Abstract
Contemporary tracking studies reveal that low migratory connectivity between breeding and non-breeding ranges is common in migrant landbirds. It is unclear, however, how internal factors and early-life experiences of individual migrants shape the development of their migration routes and concomitant population-level non-breeding distributions. Stochastic wind conditions and geography may determine whether and where migrants end up by the end of their journey. We tested this hypothesis by satellite-tagging 31 fledgling honey buzzards Pernis apivorus from southern Finland and used a global atmospheric reanalysis model to estimate the wind conditions they encountered on their first outbound migration. Migration routes diverged rapidly upon departure and the birds eventually spread out across 3340 km of longitude. Using linear regression models, we show that the birds' longitudinal speeds were strongly affected by zonal wind speed, and negatively affected by latitudinal wind, with significant but minor differences between individuals. Eventually, 49% of variability in the birds' total longitudinal displacements was accounted for by wind conditions on migration. Some birds circumvented the Baltic Sea via Scandinavia or engaged in unusual downwind movements over the Mediterranean, which also affected the longitude at which these individuals arrived in sub-Saharan Africa. To understand why adult migrants use the migration routes and non-breeding sites they use, we must take into account the way in which wind conditions moulded their very first journeys. Our results present some of the first evidence into the mechanisms through which low migratory connectivity emerges.
Keywords: bird migration, orientation, weather, behavioural development, satellite-tracking
1. Introduction
One of the most robust patterns emerging from contemporary tracking studies of migrant landbirds is one of low migratory connectivity, whereby individuals which breed in close vicinity of each other diverge across huge geographical distances during the non-breeding phase of their annual cycle [1]. Although there are exceptions where strong connectivity between breeding and non-breeding sites exists [2,3], in most migrant landbirds, individuals from different breeding populations are likely to mix during the non-breeding season. We also know that migrant landbirds are typically highly faithful to individual breeding and non-breeding sites, temporarily residing in two or more areas along their individual migration cycle [4]. It remains unclear, however, how innate and environmental factors affect the development of individual migration routines during early life [5]. A major challenge in this regard is to resolve how stochastic environmental influences shape the first outbound migration of juvenile migrants [4]. Although many migrant landbirds travel in mixed-age groups, juveniles migrate independently from their parents and other elders in numerous species [6,7]. Such unexperienced migrants are assumed to follow an innate migratory heading for a predetermined amount of time during one or more bouts of migratory flight [8,9], which explains why young and inexperienced migrants are often observed not to compensate for wind drift [10–13] or experimentally induced displacements [14], and which suggests they only manage to settle wintering territories if they do not drift too far from suitable habitat. In such a system, there is a great potential for environmental factors, and especially geography and atmospheric circulation patterns, to influence the distribution of juvenile migrants, moulding patterns of migratory connectivity within and among breeding populations [1,4,15]. Much of this theory, however, have been developed based on site-specific radar observations and experiments, and still needs to be tested by tracking juvenile migrants [7,16].
Very little is known about how environmental factors shape the first outbound migrations of juvenile migrant landbirds that rely on genetic information because most juvenile tracking studies so far have been conducted on large species such as storks [17–19], cranes [20], kites [16], eagles [21,22] and vultures [23]; all of which learn strategic migration routes and stop-over sites from elder conspecifics. Only a handful of studies have tracked juvenile migrants that travel independently from elders, and these often yielded contrasting results about the role of innate and environmental factors in the development of individual migration routines. A satellite-tracking study of juvenile ospreys Pandion haliaetus and honey buzzards Pernis apivorus from Scandinavia, for example, confirmed that juveniles did not compensate for sidewinds towards predetermined goals as their adult conspecifics did [24,25]. By contrast, juveniles of other migrant landbird species are capable of navigating towards targeted non-breeding areas without elder guidance. Juvenile Eleonora's falcons Falco eleonora [26] and juvenile common cuckoos Cuculus canorus [27], for example, independently navigate to restricted non-breeding ranges, respectively, on Madagascar and south of the Congo Basin. Juvenile Eleonora's falcons from Sardinia thereby used a complex route involving a major shift in migratory orientation after they crossed the Sahara [26], whereas a juvenile common cuckoo followed a remarkably straight path from northern Europe to northern Angola [27]. Some juvenile waders engage in long and complex but adaptive detours that appear to be programmed genetically [28,29]. Contrary to expectation, it has also been shown that juvenile songbirds can compensate for large geographical displacements from their ‘normal’ migratory route [30] and that complex migration routes can be genetically hard-wired [31]. Juvenile migrants may also engage in remarkably straight trans-oceanic autumn migrations, which may require a more sophisticated strategy than simple vector-based navigation [32]. There is, in conclusion, still little empirical information about the influence of environmental conditions on the orientation of juvenile migrants and the role of early-life experiences in shaping individual migration routines and migratory connectivity [1,4,33].
In this paper, we present the first results of an ongoing study into the ontogeny of individual migration routines of European honey buzzards. Between 2011 and 2014, thirty one fledgling honey buzzards from southern Finland were equipped with Argos tracking devices and GSM-GPS-trackers before they left the nest. Three fledglings were confirmed to have died on the nest owing to predation (two) and sickness (one) and one died in Estonia shortly upon departure (electronic supplementary material, table S1) so we obtained a good sample of tracking data for 27 juveniles. Although adult honey buzzards engage in complex detours to circumvent geographical barriers and to exploit predictable large-scale wind regimes [34], the juveniles are unable to learn these routes during their first migration because they initiate migration one to two weeks later than the adults [24]. The juveniles then seem to follow an innate migratory heading [25], as expected for the bulk of migrant landbird species, and they do not circumvent geographical barriers in the same way as larger soaring migrants do [21,23,35]. As a result, and as juvenile honey buzzards do not compensate for side winds [10], we expect stochasticity in wind conditions on migration to determine at what longitude these juvenile migrants settle in sub-Saharan Africa. We test this hypothesis by annotating tracking data with wind estimates from a global atmospheric reanalysis model [36–38] and modelling the birds' longitudinal speed in relation to wind conditions encountered en route, accounting for possible individual differences in orientation. We also map residual (i.e. predicted–observed) longitudinal speeds to identify during what parts of the journey birds moved westward or eastward faster than predicted. Some birds may, for example, orient downwind more over sea than over land, in which case we expect to see higher residual longitudinal speeds over the Baltic and the Mediterranean for those individuals.
2. Methods
(a). Origin and tracking of juvenile honey buzzards
As a part of a long-term study [39,40], 21 honey buzzard nests on 16 territories in southern Finland (latitude 61°14′–63°12′ N, longitude 21°16′–23°31′ E) were visited between 2011 and 2014. Typically, nests were visited once in June to determine occupied nests and again in mid-July to ring chicks. A third nest visit was timed to the final stage of the brood phase to equip fledglings with solar-powered Argos GPS platform terminal transmitters (PTTs) (Microwave Telemetry Inc.) or GSM-GPS-trackers (Microwave Telemetry Inc., Ecotone). Tags weighed 22–27 g corresponding to approximately 3% of the birds' body mass at the time of deployment (913 ± 82 g; avg ± s.d., n = 31). We used the body-loop attachment method with a Teflon ribbon harness [41]. The amount and type of data the PTTs/trackers delivered varied depending on tracker model and programming schedule, but also other factors such as weather (cf. [42]).
The sex of the nestlings (17 females and 14 males, electronic supplementary material, table S1) was determined from DNA as extracted from blood samples using the salt extraction method. Introns of the sex-chromosome linked CHD gene were amplified to distinguish the sexes [43]. Ten microlitres of PCR reaction contained 5 µl of Phusion master mix (Thermofisher Scientific), 10 pmol of primers 2550F and 2718R, 2 µl of dH2O and 1 µl of DNA extract. The PCR products were separated on a 2% agarose gel.
(b). Data preparation
Honey buzzards engage in pre-migratory movements in Europe and also make itinerant movements within sub-Saharan Africa during the non-breeding season. We therefore developed some simple rules on the basis of which to categorize the migration period and checked whether the endpoints we calculated were representative of the longitude at which honey buzzards settled in sub-Saharan Africa (see the electronic supplementary material, figure S1 for full explanation).
Because honey buzzards interrupt travel at night, and possibly under adverse weather conditions, we excluded all resting events from our analyses. We did this by removing all overland locations where ground speed was lower than 1.39 m s−1 (approx. 5 km h−1). We did not exclude any fixes above sea, except for the fixes of M4 in the night of 3–4 October 2013 because this bird roosted on a ship. We calculated the loxodromic distance and time interval from each location to the next to determine the birds' ground speeds and used vector trigonometry to determine the longitudinal component of the birds' ground speeds (i.e. westward/eastward speed, Ubird). We determined whether fixes were situated over land or over water using the Global Self-consistent Hierarchical High-resolution Shoreline Database [44].
(c). Influence of wind on hourly longitudinal bird speed
Using the RNCEP package [36], we annotated every fix with zonal (i.e. westward(−)/eastward(+), Uwind) and latitudinal (i.e. southward(−)/northward(+), Vwind) wind components by linearly interpolating wind data from the 925 mB pressure level (corresponding to an average flight altitude of approx. 700 m [37]) in the NCEP global atmospheric reanalysis model [38]. Reanalysis data are generated on a 2.5° × 2.5° grid four times daily and resolve large-scale circulation patterns that can be used reliably to estimate wind conditions at the altitude of flight of soaring raptors [34,45,46]. Summary statistics for Ubird, Uwind and Vwind for each of the 27 birds that were used in this analysis are provided in the electronic supplementary material, table S2.
We then constructed generalized linear regression models (GLMs) to determine how hourly longitudinal bird speed (Ubird) was affected by Uwind and Vwind. We constructed a model including only Uwind, a model including the additive effects of Uwind and Vwind, and a model including an interaction effect between Uwind and Vwind. We then selected the most parsimonious model based on Akaike's information criterion (AIC [47]).
(d). Innate and parental factors
Linear regression plots show a positive relationship between Ubird and Uwind for all individuals, regardless of sex or territory where they hatched (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). Some individuals do seem to move west or east in windless conditions faster than others. We therefore extended the most parsimonious GLM with randomly varying intercepts per individual [48]. Fledglings tagged on the same territory may behave similarly owing to parental effects. We therefore constructed a mixed linear effects model with a nested design to allow for randomly varying intercepts between individuals and territories. To identify the most parsimonious model for Ubird, we then compare AIC and log-likelihood values using a restricted maximum likelihood approach [49].
(e). Identifying influential geographical features
Several factors such as geography, topography, thermal soaring conditions and time of day may influence the rate at which birds move longitudinally. However, instead of running an exhaustive model selection procedure with possibly confounding predictor variables, we decided to map residual (i.e. observed–predicted) hourly longitudinal speeds based on the most parsimonious mixed effects model. This allows us to visualize where birds responded differently to wind than they do on average across the entire flyway.
(f). Influence of wind and geography on the total longitudinal displacement of birds
Out of 27 birds that departed from Finland, 24 ultimately survived their first migration and 23 of those yielded sufficient data to quantify wind conditions along their entire trip (electronic supplementary material, table S1). We calculated the total longitudinal displacement (Δlong[°]) of these 23 individuals by subtracting the longitude at which the birds started migration from the longitude at which they ended migration (electronic supplementary material, table S3). We then constructed multiple linear regression models to predict the birds' total longitudinal displacements as a function of the mean zonal wind () and the mean latitudinal wind () encountered en route.
The birds diverge rapidly upon departure from Finland, partly because one-third of all birds departs via Scandinavia. We also constructed two models including an additional categorical variable to account for this route choice and selected the most parsimonious model including only significant predictor variables based on AIC [47].
Mixed effect models were implemented using the lmer-package in 64-bit R v. 3.3.3. All maps were produced using the ggplot2-package [49]. The three-dimensional plot (electronic supplementary material, figure S3a) was produced with the rgl-package [50].
3. Results
Our tracking data revealed a rapid divergence of juvenile migration routes upon departure from Finland, whereby birds spread out across 1034 km longitude by the time they reached latitude 55° N at the southern end of the Baltic Sea. Just before the Mediterranean, the spread had increased to 2286 km and by the end of migration, individuals had spread out across 3340 km longitude.
(a). Influence of wind conditions on hourly longitudinal bird speed
Linear regression models confirmed that Uwind, and to a lesser extent Vwind, significantly affected Ubird. Our most parsimonious model (table 1, model 3; electronic supplementary material, figure S3a) shows that on average juvenile honey buzzards moved westward at a rate of 0.76 m s−1 in the absence of winds, and that they drifted approximately 0.5 m s−1 with every 1 m s−1 increase of Ubird in either direction. In addition, there was a significant negative effect of Vwind and a significant negative interaction effect of Uwind : Vwind on Ubird (table 1, model 3), indicating that the degree to which birds drifted with zonal winds was exacerbated by headwinds (i.e. northward winds, Vwind > 0).
Table 1.
intercept |
βUwind |
βVwind |
βUwind
:
Vwind |
AIC | adjusted R2 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | |||
model 1 | −0.63 | 3.02 × 10−20 | 0.53 | 3.46 × 10−245 | 13478.50 | 0.36 | ||||
model 2 | −0.76 | 4.44 × 10−28 | 0.52 | 3.11 × 10−243 | −0.15 | 1.55 × 10−20 | 13394.09 | 0.38 | ||
model 3 | −0.76 | 5.67 × 10−29 | 0.47 | 1.13 × 10−187 | −0.11 | 4.83 × 10−11 | −0.03 | 1.17 × 10−9 | 13313.64 | 0.40 |
There was a clear positive influence of Uwind on Ubird across most of the flyway (figure 1). Eighteen of 27 individuals initiated migration in a south-eastward direction in eastward winds across the Gulf of Finland (figure 1b). All but two (Matti and Lisa) of nine individuals that departed westward or south-westward across Scandinavia and the Baltic Sea experienced winds with a westward component (figure 1b). Individuals that departed across Scandinavia continued to fly overland, even in weak sidewinds or in moderate eastward winds, until they reached southern Sweden. They then crossed the Baltic Sea in a south-eastward direction in eastward winds (e.g. Edit, figure 1b) or south-westward in westward winds (e.g. Gilda and Valentin, figure 1b). Once over mainland Europe, most birds experienced winds with a moderate to strong eastward component (figure 1a, 45–55° N) and concomitantly moved in a south-eastward direction. Some birds did move south-westward over Eastern Europe and the Balkans when they encountered westward winds (figure 1a, 40–50° N).
One bird (Julia) ended up over the Black Sea and initiated a reverse migratory movement, flying north-westward in opposing eastward winds until she approached the coast, but then continued travelling over water until near the Bosphorus. Over the Mediterranean Sea, the honey buzzards usually encountered winds with a weak zonal component (figure 1c). Some sea-crossing individuals engaged in pronounced longitudinal movements (e.g. Hans) that were not directed towards the nearest land. Once over Africa, most birds moved south-westward in strong westward winds (figure 1a). Three of four birds that travelled south-eastward over the Sahara did so in unusual eastward winds (figure 1a, Gilda, Hans and Valentin).
(b). Innate and parental factors
Including a random intercept for individuals significantly improved the most parsimonious GLM (table 1, model 3) but did not significantly alter estimates for wind effects compared with model 1. We did not find evidence for parental effects by nesting individuals per territory, as indicated by the small increase in AIC and log-likelihood compared with the model including only individual (electronic supplementary material, table S4). Adding sex as a fixed categorical effect to our most parsimonious GLM revealed no significant differences between males and females and so we did not use it for multilevel modelling (p = 0.13).
(c). Identifying influential geographical features
We mapped residuals from model 2 from the electronic supplementary material, table S4 (electronic supplementary material, figure S3a) to see when and where birds responded differently to wind while crossing geographical barriers (electronic supplementary material, figure S3b). The birds that departed into Scandinavia moved west faster than expected from their average individual response to wind conditions, especially the two individuals that departed westward in eastward winds (Matti and Lisa; electronic supplementary material, figure S3b, blues). Over the Black Sea and over the Mediterranean Sea, there occurred many ‘events’ whereby birds travelled westward (blues) or eastward (reds) faster than predicted by our most parsimonious model (e.g. Sven and Hans; electronic supplementary material, figure S2c). Other extremely low or high residual Ubird values occurred over Africa and across the whole journey birds tended to move westward moderately faster than predicted (electronic supplementary material, figure S2, greens), and to head downwind more over water than over land.
(d). Influence of wind and geography on the total longitudinal displacement of birds
We distinguished between the nine birds that departed across Scandinavia or across the Baltic Sea and the 18 individuals that departed across the Gulf of Finland to predict the birds' total longitudinal displacements. However, we could not find a way to categorize individuals based on route choice in other parts of the flyway. For example, when birds arrive at the Mediterranean in roughly the same location, they continue along roughly the same flight direction under similar wind conditions.
Multiple linear regression models revealed that and had a significant additive effect on the birds' total longitudinal displacements (Δlong[°], table 2). Route choice at departure, by contrast, only had a marginally significant effect on Δlong[°] (table 2), probably because only one bird that departed into Scandinavia in opposing winds (Matti) survived until the end of migration. Nevertheless, we found that the individuals with the largest residual longitudinal displacements (figure 2, Hans, Matti and Venus) had all engaged in longitudinal movements that were poorly accounted for by local wind conditions at some point in their journey (figure 1).
Table 2.
intercept |
βUwind |
βVwind |
βdeparture |
βUwind:Vwind |
AIC | adjusted R2 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | estimate | p-value | |||
model 1 | −15.59 | 4.21 × 10−10 | 1.97 | 0.046 | 157.20 | 0.14 | ||||||
model 2 | −17.93 | 5.91 × 10−12 | 2.06 | 0.009 | −3.01 | 0.001 | 146.13 | 0.49 | ||||
model 3 | −22.08 | 1.60 × 10−8 | 2.69 | 0.002 | −2.02 | 0.031 | 6.49 | 0.058 | 143.68 | 0.55 | ||
model 4 | −17.71 | 1.23 × 10−11 | 2.05 | 0.009 | −2.97 | 0.001 | −0.72 | 0.180 | 145.90 | 0.51 | ||
model 5 | −21.39 | 7.96 × 10−8 | 2.61 | 0.003 | −2.12 | 0.026 | 5.65 | 0.108 | −0.49 | 0.345 | 144.51 | 0.55 |
4. Discussion
Long-distance migrant landbirds that breed in close vicinity of each other in Europe typically spread out over vast geographical ranges in sub-Saharan Africa. Our results demonstrate that for honey buzzards, such low migratory connectivity is owing largely to stochasticity in the wind conditions that juvenile migrants encounter on their first outbound migration [4]. Although most juvenile honey buzzards travel south-eastward in eastward winds over northern and central Europe, this movement is offset by the fact that on average, juvenile honey buzzards move westward at a rate of 0.87 m s−1 in windless conditions, and by the prevalence of westward winds over the Sahara, which constitutes the longest segment of their journey. There were minor but significant differences in mean migratory orientation between individuals, but wind accounted for at least half the bird's total longitudinal displacement. All juveniles, both males and females, ended up further west than where they started their journey in southern Finland, and more than half ended up west of 10° E, well within the wintering range of honey buzzards that breed as far west as Sweden [24] and the Netherlands [34,37].
Atmospheric circulation patterns seem to impact on migratory connectivity in migrant landbird populations in different ways. On the one hand, stochasticity in wind conditions drives low migratory connectivity between breeding and non-breeding ranges of the Finnish honey buzzard population. On the other hand, the Afro-Palaearctic flyways are characterized by distinct latitudinal wind regimes [34] that are likely to lead to predictable patterns of migratory connectivity between different breeding populations. Juveniles that hatched in eastern breeding populations, such as those we studied here, are likely to end up in the core wintering range of conspecifics that breed in western breeding populations. By contrast, it seems unlikely that juveniles which hatched in western Europe would end up in the non-breeding range of conspecifics that breed further east and that winter in central and eastern Africa. Atmospheric circulation patterns may therefore help explain genetic structure of extant populations of P. apivorus, as has been done for marine migrants based on ocean currents [51].
We were unable to directly account for geography to predict the total longitudinal displacement of juvenile honey buzzards. Moreover, the honey buzzards were not as reluctant to engage in long sea-crossings as many larger soaring migrants [21,23]. Nevertheless, and contrary to our expectations, geography affected the longitude at which certain individuals settled. For example, one of two birds that departed westward from Finland into Scandinavia in opposing winds survived its first migration and ended up further west in Africa than any other juvenile (Matti). In other cases, birds ended up further west or east than predicted because they engaged in protracted downwind movements over the Mediterranean or other barriers, and because they did not compensate for these movements later in their journey (e.g. Hans and Venus). We investigated all tracks in detail to obtain clues about why these birds engaged in such movements. One bird (Sven) started flying westward when night fell during its sea-crossing, roosted on a westward-sailing ship shortly thereafter, continued flying westward in westward winds the next morning and died a few days later in northern Algeria. This bird thus probably flew downwind because it was in relatively poor condition [52,53]. The fact that another bird suddenly changed travel direction at nightfall while crossing the Black Sea suggest the birds may generally be more hesitant to fly over water at night [54,55]. However, not all sea-crossing individuals behave in the same way and it remains difficult to generalize our observations into a mechanistic, deterministic model. A simple innate migration strategy that leaves room for flexible responses to highly stochastic conditions is probably highly adaptive for migrant birds [4].
(a). Wind, geography and mortality on the first outbound migration
The survival rate of juvenile honey buzzards was much higher than expected based on previously reported survival rates of migrating raptors in the Afro-Palaearctic flyways [56,57]. No less than 74.2% of all tagged fledglings and 88.8% of all individuals that initiated migration survived their first migration. Moreover, none of the juveniles died by drowning, in sharp contrast to the high mortality rate among larger soaring migrants that attempt long flights across the Mediterranean [23,58]. Interestingly, all three juveniles that died during the first autumn migration (F2, F3, M4) had left Finland through Scandinavia or around the Baltic Sea. However, the circumstances under which these birds were lost suggest they died owing to different causes and we do not think mortality is systematically higher along this flyway [59].
(b). Potential carry-over effects of early-life migration experiences
Depending on where juvenile honey buzzards end up settling at non-breeding sites, they may learn very different spring migration routes when they first return to Europe as immature birds. Honey buzzards usually spend at least one whole year in sub-Saharan Africa before they first return to Europe, and they may move further east or west before their first spring migration [60]. Of the individuals we studied here, 12 reached their third calendar year. All of these moved over long distances within Africa, but most returned to a point near the location or region as where they first settled for long periods, and initiated spring migration from there (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). Assuming that immature honey buzzards rely on a simple innate migration programme on their first return migration, we would then expect individuals that settled non-breeding grounds in West Africa to take a western route on their first return migration. However, at some point, immature honey buzzards migrate at the same time as adults from whom they then learn complex detours through the wind regimes and around the geographical barriers that characterize the African-Eurasian flyways [24,34]. By now, the juveniles we tracked have all died or reached adulthood, and we are working to determine how many birds eventually manage to learn the traditional detours, at what age learning takes place and how early-life experiences such as those described in this paper ultimately affect development of individual migration routes and natal dispersal.
(c). Implications for other migrants
We expect that wind conditions and geography will shape the first outbound migrations and non-breeding distributions of many other migrant landbirds in similar ways as we described here, at least for species that rely on a simple innate vector-based navigation during their first migration. It is possible that the influence of wind is relatively more important in the Afro-Palaearctic flyways compared with, for example, the Americas, where geography strongly affects migratory connectivity [1,2]. If so, we would also expect that there is stronger selection for stochastic rather than deterministic (cf. [27]) migration tactics in the Old World compared with the New World, because in the latter, breeding and non-breeding grounds are connected only by a narrow land mass, whereas in the former, metaphorically speaking, ‘all flyways lead to Rome’. However, theoretical studies have shown that a high within-clutch variability of innate migratory headings is also advantageous for migrants breeding in North America, because it increases the likelihood that at least one chick will be able to contend with stochastic atmospheric conditions [61]. Our results suggest that pairs of long-lived honey buzzards with small clutch size (2 eggs) benefit from a similar bet-hedging strategy across multiple years.
There are migrant landbirds in the Old World that learn complex detours without adult guidance that are not necessarily optimal with respect to seasonal winds [62], often to reach a specific non-breeding area [26]. It remains unclear, however, why and how such a high degree of migratory specialization is maintained. Red-backed shrikes Lanius collurio, for example, appear to back-track the routes along which ancestors colonized Iberia from East Europe and Africa [62], while other migrant birds have developed innovative migration strategies over much shorter time scales after colonizing new breeding areas [63]. It could be that certain innate or environmental factors constrain the ability of juveniles of conservative migrants to learn alternative strategies, and lifelong tracking studies will be crucial to understand under what conditions bet-hedging and deterministic migration strategies ultimately emerge. This matters also for conservation, because breeding populations of migrant landbirds like honey buzzards are unlikely to be conserved through protected areas but rather by innovative landscape-based conservation approaches on the wintering grounds [4,64,65].
Non-breeding distributions of migrant birds are strongly determined by connectivity to breeding areas [66] and evolution of migratory links depends on the distance birds have to travel over barriers [67]. Atmospheric circulation patterns can strongly impact on this connectivity, as favourable winds can turn a formidable barrier into a freeway for migrant birds, and vice versa [34,68]. Models that simulate juvenile migrations of birds show that it is possible for birds to reach their non-breeding areas through real-life wind fields using simple vector-based orientation [25]. Similarly, we can replicate the distribution of non-breeding adult sea-turtles and eel by modelling drift trajectories of their hatchlings or larvae through extant ocean currents [69–71]. This suggests that non-deterministic, go-with-the-flow migration strategies are highly adaptive in the juvenile life-stage of many flying and swimming organisms, even if they need to compensate for the drift they accumulated as juveniles to return to their natal site in a later stage of life [72,73].
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work would not have been possible without the help from members of the Finnish ringer community. We wish to thank Mikko Honkiniemi, Ari Rantamäki, Jari Valkama, Risto Vilén, Jouko Kivelä, Annika Rossi, Kari Palo, Hannu Vuoto, Daniel Burgas, Martti Peltola and Ismo Nousiainen. We thank Will Cresswell and one anonymous reviewer for constructive feedback on an earlier version of this paper.
Ethics
The climbing of honey buzzard nests, as well as the measuring and ringing of birds, was done as inherent of the normal ringing permit (permit 2604) as issued by the Finnish Museum of Natural History. All fieldwork requiring special permits (taking blood samples for DNA sexing, sampling and storing DNA-samples, attachment of PTTs/trackers) confirmed to five separate licences as issued by Finnish authorities (EPOELY/135/07.01.2013, ESAVI/2195/04.10.07/2014, PIRELY/49/07.01/2013, VARELY/73/07.01/2013, VARELY/215/2015).
Data accessibility
The processed and wind-annotated satellite-tracking data and code needed for reproducing our analyses are made through the Dryad repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6nj94 [74]. The global shoreline database used for mapping is publically accessible via https://www.ngdc.noaa.gov/mgg/shorelines/gshhs.html.
Authors' contributions
W.M.G.V. conducted all analyses and wrote the paper with significant input from J.K. and P.B. P.B. coordinated the fieldwork and tagging of juvenile honey buzzards. J.K. performed DNA analyses to determine the birds' sex.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
Funding for this work was provided to P.B. by Kone Foundation, Swedish Cultural Foundation in Finland, R.E. Serlachius Foundation, Svensk-Österbottniska Samfundet and Aktiastiftelserna.
References
- 1.Finch T, Butler S, Franco A, Cresswell W. 2017. Low migratory connectivity is common in long-distance migrant birds. J. Anim. Ecol. 86, 662–673. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12635) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kramer GR, Streby HM, Peterson SM, Lehman JA, Buehler DA, Wood PB, McNeil DJ, Larkin JL, Andersen DE. 2017. Nonbreeding isolation and population-specific migration patterns among three populations of golden-winged warblers. Condor 119, 108–121. ( 10.1650/CONDOR-16-143.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Stanley CQ, et al. 2015. Connectivity of wood thrush breeding, wintering, and migration sites based on range-wide tracking. Conserv. Biol. 29, 164–174. ( 10.1111/cobi.12352) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cresswell W. 2014. Migratory connectivity of Palaearctic-African migratory birds and their responses to environmental change: the serial residency hypothesis. Ibis 156, 493–510. ( 10.1111/ibi.12168) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Senner NR, Conklin J, Piersma T. 2015. An ontogenetic perspective on individual differences. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20151050 ( 10.1098/rspb.2015.1050) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Alerstam T. 1990. Bird migration. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Thorup K, Holland RA, Tøttrup AP, Wikelski M. 2010. Understanding the migratory orientation program of birds: extending laboratory studies to study free-flying migrants in a natural setting. Integr. Comp. Biol. 50, 315–322. ( 10.1093/icb/icq065) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Åkesson S, Hedenström A. 2007. How migrants get there: migratory performance and orientation. Bioscience 57, 123 ( 10.1641/B570207) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W. 2015. Avian navigation: a combination of innate and learned mechanisms. Adv. Study Behav. 47, 229–310. ( 10.1016/bs.asb.2014.12.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Thorup K, Alerstam T, Hake M, Kjellen N. 2003. Bird orientation: compensation for wind drift in migrating raptors is age dependent. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, S8–S11. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0014) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Richardson WJ. 1990. Wind and orientation of migrating birds: a review. Experientia 46, 416–425. ( 10.1007/BF01952175) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Liechti F. 2006. Birds: blowin' by the wind? J. Ornithol. 147, 202–211. ( 10.1007/s10336-006-0061-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Alerstam T. 1979. Wind as selective agent in bird migration. Ornis Scand. 10, 76–93. ( 10.2307/3676347) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Perdeck AC. 1958. Two types of orientation in migrating starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, and chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs, as revealed by displacement experiments. Ardea 46, 1–37. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Trierweiler C, Klaassen RHG, Drent RH, Exo K.-M., Komdeur J, Bairlein F, Koks BJ. 2014. Migratory connectivity and population-specific migration routes in a long-distance migratory bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20132897 ( 10.1098/rspb.2013.2897) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sergio F, Tanferna A, De Stephanis R, López Jiménez L, Blas J, Tavecchia G, Preatoni D, Hiraldo F. 2014. Individual improvements and selective mortality shape lifelong migratory performance. Nature 515, 410–413. ( 10.1038/nature13696) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Flack A, et al. 2016. Costs of migratory decisions: a comparison across eight white stork populations. Sci. Adv. 2, e1500931 ( 10.1126/sciadv.1500931) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rotics S, et al. 2016. The challenges of the first migration: movement and behavior of juvenile versus adult white storks with insights regarding juvenile mortality. J. Anim. Ecol. 85, 938–947. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12525) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chernetsov N, Berthold P, Querner U. 2004. Migratory orientation of first-year white storks (Ciconia ciconia): inherited information and social interactions. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 937–943. ( 10.1242/jeb.00853) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mueller T, O'Hara RB, Converse SJ, Urbanek RP, Fagan WF. 2013. Social learning of migratory performance. Science 341, 999–1002. ( 10.1126/science.1237139) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Mellone U, Lucia G, Mallìa E, Urios V. 2016. Individual variation in orientation promotes a 3000-km latitudinal change in wintering grounds in a long-distance migratory raptor. Ibis 158, 887–893. ( 10.1111/ibi.12401) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mellone U, Limiñana R, Mallia E, Urios V. 2011. Extremely detoured migration in an inexperienced bird: interplay of transport costs and social interactions. J. Avian Biol. 42, 468–472. ( 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2011.05454.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Oppel S, Dobrev V, Arkumarev V, Saravia V, Bounas A, Kret E, Velevski M, Stoychev S, Nikolov SC. 2015. High juvenile mortality during migration in a declining population of a long-distance migratory raptor. Ibis 157, 545–557. ( 10.1111/ibi.12258) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hake M, Kjellen N, Alerstam T. 2003. Age-dependent migration strategy in honey buzzards Pernis apivorus tracked by satellite. Oikos 103, 385–396. ( 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12145.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Thorup K, Alerstam T, Hake M, Kjellen N. 2003. Can vector summation describe the orientation system of juvenile ospreys and honey buzzards?—an analysis of ring recoveries and satellite tracking. Oikos 103, 350–359. ( 10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12163.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gschweng M, Kalko EK. V, Querner U, Fiedler W, Berthold P. 2008. All across Africa: highly individual migration routes of Eleonora's falcon. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2887–2896. ( 10.1098/rspb.2008.0575) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Vega ML, et al. 2016. First-time migration in juvenile common cuckoos documented by satellite tracking. PLoS ONE 11, 1–11. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0168940) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Alerstam T. 2001. Detours in bird migration. J. Theor. Biol. 209, 319–331. ( 10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02258-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lindström Å, Gill RE, Jamieson SE, McCaffery B, Wennerberg L, Wikelski M, Klaassen M. 2011. A puzzling migratory detour: are fueling conditions in Alaska driving the movement of juvenile sharp-tailed sandpipers? Condor 113, 129–139. ( 10.1525/cond.2011.090171) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Thorup K, Ortvad TE, Rabol J, Holland RA, Tottrup AP, Wikelski M. 2011. Juvenile songbirds compensate for displacement to oceanic islands during autumn migration. PLoS ONE 6, e17903 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0017903) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Delmore KE, Toews DPL, Germain RR, Owens GL, Irwin DE. 2017. The genetics of seasonal migration and plumage color. Curr. Biol. 26, 2167–2173. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Horton TW, Bierregaard RO, Zawar-Reza P, Holdaway RN, Sagar P. 2014. Juvenile osprey navigation during trans-oceanic migration. PLoS ONE 9, e114557 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0114557) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McKinnon EA, Fraser KC, Stutchbury BJM. 2013. New discoveries in landbird migration using geolocators, and a flight plan for the future. Auk 130, 211–222. ( 10.1525/auk.2013.12226) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Vansteelant WMG, Shamoun-Baranes J, van Diermen J, van Manen W, Bouten W. 2017. Seasonal detours by soaring migrants shaped by wind regimes along the East Atlantic Flyway. J. Anim. Ecol. 86, 179–191. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12593) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Agostini N, Coleiro C, Corbi F, Di Lieto G, Pinos F, Panucci M. 2002. Water-crossing tendency of juvenile honey buzzards Pernis apivorus during migration. Avocetta 26, 41–43. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kemp MU, Emiel van Loon E, Shamoun-Baranes J, Bouten W. 2012. RNCEP: global weather and climate data at your fingertips. Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 65–70. ( 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2011.00138.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Vansteelant WMG, Bouten W, Klaassen RHG, Koks BJ, Schlaich AE, van Diermen J, van Loon EE, Shamoun-Baranes J. 2015. Regional and seasonal flight speeds of soaring migrants and the role of weather conditions at hourly and daily scales. J. Avian Biol. 46, 25–39. ( 10.1111/jav.00457) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kalnay E, et al. 1996. The NCEP/NCAR 40-year reanalysis project. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 77, 437–470. ( 10.1175/1520-0477(1996)077%3C0437:TNYRP%3E2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Burgas D, Juutinen A, Byholm P. 2016. The cost-effectiveness of using raptor nest sites to identify areas with high species richness of other taxa. Ecol. Indic. 70, 518–530. ( 10.1016/j.ecolind.2016.06.052) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Byholm P, Burgas D, Virtanen T, Valkama J. 2012. Competitive exclusion within the predator community influences the distribution of a threatened prey species. Ecology 93, 1802–1808. ( 10.1890/12-0285.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kenward R. 2004. A manual for wildlife radio tagging. London, UK: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bouten W, Baaij EW, Shamoun-Baranes J, Camphuysen KCJ. 2013. A flexible GPS tracking system for studying bird behaviour at multiple scales. J. Ornithol. 154, 571–580. ( 10.1007/s10336-012-0908-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fridolfsson A-K, Ellegren H. 1999. A simple and universal method for molecular sexing of non-ratite birds. J. Avian Biol. 30, 116–121. ( 10.2307/3677252) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wessel P, Smith WHF. 1996. A global self-consistent, hierarchical, high-resolution shoreline database. J. Geophys. Res. 101, 8741–8743. ( 10.1029/96JB00104) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dodge S, et al. 2013. The environmental-data automated track annotation (Env-DATA) system: linking animal tracks with environmental data. Mov. Ecol. 1, 3 ( 10.1186/2051-3933-1-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Shamoun-Baranes J, Bouten W, van Loon EE. 2010. Integrating meteorology into research on migration. Integr. Comp. Biol. 50, 280–292. ( 10.1093/icb/icq011) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Akaike H. 1987. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52, 317–332. ( 10.1007/BF02294359) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zuur A, Ieno EN, Walker N. 2009. Mixed effect models and extensions in ecology with R. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wickham H. 2009. Ggplot2: elegant graphics for data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Adler D, Murdoch D.2017. rgl: 3D visualization using OpenGL. See https://r-forge.r-project.org/projects/rgl/ .
- 51.White C, Selkoe KA, Watson J, Siegel DA, Zacherl DC, Toonen RJ. 2010. Ocean currents help explain population genetic structure. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 1685–1694. ( 10.1098/rspb.2009.2214) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ronconi RA, Allard KA, Taylor PD. 2015. Bird interactions with offshore oil and gas platforms: review of impacts and monitoring techniques. J. Environ. Manage. 147, 34–45. ( 10.1016/j.jenvman.2014.07.031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Scholander SI. 1955. Land birds over the western North Atlantic. Auk 72, 225–239. ( 10.2307/4081600) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Horton KG, Van Doren BM, Stepanian PM, Hochachka WM, Farnsworth A, Kelly JF. 2016. Nocturnally migrating songbirds drift when they can and compensate when they must. Sci. Rep. 6, 21249 ( 10.1038/srep21249) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Adamík P, et al. 2016. Barrier crossing in small avian migrants: individual tracking reveals prolonged nocturnal flights into the day as a common migratory strategy. Sci. Rep. 6, 21560 ( 10.1038/srep21560) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Strandberg R, Klaassen RHG, Hake M, Alerstam T. 2010. How hazardous is the Sahara Desert crossing for migratory birds? Indications from satellite tracking of raptors. Biol. Lett. 6, 297–300. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0785) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Klaassen RHG, Hake M, Strandberg R, Koks BJ, Trierweiler C, Exo K-M, Bairlein F, Alerstam T. 2014. When and where does mortality occur in migratory birds? Direct evidence from long-term satellite tracking of raptors. J. Anim. Ecol. 83, 176–184. ( 10.1111/1365-2656.12135) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bildstein KL, Bechard MJ, Farmer C, Newcomb L. 2009. Narrow sea crossings present major obstacles to migrating griffon vultures Gyps fulvus. Ibis 151, 382–391. ( 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2009.00919.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hewson CM, Thorup K, Pearce-Higgins JW, Atkinson PW. 2016. Population decline is linked to migration route in the common cuckoo. Nat. Commun. 7 ( 10.1038/ncomms12296) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Strandberg R, Hake M, Klaassen RHG, Alerstam T. 2012. Movements of immature European honey buzzards Pernis apivorus in tropical Africa. Ardea 100, 157–162. ( 10.5253/078.100.0207) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Reilly JR, Reilly RJ. 2009. Bet-hedging and the orientation of juvenile passerines in fall migration. J. Anim. Ecol. 78, 990–1001. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01576.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Tøttrup AP, Pedersen L, Onrubia A, Klaassen RHG, Thorup K. 2017. Migration of red-backed shrikes from the Iberian Peninsula: optimal or sub-optimal detour? J. Avian Biol. 48, 149–154. ( 10.1111/jav.01352) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Berthold P, Helbig AJ, Mohr G, Querner U. 1992. Rapid microevolution of migratory behaviour in a wild bird species. Nature 360, 668–670. ( 10.1038/360668a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vickery JA, Ewing SR, Smith KW, Pain DJ, Bairlein F, Škorpilová J, Gregory RD. 2014. The decline of Afro-Palaearctic migrants and an assessment of potential causes. Ibis 156, 1–22. ( 10.1111/ibi.12118) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Osinubi ST, Hand K, Van Oijen DCC, Walther BA, Barnard P. 2016. Linking science and policy to address conservation concerns about African land use, land conversion and land grabs in the era of globalization. Afr. J. Ecol. 54, 265–267. ( 10.1111/aje.12355) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Somveille M, Rodrigues ASL, Manica A. 2015. Why do birds migrate? A macroecological perspective. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 24, 664–674. ( 10.1111/geb.12298) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Henningsson SS, Alerstam T. 2005. Barriers and distances as determinants for the evolution of bird migration links: the arctic shorebird system. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 2251–2258. ( 10.1098/rspb.2005.3221) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gill RE, Douglas DC, Handel CM, Tibbitts TL, Hufford G, Piersma T. 2014. Hemispheric-scale wind selection facilitates bar-tailed godwit circum-migration of the Pacific. Anim. Behav. 90, 117–130. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mencacci R, Bernardi E, Sale A, Lutjeharms JRE, Luschi P. 2010. Influence of oceanic factors on long-distance movements of loggerhead sea turtles displaced in the southwest Indian Ocean. Mar. Biol. 157, 339–349. ( 10.1007/s00227-009-1321-z) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Hays GC, Fossette S, Katselidis KA, Mariani P, Schofield G. 2010. Ontogenetic development of migration: Lagrangian drift trajectories suggest a new paradigm for sea turtles. J. R. Soc. Interface 7, 1319–1327. ( 10.1098/rsif.2010.0009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chang Y-L, Miyazawa Y, Béguer-Pon M. 2016. Simulating the oceanic migration of silver Japanese eels. PLoS ONE 11, e0150187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Chang Y-L, Sheng J, Ohashi K, Béguer-Pon M, Miyazawa Y. 2015. Impacts of interannual ocean circulation variability on Japanese eel larval migration in the western north Pacific Ocean. PLoS ONE 10, e0144423 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0144423) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Scott R, Marsh R, Hays G. 2014. Ontogeny of long distance migration. Ecology 95, 2840–2850. ( 10.1890/13-2164.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Vansteelant WMG, Kekkonen J, Byholm P. 2017. Data from: Wind conditions and geography shape the first outbound migration of juvenile honey buzzards and their distribution across sub-Saharan Africa. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.6nj94) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Vansteelant WMG, Kekkonen J, Byholm P. 2017. Data from: Wind conditions and geography shape the first outbound migration of juvenile honey buzzards and their distribution across sub-Saharan Africa. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.6nj94) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The processed and wind-annotated satellite-tracking data and code needed for reproducing our analyses are made through the Dryad repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.6nj94 [74]. The global shoreline database used for mapping is publically accessible via https://www.ngdc.noaa.gov/mgg/shorelines/gshhs.html.