Abstract
Mechanistically linking movement behaviors and ecology is key to understanding the adaptive evolution of locomotion. Predator evasion, a behavior that enhances fitness, may depend upon short bursts or complex patterns of locomotion. However, such movements are poorly characterized by existing biomechanical metrics. We present methods based on the entropy measure of randomness from Information Theory to quantitatively characterize the unpredictability of non-steady-state locomotion. We then apply the method by examining sympatric rodent species whose escape trajectories differ in dimensionality. Unlike the speed-regulated gait use of cursorial animals to enhance locomotor economy, bipedal jerboa (family Dipodidae) gait transitions likely enhance maneuverability. In field-based observations, jerboa trajectories are significantly less predictable than those of quadrupedal rodents, likely increasing predator evasion ability. Consistent with this hypothesis, jerboas exhibit lower anxiety in open fields than quadrupedal rodents, a behavior that varies inversely with predator evasion ability. Our unpredictability metric expands the scope of quantitative biomechanical studies to include non-steady-state locomotion in a variety of evolutionary and ecologically significant contexts.
Biomechanical understanding of animal gait and maneuverability has primarily been limited to species with more predictable, steady-state movement patterns. Here, the authors develop a method to quantify movement predictability, and apply the method to study escape-related movement in several species of desert rodents.
Introduction
Locomotion is an essential tool in the evolutionary “arms race” between predator and prey. Convergent locomotor behavior in prey species can indicate a broadly successful evasion strategy, and the success of a particular evasive maneuver can be measured by directly observing predator–prey interactions1. However, understanding the aspects of locomotion that enhance predator evasion ability makes it possible to predict prey success in broader contexts, especially where direct observation of predator-prey interaction may not be feasible.
The evasive success of prey locomotion can be defined in the context of the predation strategy encountered2. This mechanistic understanding of predator–prey interactions enables the inference of predator evasion ability, even without direct observation of predation events. For example, maintaining high uniform velocities can be useful for prey that are hunted by predators using a simple pursuit strategy3. Often, the successful animal in a pursuit is the one with greater speed or endurance. On the other hand, predation based on ballistic interception requires the prediction of prey movement to plan a predator strike. Therefore, increasing the unpredictability of prey trajectories likely increases the chance of evading a predator’s ballistic interception2, 4, 5. Although the notion of unpredictability has been formalized in the field of Information Theory, this concept has not yet been applied to quantitatively characterize animal locomotion. Rather, previous studies of prey escape trajectories are limited to qualitative descriptions of “evasive maneuvers”6–8 or measurements of variance in speed or direction9–11, neither of which is a comprehensive measure of unpredictability. We present the first quantitative method to measure the unpredictability of motion in three-dimensional space by calculating the differential entropy of animal trajectories.
We demonstrate the utility of this method by examining whether bipedal locomotion increases predator evasion ability in desert rodents via increasing trajectory unpredictability. Transient bipedal locomotion is often associated with prey escape trajectories, and is performed by a variety of terrestrial animals, including lizards and cockroaches when chased, to achieve high running speeds12. In contrast, obligate bipedal locomotion has convergently evolved in desert rodents that are hunted via ballistic interception by owls and snakes5, 6, 13. Previous research has suggested that bipedal locomotion increases predator evasion ability with respect to sympatric quadrupedal rodents14, 15. However, the mechanism by which bipedalism increases predator evasion ability has not been identified.
Here we evaluate the kinematic, dynamic, and behavioral changes associated with the evolution of bipedalism in rodents by comparing the locomotion of sympatric bipedal jerboas and quadrupedal jirds. Jerboas (family Dipodidae) are derived bipedal desert rodents16, with elongate hindlimbs, three bipedal gaits, and erratic “ricochetal” locomotion that is often assumed to enhance evasion ability17, 18. Jirds (genus Meriones) are sympatric with jerboas and are quadrupedal, like the majority of rodents, including the ancestors of jerboas16. We find that jerboas significantly deviate from the gait usage patterns stereotypic of quadrupedal steady-state cursorial locomotion. Jerboas frequently transition between gaits with distinct dynamic functions, a behavior that likely contributes to increased maneuverability, which has the potential to enhance predator evasion ability. To test the hypothesis that bipedal jerboas have higher predator evasion ability than quadrupedal rodents, we measured the three-dimensional unpredictability of bipedal and quadrupedal rodent locomotion in response to simulated predation. Indeed, we found that bipedal jerboas increase their trajectory unpredictability with respect to sympatric quadrupedal jirds by increasing the likelihood of turning and leaping, likely increasing the jerboas’ ability to evade ballistic interception predation.
Because exposed microhabitats are an important source of nutrient resources, there is a conflict in small foraging animals between exploration and risk of predation that determines how long an animal will stay in an open area19. Enhanced evasion ability decreases the risk of predation in exposed microhabitats, resulting in an inverse relationship between predator evasion ability and thigmotaxis—the behavioral affinity to shelter19–21. Thigmotaxis can therefore be used to indicate relative evasion ability between similar animals that encounter the same predators. We used standard assays of rodent behavior to test the prediction that bipedalism is associated with a decrease in thigmotaxis in jerboas, further supporting our hypothesis that bipedalism increases predator evasion ability. While this study is limited to one example of bipedalism in rodents, kangaroo rats and Australian hopping mice have similar biomechanical and ecological divergence from sympatric quadrupeds that may also be explained by the divergence in trajectory unpredictability we measured in jerboas22–25.
Our study, which integrates laboratory- and field-based analyses of biomechanics and behavior, presents a new metric for quantitatively characterizing the entropy of non-steady-state locomotion that substantially broadens and contributes meaningfully to our understanding of interspecies interactions in a natural setting.
Results
Gait dynamics
Although bipedalism has evolved multiple times in rodents, jerboas are the only group observed to employ three different footfall patterns, or gaits18, 26, 27. The jerboa’s hopping gait is similar to kangaroo hopping — both hind limbs contact the substrate simultaneously and then have extended aerial phases, in which the animal is not in contact with the ground. Skipping involves staggered but overlapping hind limb contact with the ground interposed between aerial phases. Finally, jerboa running is similar to human running, with an aerial phase following the contact of each hind limb with the ground.
Animals with multiple terrestrial gaits are often specialized for sustained locomotion at high speeds, frequently described as cursorial. As the speed of locomotion increases, cursorial animals transition between gaits to minimize the cost of transport and reduce the loading impact on the musculoskeletal system28, 29. A low cost of transport increases the endurance of cursorial animals, enhancing their performance while migrating, outrunning predators, or chasing down prey30. Thus, patterns of energy consumption can potentially indicate the selective pressures shaping the evolution of locomotion. Similarly, previous observations suggest that jerboas hop at the lowest speeds, skip at intermediate speeds, and run at the highest speeds17, 18.
We first examined whether each jerboa gait is used exclusively at the speed range expected for cursorial locomotion. The speeds at which cursorial gaits occur can be predicted by the ratio of centripetal force to gravitational force (as an animal moves over its supporting limb), or the Froude number31. Given a leg length of 0.061 m (mean hip height at mid stance), dynamic similarity based on equivalent Froude numbers predicts that hopping should occur predominantly below 0.54 ms−1, skipping should occur predominantly from 0.54 to 1.21 ms−1, and running should occur predominantly at speeds above 1.21 ms−1. We found no locomotion under 0.5 ms−1, and surprisingly found no significant difference between the mean speed of each gait (F 2,77 = 2.82, P = 0.07, one-way ANOVA, Fig. 1a), though the highest speeds (up to 3.02 ms−1) were observed during hopping and the lowest speeds during running. Skipping exhibited the greatest range of speeds, and was used most often (64 of 80 single-gait trials). Although the maximum speed exhibited by jerboas in the laboratory is lower than the maximum speeds jerboas may exhibit in the field17, the maximum speed exhibited in the laboratory exceeds the speed at which the Froude equation predicts a transition to a running gait. Furthermore, the absence of locomotion at the lowest speed range predicted by the Froude equation, the substantial overlap between the speed ranges of each gait, and the fact that all three gaits were observed at submaximal speeds, contradict the expectation based on cursorial locomotion that speed regulates gait usage in jerboas.
We next quantified acceleration and ground-reaction forces to examine the dynamics underlying each gait. We found significant differences in mean acceleration among the gaits (F 2,77 = 4.00, P = 0.02, one-way ANOVA, Fig. 1b). Hopping was associated with the greatest values of acceleration and deceleration (Supplementary Movie 1). Indeed, a path analysis of transitions between gaits showed that hopping was preferentially used to decelerate, or stop locomotion (Fig. 2). Skipping exhibited the most symmetrical variation in mean acceleration across the broadest range of speeds (Supplementary Movie 2), and was the gait to which animals transitioned most frequently (Fig. 2). Running showed the least variation in mean acceleration, and was used primarily at lower speeds (Supplementary Movie 3). The ground-reaction force data confirmed and explained these trends by revealing that gait is a strong predictor of both minimum fore-aft (or decelerative) and the absolute value (or magnitude) of fore-aft force (Table 1, Fig. 1c, Supplementary Fig. 1). Furthermore, ground-reaction forces did not show a characteristic pattern through time for each gait, as would be expected for a cursorial animal using steady-state locomotion, and the vertical forces were not significantly different between gaits (Supplementary Fig. 2). These results suggest that jerboas transition frequently between gaits that exhibit distinct dynamic functions, thus increasing their capacity for maneuverability (Supplementary Movie 4).
Table 1.
Vertical | Peak | |Peak| | Mean | Minimum |
Force | F, p | F, p | F, p | F, p |
Gait | 2.513, 0.088 | 2.513, 0.088 | 0.003, 0.997 | 1.426, 0.246 |
Fore-Aft | Peak | |Peak| | Mean | Minimum |
Force | F, p | F, p | F, p | F, p |
Gait | 0.222, 0.801 | 9.32, 2.37e − 4* | 2.055, 0.135 | 7.097, 0.015* |
All tests had 2 degrees of freedom. Out of 80 trials, 64 were “Skip,” 8 were “Hop,” and 8 were “Run.” Asterisks indicate statistical significance (α = 0.05). |Peak| is the maximum of the absolute value of force values throughout a trial
Trajectory unpredictability
While morphology determines the “capacity” to generate complex behaviors (i.e., maneuverability), this only defines the limits of theoretical performance32, 33. Predator–prey interactions are determined by the “observed” performance of locomotor trajectories that result from path planning behavior, regardless of the animal’s theoretical maneuverability2. This distinction is explicitly measured by computing the unpredictability, or entropy, of the realized performance of locomotor trajectories:
1 |
where H represents entropy, X is the set of possible outcomes, or the set of three-dimensional positions relative to an animal’s current position, and p(i) is the probability of performing action i in X, or the probability of the animal occupying a position i in three-dimensional space relative to its current position34. Morphological traits that enable bipedal rodents to move extensively in the vertical direction increase the number of locomotor possibilities (increasing X) available to the animal, but the animal must “decide” to use these specialized morphological traits to generate more unpredictable locomotor trajectories (higher H).
The variance of observed escape trajectories has been used in previous studies to estimate predator evasion ability (reviewed in ref. 9), but unpredictability more accurately estimates a predator’s inability to compute a successful intercept course35. To illustrate, consider a prey animal with a sinusoidal trajectory (Fig. 3). The animal can increase its variability by increasing the amplitude of the lateral displacements, yet the overall pattern, and thus the unpredictability, of the locomotion remains the same. To quantify the effect of bipedal locomotion on trajectory unpredictability, we therefore developed methods to directly measure the unpredictability of rodent locomotion by using the notion of differential entropy that generalizes the entropy Eq. (1), below, from discrete to continuous probability distributions.
In an effort to quantify the effect of bipedalism on trajectory unpredictability, we observed the locomotion of freely moving wild-caught sympatric bipedal and quadrupedal rodents in response to simulated predation in their natural habitat (Supplementary Movies 5 and 6). We captured and observed two species of bipedal jerboa (Allactaga elater and Dipus sagitta) and one species of quadrupedal jird (genus Meriones). D. sagitta is a three-toed jerboa closely related, and of the same morphotype, as the J. jaculus jerboas in our laboratory experiments, and A. elater is a bipedal five-toed species that only makes contact with the ground via their three central hindlimb digits16. As expected, bipedal jerboa trajectories were more unpredictable (higher entropy) than quadrupedal jird trajectories (H(A. elater) = −9.936 > H(D. sagitta) = −12.60 > H(Meriones sp.) = −13.91). When each is compared to a null-hypothesis generated from a mixed data set selected from each pair of species (See Supplementary Methods for a description of this modified t-test), significant differences were found between the entropy (and therefore unpredictability) of bipedal A. elater and quadrupedal Meriones sp. (Supplementary Fig. 3A, A. elater P = 0, Meriones sp. P = 0.0002, modified t-test). However, we were surprised to find that the difference in trajectory entropy between the bipedal jerboa D. sagitta and quadrupedal jird Meriones sp. was only trending towards significant (Supplementary Fig. 3C, D. sagitta P = 0.0561, Meriones sp. P = 0.0605, modified t-test) at the α = 0.05 level. We also found that the two bipedal jerboa distributions differed significantly (Supplementary Fig. 3B, A. elater P = 0, D. sagitta P = 0.0002, modified t-test). Thus, each sympatric species moved with a distinct pattern of trajectory unpredictability.
To identify the locomotor behaviors that contribute to unpredictability in each species, we projected the multidimensional probability distribution into two marginal distributions: one illustrating the distribution of speeds (Fig. 4a) and another illustrating the distribution of angles (Fig. 4b–d). Quadrupedal jirds (Meriones sp.) used lower speeds most frequently, and the bipedal jerboa A. elater exhibited the most even distribution of speeds, reflecting a greater probability of acceleration and deceleration from step to step (Fig. 4a). The distributions of angles of motion reveal spatial preferences for each species. Despite more frequent contact with the ground, and therefore more opportunities to change direction in the horizontal plane, quadrupedal jirds showed the greatest preference for forward locomotion with few turns (Fig. 4b). In contrast, both jerboa species showed an increased preference for vertical motion (indicated by the longitudinal extent of the colored clouds in Fig. 4c, d), suggesting that the evolution of bipedalism likely facilitated the divergence in trajectory unpredictability between jerboas and jirds by allowing jerboas to utilize three-dimensional space and increasing their capacity for acceleration. Differences in entropy between the two jerboa species resulted primarily from the stronger preference of A. elater for turning (indicated by the extent of the colored clouds away from the y axis in Fig. 4c, d), and to a lesser extent a difference in the propensity for vertical motion. The difference in preference of horizontal movement direction explains how two species of bipedal jerboa can exhibit such significant differences in locomotor unpredictability.
Risk aversive behavior
Small, foraging animals that are susceptible to predation are subject to a conflict between the desire to explore new areas to discover food and the desire to remain in covered areas safe from predators. Due to this conflict, the amount of time an animal spends in an open area varies with their predator evasion ability (see Eq. (1)19), assuming that there is a minimum acceptable probability of death. Therefore, to determine whether prey with unpredictable trajectories have greater predator evasion ability, we measured rodent thigmotaxis.
During the simulated-predation trials we observed that the bipedal species explored the entire arena, while quadrupedal jirds had an apparent affinity for the walled periphery (Supplementary Movies 5 and 6). We therefore retrospectively analyzed the same locomotor trajectories according to the Open-Field Exploration Test, frequently used to assess thigmotaxis in rodents36. This test interprets a decrease in exploration and general locomotor activity in a brightly lit and exposed environment as a stress response to being without refuge from predators. Despite the stress being provided by simulated-predation rather than the more conventional bright lights, we found a significant increase in the percentage of time bipedal species spent in exposed areas (Fig. 5a).
To determine whether the divergence in microhabitat preference could be replicated under more controlled conditions, we performed the Light-Dark Box Exploration test, a standard assay of thigmotaxis with unambiguous categories of behavior and minimal restriction on locomotion36. We recorded the amount of time each animal spent in either the protection of a dark box or in exposed and brightly-lit open areas (Supplementary Movies 7 and 8). Lab-reared bipedal jerboas (J. jaculus) spent significantly more time exploring the open area than the lab-reared quadrupedal jirds (Meriones unguiculatus) (Fig. 5b, Welch Two Sample t-test: P = 0.005, t = −3.4113, df = 11.756, Supplementary Table 1). These results corroborate the pattern we found in the natural habitat and suggest bipedalism is associated with less thigmotaxis and greater predator evasion ability in open microhabitats.
Discussion
While laboratory-based studies of steady-state locomotion establish functional relationships between morphology and motion, these studies are highly abstracted from the way animals behave in natural circumstances11, 37. In predator–prey interactions, prey survival is often determined by momentary bursts of acceleration and rapid evasive maneuvers1, 19. Creating a tool that specifically characterizes these types of transient, non-steady-state locomotion makes it possible to more closely relate biomechanical performance to evolutionary fitness.
In general, non-steady-state locomotion enables prey to evade predators that hunt using a pre-calculated intercept course2, 5, 8. Ricochetal bipedal locomotion in rodents may be an adaptation to enhance predator evasion18, 38. For example, Australian hopping mice transition from quadrupedal to bipedal locomotion during evasive maneuvers27. Kangaroo rats are obligately bipedal, produce evasive maneuvers in response to predator cues, and have greater evasion success than sympatric quadrupedal rodents6, 7, 39. To determine whether the shift to bipedal locomotion enhances predator evasion, we developed metrics for quantifying the evasiveness of a maneuver. Since bipedal locomotion with large aerial phases increases the dimensionality of rodent locomotion, we sought out analytical methods capable of quantifying the contributions of both velocity and dimensionality to identify differences observed between bipedal and quadrupedal rodent locomotion.
Highly variable animal behavior is often characterized with ethograms to record the frequency and variety of behavioral components40, 41. Unfortunately, these methods require discretization of continuously varying behavior, which introduces bias into the identification of behavioral components42. As we have shown, jerboas move with step-to-step changes in stride length, direction, gait, and speed, making it difficult to determine an appropriate discretization method. Sorting jerboa locomotion by each of these unique factors would require an ethogram with an infinite number of states or a binning that is so coarse that it obscures underlying patterns of behavior.
Instead, to quantify the unpredictability of locomotion that is continuously varying in both space and time, we developed a method to measure the differential entropy of locomotor trajectories. In addition to a quantitative metric of unpredictability that corresponds to predator evasion ability, an initial step in calculating entropy is constructing a continuous model of the animal’s locomotion. When interpreted together, the model explains the components of movement that contribute to the animal’s overall trajectory unpredictability.
Many existing metrics of characterizing terrestrial motion use animal morphology as the frame of reference, often standardizing by body size to compare the theoretical relationship between morphology and performance between multiple individuals of the same species, or multiple dynamically similar species. In the context of predator–prey interactions, defining motion with respect to prey morphology is less relevant to prey fitness than defining motion from the perspective of the predator. Because our entropy calculation is agnostic to the theoretical capabilities of an animal’s morphology, it can quantitatively compare motion between animals that move with different mean stride lengths and in different planes of locomotion. Just as many predators are capable of hunting multiple species of prey, our entropy method enables relevant comparison between sympatric species that encounter similar selective pressures.
By revealing the biomechanical principles underlying interspecies interactions, the entropy method has the potential to explain the ecological structure of a community. For example, in desert ecosystems, small quadrupedal rodents tend to forage near bushes (high thigmotaxis), where the risk of predation is low and the likelihood of finding seeds is high43–46. This behavioral aversion to exposure is so ingrained in mice that it is a standard experimental system for understanding the genetics of anxiety and for measuring the effectiveness of therapeutic pharmaceuticals36. The increased trajectory unpredictability in bipedal jerboas enhances predator evasion and therefore decreases the risk associated with foraging in the exposed microhabitats common to sparsely vegetated deserts. Furthermore, competition with quadrupedal rodents would decrease if jerboas evolved a behavioral propensity to forage in exposed areas, where quadrupedal rodents are at higher risk of predation.
On the basis of the relationship we found between trajectory unpredictability and microhabitat preference, the evolution of bipedalism has the potential to influence desert community ecology. It is clear that divergence in microhabitat preference can enable resource partitioning to decrease competition in resource-poor ecosystems14, 47. The substantial differences we have recorded in trajectory unpredictability and open-field anxiety between sympatric jerboas and jirds (Fig. 5), along with several studies noting similar biomechanical and ecological divergence between bipedal and quadrupedal rodents on other continents22–25, suggest that bipedalism may have evolved to limit interspecific competition, thereby maintaining diversity among desert rodents14, 15. Indeed, many communities of desert rodents in Africa18, 48 and North America20, 49, 50, exhibit similar patterns of locomotor, behavioral, and ecological stratification that include the evolution of obligate bipedalism, whereas South American desert communities without bipedal rodents are less taxonomically diverse51. Thus, since diversity in predator evasion ability and foraging strategy can enable resource partitioning and species coexistence52, 53, we propose that locomotor innovation is an important factor that should be examined for its influence on the taxonomic diversity of animal communities.
This analysis of predator evasion ability in desert rodents demonstrates how our computational framework can be used to integrate biomechanical studies with other fields of biology. In addition to predicting prey fitness, this metric can be applied to quantitatively assess a variety of important motion behaviors. Measuring the entropy of movements through time can reveal how motor control is learned ontogenetically or evolutionarily54, 55. Just as the complexity of a bird song is an important signal to female songbirds, the complexity of behavioral mating displays can now be tested for its effect on fitness56, 57. Furthermore, combining entropy-based measurements of behavioral models and mimics with phylogenetic comparative methods can reveal the pattern and process of evolving behavioral mimicry58. Thus, the framework we present for characterizing non-steady-state locomotion encourages an integrative biomechanical approach that complements and mechanistically informs behavior, ecology, and evolution.
Methods
Animal care and use
All animal care and use protocols were approved by the Harvard Faculty of Arts and Sciences Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC protocols 28–23, 20–09) and the United States Department of Agriculture.
Laboratory gait analysis
We collected 139 trials from 5 male jerboas (J. jaculus) from a breeding colony at the Harvard Concord Field Station. Each animal was encouraged to travel along a narrow trackway (2 × 0.15 × 0.4 m3) over a two-axis force platform (0.06 × 0.12 m2) and past a high-speed video camera recording at 500 fps. The camera field of view captured one to three strides, depending on speed. The size of the trackway had no detectable effect on the range of speeds used by jerboas when compared to a previous study of unrestricted jerboa locomotion indoors (0.5 to 3.21 ms−1)18.
We visually categorized the gait of each stride by footfall pattern following terminology specific to bipedal animals12, rather than terminology traditionally used to describe quadrupedal gaits18. A total of 59 trials were excluded from speed and force analysis: 28 involved transitions between gaits, 8 included a stop or start from standstill (these 36 trials were used to generate Fig. 2), 2 involved changes between the leading foot between strides, and the gait in 20 trials was ambiguous. The remaining data set included 80 trials, with 7–29 from a single individual.
We calculated the speed of each jerboa by tracking its eye using DLTdv5 tracking software for Matlab59. The eye was used to estimate the center of mass, as the J. jaculus have fused cervical vertebrae16, 60, which limit head motion with respect to the body.
Gait transition predictions were made assuming that the transition from slow-to-medium gaits occurs at Fr = 0.5, and the transition from medium to fast gaits occurs at Fr = 2.5, and using the equation
2 |
where l is the leg length, g is gravity, and speed is u 31. Leg length was estimated as the height of the hip at mid-stance for each trial. For n = 80 trials, the mean leg length was 0.0605 m, 0.0066 m standard deviation.
Field trajectory unpredictability
We captured rodents from their natural habitat in the desert north of Fukang, in Xinjiang Province, People’s Republic of China. Bipedal jerboas (A. elater and D. sagitta) and quadrupedal jirds (Meriones sp.) were captured at night, then observed and released the following night. Animals were recorded moving within a 5 × 5 m2 fenced area in the natural habitat during their naturally active hours (21:00–1:00 CST, 19:00–23:00 Xinjiang Local Time). We illuminated the enclosure using infrared floodlights and recorded animal movements using cameras from which the infrared filter was removed (Casio ZR100). Each animal was filmed in HD (1920 × 1080 pixels) at 30 fps by two cameras as it responded to stimuli (e.g., looming, loud noises) for 15 min or until it presented signs of fatigue. In light of humans being slower and less maneuverable than the rodents’ natural predators, each trial consisted of one human stationed at each end of the enclosure. This had the effect of scaring the rodent towards the other human, increasing the rapidity of each encounter. The asymmetry in movement preference about the forward direction in A. elater (Fig. 4b) was likely in response to asymmetry in the human-generated stimulus, as random pairs of four individuals produced the stimulus from opposite ends of the enclosure, and one individual often provided a more effective stimulus than the other.
The center of the rodent body was tracked in three-dimensional space using DLTdv5 tracking software59. The calibrated volume was 4.3 × 2.5 × 0.4 m3. In each video, continuous segments of the tracked data < 20 frames were excluded, and each remaining segment of the continuously tracked data is hereafter referred to as a “trial.” To reduce the effect of outliers, we retained only the most similar 97% of the data for each species. This data set included 25,397 frames for 5 A. elater, 11,185 frames for 3 individuals of D. sagitta, and 20,609 frames for 3 individuals of Meriones sp. We chose to use kinematics tracking rather than other tracking devices (e.g., radio-frequency ID or PIT tags), which do not operate well over longer ranges and do not provide information regarding three-dimensional movement patterns.
Entropy calculation
To compare the locomotor unpredictability of each species, we developed methods to measure the entropy of trajectories in continuous three-dimensional space. Previous methods of entropy calculation require that data be either discretized and organized into a histogram or that the data be in the form of a continuous distribution. While our data are finite, we sought to avoid arbitrary discretization of three-dimensional space. Therefore, we used optimization to find the continuous probability distribution that best fit our finite data while making the fewest assumptions about the shape of the data. Measuring the entropy of the continuous probability distribution that best fits the observed data provides the unpredictability of the trajectories performed by each species. The mathematical steps leading to the optimization problem are presented below. The algorithmic solution to the optimization problem and the necessary mathematical proofs are presented in the Supplementary Methods.
To measure the entropy of a species, we first determined the probability with which each species travels to any given location in a given interval of time. For example, if the species prefers to move forward on the horizontal plane at high speeds, the animal would have a high probability of occupying locations directly in front and far from the present location; all other locations would be associated with a low probability. Since three-dimensional space and speed are continuous variables, the probability of occupying any potential location, given the current location, is described by a continuous probability distribution. Because our data are finite, we assume that they are sampled from a continuous probability distribution that represents the true movement preferences of each species. We seek to measure the unpredictability of each species’ true movement preferences, therefore we measure the entropy, H[f], of the continuous probability distribution, f, that best fits the finite data collected for each species, d.
The entropy of continuous probability distributions is called “differential entropy.” Given a continuous probability density function, with distribution f, the differential entropy is defined as:
3 |
where the limits of the integral are defined by the maximum and minimum distance that the animal can move in any direction in an instance of time, and x is a point in three-dimensional space. Note, in this case we have chosen −1 and 1 for convenience in the presented equations, but these values represent the minimum and maximum distance an animal can travel in three-dimensional space within one frame. Negative distances in this case correspond to backwards movement with respect to the previous frame.
However, constructing a continuous probability distribution that best explains the finite observations is non-trivial because a finite amount of data can be explained by an infinite number of continuous probability distributions. A continuous probability distribution is said to “fit” a finite set of observed data if the moments of the distribution match the moments of the data61. Statistical moments are quantitative measures that describe the shape of a data set. For example, the first moment, m 1, of a scalar dataset is the mean, the second moment, m 2, of a scalar dataset corresponds to variance, etc.
The moments, m α, for a continuous probability density function, f, are defined as:
4 |
where , denotes the set of three dimensional real numbers, x α denotes with , and denotes the set of nonnegative integers. Moving forward, we write where denotes the three-dimensional set of nonnegative integers.
On the other hand, empirical moments, , describe the moments of finite observations. Suppose we are given individual data points d 1, …, d N where for all i ∈ {1, …, N}. Then the empirical moments, , of the data are defined as:
5 |
Under mild assumptions, with an infinite set of empirical moments, a continuous probability distribution could be selected that perfectly matches the observed data61. However, computing an infinite number of moments is practically infeasible. A finite number of moments results in many possible continuous probability distributions that fit the empirical data. While making assumptions regarding the type of distribution producing the observed data (e.g., Gaussian) would reduce the number of possible continuous probability distributions, we have no basis for making such assumptions. To make the fewest assumptions about the type of distribution producing the observed data (“epistemic modesty”), we follow the Principle of Maximum Entropy62, which states that, amongst all the continuous probability distributions that fit the data, the best distribution is the one that maximizes the entropy. Therefore, we seek the continuous probability distribution that maximizes the entropy while matching the empirical moments of the observed data.
We can therefore restate this as an optimization problem:
6 |
where refer to those with (see Supplementary Methods). That is, we seek to find a continuous probability density function, f, that simultaneously maximizes the differential entropy while having its first 2k moments equal to the given empirical moments (see Algorithm 1 in Supplementary Methods to solve this optimization problem). Once the continuous probability function that solves this optimization problem is found, the differential entropy of the distribution can be measured. Furthermore, the continuous probability function can be visualized by examining projections of the distribution with respect to different variables (e.g., speed or angle of motion in three-dimensional space).
Field-based open-field exploration test
The same videos used to capture field trajectory unpredictability were visually analyzed for field usage following the “Open Field Exploration Test” as described in ref. 36. Animals within two body-lengths of the enclosure walls were categorized as “edge,” including climbing on the walls of the enclosure. All other times the animals were visible were categorized as “open.”
Laboratory-based light-dark box exploration test
We used four adult male J. jaculus jerboas born (in March 2013) and raised in the laboratory colony at the Concord Field Station mentioned above. 6 outbred adult gerbils (M. unguiculatus) were obtained from Charles River, had 3 weeks to acclimate, and were 70–72 days old at the time of experimentation. All rodents were housed individually, and animal housing, care, and experimental protocols were approved by Harvard IACUC and USDA. Each animal performed three trials. The location of the dark box and the animal trial order were randomized prior to experimentation. The trials were performed throughout the periods in which their housing rooms were dark and illuminated (light cycle).
We performed this anxiety test following the procedures in ref. 36. Animals were transported from their home cages in an opaque plastic transportation case that was sanitized between trials. We recorded the amount of time it took to capture each animal prior to experimentation to account for the stress of capture. Video was recorded from above with the same camera as in the simulated predation (Casio ZR100) in HD (1920 × 1080 pixels) at 30 fps. Recording began before the animal was brought into the experimental area in a sterilized plastic box, and ended after the animal was removed from the experimental arena. The experimental arena (1 × 1 × 0.35 m3) was made from clear plexiglas to aid in sterilization, with a yoga mat lining the bottom to provide grip. The shelter was an opaque plastic box (0.5 × 0.4 × 0.35 m3) with a 5 cm radius semicircular opening. The experimental arena was lit from above by a combination of fluorescent and incandescent lights, at 9000 lx across the exposed area. Animals were placed inside the experimental arena in the exposed area facing away from the shelter. The experimenter left the room for 5 min, retrieved the animal, and returned it to its cage. The experimental area, shelter, and transport box were sterilized with a disinfectant (Quatricide) between trials to limit the possibility of scent signals from a previous animal affecting the behavior of other animals. We visually recorded the amount of time each animal spent within the shelter vs. outside of the shelter, time spent in risk-assessment (face and forelegs outside of the shelter, with rest of the body within the shelter), and number of transitions between light, dark, and risk-assessment.
Code availability
Matlab code is available from the authors upon request.
Data availability
The data are available from the authors upon request.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This study was funded in part by the Chapman Memorial Scholarship to T.Y.M. The authors thank S (Paine) Valdes, Y.C. Yin, J.F. Chu, K. Jayaram for experimental assistance, J.A. Miyamae for digitization, and P.A. Ramirez for animal care.
Author contributions
All authors contributed to writing the text of the paper. T.Y.M.: Designed the study, performed experiments, and analyzed the data. K.L.C.: Designed the study and performed experiments. A.A.B.: Oversaw the experimental design. R.V.: Designed and performed the data analysis.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41467-017-00373-2.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Domenici P, Blagburn JM, Bacon JP. Animal escapology I: theoretical issues and emerging trends in escape trajectories. J. Exp. Biol. 2011;214:2463–2473. doi: 10.1242/jeb.029652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Moore TY, Biewener AA. Outrun or Outmaneuver: predator-prey interactions as a model system for integrating biomechanical studies in a broader ecological and evolutionary context. Int. Comp. Biol. 2015;55:1188–1197. doi: 10.1093/icb/icv074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Scott, W. & Leonard, N. E. Pursuit, herding, and evasion: A three-agent model of caribou predation. in American Control Conference (ACC)n 2978–2983 (2013).
- 4.Chance MRA, Russell WMS. Protean displays: a form of allaesthetic behaviour. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 1959;132:65–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1959.tb05513.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Edut S, Eilam D. Protean behavior under barn-owl attack: voles alternate between freezing and fleeing and spiny mice flee in alternating patterns. Behav. Brain. Res. 2004;155:207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Clark RW, Tangco S, Barbour MA. Field video recordings reveal factors influencing predatory strike success of free-ranging rattlesnakes (Crotalus spp.) Anim. Behav. 2012;84:183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Longland WS, Price MV. Direct observations of owls and heteromyid rodents: Can predation risk explain microhabitat use? Ecology. 1991;72:2261–2273. doi: 10.2307/1941576. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nishiumi N, Mori A. Distance-dependent switching of anti-predator behavior of frogs from immobility to fleeing. J. Ethol. 2014;33:117–124. doi: 10.1007/s10164-014-0419-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Domenici P, Blagburn JM, Bacon JP. Animal escapology II: escape trajectory case studies. J. Exp. Biol. 2011;214:2474–2494. doi: 10.1242/jeb.053801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mischiati M, et al. Internal models direct dragonfly interception steering. Nature. 2014;517:333–338. doi: 10.1038/nature14045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Combes SA, et al. Linking biomechanics and ecology through predator-prey interactions: flight performance of dragonflies and their prey. J. Exp. Biol. 2012;215:903–913. doi: 10.1242/jeb.059394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Alexander RM. Bipedal animals, and their differences from humans. J. Anat. 2004;204:321–330. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-8782.2004.00289.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Berman SL. Convergent evolution in the hindlimb of bipedal rodents. J. Zool. Syst. Evol. Res. 1985;23:59–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0469.1985.tb00570.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kotler BP, Brown JS, Mitchell WA. The role of predation in shaping the behaviour, morphology and community organisation of desert rodents. Aust. J. Zool. 1994;42:449–466. doi: 10.1071/ZO9940449. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rogovin K. On the origin of bipedal locomotion on (sic) rodents: Ecological correlates of jerboa’s bipedal hopping. Zool. Zh. 1999;78:228–239. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Moore TY, et al. Multiple phylogenetically distinct events shaped the evolution of limb skeletal morphologies associated with bipedalism in the jerboas. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:2785–2794. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Happold DCD. Biology of the jerboa, Jaculus jaculus butleri (Rodentia, Dipodidae), in the Sudan. J Zool. Lond. 1967;151:257–275. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1967.tb02114.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Schröpfer R, Klenner-Fringes B, Naumer E. Locomotion pattern and habitat utilisation of the two jerboas Jaculus jaculus and Jaculus orientalis (Rodentia, Dipodidae) Mammalia. 1985;49:445–454. doi: 10.1515/mamm.1985.49.4.445. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lima SL, Dill LM. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 1990;68:619–640. doi: 10.1139/z90-092. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kotler BP. Risk of predation and the structure of desert rodent communities. Ecology. 1984;65:689–701. doi: 10.2307/1938041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hendrie CA, Weiss SM, Eilam D. Behavioural response of wild rodents to the calls of an owl: a comparative study. J. Zool. 1998;245:439–446. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1998.tb00118.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Biewener A, Blickhan R. Kangaroo rat locomotion: design for elastic energy storage or acceleration? J. Exp. Biol. 1988;140:243–255. doi: 10.1242/jeb.140.1.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Clark R, et al. Comparison of anti-snake displays in the sympatric desert rodents Xerospermophilus tereticaudus (round-tailed ground squirrels) and Dipodomys deserti (desert kangaroo rats) J. Mammal. 2016;97:1709–1717. doi: 10.1093/jmammal/gyw137. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Baudinette R, Nagle K, Scott R. Locomotory energetics in a marsupial (Antechinomys spenceri) and a Rodent (Notomys alexis) Experientia. 1976;32:583–585. doi: 10.1007/BF01990175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dickman C, et al. Social organization of desert rodents during population “booms” and “busts” in central Australia. J. Mammal. 2010;91:798–810. doi: 10.1644/09-MAMM-S-205.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hayes G, Alexander RM. The hopping gaits of crows (Corvidae) and other bipeds. J. Zool. Lond. 1983;200:205–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1983.tb05784.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Marlow BJ. A comparison of the locomotion of two desert-living Australian mammals, Antechinomys spenceri (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae) and Notomys cervinus (Rodentia: Muridae) J. Zool. Lond. 1969;157:159–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1969.tb01695.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lee DV, et al. A collisional perspective on quadrupedal gait dynamics. J. R. Soc. Interface. 2011;8:1480–1486. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2011.0019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Farley CT, Taylor R. A mechanical trigger for the trot-gallop transition in horses. Science. 1991;253:306–308. doi: 10.1126/science.1857965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lull RS. Adaptations to aquatic, arboreal, fossorial and cursorial habits in mammals IV: Cursorial adaptations. Am. Nat. 1904;38:1–11. doi: 10.1086/278375. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Alexander RM, Jayes AS. A dynamic similarity hypothesis for the gaits of quadrupedal mammals. J. Zool. Lond. 1983;201:135–152. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1983.tb04266.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Webb PW. Speed, acceleration and manoeuvrability of two teleost fishes. J. Exp. Biol. 1983;102:115–122. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Norberg RA. Swallow tail streamer is a mechanical device for self deflection of tail leading edge, enhancing aerodynamic efficiency and flight manoeuvrability. Proc. R Soc. B. 1994;257:227–233. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1994.0119. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Shannon CE. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 1948;27:379–423. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Humphries DA, Driver PM. Protean defence by prey animals. Oecologia. 1970;5:285–302. doi: 10.1007/BF00815496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bailey, K. R. & Crawley, J. N. in Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience (ed Buccafusco, JJ) Ch 5. (CRC Press, Boca Raton, 2009). [PubMed]
- 37.Astley HC, Abbott EM, Azizi E, Marsh RL, Roberts TJ. Chasing maximal performance: A cautionary tale from the celebrated jumping frogs of Calaveras County. J. Exp. Biol. 2013;216:3947–3953. doi: 10.1242/jeb.090357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bartholomew GA, Caswell HH. Locomotion in kangaroo rats and its adaptive significance. J. Mammal. 1951;32:155–169. doi: 10.2307/1375371. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Webster DB. A function of the enlarged middle-ear cavities of the kangaroo rat. Dipodomys. Physiol. Zool. 1962;35:248–255. doi: 10.1086/physzool.35.3.30152809. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Grant EC. An analysis of the social behaviour of the male laboratory rat. Behaviour. 1963;21:261–281. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kolmes SA. An information-theory analysis of task specialization among worker honey bees performing hive duties. Anim. Behav. 1985;33:181–187. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80131-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.MacNulty DR, Mech LD, Smith DW. A proposed ethogram of large-carnivore predatory behavior, exemplified by the wolf. J. Mammal. 2007;88:595–60. doi: 10.1644/06-MAMM-A-119R1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Clarke JA. Moonlight’s influence on predator/prey interactions between short-eared owls (Asio flammeus) and deermice (Peromyscus maniculatus) Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1983;13:205–209. doi: 10.1007/BF00299924. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kotler BP. Effects of illumination on the rate of resource harvesting in a community of desert rodents. Am. Midl. Nat. 1984;111:383–389. doi: 10.2307/2425333. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Griffin PC, Griffin SC, Waroquiers C, Mills LS. Mortality by moonlight: Predation risk and the snowshoe hare. Behav. Ecol. 2005;16:938–944. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ari074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ellard CG, Eller MC. Spatial cognition in the gerbil: Computing optimal escape routes from visual threats. Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:333–345. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0193-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Schröpfer R, Klenner-Fringes B. Minimizing interspecific competition by different foraging strategies in 2 North African desert rodents. Acta Theriol. 1991;36:109–117. doi: 10.4098/AT.arch.91-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Petter F. Répartition géographique et écologie des rongeurs désertiques (du Sahara occidental et l’Iran oriental) Mammalia. 1961;25:1–222. doi: 10.1515/mamm.1961.25.4.485. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Harris JH. Microhabitat segregation in two desert rodent species: the relation of prey availability to diet. Ecology. 1986;68:417–421. doi: 10.1007/BF01036749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Price M. The role of microhabitat in structuring desert rodent communities. Ecology. 1978;59:910–921. doi: 10.2307/1938543. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kelt DA, Brown JH, Heske EJ, Morton SR. Community structure of desert small mammals: comparisons across four continents. Ecology. 1996;77:746–761. doi: 10.2307/2265499. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Schoener TW. Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science. 1974;185:27–39. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4145.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chesson P. Mechanisms of Maintenance of Species Diversity. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 2000;31:343–366. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.31.1.343. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Putman BJ, Coss RG, Clark RW. The ontogeny of antipredator behavior: age differences in California ground squirrels (Otospermophilus beecheyi) at multiple stages of rattlesnake encounters. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2015;69:1447–1457. doi: 10.1007/s00265-015-1957-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.MacNeilage, P. F. Vision and Action: the Control of Grasping 1–13 (Ablex, 1990).
- 56.Leitão A, ten Cate C, Riebel K. Within-song complexity in a songbird is meaningful to both male and female receivers. Anim. Behav. 2006;71:1289–1296. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Johnstone RA. Sexual selection, honest advertisement and the handicap principle: reviewing the evidence. Biol. Rev. 1995;70:1–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1995.tb01439.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Davis Rabosky AR, et al. Coral snakes predict the evolution of mimicry across New World snakes. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:11484. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hedrick TL. Software techniques for two- and three-dimensional kinematic measurements of biological and biomimetic systems. Bioinspir. Biomim. 2008;3:1–6. doi: 10.1088/1748-3182/3/3/034001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Hatt RT. The vertebral columns of ricochetal rodents. Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 1932;63:599–738. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lasserre, J. B. Moments, Positive Polynomials and Their Applications, Vol. 1 (World Scientific, 2009).
- 62.Uffink J. Can the maximum entropy principle be explained as a consistency requirement? Stud. Hist. Philos. MP. 1995;26:223–261. doi: 10.1016/1355-2198(95)00015-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data are available from the authors upon request.