Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2017 Jul 18;7:5665. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06015-3

The natural Disc1-deletion present in several inbred mouse strains does not affect sleep

Lars Dittrich 1, Alessandro Petese 1, Walker S Jackson 1,
PMCID: PMC5515846  PMID: 28720848

Abstract

The gene Disrupted in Schizophrenia-1 (DISC1) is linked to a range of psychiatric disorders. Two recent transgenic studies suggest DISC1 is also involved in homeostatic sleep regulation. Several strains of inbred mice commonly used for genome manipulation experiments, including several Swiss and likely all 129 substrains, carry a natural deletion mutation of Disc1. This constitutes a potential confound for studying sleep in genetically modified mice. Since disturbed sleep can also influence psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease models, this putative confound might affect a wide range of studies in several fields. Therefore, we asked to what extent the natural Disc1 deletion affects sleep. To this end, we first compared sleep and electroencephalogram (EEG) phenotypes of 129S4 mice carrying the Disc1 deletion and C57BL/6N mice carrying the full-length version. We then bred Disc1 from C57BL/6N into the 129S4 background, resulting in S4-Disc1 mice. The differences between 129S4 and C57BL/6N were not detected in the 129S4 to S4-Disc1 comparison. We conclude that the mutation has no effect on the measured sleep and EEG characteristics. Thus, it is unlikely the widespread Disc1 deletion has led to spurious results in previous sleep studies or that it alters sleep in mouse models of psychiatric or neurodegenerative diseases.

Introduction

Many psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia, major depression, and bipolar disorder, are accompanied by pronounced sleep abnormalities1, 2. This was long viewed as a secondary effect downstream of the primary disorder. However, recent evidence suggests that the relation is bidirectional, i.e. sleep problems exacerbate psychiatric diseases36. The cause for the high rate of co-occurrence of psychiatric disorders and sleep abnormalities is unknown, but a common genetic predisposition might increase the risk for both35, 7, 8. A Scottish pedigree with high prevalence of psychiatric disease carries a mutated gene associated with schizophrenia, major depression and bipolar disorder9. The gene, known as Disrupted in Schizophrenia-1 (DISC1) encodes an intracellular scaffold protein known to play a role in neuronal development and synaptic function10. A role for Disc1 in sleep regulation was suggested in two transgenic studies ectopically expressing human DISC1. In flies human DISC1 caused increased total sleep and increased sleep bout duration whereas in mice it caused increased wakefulness and decreased sleep11, 12. Although these two reports seem contradictory, it should be noted that flies lack a native Disc1 gene and the transgenic mice expressed both human and mouse Disc1 and that the measurement techniques for flies and mice are necessarily different. Nonetheless, these reports indicate that expression of DISC1 related genes can alter sleep.

In contrast to most inbred mouse strains such as C57BL/6, several strains including “Swiss” strains (e.g. FVB, SJL, SWR), and all 129 substrains naturally carry a 25 base pair deletion mutation in exon 6 of Disc1 1315. Importantly, many of these strains have been widely used for genome manipulation techniques. The molecular consequence of the deletion is that the normal Disc1 protein can be made up to amino acid 528, after which a stretch of 13 incorrect amino acids are added until a newly in-frame stop codon is reached and translation is terminated. Since full-length wild-type Disc1 protein has 852 amino acids, the deletion allele theoretically removes 38% of the C-terminal end of Disc1 protein and replaces it with 13 amino acids of unknown consequence. It is conceivable that this mutant form loses all or most of the normal function or gains a negative function. Importantly, although western blot analysis confirmed that the corresponding region of Disc1 protein is not detected in brains of mice homozygous for the Disc1 deletion, antibodies specific for peptides on either side of the deleted region indicate that a nearly full-length Disc1 protein is expressed, most likely the result of alternative splicing or exon skipping16. Despite synthesizing much of the protein, mice congenic for the Disc1 deletion show abnormalities in behavioral tests, pre-pulse inhibition, neuron morphology, and neuronal excitability1719. Thus, the full significance of the deletion remains uncertain.

Considering the above mentioned reports of altered sleep in human DISC1 expressing transgenic flies and mice and that the mouse Disc1 deletion creates physiological changes, we wondered if the Disc1 deletion would also affect sleep. This is important both for the quest of understanding links between sleep and psychiatric disease and to determine if it creates a potential confound for a large number of existing studies. A search on the Jackson laboratories website suggests that in their collection alone over 4,000 mouse lines have some contribution of a 129 genome, and over 1,000 of FVB (as of February 2017). Considering that sleep influences several non-sleep measures in mice, such as psychiatric behaviors8, memory20, and neurodegeneration21, this confound could affect a wide range of published mouse studies. To test if the Disc1 deletion does affect sleep, we characterized the sleep phenotype of a commonly used substrain, 129S4 (formerly 129/SvJae, hereafter S4). To our knowledge, the sleep phenotype of this substrain has never been characterized, despite its wide use. Sleep has been well characterized in the 129P2 substrain formerly 129/Ola22, 23. However, 129 substrains are highly diverse genetically24, 25, which cautions against transferring phenotyping results between substrains. We identified several differences in sleep-related characteristics between S4 and C57BL/6N (hereafter B6N). We then asked whether any of these differences were caused by the Disc1 deletion. To this end we bred the full length B6N version of the Disc1 gene to the S4 background yielding a mouse line that carries the Disc1 locus from B6N in the context of a > 99.6% S4 genome, a line we call S4-Disc1. We reasoned that any sleep characteristic that was disrupted by the Disc1 deletion should resemble the B6 phenotype and differ from the S4 phenotype in these mice. Surprisingly, we detected no differences between S4 and S4-Disc1 mice.

Results

Vigilance states can be classified into wake, REM (rapid eye movement sleep) and NREM (non-rapid eye movement sleep). To compare the sleep phenotype of S4 mice to B6N mice, EEG and EMG (electromyogram) were recorded using wireless implanted transmitters that allowed unrestricted movement in the cage. We manually scored vigilance states in 10-second epochs. We then tested if the distribution or architecture of these states differed across an undisturbed 24 h baseline period. During baseline, S4 mice spent less time in NREM (T12 = 2.33, p = 0.038) and more time in REM (T12 = 3.59, p = 0.004) than B6 mice (Fig. 1A). Group means with standard deviations were 45.5 ± 1.8% wake, 49.0 ± 1.7% NREM, 5.5 ± 0.3% REM for S4 and 42.2 ± 1.1% wake, 53.6 ± 1.2% NREM, 4.2 ± 0.2% REM for B6N. Average REM bout duration was shorter (T12 = 5.67, p < 0.001), whereas the number of REM (T12 = 6.79, p < 0.001) as well as wake (T12 = 3.08, p = 0.009) bouts was higher (Fig. 1B and C). Group means for bout durations were 4.0 ± 0.5 min wake, 2.1 ± 0.1 min NREM, 1.1 ± 0.04 min REM for S4, 5.0 ± 0.4 min wake, 2.5 ± 0.2 min NREM, 1.5 ± 0.06 min REM for B6N. Group means for bout numbers were 160.8 ± 14.5 wake, 349.2 ± 26.9 NREM, 73.3 ± 3.9 REM for S4, 117.4 ± 5.8 wake 317.9 ± 20.4 NREM, 39.9 ± 3.1 REM for B6N. The distribution of sleep time across the 24 h period illustrates that differences in sleep amount were not apparently restricted to certain times of day (Fig. 1D–F; no interactions of factors ‘Zeitgeber Time’ and ‘strain’).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Vigilance states of S4 and B6 mice during 24 h undisturbed baseline. (A) percentage of time spent in wake, NREM, and REM. Asterisks indicate differences between strains at p < 0.05. (B) Average durations of the bouts of each state. (C) Average number of bouts during baseline. (D) Wake time across the 24 h in 1 h bins. Curves depict group averages, surrounding shaded areas depict s.e.m. Shaded rectangle indicates time during lights off. F-values for interactions of factors ‘time’ and ‘strain’ are indicated in the panels. Degrees of freedom for ANOVAs in D-F are 23, 276. (E) NREM time across the 24 h (F) REM time across the 24 h. (GI) EEG power spectra. The power in one frequency bin is expressed as percentage of the cumulative power of all frequencies (0–50 Hz). Curves depict group averages, shaded areas depict s.e.m. Interactions of factors ‘frequency’ and ‘strain’ (permutation ANOVA) are indicated for each panel. The degrees of freedom are 409 and 4908 for all interactions in GJ. The p-values for post hoc uncorrected bin-by-bin t-tests are indicated in grey below the spectra (G) Average wake power spectra (H) Average NREM EEG power spectra. (I) Average REM EEG power spectra. (J) Average REM EEG power spectra normalized by the respective wake power spectra. (K) Average activity during baseline in 1 h bins. Error bars depict s.e.m. F-value for interaction of factors ‘time’ and ‘strain’ is indicated in the panel. Degrees of freedom are 23, 276. (L) Frequency time histogram (71 bins) of activity during baseline. Curves depict group averages, shaded areas depict s.e.m. F-value for interaction of factors ‘activity level’ and ‘strain’ (permutation ANOVA) is indicated in the panel. Degrees of freedom are 70, 840. P-values of non-corrected post-hoc t-tests (bottom) indicate that B6 mice spent more time at activity levels > 0.4.

To test if the S4 EEG differs from that of B6N mice, we calculated the average EEG power spectra. This analysis quantifies the relative contribution of different frequencies to the EEG and thus enables the detection of differences across a wide range of frequencies. EEG strongly varies with vigilance state, which can compromise EEG analyses if sleep/wake behavior is not considered. Therefore, power spectra were analyzed for each of the three vigilance states separately. During wake, S4 mice produced lower relative power in the delta range (1–4 Hz) and higher relative power in the theta range (4–10 Hz) and in frequencies between 15 and 40 Hz (Fig. 1G). During NREM, S4 mice produced lower relative power in the delta range and higher power in all higher frequencies (Fig. 1H). During REM, the relative power at frequencies > 25 Hz was higher in S4 (Fig. 1I). The average theta shifted towards lower frequencies in S4 mice, resulting in higher power at 6–7 Hz and lower power at 7.5–8 Hz. Expressing power spectra as relative values is a useful way for normalization when comparing recordings from different individuals. However, it can be difficult to discern if an observed difference is driven by higher absolute power in one frequency range or lower absolute power in another one. Therefore, it can be useful to normalize the data additionally in a different, non-redundant way. To this end we plotted the REM spectra normalized by the corresponding wake spectra (Fig. 1J). The difference between REM and wake was smaller in the S4 than in the B6N mice, because of their somewhat REM-like wake spectra with a large theta peak and high relative power in the 10–30 Hz range. Thus, the strains differed not just in EEG composition, but also in the way the EEG changed when transitioning between vigilance states. The differences in EEG spectra between B6 and S4 were so pronounced that they were also discernible in non-normalized raw spectra (Supplementary Fig. 1A–C).

To test if the mouse strains showed overall differences in locomotor activity, we made use of a semi-quantitative measure based on changes of signal strength when the transmitter moves relative to the receiver antennae. S4 mice showed on average lower locomotor activity. Analysis of diurnal activity patterns in hourly bins (Fig. 1K) revealed main effects for the factors ‘strain’ (F1,12 = 6.05, p = 0.03) and ‘Zeitgeber Time’ (F23,276 = 7.23, p < 0.001) as well as their interaction (F23,276 = 2.07, p = 0.003). Correspondingly, Fig. 1K indicates that differences in average locomotor activity were most pronounced during lights-off. For a more detailed analysis, we plotted the activity recordings from all 10 s-epochs as histograms (Fig. 1L). This kind of analysis compares the distribution of activity levels, which can reveal more subtle information than comparison of means. For example, lower average activity could be caused by a generally slower locomotion, but just as well by more time spent without movement or a lower number of bursts of very high activity levels. We found that S4 mice spent less time at the highest activity levels of values > 0.4 (statistics in Fig. 1L). Analysis of the “activity count” measure provided by the recording software confirmed that S4 showed lower locomotor activity than B6 (Supplemental Fig. 1D–F).

For a more detailed analysis of sleep regulation, we subjected the mice to a sleep homeostatic challenge. Mice were kept awake for 6 h, after which they were left undisturbed, providing an opportunity for recovery sleep. In the first 2 h of RS opportunity following sleep deprivation, S4 mice spent more time asleep than B6N mice (74.0% ± 2.5 and 53.7% ± 6.5, respectively; T12 = 2.58, p = 0.024; Fig. 2A). An established measure of accrued sleep pressure is the NREM EEG power in the 1–4 Hz range (NREM delta power). Figure 2B depicts NREM delta power during baseline and following 6 h of sleep deprivation, both normalized to the average 24 h baseline values. During baseline, the diurnal oscillation was smaller in S4 than B6N mice (24 h trough-to-peak amplitude of hourly time bins 0.38 ± 0.02 for S4, 0.61 ± 0.08 for B6, T12 = 2.32, p = 0.038). The expected compensatory peak of NREM delta power following sleep deprivation did not differ between strains. A measure for sleep homeostasis complementary to NREM delta power is NREM delta energy (NRDE, called Delta energy in SWS by Franken et al.26). It is the mathematical product of NREM delta power and time spent in NREM, and thus reflects both homeostatic responses to sleep loss, increasing sleep time and increasing sleep depth. Following 6 h of sleep deprivation, S4 mice regained cumulative NREM delta energy quicker than B6N mice (Fig. 2C). Correspondingly, ANOVA revealed an interaction between factors ‘Zeitgeber Time’ and ‘strain’, indicating that the cumulative NRDE curves differ (F23,276 = 15.48, p < 0.001). To analyze the EEG response to sleep deprivation in more detail, we plotted the average NREM spectra during the 2 h recovery sleep opportunity normalized by the NREM spectra of the corresponding 2 h of baseline recording, i.e. Zeitgeber Time (ZT) 7–8. This depiction illustrates the changes in NREM EEG spectra in response to sleep deprivation (statistics in figure). S4 mice responded with a weaker increase in low delta power (<2 Hz), and a stronger increase in the frequencies between 7 and 15 Hz than B6N (Fig. 2D).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Responses to sleep homeostatic challenges in S4 and B6 mice. (A) Wakefulness in hourly bins during undisturbed baseline, sleep deprivation, and recovery opportunity. Black bar at bottom indicates sleep deprivation. Shaded background indicates lights-off. Curves depict group means and s.e.m. The baseline values are replotted from Fig. 1D. The asterisk indicates the significant difference between strains in sleep amounts during the first 2 h of recovery sleep opportunity (p = 0.02). SD = sleep deprivation, RS = recovery sleep (B) NREM delta power in hourly bins during baseline and recovery sleep opportunity. Black bar at bottom indicates sleep deprivation. Shaded background indicates lights-off. Curves depict group means and s.e.m. The amplitude of oscillation was lower in S4 mice. (C) Cumulative NREM delta energy over 24 h. Dashed curves depict baseline values, solid curves depict values during sleep deprivation and recovery sleep. Curves depict mean values and s.e.m. F-value for interaction of factors ‘time’ and ‘strain’ for the sleep deprivation + recovery days is indicated in the panel. Degrees of freedom are 23, 276. Bsl = baseline. (D) NREM EEG power spectra of the first 2 h of RS normalized by the corresponding 2 h of baseline NREM (ZT7 + 8). Curves depict group averages and s.e.m. Interaction of factors ‘frequency’ and ‘strain’ (permutation ANOVA) is indicated at the bottom. The degrees of freedom are 409 and 4908. The p-values for post hoc uncorrected bin-by-bin t-tests are indicated below the spectra. (E) Wake time during the multiple sleep latency test. Curves depict group means and s.e.m. Black bars at bottom indicate the five consecutive 20 min sleep deprivations. (F) Sleep latency and time spent in NREM during the 5 nap opportunities of the MSLT. Asterisk indicates shorter sleep latencies for S4 mice (p < 0.05). (G) NREM delta power during the nap opportunities of the MSLT, normalized by the average baseline values. Curves depict group means in 200 s time bins. Error bars depict s.e.m. Shaded rectangles depict group means ± s.e.m. for a complete nap opportunity. The asterisk by the legend indicates a significant main effect of ANOVA for the factor ‘strain’ (p < 0.05).

To further investigate sleep regulation, we subjected both strains to the murine version of the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT)27, a test that measures sleep propensity and is used similarly in humans. In this test, S4 mice showed a shorter latency to sleep than B6N (4.4 ± 0.5 min and 8.6 ± 1.4 min, respectively; T12 = 2.47, p = 0.029), indicating higher sleepiness (Fig. 2E,F). When we analyzed NREM delta power during the nap opportunities in the MSLT, we found that B6N mice responded to that test with a stronger increase in delta power relative to baseline. ANOVA revealed a main effect for ‘strain’ (F1,9 = 23.39, p = 0.001) but no main effect of ‘nap opportunity’ or interaction.

To test if any of the differences identified in Figs 1 and 2 might be influenced by the Disc1 deletion of S4, we compared a second group of S4 mice with S4-Disc1 mice, which carry the B6N version of the gene on an S4 background. The S4-Disc1 line was developed by passing the B6N Disc1 gene through the S4 background for 9 generations. Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) are DNA substitutions in coding and non-coding regions distributed throughout the genome and are commonly used as markers to determine the genetic relatedness of different mouse strains. At generation seven, the developing S4-Disc1 line was determined to be 99.58% S4 by a genome wide SNP survey, leading us to conclude that by generation nine it was >99.6% S4.

Figure 3 depicts sleep-related characteristics of S4 and S4-Disc1 mice during 24 h of undisturbed baseline. There were no statistically significant differences in amount of time spent in any of the three analyzed vigilance states (Fig. 3A), average bout durations (Fig. 3B), or bout numbers (Fig. 3C). Group means for vigilance states were 44.8 ± 1.0% wake, 49.9 ± 1.0% NREM, 5.3 ± 0.4% REM for S4, 46.4 ± 1.0% wake, 48.2 ± 0.9% NREM, 5.6 ± 0.16% REM for S4-Disc1. Group means for bout durations were 4.3 ± 0.3 min wake, 2.0 ± 0.1 min NREM, 1.2 ± 0.05 min REM for S4, 4.7 ± 0.2 min wake, 1.9 ± 0.1 min NREM, 1.2 ± 0.06 min REM for S4-Disc1. Group means for bout numbers were 143.0 ± 8.7 wake, 366.6 ± 19.3 NREM, 65.5 ± 6.0 REM for S4, 132.3 ± 4.1 wake, 364.0 ± 23.1 NREM, 66.0 ± 4.4 REM for S4-Disc1.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Vigilance states of S4 and S4-Disc1 mice during 24 h undisturbed baseline. This figure directly parallels the comparisons made between S4 and B6 in Fig. 1. (A) Percentage of time spent in wake, NREM, and REM. (B) Average durations of the bouts of each state. (C) Average number of bouts during baseline. (D) Wake time across the 24 h in 1 h bins. Curves depict group averages, shaded areas depict s.e.m. Shaded rectangle indicates lights off. F-values for interactions of factors ‘time’ and ‘strain’ are indicated in the panels. Degrees of freedom for ANOVAs in D-F are 23, 276. (E) NREM time across the 24 h. (F) REM time across the 24 h. (G-I) EEG power spectra. The power in one frequency bin is expressed as percentage of the cumulative power of all frequencies (0–50 Hz). Curves depict group averages, shaded areas depict s.e.m. Interactions of factors ‘frequency’ and ‘genotype’ (permutation ANOVA) are indicated for each panel. The degrees of freedom are 409 and 4908 for interactions in G-J. The p-values for post hoc uncorrected bin-by-bin t-tests are indicated below the spectra (G) Average wake power spectra (H) Average NREM EEG power spectra. (I) Average REM EEG power spectra. (J) Average REM EEG power spectra normalized by the respective wake power spectra. (K) Average activity during baseline in 1 h bins. Error bars depict s.e.m. F-value for interaction between factors ‘time’ and ‘strain’ is in the panel. Degrees of freedom are 23, 276. (L) Frequency time histogram (71 bins) of activity during baseline. Curves depict group averages, shaded areas depict s.e.m. F for interaction of factors ‘activity level’ and ‘genotype’ (permutation ANOVA) is indicated in the panel. Degrees of freedom are 70, 840.

After we could not detect differences, we calculated the 90% confidence interval of the differences of the means. This is equivalent to the two one-sided test procedure28. We can conclude at the alpha = 0.05 level that the real difference of the means is not larger in either direction than indicated by this interval. Confidence bounds were for wake time (percentage points) −4.1, 1.2; NREM time −0.84, 4.3; REM time −1.2, 0.65; for wake bout durations −1.0, 0.25 min; NREM bout durations −0.22, 0.34 min; REM bout durations −0.19, 0.08 min; for wake bout number −8.5, 29.8; NREM bout number −50.7, 55.9; REM bout number −14.6, 13.6. The distribution of sleep time across the 24 h period did not differ between the genotypes (Fig. 3D–F; no interaction between factors “Zeitgeber Time” and “genotype”).

Averaged EEG power spectra did not differ for wake (Fig. 3G), NREM (Fig. 3H), or REM (Fig. 3I). REM spectra normalized by the corresponding wake spectra did not differ between the genotypes, indicating that similar changes in brain activity occur when either group switches between these states (Fig. 3J). Both genotypes showed higher locomotor activity during lights-off, which is expected for a nocturnal species (Fig. 3K). Correspondingly, ANOVA revealed a main effect for the factor ‘Zeitgeber Time’ (F23,276 = 5.65, p < 0.001). However, no main effect was found for the factor ‘genotype’ (F1,12 = 0.01, p = 0.911) and there was no significant interaction (F23,276 = 0.71, p = 0.834), suggesting that diurnal activity patterns did not differ between the mouse lines. A more fine-grained analysis of locomotor activity using a histogram plot also did not reveal any differences between the genotypes (Fig. 3L).

Analyses of sleep homeostasis (Fig. 4) also did not recapitulate any of the differences detected between the background strains. There was no difference between S4 and S4-Disc1 in the time spent asleep in the first 2 h of recovery sleep opportunity following 6 h of sleep deprivation (71.7 ± 2.8 and 69.9 ± 2.7, respectively; T12 = 0.434, p = 0.67; Fig. 4A). 90% confidence bounds for the difference of the means were −8.9, 5.4. Figure 4B depicts NREM delta power during baseline and following 6 h of sleep deprivation, both normalized to the average baseline values. The amplitudes of the undisturbed diurnal oscillations did not differ between S4 and S4-Disc1 strains (0.49 ± 0.03 and 0.40 ± 0.04, respectively; T12 = 1.75, p = 0.11). 90% confidence bounds for the differences of the mean were −0.002, 0.18. Analysis of NRDE detected no interaction of factors ‘Zeitgeber Time’ and ‘genotype’ (F23,276 = 1.01, p = 0.45; Fig. 4C). Analysis of NREM spectra during the first 2 h of recovery sleep opportunity also detected no differences in additional EEG frequencies (Fig. 4D), implicating that the EEG response to acute sleep pressure was identical for mice with either version of the Disc1 gene. In the murine multiple sleep latency test, we found no difference of latency to sleep between S4 and S4-Disc1 mice (5.7 ± 1.0 min and 6.5 ± 1.1 min, respectively; T12 = 0.539, p = 0.60; Fig. 4E,F). 90% confidence bounds of the difference of the mean were −3.4, 1.8. The NREM delta power during the MSLT did not differ, either (Fig. 4G). For the mean values per nap opportunity, there was no main effect for ‘genotype’ (F1,10 = 0.32, p = 0.58). However, there was a main effect for ‘nap opportunity’ (F4,40 = 7.35) with no interaction (F4,40 = 1.94, p = 0.122), suggesting that there was a net accumulation of sleep pressure during this task.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Responses to sleep homeostatic challenges in S4 and S4-Disc1 mice. This figure directly parallels the comparisons made between S4 and B6 in Fig. 2. (A) Wakefulness in hourly bins during undisturbed baseline, sleep deprivation, and recovery opportunity. Black bar at bottom indicates sleep deprivation. Shaded background indicates lights-off. Curves depict group means and s.e.m. The baseline values are replotted from Fig. 3D. SD = sleep deprivation, RS = recovery sleep. (B) NREM delta power in hourly bins during baseline and recovery sleep opportunity. Black bar at bottom indicates sleep deprivation. Shaded background indicates lights-off. Curves depict group means and s.e.m. (C) Cumulative NREM delta energy over 24 h. Dashed curves depict baseline values, solid curves depict values during sleep deprivation and recovery sleep. Curves depict mean values and s.e.m. F-value for interaction of factors ‘time’ and ‘strain’ for the sleep deprivation + recovery days is indicated in the panel. Degrees of freedom are 23, 276. Bsl = baseline. (D) NREM EEG power spectra of the first 2 h of RS normalized by the corresponding 2 h of baseline NREM (ZT7 + 8). Curves depict group averages and s.e.m. F-value for interaction of factors ‘frequency’ and ‘genotype’ (permutation ANOVA) is indicated at bottom. The degrees of freedom are 409 and 4908. The p-values for post hoc uncorrected bin-by-bin t-tests are indicated below the spectra. (E) Wake time during the multiple sleep latency test. Curves depict group means and s.e.m. Black bars at bottom indicate the five consecutive 20 min sleep deprivations. (F) Sleep latency and time spent in NREM during the 5 nap opportunities of the MSLT. (G) NREM delta power during the nap opportunities of the MSLT, normalized by the average baseline values. Curves depict group means in 200 s time bins. Error bars depict s.e.m. Shaded rectangles depict group means ± s.e.m. for a complete nap opportunity.

Discussion

We found several differences in EEG, sleep, and sleep homeostasis between the inbred mouse strains, S4 and B6N. None of these differences persisted in a comparison of S4 and S4-Disc1 mice, which argues against the natural Disc1 deletion altering sleep regulation. We will first discuss the differences between the strains and second the apparent absence of a sleep phenotype linked to the Disc1 deletion.

The present study is, to our knowledge, the first description of the sleep phenotype of the S4 mouse strain. Although another 129 substrain was included in a comprehensive description of sleep and EEG phenotypes of different inbred mouse strains from a series of reports by Franken et al.22, 23, 26, that line 129P2, (formerly 129/Ola, hereafter P2) is very different from S4. The two strains were separated in 1948 and contamination of the P2 line appeared to occur sometime after 197724. This is evidenced by a recent large-scale genomics study. Among 17 strains compared to the C57Bl/6J reference genome (hereafter B6J, also studied by Franken), S4 was not included but two closely related strains were, 129S1 and 129S5, as well as the B6N strain we studied and P2 studied by Franken. The number of mouse strain specific SNPs for each strain were B6N = 1,488; 129S1 = 1,489; 129S5 = 1,991; P2 = 23,67729. The large number of SNPs specific to P2 indicates it is very genetically different from B6 and 129S substrains and that the 129S substrains are very similar to each other.

It is well known that characteristics of EEG are heritable30, 31 and consequently differ systematically between diverse inbred mouse strains23. As expected, we found that the EEG of S4 mice was clearly distinguishable from B6N mice in all vigilance states. Most prominently, the EEG power spectra of S4 mice showed lower relative delta power in NREM, and higher relative theta power in wake. Wake theta power correlates with certain behaviors, such as exploration. However, amplitude and peak frequency of the theta peak can also differ between inbred mouse strains when behavior type is controlled23. Either factor might account for the difference in wake theta in this study.

Diurnal oscillation of NREM delta power was less pronounced in S4 than in B6N, although a normal rebound following sleep deprivation excluded a deficit in delta power generation. In combination with our finding that their sleep/wake architecture tended to be more fragmented (more but shorter bouts), it can be speculated that S4 mice tended to respond to sleep pressure with an increase of sleep propensity rather than delta power, and thus might have entered sleep before much sleep pressure had accrued during the baseline recording, preventing further build up. This would be in agreement with the quicker sleep onset in the multiple sleep latency test.

Although their work also included a comparison between B6 and 129 substrains, direct comparisons of Franken et al.‘s work and ours should be made cautiously for several reasons. First, the B6 mice were from two different substrains; B6J in Franken et al.‘s work, B6N in ours. Despite their genetic similarity e.g. refs 32 and 33 B6N and B6J substrains show differences in a number of behavioral tests, although video-based analysis shows their rest/activity behavior to be indistinguishable at the relevant age34. Second, our study used a different EEG electrode configuration, which affects the frequency contributions of the recorded signal. Third, the specific tests performed were not identical. Nonetheless, many relative changes can be compared between studies.

Despite the subtle genetic and behavioral differences cited above, the phenotypes of B6N in our study were very similar to that of B6J in Franken et al.’s reports. Similarly, many of the qualitative differences between S4 and B6N mice in our study were also reported for P2 and B6J mice though we also noted some differences. Resembling our S4 data, P2 mice showed decreased NREM delta power and higher (although not significantly) sigma power (11–15 Hz) during 24 h baseline, as well as more time spent in REM than B623. Franken et al.26 noted an “additional rest period” in B6J but not P2 during the second half of the lights-off period. This resembles the curves of wake and NREM in our B6N and S4 groups (Fig. 1D,E). However, in contrast to S4 in our study, P2 mice did not show a blunted oscillation of NREM delta power. Franken et al. also did not detect differences between B6J and P2 in the rate at which lost NRDE is regained26, in contrast to our findings in S4. However, to test if delta power kinetics truly differ between S4 and P2, a direct comparison with identical electrode configurations would be required.

The major goal of this study was to evaluate a potential effect for the widespread Disc1 deletion on sleep. Since we hypothesized that the Disc1 deletion affects sleep regulation, rather than moving the deleted Disc1 gene into the B6 background we chose to move the wild-type Disc1 gene into the S4 background because, in addition to testing the hypothesis, the resulting line would be a useful resource for correcting this deficit. Unexpectedly, none of the sleep or EEG characteristics of S4 mice were altered when we replaced the deleted Disc1 allele with the full-length version. Given the small sample size of our study, we cannot exclude that the Disc1 deletion causes smaller effects, which would require a larger study to detect. However, our experimental setup robustly detected differences of sleep and EEG in the background strains, B6N versus S4, but not in the S4 versus S4-Disc1 comparison. We conclude that it is unlikely that the differences between S4 and B6N mice were caused by the Disc1-deletion mutation present in S4 and other strains. It is a theoretical possibility that S4 carries additional mutations in a putative Disc1 dependent, sleep-related pathway. This would make the presence of full-length Disc1 insufficient to rescue the phenotype. An experiment conversely to ours, where the deleted allele is transferred to a B6 background, would be able to control for this possibility.

Another caveat with this study is that a region of genome flanking the Disc1 allele in S4-Disc1 mice was from the B6N genetic background and it is possible these linked genes, some of which will have differences from their S4 counterpart, could have resulted in the lack of a detectable phenotypic difference between S4 and S4-Disc1 mice. However, if our conclusion that the Disc1 deletion does not affect sleep is false, the simplest (although admittedly still complicated) explanation for the lack of a phenotypic difference being due specifically to the presence of a linked B6 derived gene is that the linked B6 gene would make the B6 Disc1 gene function like the S4 Disc1 gene when in the S4 but not the B6 background. This could be tested by creating both a new knock-in mouse line that corrects the deletion in inbred S4 mice and one that creates the same 25 base pair Disc1 deletion in an inbred B6 background. However, since our experiments did not detect differences in congenic mice, it is very unlikely that a knock-in approach will reveal differences either.

Although the purpose of our experiment was to test if the Disc1 deletion affects sleep, it might be tempting to speculate based on ours and others work if there is a role for wild-type Disc1 in sleep. Some isoforms of Disc1 are still expressed in mice carrying the deletion16. In contrast to the lack of phenotype in our study, the same mutation affected cognitive function and neuronal development on C57BL/6J or mixed C57BL/6J;129S1/SvImJ backgrounds1719, indicating the deleted Disc1 gene is not equivalent to the wild-type gene. It is possible that the remaining isoforms are sufficient for retaining a putative sleep-regulatory function, but not other functions of Disc1. Alternatively, Disc1 might not be involved in sleep regulation after all. Indication for a putative role of Disc1 for sleep comes from two recent transgenic studies, one in Drosophila12, the other in mouse11. In mouse, the authors expressed the human DISC1 under the promoter of alpha-CaMKII with a random integration approach. While such models are useful, several potential confounds have to be considered, as we have discussed in detail elsewhere35, and briefly summarize as follows. First, random integration of transgenes can produce off-target effects by disrupting other genes at the insertion sites. Second, the location of the transgene’s insertion influences the cell type-specificity of its expression pattern, as does the use of a heterologous promoter. Effects of the gene product in cell types where it would not naturally be expressed may result in a phenotype that does not reflect the physiological function of the transgene. Moreover, expression of both native and exogenous DISC1 protein might also produce effects by non-physiological mechanisms, such as aggregation of excess protein. Indeed, the DISC1 protein appears to be prone to aggregation36. Some of these gain-of-function mechanisms might indeed link DISC1 to brain functions in human pathologies36, possibly including disturbed sleep. However, this could be the case also in the absence of a physiological role of DISC1 in sleep-regulation. Likewise, our study was focused on a natural deletion mutation encoding a premature stop codon that appears to be partially skipped over by alternative splicing16. These caveats must be addressed before a firm conclusion about a putative role of wild-type Disc1 for sleep regulation is reached. A mouse line in which no Disc1 gene product is made could more conclusively address whether there is a role for wild-type Disc1 in sleep.

We can conclude, however, that the natural deletion mutation of Disc1 found in 129 and other widely used mouse strains does not noticeably affect sleep regulation. This is important for the field of sleep research. Many relevant transgenic mouse lines are created using stem cells derived from affected lines, including complete or partial knockout mice e.g. refs 3739, cre lines for sleep-related neurotransmitters e.g. refs 4043 and reporter lines for selective manipulation of sleep-related cell types e.g. refs 44 and 45. It has been cautioned that the possible presence of the Disc1 deletion mutation in such mice could confound the study of behaviors that are affected by this mutation13. Our finding suggests that this is no reason for concern for sleep research.

Methods

Breeding of S4-Disc1 mice

S4 mice congenic for the wild-type (WT) Disc1 allele (S4-Disc1) were derived by nine rounds of backcrossing of the Disc1 allele derived from B6N into the S4 background aided by a PCR assay developed previously13. Briefly, PCR with 36 cycles of 94 °C 20 sec, 54 °C 60 sec, 72 °C 40 sec using two primers (Forward-5′-GCTGTGACCTGATGGCACT and Reverse-5′-GCAAAGTCACCTCAATAACCA) produced a 196 base pair product for the WT (B6N derived) allele and a 171 base pair product for the deleted (S4 derived) allele. Early in this process the WT allele was passed through the female germline directly for two generations (2 and 3), ensuring that the developing line carried the S4 Y chromosome. Subsequently, the WT allele was passed through the male germline directly for 5 generations (4 through 8), ensuring that the X chromosomes were S4 derived and essentially all B6 mitochondria were diluted out. At generation four a SNP (single nucleotide polymorphism) analysis measuring 385 loci identified a male that was 95% S4, compared to the 93.75% predicted by chance. The lineage was again analyzed at generation seven with a 1417 SNP panel and a male that was 99.58% S4 was identified, compared to the predicted 99.375%. Since the allele was subsequently passed through S4 two more times, the resulting mice were theoretically 99.895% S4. This is possibly an overestimate but we can safely conclude the mice are >99.6% S4.

Experimental mice

For the first set of experiments we used male mice of strains C57BL/6NCrl (N = 8, age 28.9 ± 1.9 weeks, mean ± standard deviation), and S4 (N = 6, age 27.7 ± 1.1 weeks). For the second set of experiments, we used male mice of strains S4-Disc1 (N = 6, age 26.6 ± 1.5 weeks) and S4 (N = 8, age 25.0 ± 3.1 weeks). All mice were bred in house.

Mice were single housed in cages, which were placed in ventilated cabinets. The cabinets were not temperature- or humidity controlled, but the rooms were kept at 23 ± 1 °C and 45–55% humidity. Mice were provided food and water ad libitum, bedding, and nesting material. Light conditions were 12h/12h of lights on/lights off. In an effort to standardize recording conditions in the lab, the light sources were changed between the two sets of experiments. In the first set, cabinets were illuminated by LEDs, providing ~30 lux at cage level, and experiments were performed in spring. In the second set, cabinets were illuminated by fluorescent tubes, providing ~100 lux at cage level, and experiments were performed in late summer. Experiments were approved by the State Office of North Rhine-Westphalia, Department of Nature, Environment and Consumerism (LANUV NRW). Experiments were carried out in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. All measures were taken to minimize the number of animals used, as well as to maximally reduce their discomfort and pain.

Surgical Procedures

Mice were implanted with F20-EET transmitters (channel bandwith 1–50 Hz, Data Sciences International, St. Paul, MN) under isoflurane anesthesia. Transmitters were placed intraperitoneally in the first set of experiments and subcutaneously in the second set. The change of transmitter placement was part of standardizing the temperature recordings using this setup, which were used for unrelated studies. EEG leads were placed epidurally above the right parietal cortex (AP-0.5, ML 2.5 mm from bregma, negative lead) and above the cerebellum (center of interparietal bone, positive lead). EMG leads were anchored in the neck muscles. Mice were allowed at least two weeks of recovery before recordings.

Data acquisition and analysis

Sleep scoring and analysis was done as reported before38, 46, 47. EEG, EMG, and signal strength (as proxy for locomotor activity) were recorded via telemetry using DQ ART software (Data Sciences International). Sampling frequencies were 500 Hz, low-pass filter cut offs were 100 Hz (in addition to the 1 Hz high pass and 50 Hz low pass antialiasing filtering built in the transmitter). EEG and EMG recordings were scored in 10 s epochs as wake, rapid eye movement sleep (REM), or non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM) by expert scorers who examined the recordings visually using NeuroScore 3.0 software (Data Sciences International). To calculate bout durations, a bout was defined as consisting of a minimum of two consecutive epochs of a given state and ending with a single state change epoch. Latency to sleep was the time from start of a nap opportunity to the first occurrence of a NREM bout. EEG spectra were analyzed with a fast Fourier transform algorithm using a Hanning Window without overlap (NeuroScore) on all epochs without artifact. For direct comparisons of EEG power spectra, power was expressed as relative power, i.e. each frequency bin (0.122 Hz) was divided by the sum of the values between 0 and 50 Hz. Normalized NREM delta power was calculated as the raw power between 1–4 Hz (i.e. summed values of the respective frequency bins) of any epoch scored as NREM, divided by the average NREM delta power of the 24 h baseline recording. “Signal strength” is a measure that reflects position of the transmitter relative to the receiver antennae. It does not influence quality or amplitude of any of the transmitted data (i.e. EEG/EMG). The manufacturer DSI uses changes of signal strength as the mouse moves across the receiver plate to derive an “activity count”, which reflects amount and speed of motion per time. This is a semi-quantitative measure, which gives results comparable to the established beam break method48, and has been used to compare the overall activity between groups of mice49. To obtain a continuous rather than a discrete measure (as in DSI’s “activity count”), we expressed activity as the standard deviation of the signal strength values for each epoch, which gives a redundant (but continuous) measure.

Sleep deprivation

To probe the homeostatic response to sleep loss, mice were sleep deprived for six hours. Sleep deprivation started at lights on, i.e. Zeitgeber Time (ZT) 0, followed by 18 h of undisturbed recovery sleep opportunity. When a mouse assumed pre-sleep behavior, e.g. lowering the head to the floor, sleep was prohibited by tapping the cage or, if necessary, touching fur or whiskers with a soft brush.

To probe sleep propensity, mice were subjected to the murine multiple sleep latency test MSLT27. Starting six hours after lights on (ZT6), mice were sleep deprived for 20 min as described above. This was followed by a 20 minutes nap opportunity. This cycle was directly repeated 5 times.

Statistics

Statistical tests were performed using MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) and SPSS (IBM, Armonk, NY). For comparisons of EEG power spectra, we first performed two-way permutation ANOVA50 with 5000 iterations with factors ‘frequency bin’ and ‘mouse line’. If interactions were found, the source of the interaction was evaluated through bin-by-bin uncorrected t-tests between the groups. Only changes that affected a range of neighboring frequency bins were considered potentially meaningful. The same analysis was done for activity histograms, with factors ‘activity’ and ‘mouse line’. If not indicated otherwise, reported values are group means ± standard error of the mean.

Electronic supplementary material

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by a grant from Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DI1718/3-1) and internal funding from the DZNE. We are also grateful for technical support from Theresa Hundt, technological insight from Drs. Marco Weiergraeber and Cristina Baciu, and helpful suggestions from a journal appointed reviewer.

Author Contributions

L.D. and W.J. designed the project and wrote the manuscript, L.D. and A.P. performed experiments. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-06015-3

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Chouinard S, Poulin J, Stip E, Godbout R. Sleep in untreated patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Bull. 2004;30:957–967. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Benca, R. M., Obermeyer, W. H., Thisted, R. A. & Gillin, J. C. Sleep and psychiatric disorders. A meta-analysis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry49, 651–668; discussion 669–670 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 3.Harvey AG. Insomnia, Psychiatric Disorders, and the Transdiagnostic Perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17:299–303. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00594.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Harvey AG, Murray G, Chandler RA, Soehner A. Sleep disturbance as transdiagnostic: consideration of neurobiological mechanisms. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2011;31:225–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spiegelhalder K, Regen W, Nanovska S, Baglioni C, Riemann D. Comorbid sleep disorders in neuropsychiatric disorders across the life cycle. Current psychiatry reports. 2013;15:364. doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0364-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wulff K, Gatti S, Wettstein JG, Foster RG. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2010;11:589–599. doi: 10.1038/nrn2868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Monti JM, et al. Sleep and circadian rhythm dysregulation in schizophrenia. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Bol. Psychiatry. 2013;43:209–216. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pritchett D, et al. Evaluating the links between schizophrenia and sleep and circadian rhythm disruption. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2012;119:1061–1075. doi: 10.1007/s00702-012-0817-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Millar JK, et al. Disruption of two novel genes by a translocation co-segregating with schizophrenia. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2000;9:1415–1423. doi: 10.1093/hmg/9.9.1415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tomoda T, Sumitomo A, Jaaro-Peled H, Sawa A. Utility and validity of DISC1 mouse models in biological psychiatry. Neuroscience. 2016;321:99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jaaro-Peled H, et al. Abnormal wake/sleep pattern in a novel gain-of-function model of DISC1. Neurosci. Res. 2016;112:63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2016.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sawamura, N. et al. Nuclear DISC1 regulates CRE-mediated gene transcription and sleep homeostasis in the fruit fly. Mol. Psychiatry13, 1138–1148, 1069, doi:10.1038/mp.2008.101 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 13.Clapcote SJ, Roder JC. Deletion polymorphism of Disc1 is common to all 129 mouse substrains: implications for gene-targeting studies of brain function. Genetics. 2006;173:2407–2410. doi: 10.1534/genetics.106.060749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ritchie DJ, Clapcote SJ. Disc1 deletion is present in Swiss-derived inbred mouse strains: implications for transgenic studies of learning and memory. Lab. Anim. 2013;47:162–167. doi: 10.1177/0023677213478299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Clapcote, S. J. & Roder, J. C. In MGI Direct Data Submission Vol. MGI Ref ID J:118317 (2007).
  • 16.Ishizuka K, et al. Evidence that many of the DISC1 isoforms in C57BL/6J mice are also expressed in 129S6/SvEv mice. Mol. Psychiatry. 2007;12:897–899. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Koike H, Arguello PA, Kvajo M, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA. Disc1 is mutated in the 129S6/SvEv strain and modulates working memory in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:3693–3697. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511189103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Juan LW, et al. Phenotypic characterization of C57BL/6J mice carrying the Disc1 gene from the 129S6/SvEv strain. Brain structure & function. 2014;219:1417–1431. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0577-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gomez-Sintes R, Kvajo M, Gogos JA, Lucas JJ. Mice with a naturally occurring DISC1 mutation display a broad spectrum of behaviors associated to psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2014;8:253. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Colavito V, et al. Experimental sleep deprivation as a tool to test memory deficits in rodents. Frontiers in systems neuroscience. 2013;7:106. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Musiek ES, Xiong DD, Holtzman DM. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and the pathogenesis of Alzheimer disease. Exp. Mol. Med. 2015;47:e148. doi: 10.1038/emm.2014.121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Franken P, Chollet D, Tafti M. The homeostatic regulation of sleep need is under genetic control. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:2610–2621. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02610.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Franken P, Malafosse A, Tafti M. Genetic variation in EEG activity during sleep in inbred mice. Am. J. Physiol. 1998;275:R1127–1137. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.4.R1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Simpson EM, et al. Genetic variation among 129 substrains and its importance for targeted mutagenesis in mice. Nat. Genet. 1997;16:19–27. doi: 10.1038/ng0597-19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Threadgill DW, Yee D, Matin A, Nadeau JH, Magnuson T. Genealogy of the 129 inbred strains: 129/SvJ is a contaminated inbred strain. Mamm. Genome. 1997;8:390–393. doi: 10.1007/s003359900453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Franken P, Malafosse A, Tafti M. Genetic determinants of sleep regulation in inbred mice. Sleep. 1999;22:155–169. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Veasey SC, Yeou-Jey H, Thayer P, Fenik P. Murine Multiple Sleep Latency Test: phenotyping sleep propensity in mice. Sleep. 2004;27:388–393. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.3.388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Barker LE, Luman ET, McCauley MM, Chu SY. Assessing equivalence: an alternative to the use of difference tests for measuring disparities in vaccination coverage. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2002;156:1056–1061. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Keane TM, et al. Mouse genomic variation and its effect on phenotypes and gene regulation. Nature. 2011;477:289–294. doi: 10.1038/nature10413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ambrosius U, et al. Heritability of sleep electroencephalogram. Biol. Psychiatry. 2008;64:344–348. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.van Beijsterveldt CE, van Baal GC. Twin and family studies of the human electroencephalogram: a review and a meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 2002;61:111–138. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00055-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Simon MM, et al. A comparative phenotypic and genomic analysis of C57BL/6J and C57BL/6N mouse strains. Genome biology. 2013;14:R82. doi: 10.1186/gb-2013-14-7-r82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Matsuo N, et al. Behavioral profiles of three C57BL/6 substrains. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience. 2010;4:29. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Banks G, et al. Genetic background influences age-related decline in visual and nonvisual retinal responses, circadian rhythms, and sleep. Neurobiol. Aging. 2015;36:380–393. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.07.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kaczmarczyk L, Jackson WS. Astonishing advances in mouse genetic tools for biomedical research. Swiss medical weekly. 2015;145:w14186. doi: 10.4414/smw.2015.14186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Korth C. Aggregated proteins in schizophrenia and other chronic mental diseases: DISC1opathies. Prion. 2012;6:134–141. doi: 10.4161/pri.18989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mang GM, et al. Altered Sleep Homeostasis in Rev-erbalpha Knockout Mice. Sleep. 2016;39:589–601. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Morairty SR, et al. A role for cortical nNOS/NK1 neurons in coupling homeostatic sleep drive to EEG slow wave activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:20272–20277. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314762110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ingiosi AM, Raymond RM, Jr., Pavlova MN, Opp MR. Selective contributions of neuronal and astroglial interleukin-1 receptor 1 to the regulation of sleep. Brain. Behav. Immun. 2015;48:244–257. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Anaclet C, et al. The GABAergic parafacial zone is a medullary slow wave sleep-promoting center. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1217–1224. doi: 10.1038/nn.3789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bocchio, M. et al. Sleep and Serotonin Modulate Paracapsular Nitric Oxide Synthase Expressing Neurons of the Amygdala. eNeuro3, doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0177-16.2016 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 42.Vetrivelan R, et al. Melanin-concentrating hormone neurons specifically promote rapid eye movement sleep in mice. Neuroscience. 2016;336:102–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.08.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bjorness TE, Kelly CL, Gao T, Poffenberger V, Greene RW. Control and function of the homeostatic sleep response by adenosine A1 receptors. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:1267–1276. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2942-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Xu M, et al. Basal forebrain circuit for sleep-wake control. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:1641–1647. doi: 10.1038/nn.4143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Tsunematsu T, et al. Optogenetic manipulation of activity and temporally controlled cell-specific ablation reveal a role for MCH neurons in sleep/wake regulation. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:6896–6909. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5344-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dittrich L, Morairty SR, Warrier DR, Kilduff TS. Homeostatic sleep pressure is the primary factor for activation of cortical nNOS/NK1 neurons. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:632–639. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Parks GS, et al. The Dual Hypocretin Receptor Antagonist Almorexant is Permissive for Activation of Wake-Promoting Systems. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:1144–1155. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Streng T, Klemola T, Bastman S, Nevalainen J, Scheinin M. Comparison of simultaneous measurement of mouse locomotor activity by radiotelemetry and photobeam methods. J Pharmacol Toxicol Methods. 2015;71:90–94. doi: 10.1016/j.vascn.2014.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Gaburro S, et al. A mouse model of high trait anxiety shows reduced heart rate variability that can be reversed by anxiolytic drug treatment. The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;14:1341–1355. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711000058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Manly, B. F. J. Randomization, Bootstrap and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology. 3rd edn (Chapman & Hall/CRC, 2007).

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES