion artifacts, subject-specific realignment parameters were modeled as covariates of no interest. Linear contrasts of regression coefficients were computed at the subject level and then taken to group-level random effect analyses.
Neural correlates of choice temperature and learning rate were identified in placebo sessions using a whole-brain one-sample t-test (cluster generating threshold P<0.001 uncorrected, cluster level threshold P<0.05 family-wise error corrected). The impact of ketamine on these networks was assessed using a paired t-test between ketamine and placebo sessions (cluster generating threshold P<0.01 uncorrected, cluster level threshold P<0.05 family-wise error corrected). In order to maximize sensitivity and to ensure that drug effects were only assessed within task-relevant networks, this analysis was masked by the parametric modulations (by choice temperature or learning rate) obtained when pooling placebo and ketamine sessions.
For ROI analyses, we extracted the regression estimates (betas) from spheres of 8mm in diameter (corresponding to the full-width at half-maximum of the Gaussian kernel used for spatial smoothing), centered on group-level activation peaks. The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) ROI, that was used to perform a comparison between placebo and ketamine session, was defined from the second-level analysis pooling both placebo and ketamine sessions in order to avoid biasing this comparison in favor of placebo sessions.
Additional GLMs were computed for illustrative purpose only. In these GLMs, trials were sorted in six bins of confidence (as defined in the best computational model) or trial number in a block (as in the model-free analysis: the first ten trials of each block, the following ten and so on). These GLMs were used to plot the hemodynamic response at cue and outcome onsets.
Results
Clinical assessments
The mean blood plasma concentration of ketamine during infusion was 96.01±19.11 ng ml−1. Paired t-tests indicated that ketamine caused a significant increase in positive psychotic symptoms as measured by the Rating Scale for Psychotic Symptoms (t(20)=5.43, P<0.001) and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (t(20)=2.8, P=0.011), as well as in dissociative symptoms as measured by the Clinician Administered Dissociative States Scale (t(20)=3.72, P=0.0013).
Behavioral results
Choice and motor impulsivity did not differ between drug conditions (risky choice: 48.2% vs 47.8%, t(20)=0.29, P=0.8; button press: 53.0% vs 51.7%, t(20)=1.05, P=0.3). There was a main effect of learning, with optimal choices increasing across bins (F(5,100)=66.77, P<0.001), a main effect of block (F(3,60)=4.57, P<0.01) with more optimal choices during the first (pre-reversal) block (80%) compared with others (74%). There was no other main effect and no interaction between factors (all P>0.1). Post-hoc analysis showed a significant effect of drug status in the last trial bin (see Figure 2a), with higher performance under placebo (F(1,20)=5.641, P=0.028) without main effect nor interaction with block (both P>0.1). Indeed, during ketamine infusion, participants apportioned their responses in a way that matched or slightly exceeded the 80% probability of positive reinforcement (81.1%, t(20)=0.37, P=0.7 in comparison with 80%). In contrast, they optimized their behavior under placebo (87.2%, t(20)=2.52, P=0.02 compared to 80%). In summary, this preliminary behavioral analysis suggests that ketamine reduced the ability to go beyond probability matching, that is, to stabilize behavior in the face of probabilistic (misleading) unexpected outcomes. This hypothesis was formally assessed by using computational modeling.
Computational modeling results
To explore a comprehensive set of possible strategies, we fitted qualitatively different models to the observed choices (see SOM for details). All models estimate the trial-wise values attached to the two cues, and use these values to predict choices, through a softmax function.
A first series of models were designed to account for low-level reinforcement learning. Following a standard ‘delta' rule,38 these models update after each trial the current cue value in proportion to prediction error, defined as the outcome value minus the expected value.
In a basic version, the outcome was simply the monetary amount (+£1, +0.1£, −0.1£ or -1£). In a second version, we integrated some understanding of the task structure by including the possibility that cue values were coded at a more abstract level, as if subjects figured out that all the information needed was the outcome valence (+ or −). In a third version the two cue values were updated after every outcome, to model the possibility that subjects realized that they always had an opposite valence, that is, information about the status of one cue also gave information about the status of the other.
Reinforcement learning models have constant parameters (learning rate α and choice stochasticity β). This limits the capacity to optimize the behavioral policy around the end of learning blocks, once subjects believe themselves to have a reasonably good estimation of contingencies. At this point, prediction errors should be tempered, and choices tuned to a more deterministic exploitation of learned contingencies.29, 39, 40 Conversely, when contingencies suddenly change after reversals, prediction errors should be given more weight, and choices should be more exploratory. This can be implemented in an optimal way using a hierarchical Bayesian architecture.29, 40 Some evidence has been found that human behavior can be accounted for by hierarchical Bayesian models.41, 42 However, Bayesian updates of probability distributions may become computationally cumbersome, and human subjects sometimes follow simpler heuristics, particularly when they are uncertain about the task structure.43, 44 Another way to optimize behavior is to subordinate the reinforcement learning parameters to a higher level of control that monitors performance. This idea has been proposed and formalized in the so-called meta-learning theoretical framework,45 which addresses the question of how machines can learn how to learn. This principle has been implemented for instance to adjust the exploration rate during the course of learning, and provides a good fit of nearly optimal primate behavior.46, 47
A second series of models followed this latter principle: they included a meta-cognitive level consisting in updating confidence (the belief that current representations are correct) so as to downregulate contingency learning and choice stochasticity. These hierarchical models allowed us to determine more precisely which level of learning was altered by ketamine infusion. Confidence was monitored using a delta rule in all the following models, which differed in the way outcomes were used to assess performance. A first variant used the absolute value of the prediction error generated in the lower reinforcement learning level, implementing the intuition that subjects should be more confident when prediction errors are reduced.48, 49 A second variant (following Khamassi et al.47) coded the outcome in terms of optimality: 0 for non-optimal outcomes (losing £1 or winning only 10p) and 1 for optimal outcomes (winning £1 or losing only 10p). In both variants, confidence could be used to modulate learning rate (αm), choice temperature (βm) or both, with different or identical weight. Optimizing choice temperature means favoring exploitation when confidence increases. Optimizing learning rate means increasing sensitivity to confirmatory outcomes and decreasing sensitivity to contradictory outcomes when confidence increases. Confirmatory means that the valence of the outcome is the same as the valence estimated by the model. Thus, when confidence was close to 0, the learning rate was similar for confirmatory and contradictory outcomes, but as confidence increased, it got closer to 1 for confirmatory outcomes and to 0 for contradictory outcomes.
Bayesian model selection was performed separately for placebo and ketamine sessions (see Figure 3 and Figure 4). The best model was the same in both sessions but the evidence was higher for placebo (xp=0.96; Supplementary Table S1) than for ketamine (xp=0.45; Supplementary Table S2). At the low level, this best model implemented an informed reinforcement learning rule, using the outcome valence (+ or −) to update the two cue values. At the high level, confidence was updated using the outcome optimality, and impacted both learning rate and choice temperature, with identical weights. Family model comparison37 confirmed that the best model was the same in both sessions though in ketamine sessions there was less clear evidence for the necessity of a meta-cognitive level that monitors confidence and allows confidence to modulate low-level parameters (see SOM for details).
We next compared the free parameters of this best model between placebo and ketamine sessions, with paired-tests (Figure 2b, Supplementary Table S3). The parameter that significantly differed between sessions was the weight that confidence had on learning rate and choice temperature (t(20)=2.3, P=0.027). Thus, ketamine reduced the impact of confidence on low-level parameters. This attenuation could therefore explain the deleterious effect of the drug on ability to optimize behavior when confidence increases, towards the end of learning blocks.
Neuroimaging results
The computational analysis demonstrated that the behavioral effects of ketamine were underpinned by a shift in the dynamics of choice temperature and learning rate (βm and αm), which were insufficiently tuned by the confidence increases within learning blocks. To identify the underlying neural effects, we therefore focused on the neural representation of βm and αm, which, in principle, should be used to make choices at cue onsets and to update values at outcome onsets respectively. For each time point (cue and outcome onsets), we first analyzed the placebo session to identify the neural representation of βm or αm in the normal brain. We then directly compared placebo and ketamine sessions.
At choice onset
Under placebo, βm was correlated with activity in a large fronto-parietal network, including dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), frontopolar cortex and bilateral lateral prefrontal cortex. Other correlations were observed in the anterior insula, in addition to subcortical regions encompassing bilateral caudate nucleus, thalamus and cerebellum (Figure 5, Table 1). Put simply, elevated temperature was associated with enhanced activity in these regions. Conversely, βm was negatively correlated with activity in a bilateral network including cuneus, precuneus, posterior cingulate and medial temporal lobe.
Table 1. Brain regions reflecting confidence-modulated choice temperature (βm).
Structure | MNI coordinates (x, y, z) | Z-score | |
---|---|---|---|
Positive | Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex | −4, 24, 48 | 4.83 |
48, 20, 52 | 4.78 | ||
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex | 48, 28, 42 | 5 | |
36, 52, 24 | 4.31 | ||
−28, 56, 26 | 4.18 | ||
−46, 14, 44 | 4.26 | ||
−38, 30, 42 | 4.24 | ||
36, 54, −4 | 3.91 | ||
Frontopolar cortex | 14, 60, −10 | 4.66 | |
−42, 48, −4 | 4.05 | ||
−22, 58, −6 | 4.49 | ||
50, −52, 46 | 4.01 | ||
(Inferior) parietal cortex | −42, −52, 42 | 4.73 | |
34, 22, −12 | 4.09 | ||
Anterior insula | 38, 22, 6 | 3.99 | |
−36, 18, −6 | 3.96 | ||
40, −66, −50 | 3.74 | ||
Cerebellum | −28, −74, −36 | 4.72 | |
16, 2, 14 | 4.6 | ||
Caudate nucleus | 10, −12, 0 | 4.17 | |
Thalamus | −8, −14, 0 | 4.35 | |
Negative | Precuneus | −12, −48, 12 | 4.31 |
Posterior cingulate | 22, −52, 20 | 4.25 | |
−12, −58, 22 | 3.93 | ||
Cuneus | 22, −88, 26 | 3.41 | |
Medial temporal lobe | 36, −38, −2 | 4.55 | |
−40, −36, −6 | 3.53 | ||
Post-central gyrus | 24, −48, 66 | 3.49 | |
Placebo vs ketamine | Insula | 48, 12, 8 | 3.77 |
36, 20, 8 | 2.82 | ||
Cerebellum | 38, −66, −46 | 3.87 | |
−10, −82, −24 | 3.15 | ||
Middle frontal gyrus | 30, 18, 38 | 3.6 | |
−42, 16, 42 | 3.27 | ||
Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex | 6, 26, 60 | 3.07 | |
12, 26, 52 | 3.03 | ||
−4, 26, 58 | 2.96 | ||
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex | 40, 56, 20 | 2.81 | |
46, 30, 38 | 2.62 | ||
−42, 16, 42 | 3.27 | ||
−36, 12, 52 | 2.79 | ||
(Inferior) parietal cortex | −54, −44, 42 | 3.26 | |
frontopolar cortex | −36, 54, 12 | 3.1 | |
42, 58, −4 | 2.67 |
In the ketamine session, the positive correlation with βm was significantly reduced compared to placebo in a bilateral fronto-parietal network, including the dmPFC, bilateral frontopolar cortex, bilateral lateral prefrontal cortex and left parietal cortex, as well as the anterior insula (Figure 5, Table 1). Thus, trial-to-trial variations in temperature expressed in the fronto-parietal network were diminished under ketamine. There was no significant difference between sessions for the negative correlation with βm.
At outcome onset
Under placebo, we observed a positive correlation with αm in the vmPFC and bilateral posterior insula extending to the superior temporal cortex (Figure 5, Table 2). These regions therefore increased their responses to confirmatory outcomes, and decreased their responses to contradictory outcomes, as confidence accumulated within learning blocks. Conversely, there was a negative correlation in the right anterior insula.
Table 2. Brain regions reflecting confidence-modulated learning rate (αm).
Structure | MNI coordinates (x, y, z) | Z-score | |
---|---|---|---|
Positive | Posterior insula | 58, −8, −2 | 4.52 |
40, −18, −2 | 3.69 | ||
−36, −18, −2 | 3.68 | ||
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex | 6, 36, −10 | 4.09 | |
Negative | Anterior insula | 38, 26, −6 | 3.7 |
There was no significant difference in the correlation with αm between placebo and ketamine sessions at the whole-brain level, nor in a ROI analysis focusing on the vmPFC (P>0.1). Correlation with αm corresponds to an interaction between confidence and outcome category (confirmatory or contradictory). We verified that the correlation was not reducible to the main effect of outcome category: when this was regressed out, the correlation with αm was still significant in our vmPFC ROI under placebo (t(20)=2.79; P=0.01) but not under ketamine (t(20)=1.41; P=0.18), though the direct comparison was not significant (P>0.1). In short, under placebo but not ketamine, the difference between confirmatory and contradictory outcomes was amplified following the trial-wise increase in confidence within learning blocks.
Discussion
Our working hypothesis was that early psychosis is characterized by a state in which the ability to acquire a robust and confident model of the world is lost. We tested this hypothesis at both the computational and neural levels, by combining a pharmacological model of early psychosis through NMDA blockade with model-based analysis of behavioral choices and fMRI data. The effects of NMDA blockade manifested in two ways:1 a decreased ability to optimize contingency learning in conditions of high confidence,2 a concurrent alteration in the regulation of brain systems reflecting choice stochasticity, notably in a bilateral fronto-parietal network including the dmPFC. Through use of a low dose of ketamine (rather than a higher one which would cause global cognitive difficulties), we have been able to identify a subtle and interpretable effect. Our findings have implications both for our understanding of contingency learning mechanisms and for theoretical perspectives on the emergence of psychosis. Because our experiment was carried out in a limited number of participants, as is common to pharmaco-MRI studies for obvious ethical reasons, we consider the implications below as primarily theoretical suggestions that will guide further investigations.
Contingency learning mechanisms in an unstable environment
Our benchmark computational model was a standard Q-learning algorithm, which has been shown to provide a good account of instrumental learning in a variety of situations.50 However, the task was expressly designed such that Q-learning would not be optimal. This is because Q-learning gives a constant weight to outcomes in value updating, and a constant weight to value estimates in decision making. Yet it is adaptive to adjust these weights in unstable environments, where contingencies are stochastic and susceptible to sudden reversals, depending on the confidence in value estimates. Behavioral data suggested that participants did modulate choice and learning parameters as a function of confidence. To analyze this we developed a hierarchical model with a meta-cognitive level that monitors confidence and modulates first-level Q-learning parameters, an approach that has been formalized in the meta-learning framework.45, 46, 51 At the meta-cognitive level, Bayesian model selection indicated that an independent delta rule on outcome optimality (similar to that used in Khamassi et al.47) provided a better fit than a direct accumulation of unsigned prediction errors (as implemented in 48). Our construct of confidence can therefore be considered as surface monitoring, since it remains blind to the computations driving choices. In the model that best captured the behavioral data, both choice temperature and learning rate were dynamically adjusted as a function of confidence. Moreover, confidence had a differential impact on confirmatory outcomes (whose weight was amplified) and contradictory outcomes (whose weight was reduced). Together, these confidence-based adjustments enabled stabilizing internal representations of environmental contingencies (cue value estimates) and optimizing behavioral policy (exploitation of cue values).
Our concept of confidence can be linked to several recent theoretical propositions, in which higher level representations control lower-level processes. For example, it has been suggested that uncertainty, which quantifies ignorance about true values, drives the trade-off between exploitation and exploration.52 In the predictive coding framework, the precision of (or confidence in) beliefs determines the weight that prediction errors have in belief updating. Indeed, aberrant encoding of precision has been recently proposed to account for various aspects of psychosis.10 Some implementations of hierarchical Bayesian modeling can also be seen as very close to our approach, particularly when both the learning and decision rules are modulated by precision estimates.42 Note however that a new and important feature of our model is the differential impact of confidence on learning depending on the nature of the outcome (confirmatory or not), which allows neglect of contradictory information. We acknowledge that the concept of confidence is used for convenience, and corresponds in fact to a running estimate of performance. Whether this measure matches what participants would report as a feeling of confidence remains to be demonstrated.
Neuroimaging data provided additional support for our hierarchical model. At the time of cues, trial-wise variation in choice temperature was reflected in activation of a fronto-parietal network that has been previously implicated in cognitive control.53, 54, 55, 56, 57 This does not imply that all these regions have the function of representing choice temperature. Their activity might represent an indirect correlate of variations in this computational variable. In particular, regions such as the dmPFC has been involved in monitoring errors,58, 59 detecting conflicts60, 61 and making decisions under uncertainty.59, 62, 63 This region might signal the necessity of additional control, or even implement this necessary control, in periods of doubt regarding which choice is the best.64, 65 At the time of outcomes, trial-wise variation in learning rate was positively reflected in regions such as the vmPFC, which has been implicated in encoding the subjective value of stimuli.66, 67 Here, this region increased its response to confirmatory outcomes, and decreased its response to contradictory outcomes, from the beginning to the end of learning blocks. This finding extends a previous report that the vmPFC integrates option value and choice confidence68 by showing that this integration also applies to outcomes. Interestingly, the reverse pattern of activity was observed in the anterior insula, a region involved in signaling aversive values.69, 70 Thus, these two regions appeared to mediate the influence of meta-cognitive control on proximal reactions to gains and losses, such that they align to the distal goal of optimizing performance.
Emergence of psychosis through NMDA blockade
Model-based analysis of the behavior suggested that NMDA blockade was associated with a reduced capacity to stabilize an internal model in order to capitalize on environmental regularities. This was evidenced by a reduced weight of confidence on choice temperature and learning rate. The performance deficit induced by ketamine infusion was therefore observed at the end of learning blocks, when confidence should be high enough to stabilize cue value estimation and exploitation policy. Our findings thus show that ketamine was associated with diminished ability to stabilize cue value estimates in the presence of probabilistic errors, as if a persistent doubt undermined optimization of behavior and made them more vulnerable to the effects of ‘noise' trials.
In a very simple environment as in our task (two cues with opposite values), such an impairment has limited impact and could hardly induce strange beliefs. In a more complex environment, where multiple internal explanatory models can be held at the same time, we would expect this impairment to forge strange beliefs, by combination of existing models or through the emergence of unexpected explanations. Our results therefore extend previous accounts of early psychosis, in which altered prediction errors lead to a sense of strangeness and to abnormalities in belief updating.9, 15, 26 Our findings suggest that it is important to take into account not just how prediction errors are used in low-level associative learning, but in how outcome optimality is integrated to modulate low-level parameters, via confidence monitoring.
We note that changes in key behavioral parameters did not correlate with the subtle psychopathology induced by this low dose of ketamine. This is perhaps unsurprising given the lack of statistical power—our experiment was devised with a view to identifying differences between ketamine and placebo rather than across-subject correlations. We see two other reasons that could account for this limitation. First, the neuro-cognitive perturbations that we demonstrated here might have different kinetics from those of psychotic symptoms (the former preceding the latter). Therefore, these two dimensions might remain uncorrelated at a given time. Second, if we assume that psychotic-like symptoms yield from more elementary cognitive dysfunctions, this link could be modulated (and hence blurred) by several factors, such as the existence of baseline (pre-ketamine) bizarre ideas, or the ability to introspect and conscious access to these dysfunctions and therefore to report psychotic-like symptoms.
In line with the behavioral analysis, the fMRI data showed that the confidence-based modulation of Q-learning parameters was significantly altered during ketamine infusion. Specifically, brain activity reflecting choice temperature was significantly less modulated by confidence under ketamine than placebo. This difference was observed in a bilateral fronto-parietal network, including the dmPFC. A detrimental effect of ketamine on dmPFC activation is in line with repeated observations of dorsal cingulate cortex impairment in patients with schizophrenia.71 Critically, here we offer a computational account of this effect, suggesting that that dmPFC impairment might play a key role in early symptoms of psychosis by compromising belief updating and policy adjustment in unstable environments. This dmPFC dysfunction could either alter confidence level or perturb the impact of confidence on behavioral policy.
The effect of ketamine on fronto-parietal regions might also relate to the well-established changes in consciousness produced by higher doses of ketamine,72 since the global workspace theory.73, 74 implicates these regions in conscious access by Interestingly, modulation of choice temperature by confidence was initially proposed to regulate the activity of workspace neurons whose role is to determine the degree of effort invested in decision making46, 47 in keeping with the concept of vigilance.73 One may speculate that the meta-cognitive component of our model, notably confidence monitoring and down-regulation of choice temperature, requires conscious processing. Thus, dysfunction of this part could be linked to both alteration of consciousness with higher doses of ketamine and to dysfunction of conscious processing in schizophrenic patients,75, 76 who would perform contingency learning in a more implicit way. Evidence for such a speculation would require further experiments manipulating consciousness levels.
The earliest stages of psychotic illness present an intriguing and puzzling set of cognitive changes. Computational psychiatry1, 3 offers new and rich frameworks for considering these changes and linking them to underlying neural alterations. Here we have shown that pharmacological fMRI, employing a well-established drug model of psychosis, presents a powerful tool in developing such frameworks, offering an opportunity to determine how controlled perturbations in glutamate function relate to altered balance in the dynamic control of optimal learning and behavior.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Mael Lebreton and Jean Daunizeau for helpful conversations and advices. FV was supported by the Groupe Pasteur Mutualité. RG was supported by the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale and the Fondation Bettencourt Schueller. SP is supported by a Marie Curie Intra-European fellowship (FP7-PEOPLE-2012-IEF). AF was supported by National Health and Medical Research Council grants (IDs: 1050504 and 1066779) and an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship (ID: FT130100589). This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust and the Bernard Wolfe Health Neuroscience Fund.
Footnotes
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Molecular Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/mp)
RG has received compensation as a member of the scientific advisory board of Janssen, Lundbeck, Roche, Takeda. He has served as consultant and/or speaker for Astra Zeneca, Pierre Fabre, Lilly, Otsuka, SANOFI, Servier and received compensation, and he has received research support from Servier. PCF has consulted for Glaxo SmithKline and Lundbeck and received compensation. MOK has received compensation as a member of the scientific advisory board of Roche. She has served as speaker for Janssen and received unrestricted support for conference organization from Janssen and Otsuka-Lunbeck, and she has been invited to scientific meetings by Lundbeck and Takeda. AS has consulted for Servier and received compensation. FV has served as speaker for Servier and received compensation.
Supplementary Material
References
- Montague PR, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ, Dayan P. Computational psychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16: 72–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maia TV, Frank MJ. From reinforcement learning models to psychiatric and neurological disorders. Nat Neurosci 2011; 14: 154–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Stephan KE, Montague R, Dolan RJ. Computational psychiatry: the brain as a phantastic organ. Lancet Psychiatry 2014; 1: 148–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Murray GK, Honey GD, Aitken MRF, Shanks DR, Robbins TW et al. Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain 2007; 130: 2387–2400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gradin VB, Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Stickle C, Milders M et al. Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain 2011; 134: 1751–1764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RW, Vercammen A, Lenroot R, Moore L, Langton JM, Short B et al. Disambiguating ventral striatum fMRI-related BOLD signal during reward prediction in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 2012; 17: 235, 80-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray GK, Corlett PR, Clark L, Pessiglione M, Blackwell AD, Honey G et al. Substantia nigra/ventral tegmental reward prediction error disruption in psychosis. Mol Psychiatry 2008; 13: 239, 67-76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waltz JA, Schweitzer JB, Ross TJ, Kurup PK, Salmeron BJ, Rose EJ et al. Abnormal responses to monetary outcomes in cortex, but not in the basal ganglia, in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35: 2427–2439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher PC, Frith CD. Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10: 48–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams RA, Stephan KE, Brown HR, Frith CD, Friston KJ. The computational anatomy of psychosis. Front Psychiatry 2013; 4: 47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch DM, Dowd EC. Goal representations and motivational drive in schizophrenia: the role of prefrontal-striatal interactions. Schizophr Bull 2010; 36: 919–934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JM, Waltz JA, Prentice KJ, Morris SE, Heerey EA. Reward processing in schizophrenia: a deficit in the representation of value. Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 835–847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deserno L, Boehme R, Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F. Reinforcement learning and dopamine in schizophrenia: dimensions of symptoms or specific features of a disease group? Front Psychiatry 2013; 4: 172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitson JA, Galinsky AD. Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science 2008; 322: 115–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 13–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett P, Fletcher P. The neurobiology of schizotypy: fronto-striatal prediction error signal correlates with delusion-like beliefs in healthy people. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50: 3612–3620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Aramaki M, Merer A, Cermolacce M, Ystad S, Kronland-Martinet R et al. Toward an exploration of feeling of strangeness in schizophrenia: perspectives on acousmatic and everyday listening. J Abnorm Psychol 2012; 121: 628–640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor K. Cognitive and meta-cognitive dimensions of psychoses. Can J Psychiatry 2009; 54: 152–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coltheart M, Langdon R, McKay R. Delusional belief. Annu Rev Psychol 2011; 62: 271–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fyfe S, Williams C, Mason OJ, Pickup GJ. Apophenia, theory of mind and schizotypy: perceiving meaning and intentionality in randomness. Cortex 2008; 44: 1316–1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broome MR, Johns LC, Valli I, Woolley JB, Tabraham P, Brett C et al. Delusion formation and reasoning biases in those at clinical high risk for psychosis. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 2007; 51: s38–s42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colbert SM, Peters ER. Need for closure and jumping-to-conclusions in delusion-prone individuals. J Nerv Ment Dis 2002; 190: 27–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal JH, Karper LP, Seibyl JP, Freeman GK, Delaney R, Bremner JD et al. Subanesthetic effects of the noncompetitive NMDA antagonist, ketamine, in humans. Psychotomimetic, perceptual, cognitive, and neuroendocrine responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51: 199–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Javitt DC, Zukin SR, Heresco-Levy U, Umbricht D. Has an angel shown the way? Etiological and therapeutic implications of the PCP/NMDA model of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2012; 38: 958–966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomarol-Clotet E, Honey GD, Murray GK, Corlett PR, Absalom AR, Lee M et al. Psychological effects of ketamine in healthy volunteers. Phenomenological study. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 189: 173–179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corlett PR, Honey GD, Krystal JH, Fletcher PC. Glutamatergic model psychoses: prediction error, learning, and inference. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011; 36: 294–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 2006; 442: 1042–1045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessiglione M, Petrovic P, Daunizeau J, Palminteri S, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 2008; 59: 561–567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Walton ME, Rushworth MF. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat Neurosci 2007; 10: 1214–1221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Absalom AR, Lee M, Menon DK, Sharar SR, De Smet T, Halliday J et al. Predictive performance of the Domino, Hijazi, and Clements models during low-dose target-controlled ketamine infusions in healthy volunteers. Br J Anaesth. 2007; 98: 615–623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chouinard G, Miller R. A rating scale for psychotic symptoms (RSPS): Part I: theoretical principles and subscale 1: perception symptoms (illusions and hallucinations). Schizophr Res 1999; 38: 101–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overall JE, Gorham D. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS): recent developments in ascertainment and scaling. Psychopharmacol Bull 1988; 24: 97–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Putnam FW, Southwick SM, Marmar C, Charney DS et al. Measurement of dissociative states with the clinician-administered dissociative states scale (CADSS). J Trauma Stress 1998; 11: 125–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dandash O, Harrison BJ, Adapa R, Gaillard R, Giorlando F, Wood SJ et al. Selective augmentation of striatal functional connectivity following NMDA receptor antagonism: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015; 40: 622–631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Mattout J, Trujillo-Barreto N, Ashburner J, Penny W. Variational free energy and the Laplace approximation. Neuroimage 2007; 34: 220–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daunizeau J, Adam V, Rigoux L. VBA: a probabilistic treatment of nonlinear models for neurobiological and behavioural data. PLoS Comput Biol 2014; 10: e1003441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigoux L, Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Daunizeau J. Bayesian model selection for group studies – revisited. Neuroimage 2014; 84: 971–985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton RS, Barto AG. Reinforcement Learning, a Bradford book. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA: Cambridge, MA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF, Behrens TE. Choice, Uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 2008; 11: 389–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathys C, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ, Stephan KE. A bayesian foundation for individual learning under uncertainty. Front Human Neurosci 2011; 5: 39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iglesias S, Mathys C, Brodersen KH, Kasper L, Piccirelli M, den Ouden HE et al. Hierarchical prediction errors in midbrain and basal forebrain during sensory learning. Neuron 2013; 80: 519–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diaconescu AO, Mathys C, Weber LA, Daunizeau J, Kasper L, Lomakina EI et al. Inferring on the intentions of others by hierarchical Bayesian learning. PLoS Comput Biol 2014; 10: e1003810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins A, Koechlin E. Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS Biol 2012; 10: e1001293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payzan-LeNestour E, Bossaerts P. Risk, unexpected uncertainty, and estimation uncertainty: Bayesian learning in unstable settings. PLoS Comput Biol 2011; 7: e1001048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doya K. Metalearning and neuromodulation. Neural Netw. 2002; 15: 495–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khamassi M, Enel P, Dominey PF, Procyk E. Medial prefrontal cortex and the adaptive regulation of reinforcement learning parameters. Prog Brain Res. 2013; 202: 441–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khamassi M, Lallee S, Enel P, Procyk E, Dominey PF. Robot cognitive control with a neurophysiologically inspired reinforcement learning model. Front Neurorobot 2011; 5: 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krugel LK, Biele G, Mohr PN, Li SC, Heekeren HR. Genetic variation in dopaminergic neuromodulation influences the ability to rapidly and flexibly adapt decisions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009; 106: 17951–17956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SW, Shimojo S, O'Doherty JP. Neural computations underlying arbitration between model-based and model-free learning. Neuron 2014; 81: 687–699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rangel A, Camerer C. Montague PR. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008; 9: 545–556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doya K. Modulators of decision making. Nat Neurosci 2008; 11: 410–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P. Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8: 1704–1711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Fronto-parietal network: flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci 2013; 17: 602–603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, Repovs G, Anticevic A, Braver TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16: 1348–1355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007; 7: 1–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glascher J, Adolphs R, Damasio H, Bechara A, Rudrauf D, Calamia M et al. Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2012; 109: 14681–14686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, Carter CS. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2012; 12: 241–268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch D, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 1998; 280: 747–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown JW, Braver TS. Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science 2005; 307: 1118–1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick M, Nystrom LE, Fissell K, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 1999; 402: 179–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 2004; 303: 1023–1026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MF, Walton ME, Kennerley SW, Bannerman DM. Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2004; 8: 410–417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkatraman V, Huettel SA. Strategic control in decision-making under uncertainty. Eur J Neurosci. 2012; 35: 1075–1082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 2013; 79: 217–240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JD, McClure SM, Yu AJ. Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2007; 362: 933–942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebreton M, Jorge S, Michel V, Thirion B, Pessiglione M. An automatic valuation system in the human brain: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuron 2009; 64: 431–439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy DJ, Glimcher PW. The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2012; 22: 1027–1038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Martino B, Fleming SM, Garrett N, Dolan RJ. Confidence in value-based choice. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16: 105–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palminteri S, Justo D, Jauffret C, Pavlicek B, Dauta A, Delmaire C et al. Critical roles for anterior insula and dorsal striatum in punishment-based avoidance learning. Neuron 2012; 76: 998–1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Büchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditionning: an event-related fonctional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron 1998; 20: 947–957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fornito A, Yucel M, Dean B, Wood SJ, Pantelis C. Anatomical abnormalities of the anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia: bridging the gap between neuroimaging and neuropathology. Schizophr Bull 2009; 35: 973–993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marland S, Ellerton J, Andolfatto G, Strapazzon G, Thomassen O, Brandner B et al. Ketamine: use in anesthesia. CNS Neurosci Therapeut 2013; 19: 381–389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Kerszberg M, Changeux JP. A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998; 95: 14529–14534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Naccache L. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 2001; 79: 1–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene S, Artiges E, Naccache L, Martelli C, Viard A, Schurhoff F et al. Conscious and subliminal conflicts in normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia: the role of the anterior cingulate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003; 100: 13722–13727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Cul A, Dehaene S, Leboyer M. Preserved subliminal processing and impaired conscious access in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63: 1313–1323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.