Abstract
Episodic future thinking refers to the capacity to imagine or simulate experiences that might occur in one’s personal future. Cognitive, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging research concerning episodic future thinking has accelerated during recent years. This article discusses research that has delineated cognitive and neural mechanisms that support episodic future thinking as well as the functions that episodic future thinking serves. Studies focused on mechanisms have identified a core brain network that underlies episodic future thinking and have begun to tease apart the relative contributions of particular regions in this network, and the specific cognitive processes that they support. Studies concerned with functions have identified several domains in which episodic future thinking produces performance benefits, including decision making, emotion regulation, prospective memory, and spatial navigation.
Introduction
During the past decade, one of the most rapidly growing areas of research in cognitive neuroscience and psychology has focused on episodic future thinking: the capacity to imagine or simulate events that might occur in one’s personal future (cf., 1–3). This surge of research has been fueled in part by experimental demonstrations of striking cognitive and neural similarities when people are asked to imagine future experiences and remember past experiences (for detailed review, see 4). Here we review recent studies of episodic future thinking in human adults that have yielded new insights into the mechanisms that support episodic future thinking and the functions that it serves (for recent research on episodic future thinking in children and non-human primates, see 5–7).
Episodic future thinking is just one of several forms of future thinking or prospection (8). A recently proposed taxonomy (9) distinguishes among four basic forms of future thinking: simulation (construction of a specific mental representation of the future), prediction (estimation of the likelihood of a future outcome), intention (setting of a goal), and planning (organization of steps for achieving a goal). Each form of future thinking is proposed to vary according to its representational contents on a gradient from episodic (specific autobiographical experiences that may happen in the future, such as a meeting with a friend that will take place next week) to semantic (more general or abstract states of the world that may occur in the future, such as the political landscape of the United States after an upcoming election). In the context of this taxonomy, episodic future thinking could potentially refer to episodic simulation, prediction, intention, or planning. In practice, however, studies of episodic future thinking almost always focus on episodic simulation; indeed, the terms episodic future thinking and episodic simulation are frequently used interchangeably (cf., 2–3), and we will follow that practice here.
Mechanisms of episodic future thinking
We first discuss recent research on cognitive mechanisms of episodic future thinking and then turn to research on neural mechanisms.
Cognitive mechanisms
It has been proposed that episodic future thinking depends critically on the episodic memory system, which is thought to underlie an individual’s ability to recollect past personal experiences (10). One formulation of this view, referred to as the constructive episodic simulation hypothesis (11), holds that episodic memory supports future simulation by allowing people to flexibly retrieve and recombine elements of past experiences into novel representations of events that might occur in the future. Moreover, this view holds that flexible retrieval and recombination abilities, though adaptive for purposes of constructing episodic simulations, also may create memory errors that can result from mistakenly combining elements of distinct past experiences, such as source misattribution and false recognition. Consistent with this idea, recent experimental evidence points toward similar constructive mechanisms operating during episodic memory and future thought (12), and directly links flexible retrieval/recombination processes with source memory errors that result from mixing up elements of related episodes (13; see also, 14).
Some of the key cognitive evidence supporting a role for episodic memory in future imagining comes from studies using the Autobiographical Interview (AI; 15). In this procedure, individuals recall past and imagine future personal experiences. The details that they produce are then categorized as either internal (episodic details such as what happened, where it happened, and so forth) or external (facts/semantic details, commentary). For example, studies that have used the AI to compare healthy older adults with young adults consistently show that older adults produce fewer internal/episodic details and more external/semantic details for both remembered past events and imagined future events, suggesting a common role of episodic memory in both event types (for review of early studies, see 16, and for recent and related evidence, see 5, 17–21). Reductions in episodic detail for both past and future events using the AI and related procedures have also been documented in various patient populations, including in recent studies of patients with depression (22), post-traumatic stress disorder (23), amnesic syndrome (24–27; but see 28 for relatively preserved future imagining in amnesics), Alzheimer’s disease (29–30), unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy (31), schizophrenia (32), prefrontal lesions (33), and long-term opiate users (34).
While these and other findings point toward a role for episodic memory in future imagining, procedures such as the AI can be influenced by non-episodic factors. For example, in one study (35) that used pictures of everyday scenes as cues, young and old participants either recalled a past experience or imagined a future experience related to the picture (tasks that are thought to tap episodic memory), or simply described the picture (a task that should not involve episodic memory). Older adults produced fewer internal but not external details on the memory and imagination tasks, replicating earlier results, but they also showed the same pattern on the picture description task. Although this study also revealed effects of aging on memory and imagination above-and-beyond picture description performance, such findings nonetheless suggest that non-episodic factors that similarly impact remembering, imagining, and describing, such as narrative style or cognitive control, are at least partly responsible for similarities between remembering past experiences and imagining future experiences on tasks such as the AI (for discussion, see 16, 35). Other evidence similarly points toward picture description deficits in amnesic patients (27; but see 36 for contrasting results) and indicates that age effects on episodic future thinking are related to changes in more general cognitive functions (5).
In light of these considerations, a critical issue for theoretical interpretation is to identify episodic influences on task performance and to distinguish them from non-episodic influences. One recently developed approach to accomplishing this objective is via the use of an episodic specificity induction (ESI): brief training in recollecting details of past experiences (37–38). As discussed in Box 1, several experiments have shown that the ESI selectively dissociates the contributions of episodic retrieval to future imagining from such non-episodic factors as semantic retrieval and narrative style.
Box 1. Episodic specificity induction: Selective effects on remembering and imagining.
The approach known as episodic specificity induction (ESI; 37) involves administering a detailed interview that encourages people to focus on retrieving specific episodic details of a past experience (i.e., people, objects, and actions in a recently viewed videotape of an everyday event). According to the logic of the ESI approach, if a cognitive task relies at least in part on episodic retrieval, then performance on that task should be affected by an ESI administered just prior to the task (compared with a control induction). By contrast, if performance on a cognitive task does not rely on episodic retrieval, then task performance should not be influenced by a prior ESI. In an initial study using ESI (37), young and old participants viewed a videotape of people performing routine actions in a kitchen and received either an ESI or a control induction (providing their general impressions of the video). After each induction, participants received picture cues and either remembered a related past experience, imagined a related future experience, or described the picture (as in 35). Protocols were scored for internal and external details using the AI.
As shown in Figure 1, an ESI boosted subsequent production of internal details on the memory and imagination tasks, but had no effect on internal details in the picture description task and had no effect on the production of external details on any of the three tasks (effects of induction are collapsed across young and old participants because they were nearly identical for the two groups). Thus, ESI dissociated performance on the two tasks thought to draw on episodic retrieval (memory and imagination) from the non-episodic task (picture description), and also dissociated retrieval of episodic details from retrieval of semantic details during memory and imagination. These results were replicated in a subsequent experiment using different cues and procedures (97), and effects of ESI have also been extended to other tasks that draw on episodic retrieval and simulation, including social problem solving and divergent creative thinking (for discussion, see 38).
The foregoing observations indicate a distinct role for episodic retrieval in imagining future experiences, and various suggestions have been advanced concerning the precise nature of episodic mechanisms that support future thinking (cf., 4, 10, 38–39). Nonetheless, it seems clear that episodic future thinking is not simply a direct expression of episodic memory (39). In addition to the evidence for an impact of non-episodic factors considered earlier, other studies indicate that patients with semantic dementia, who typically exhibit severe problems with semantic memory despite relatively preserved episodic memory, exhibit marked impairments when imagining future experiences along with an intact ability to remember past experiences (30). These findings have led to the semantic scaffolding hypothesis, which holds that semantic memory provides a framework that guides future thinking, including episodic future thinking (for discussion, see 30).
A related role for higher-order (e.g., semantic and organizational) mechanisms is indicated by studies that have distinguished components of episodic future thinking. When constructing a future episode people typically access general personal knowledge before generating episodic details (40), organize episodic future thoughts into event clusters that reflect the influence of higher-order schematic, conceptual, and affective knowledge (41–42), and indicate a stronger subjective sense of “pre-experiencing” for future events that are associated with personal goals than for those that are not (43). Thus the construction of episodic details and integration of those details with higher-order autobiographical knowledge may represent distinct components of episodic future thinking (43).
Neural mechanisms
An important observation linking episodic future thinking with memory comes from neuroimaging studies indicating that a common core network of brain regions show increased activity when people remember past experiences and imagine future experiences (e.g., 44). This core network, comprising regions in the medial temporal lobe (MTL), the posterior cingulate including retrosplenial cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and lateral temporal and parietal regions largely corresponds to the well-known default network (45). Recent meta-analyses have confirmed that the core/default network is engaged during episodic simulation (46) and further that it is jointly engaged during episodic simulation and episodic memory (47).
In addition, the latter meta-analysis also revealed that core network regions including left posterior inferior parietal lobe and posterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex show increased activity during episodic simulation compared with episodic memory, as do several regions that are part of a distinct fronto-parietal control network (47). Related evidence reveals differences in functional connectivity among regions within the default network during episodic remembering and future imagining (48) and indicates that some default network regions (i.e., parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices) show increased activity during episodic remembering relative to future imagining, likely reflecting enhanced contextual processing for the former compared with the latter (49).
A critical task for research focused on neural mechanisms is to characterize the contributions of specific brain regions to episodic future thinking. Although this issue was addressed in several early studies of episodic future thinking (for review, see 4), novel approaches have appeared during the past few years (42, 50, 51). One involves adapting the widely used fMRI repetition suppression paradigm (52), where repetition-related reductions in neural activity are taken as indications that specific brain regions are involved in the processing of particular types of stimuli, by varying the content and frequency of future event simulation. In two experiments (53), participants simulated future events involving interactions with familiar people (social scenarios) or objects (non-social scenarios) in personally familiar locations. The frequency with which the participants simulated particular people, objects, and locations was manipulated, and reduced fMRI signal in a specific region as consequence of repeating a particular element was taken as evidence for the involvement of that region in processing the repeated element. Results revealed that distinct regions contribute to simulation of people (medial prefrontal cortex), interactions with objects (inferior frontal and premotor cortices) and locations (posterior cingulate/retrosplenial, parahippocampal and lateral parietal cortices). Moreover, simulated social scenarios (i.e., location-specific interactions with people) were linked with medial prefrontal, posterior cingulate, temporal–parietal and middle temporal cortices. The hippocampus, however, was responsive to entirely novel events for which all elements (i.e., person, place, and scenario) had not been previously simulated, pointing toward a link between hippocampal activity and event novelty, a finding that was also reported in an independent fMRI study of repeated event simulations (54). The repetition suppression technique has also been used to identify brain regions that contribute to emotional future simulations, with initial results linking the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus to simulations of negative future events and the orbitofrontal cortex to positive events (55).
The ESI procedure described earlier in behavioral experiments was recently applied to the analysis of neural mechanisms in an fMRI study in which participants were scanned as they imagined possible future experiences following either an ESI or a control induction (56). Participants exhibited significantly more activity in several core network regions during the construction of imagined events after the ESI than control induction. These regions included the left anterior hippocampus, right inferior parietal lobule, right posterior cingulate cortex, and right ventral precuneus. Further, a parametric modulation analysis indicated that induction-related differences in the episodic detail of imagined events were related to induction-related differences in the engagement of left anterior hippocampus and right inferior parietal lobule during the construction of imagined events. Resting-state functional connectivity analyses seeded in these two regions further revealed stronger coupling with core network regions following ESI compared with the control induction.
These findings point toward a link between hippocampal activity and the construction of specific and detailed imaginary future episodes, which strengthen previous observations along similar lines (57–60). Other fMRI studies have linked hippocampal activity during imagining to the process of scene construction (61–62), encoding of episodic simulations into long-term memory (63), and detail recombination (64). Mapping these subtly different cognitive constructs onto task manipulations can pose challenges. For example, Addis et al. (57) manipulated whether participants imagined specific events (e.g., proposing marriage) or general/routine events (e.g., commuting to a job), and documented increased hippocampal activity in the former condition compared with the latter. More recently, Palombo et al. (61) used a nearly identical manipulation that they characterized as high vs. low scene construction, and reported extremely similar hippocampal results to those of Addis et al. (57). Although they did not cite the Addis et al. paper, Palombo et al. nonetheless acknowledged that it is unclear whether their findings are a consequence of imagining specific vs. general events or high vs. low scene construction demands (for further discussion, see 65). An important task for future research is to try to disentangle these and other closely related cognitive constructs (see also 4, 34, 50).
Complimentary studies of neuropsychological patients have reported that some amnesic patients with hippocampal damage also exhibit deficits when imagining future experiences (25–26), but not all studies reveal such deficits (28). Thus questions concerning the precise role of the hippocampus in episodic future thinking continue to be debated, with the evidence suggesting that different subregions of the hippocampus make distinct contributions to particular subprocesses that comprise episodic future thinking and related phenomena (59–60, 62).
Finally, neuroimaging and patient studies have also begun to delineate the role of another key core network region in episodic future thinking, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). These studies suggest that the vmPFC contributes to both accessing schematic knowledge used in episodic simulations as well signaling their affective value (see Box 2).
Box 2. Contributions of the vmPFC to episodic simulation.
The vmPFC is part of the core network involved in episodic simulation (47), yet relatively little is known about the exact processes supported by this region. The vmPFC has been implicated in various different functions, including memory but also the representation of value and affect (98). Recent evidence suggests that during episodic simulation, vmPFC may support the intersection of those functions. Activation in rostral and ventral mPFC signals the emergent affective value of an imagined episode, and this value signal is difficult to account for by the nominal values of the individual elements that make up the episode (e.g., its location and present people or objects; 99–100). By signaling the anticipated value of an event, the vmPFC can mediate farsighted decisions (68–69).
Importantly, the contribution of the vmPFC is insufficiently characterized as valuating imagined scenarios because it also plays a role in constructing such scenarios. Lesions to this region can reduce the episodic detail and coherence of imagined events (101–102). This impairment seems to be particularly pronounced when patients have to imagine a broad scenario (e.g., hosting a dinner), because such patients have been shown to perform within the normal range of healthy controls when they are instructed to focus on a circumscribed moment within such a scenario (e.g., cutting vegetables; 25). However, there were also other important differences between the aforementioned studies, such as center of lesion overlap, etiology, and amnesic status of the patients. Nonetheless, the pattern suggests that the vmPFC supports access to schemata or conceptual knowledge of the respective scenarios that then foster the ability to construct specific episodes (101; see also 103). Consistent with this interpretation, fMRI data indicate that the vmPFC particularly supports simulations that can draw on rich knowledge (99) and that a more dorsal part of the mPFC is similarly more strongly engaged during simulations of episodes that are part of the same event cluster (42).
Taken together, the evidence suggests that the contribution of the vmPFC to episodic simulation may be twofold: accessing schematic knowledge and processing anticipated affect. Further research is needed to examine interactions between the two.
Functions of episodic future thinking
Evidence from thought sampling procedures indicates that episodic future thoughts occur frequently in everyday life and tend to be positively biased (66). Early laboratory and clinical studies provided initial evidence that they serve a range of functions, including decision making, emotion regulation, intention formation, and planning (67), and recent work has both strengthened and broadened this evidence.
An impact of episodic future thinking on decision making has been clearly revealed in studies on inter-temporal choice, where people make decisions regarding two reward options that differ in magnitude and delay until delivery, such as a smaller but more proximal vs. a larger but more distal reward. Typically, future rewards are devalued in relation to the extent of delay (i.e., temporally discounted), often leading to shortsighted choices of the smaller reward option. However, when people simulate consuming the larger reward, they become more patient and shift to favoring this more farsighted choice (68–70). Such effects have been extended to the domain of eating behavior, where engaging in episodic future thinking reduces calorie intake in both female undergraduates (71) and in overweight or obese women (72–73), thus biasing dietary decisions away from immediate food rewards and toward fulfilling longer-term health goals related to weight loss (72). Analogous effects have been observed on consumption of hypothetical alcoholic drinks in individuals with alcohol dependency problems (74) and on cigarette consumption in smokers (75). Recent studies have shown that such effects of episodic future thinking are more pronounced when the simulated event is related to the respective decision (e.g., imagining the future moment of buying a computer when making monetary choices or of an upcoming meal when being tempted by readily available snacks; 70–71). The observation that the impact of episodic future thinking is contingent on the content of the imagined event indicates that it does not merely reflect a generic change in future orientation.
Initial fMRI evidence pointed toward a role for the hippocampus in mediating effects of simulation on temporal discounting (68–69; 76). These observations have been supported by some evidence from patients with hippocampal damage (77) or atrophy following Alzheimer’s disease (76) showing that temporal discounting is not attenuated by situations requiring episodic future thinking in these populations (see also [78] for similar findings in healthy older adults). However, another study of amnesic patients (79) provided seemingly contradictory results, i.e., patients showed attentuation of temporal discounting in a situation also thought to induce episodic future thinking. This outcome may have occurred because in the latter study (79), amnesics could have benefited from mere semantic (rather than episodic) future thinking (see also 80). Consistent with this interpretation, recent fMRI evidence indicates that the role of the hippocampus may be less pronounced when people imagine familiar personal scenarios, i.e., in cases where they can draw on prior semantic knowledge (81).
Episodic future thinking has also been linked to emotional regulation, partly on the basis of evidence of reduced specificity and vividness of episodic future thinking in anxious individuals with emotion regulation problems (82–84). Recent evidence indicates that administering the ESI procedure described earlier just before individuals simulate possible solutions to personally worrisome future events has beneficial effects on emotion regulation: following ESI versus a control induction, participants generated more constructive steps to address a future worrisome event, were better able to reappraise the event, and showed improvements on several measures of subjective well-being (85). However, other evidence indicates that it can also be beneficial to instead suppress simulations of events that people fear may happen in their life (86): such suppression caused forgetting of details typically associated with the dreaded events, hindered the ability to subsequently imagine the events and, critically, also reduced apprehensiveness. People that were particularly efficient at down-regulating their fears of the future by suppression were also less trait-anxious, suggesting that suppression constitutes a natural coping mechanism. It will be important for future work to identify the relative benefits of simulation versus suppression. Related work shows that vivid and detailed episodic simulations of helping behaviors can facilitate empathy or prosocial intentions toward individuals in need (87–88).
Several studies have shown that episodic future thinking can boost prospective memory, i.e., the ability to remember carrying out a designated intention (e.g., action) at a future time. Simulating performing an upcoming intention makes it more likely that this intention will actually be carried out (89–91). These beneficial effects were of similar magnitude in older and younger adults despite the existence of episodic future thinking deficits in older adults (89), but were reduced in heavy social drinkers, who also show episodic simulation deficits (91). Overall these findings both highlight a functional benefit of episodic future thinking for prospective memory and also suggest a link between the two forms of prospection, which is further supported by a study that revealed significant positive correlations between episodic future thinking and prospective memory abilities in both young and old adults (19).
Finally, cognitive evidence points toward a role for episodic future thinking in shaping an individual’s sense of self and identity (92–93), both cognitive and neuroimaging evidence indicate a connection between episodic future thinking and divergent creative thinking (94–95), and studies of spatial navigation show that episodic simulation makes an important functional contribution to planning routes and achieving navigational goals (see Box 3).
Box 3. Episodic future thinking and simulation in navigational planning.
A possible role for hippocampal-dependent episodic future thinking in route planning has been suggested by experiments with rats revealing the involvement of hippocampal place cells in representing future locations during spatial navigation (for a recent example, see 104). These findings have been extended to humans by using fMRI to scan participants while they perform a virtual navigation task: multi-voxel pattern analysis revealed that hippocampal activity patterns were linked to prospective navigational goals as participants planned their routes, and that the strength of these goal-related representations was associated with goal-related activity in other core network regions (105). Related work has shown, intriguingly, that imagined spatial navigation is accompanied by fMRI signals in entorhinal cortex that display features characteristic of grid cells (106).
Evidence for a functional/adaptive role of navigation-related episodic simulations comes from behavioral experiments in which participants first became familiar with a virtual environment by taking a virtual walk through it streets, and after this encoding phase, simulated which route they would take prior to engaging in a goal-directed navigation task (107). An initial experiment showed that simulation times were positively correlated with navigation times. Importantly, participants mentally simulated a route at a much faster rate (2.39 times) than their time to actually navigate the route, that is, their episodic simulations exhibited temporal compression. A second experiment tested the hypothesis that such temporal compression reflects an adaptive feature of episodic simulations by varying the speed at which the spatial environment was initially encoded prior to simulation. If episodic simulations are adaptive, then they should be particularly compressed following slower initial encoding of a route, because such compression would allow people to simulate navigation of an upcoming route more quickly than if they were rigidly tied to the slow initial encoding time. Consistent with this hypothesis, there was a higher compression rate after a slow vs. a fast encoding condition (but no difference between a medium and slow encoding condition, perhaps because an optimal compression rate had already been achieved in the medium encoding condition). Importantly, faster simulation was positively related to how effectively and efficiently participants navigated a virtual route. Overall, these data indicate a functional role for episodic simulation in navigational planning.
Conclusions
During the past few years, the pace of research concerning episodic future thinking has accelerated, continuing a trend that began about a decade ago. There has been significant progress in characterizing the mechanisms that support episodic future thinking, and in identifying functions that episodic future thinking serves (see Figure 2).
Some key issues remain controversial, such as the nature of hippocampal contributions to episodic future thinking, and the related question of the extent to which neural and cognitive signatures of episodic future thinking are specific to imagining future experiences as opposed to atemporal imagining or imagining counterfactuals to past experiences (cf., 4, 28, 34, 39, 50, 58–61, 65, 96). Nonetheless, we believe that further attempts to integrate cognitive, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging evidence will eventually resolve such issues. Research regarding the functions of episodic future thinking has, if anything, accelerated even more rapidly. By linking episodic future thinking to an expanding range of functions and populations reviewed here, this research has opened up new questions and broadened the potential impact of studies focused on mechanisms. The future of research on episodic future thinking thus seems to us quite bright.
Highlights.
Research on episodic future thinking has increased dramatically during the past decade
Cognitive studies have begun to separate the contributions of episodic retrieval from non-episodic processes
Studies of patient populations have shown episodic future thinking deficits in various conditions
Neuroimaging studies are identifying the role of individual regions in a core brain network that supports episodic future thinking
Episodic future thinking serves diverse functions, ranging from decision making to spatial navigation
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this article was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH060941 and National Institute on Aging Grant AG08441 to DLS. RGB is supported by the Max Planck Society. We thank Alexis Carpenter, Aleea Devitt, Helen Jing, Kevin Madore, and Preston Thakral for helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement
None declared.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References and recommended reading
- 1.Atance CM, O’Neill D. Episodic future thinking. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:533–539. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01804-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Episodic simulation of future events: concepts, data, and applications. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2008;1124:39–60. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Szpunar KK. Episodic future thought: an emerging concept. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2010;5:142–162. doi: 10.1177/1745691610362350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schacter DL, Addis DR, Hassabis D, Martin VC, Spreng RN, Szpunar KK. The future of memory: remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron. 2012;76:677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Abram M, Picard L, Navarro B, Piolino P. Mechanisms of remembering the past and imagining the future: new data from autobiographical memory tasks in a lifespan approach. Conscious Cogn. 2014;29:76–89. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Atance CM, Louw A, Clayton NS. Thinking ahead about where something is needed: new insights about episodic foresight in preschoolers. J Exp Child Psychol. 2015;129:98–209. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Suddendorf T, Redshaw J. Children’s and apes’ preparatory responses to two mutually exclusive possibilities. Curr Biol. 2016;26:1758–1762. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gilbert DT, Wilson TD. Prospection: experiencing the future. Science. 2007;317:1351–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.1144161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Szpunar KK, Spreng RN, Schacter DL. A taxonomy of prospection: introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:18414–18421. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417144111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Ann Rev Psychol. 2002;53:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Schacter DL, Addis DR. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2007;362:773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12•.McDermott KB, Woolridge CL, Rice HJ, Berg JJ, Szpunar KK. Visual perspective in remembering and episodic future thought. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:243–253. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1067237. This study finds that people adopt comparable spatial viewpoints when remembering and imagining events from third-person perspectives, supporting the hypothesis that similar constructive mechanisms support memory and future thinking. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13•.Carpenter AC, Schacter DL. Flexible retrieval: when true inferences produce false memories. J Exp Psychol: Learn Mem Cogn. 2017;43:335–349. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000340. This study tests the idea that flexible retrieval and recombination processes, which are thought to be important for simulating future events, can also lead to memory errors. Adapting an associative inference paradigm that requires combining information from separate episodes, the authors showed that participants are more susceptible to false memories resulting from mistakenly combining details from related episodes for successful than unsuccessful inferences about the relations between these episodes, but only when contextual details were probed after (versus before) an associative inference test that engaged flexible retrieval processes. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dewhurst SA, Anderson RJ, Grace L, van Esch L. Adaptive false memory: imagining future scenarios increases false memories in the DRM paradigm. Mem Cogn. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0620-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psych Aging. 2002;17:677–689. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schacter DL, Gaesser B, Addis DR. Remembering the past and imagining the future in the elderly. Gerontology. 2013;59:143–151. doi: 10.1159/000342198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Anelli F, Ciaramelli E, Arzy S, Frassinetti F. Age-related effects on future mental time travel. Neural Plasticity. 2016 doi: 10.1155/2016/1867270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/1867270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 18.Lyons AD, Henry JD, Rendell PG, Corballis MC, Suddendorf T. Episodic foresight and aging. Psych Aging. 2014;29:873–884. doi: 10.1037/a0038130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Terrett G, Rose NS, Henry JD, Bailey PE, Altgassen M, Phillips LH, Kliegel M, Rendell PG. The relationship between prospective memory and episodic future thinking in younger and older adulthood. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:310–323. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1054294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Devitt A, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Episodic and semantic content of memory and imagination: A multilevel analysis. Mem Cogn. 2017 doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0716-1. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lapp LK, Spaniol J. Impact of age-relevant goals on future thinking in younger and older adults. Memory. 2017 doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1284240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Addis DR, Hach S, Tippett LJ. Do strategic processes contribute to the specificity of future simulation in depression? Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:167–186. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Brown AD, Addis DR, Romano TA, Marmar CR, Bryant RA, Hirst W, Schacter DL. Episodic and semantic components of autobiographical memories and imagined future events in posttraumatic stress disorder. Memory. 2014;22:594–604. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.807842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cole SN, Morrison CM, Barak O, Pauly-Takacs K, Conway MA. Amnesia and future thinking: exploring the role of memory in the quantity and quality of episodic future thoughts. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:206–224. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kurzcek J, Wechsler E, Ahuja S, Jensen U, Cohen NJ, Tranel D, Duff MC. Differential contributions of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to self-projection and self-referential processing. Neuropsychologia. 2015;73:116–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Race E, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. Medial temporal lobe damage causes deficits in episodic memory and episodic future thinking not attributable to deficits in narrative construction. J Neurosci. 2011;31:10262–10269. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1145-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zeman AZJ, Beschin N, Dewar M, Della Salla S. Imagining the present: amnesia may impair descriptions of the present as well as of the future and the past. Cortex. 2013;49:637–645. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dede AJO, Wixted JT, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Autobiographical memory, future imagining, and the medial temporal lobe. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:13474–13479. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1615864113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Haj ME, Antoine P, Kapogiannis D. Flexibility decline contributes to similarity of past and future thinking in Alzheimer’s disease. Hippocampus. 2015;25:1447–1455. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30•.Irish M, Piolino P. Impaired capacity for prospection in the dementias – theoretical and clinical implications. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:49–68. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12090. A thoughtful overview and theoretical analysis of recent studies that have revealed impaired episodic future thinking in patients with dementia. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lechowicz M, Miller L, Irish M, Addis DR, Mohamed A, Lah S. Imagining future events in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:187–205. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32•.Lyons AD, Henry JD, Rendell PG, Robinson G, Suddendorf T. Episodic foresight and schizophrenia. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:107–122. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12092. Previous studies had shown that schizophrenic patients are impaired when asked to imagine future events, but the present study used a recently developed measure, called Virtual Week-Foresight, that attempts to capture everyday situations where episodic future thinking (called episodic foresight by the authors) is required to solve problems that are presented in an electronic board game. Schizophrenic patients were less able than controls to apply episodic future thinking adaptively to solve problems. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rasmussen KW, Bernsten D. Deficits in remembering the past and imagining the future in patients with prefrontal lesions. J Neuropsychology. 2016 doi: 10.1111/jnp.12108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34•.Mercuri K, Terrett G, Bailey PE, Henry JD, Curran HV, Rendell PG. Deconstructing the nature of episodic foresight deficits associated with chronic opiate use. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:401–413. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12110. This study shows that chronic opiate users generated significantly fewer episodic details than did controls when imagining a personal future experience, but performed normally on an atemporal scene construction task. The authors suggest that this pattern reflects a specific deficit in self-projection into the future among the opiate users. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gaesser B, Sacchetti DC, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Characterizing age-related changes in remembering the past and imagining the future. Psych Aging. 2011;26:80–84. doi: 10.1037/a0021054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Race E, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. Living in the moment: patients with MTL amnesia can richly describe the present despite deficits in past and future thinking. Cortex. 2013;49:1764–1766. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Madore KP, Gaesser B, Schacter DL. Constructive episodic simulation: dissociable effects of a specificity induction on remembering, imagining, and describing in young and older adults. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2014;40:609–622. doi: 10.1037/a0034885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schacter DL, Madore KP. Remembering the past and imagining the future: identifying and enhancing the contribution of episodic memory. Memory Studies. 2016;9:245–255. doi: 10.1177/1750698016645230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Klein SB. Autonoetic consciousness: reconsidering the role of episodic memory in future-oriented self-projection. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:381–401. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1007150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.D’Argembeau A, Mathy A. Tracking the construction of episodic future thoughts. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2011;140:258–271. doi: 10.1037/a0022581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Demblon J, D’Argembeau A. Networks of prospective thoughts: the organizational role of emotion and its impact on well-being. Cogn Emotion. 2016;30:582–591. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1015967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42•.Demblon J, Bahri MA, D’Argembeau A. Neural correlates of event clusters in past and future thoughts: how the brain integrates specific episodes with autobiographical knowledge. NeuroImage. 2016;127:257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.062. This fMRI study is the first to examine neural correlates of event clusters – past or future experiences that are thematically or casually related to one another. The authors found that compared to unrelated events, experiences that were part of event clusters in either the future or the past elicited increased activity in medial and rostrolateral prefrontal cortex as well as in lateral temporal and parietal regions. The results provide insights into how specific events in either the future or the past are linked to higher-order autobiographical knowledge. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lehner E, D’Argembeau A. The role of personal goals in autonoetic experience when imagining future events. Conscious Cogn. 2016;42:267–276. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45•.Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030. A highly informative review of the discovery, characteristics, and functions of the default mode network, which is closely linked with episodic future thinking and related capacities. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stawarzyck D, D’Argembeau A. Neural correlates of personal goal processing during episodic future thinking and mind-wandering: an ALE meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:2929–2947. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Benoit RG, Schacter DL. Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia. 2015;75:450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48•.Bellana B, Liu Z-X, Diamond NB, Grady CL, Moscovitch M. Similarities and differences in the default mode network across rest, retrieval, and future imagining. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016 doi: 10.1002/hbm.23445. The authors acquired fMRI data while participants either thought about the past, imagined the future, or were not instructed to engage in any particular activity (i.e., they ‘rested’). Though all of these conditions were associated with activity in default-network regions, they yielded substantial differences in the connectivity pattern between those regions. These data suggest that the default network is not a static entity but that it gets dynamically configured according to current processing demands. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49•.Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, McDermott KB. The contextual association network activates more for remembered than for imagined events. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:611–617. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu223. The authors provide important insights related to task constraint and statistical power in addressing why neuroimaging studies of memory and imagination typically do not report regions of the brain that show more activity for remembering than future thinking. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Xu X, Yuan H, Lei X. Activation and connectivity within the default mode network contribute independently to future-oriented thought. Sci Reports. 2016;6:2100. doi: 10.1038/srep21001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Thakral PP, Benoit RG, Schacter DL. Imagining the future: The core episodic simulation network dissociates as a function of timecourse and the amount of simulated information. Cortex. 2017;90:12–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baron HC, Garvert MM, Behrens TEJ. Repetition suppression: a means to index neural representations using BOLD? Phil Trans R Soc B. 2016;371:20150355. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Szpunar KK, St Jacques PL, Robbins CA, Wig GS, Schacter DL. Repetition- related reductions in neural activity reveal component processes of mental simulation. Soc Cogn Affective Neurosci. 2014;9:712–722. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.van Mulukom V, Schacter DL, Corballis MC, Addis DR. Re-imagining the future: repetition decreases hippocampal involvement in future simulation. PloS One. 2013;8:e69596. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Szpunar KK, Jing HG, Benoit RG, Schacter DL. Repetition-related reduction in neural activity during emotional simulations of future events. PloS One. 2015;10:e0138354. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56•.Madore KP, Szpunar KK, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Episodic specificity induction impacts activity in a core brain network during construction of imagined future experiences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:10696–10701. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612278113. This fMRI study is the first to examine the effects of episodic specificity induction (see Box 1) on imagined future experiences. After receiving a specificity induction, participants imagined future experiences in greater episodic detail than after a control induction, and exhibited increased activity in several regions within the core brain network associated with episodic future thinking, including hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Addis DR, Cheng T, Roberts R, Schacter DL. Hippocampal contributions to the episodic simulation of specific and general future events. Hippocampus. 2011;21:1045–1052. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kirwan CB, Ashby SR, Nash MI. Remembering and imagining differentially engage the hippocampus: a multivariate fMRI investigation. Cog Neurosci. 2014;5:177–185. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2014.933203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59•.Schacter DL, Addis DR, Szpunar KK. Escaping the past: contributions of the hippocampus to future thinking and imagination. In: Hannula DE, Duff MC, editors. The Hippocampus from Cells to Systems: Structure, Connectivity, and Functional Contributions to Memory and Flexible Cognition. Springer; 2017. pp. 439–465. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Sheldon S, Levine B. The role of the hippocampus in memory and mental construction. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2016;1369:76–92. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Palombo DJ, Hayes SM, Peterson KM, Keanue MM, Verfaellie M. Medial temporal lobe contributions to episodic future thinking: scene construction or future projection? Cereb Cortex. 2016 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62•.Zeidman P, Maguire EA. Anterior hippocampus: the anatomy of perception, imagination and episodic memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17:173–182. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.24. An excellent review of cognitive activities associated with anterior hippocampus that highlights the important contribution of scene construction processes. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Martin VC, Schacter DL, Corballis M, Addis DR. A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:13858–13863. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105816108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Gaesser B, Spreng RN, McLelland VC, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Imagining the future: evidence for a hippocampal contribution to constructive processing. Hippocampus. 2013;12:1150–1161. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Roberts RR, Schacter DL, Addis DR. Scene construction and relational processing: Separable constructs? Cereb Cortex. 2017 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Barsics C, Van der Linden M, D’Argembeau A. Frequency, characteristics, and perceived functions of emotional future thinking in daily life. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:217–233. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1051560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Schacter DL. Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. Am Psychol. 2012;67:603–613. doi: 10.1037/a0029869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Benoit RG, Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. J Neurosci. 2011;31:6771–6779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6559-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Peters J, Büchel C. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron. 2010;66:138–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.O’Donnell S, Daniel TO, Epstein LH. Does goal relevant episodic future thinking amplify the effect on delay discounting? Conscious Cogn. 2017;51:10–16. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Dassen FCM, Jansen A, Nederkoorn C, Houben K. Focus on the future: episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite. 2016;96:327–332. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72•.O’Neill J, Daniel TO, Epstein LH. Episodic future thinking reduces eating in a food court. Eating Behaviors. 2016;20:9–13. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.10.002. The authors examined the effects of inducing overweight or obese women to engage in episodic future thinking about personal health goals while eating dinner in a public food court. Compared with a control group of obese/overweight women who thought about recent habitual behaviors, the episodic future thinking group consumed fewer calories. This group also consumed a reduced percentage of calories from fat and an increased percentage of calories from protein. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Sze YY, Stein JS, Bickel WK, Paluch RA, Epstein LH. Bleak present, bright future: online episodic future thinking, scarcity, delay discounting, and food demand. Clin Psychol Sci. 2017 doi: 10.1177/2167702617696511. doi:101177/2167702617696511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Snider SE, LaConte SM, Bickel WK. Episodic future thinking: expansion of the temporal window in individuals with alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016;40:1558–1566. doi: 10.1111/acer.13112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Stein JS, Wilson AG, Koffarnus AN, Daniel TO, Epstein TO, Bickel WK. Unstuck in time: episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:3771–3778. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4410-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lebreton M, et al. A critical role for the hippocampus in the valuation of imagined outcomes. PLoS Biology. 2013;11:e1001684. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Palombo DJ, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. The medial temporal lobes are critical for reward-based decision making under conditions that promote episodic future thinking. Hippocampus. 2015;25:345–353. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Sasse LK, Peters J, Brassen S. Cognitive control modulates effects of episodic simulation on delay discounting in aging. Frontiers Aging Neurosci. 2017 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kwan D, Craver CF, Green L, Myerson J, Gao F, Black SE, Rosenbaum RS. Cueing the personal future to reduce discounting in intertemporal choice: is episodic prospection necessary? Hippocampus. 2015;25:444–455. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Palombo DJ, Keane MM, Verfaellie M. Using future thinking to reduce temporal discounting: under what circumstances are the medial temporal lobes critical? Neuropsychologia. 2016;89:437–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Sasse LK, Peters J, Büchel C, Brassen S. Effects of prospective thinking on intertemporal choice: the role of familiarity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:4210–4221. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82•.MacLeod AK. Prospection, well-being and memory. Memory Studies. 2016;9:266–274. An insightful disussion of the ways in which various kinds of future thinking impact emotional well being. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Miloyan B, Bulley A, Suddendorf T. Episodic foresight and anxiety: proximate and ultimate perspectives. Br J Clin Psychol. 2016;55:4–22. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Wu J, Szpunar KK, Godovich S, Schacter DL, Hofmann SG. Episodic future thinking in generalized anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2015;36:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85•.Jing HG, Madore KP, Schacter DL. Worrying about the future: an episodic specificity induction impacts problem solving, reappraisal, and well-being. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016;145:402–418. doi: 10.1037/xge0000142. The authors examined the impact of an episodic specificity induction (i.e., brief training in recollecting specific details of a recent experience) on worry. Young adults first generated a series of worrisome future events, received either a specificity or control induction, and then performed tasks that focused on how to deal with the worrisome event. After receiving the specificity induction, participants generated more constructive behaviors to address the worrisome event, were able to reframe or reappraise the event in more detail, and showed improvements on several measures of subjective well being. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86•.Benoit RG, Davies DJ, Anderson MC. Reducing future fears by suppressing the brain mechanisms underlying episodic simulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:E8492–E8501. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606604114. The authors tested the hypothesis that future fears can be regulated by a mechanism akin to memory suppression. Volunteers provided events that they dreaded may happen to them, before they blocked out any thought or image of some of the events. Such suppression caused participants to forget details that were typical for their imaginings of the respective situation, hindered their ability to freshly simulate those events and, critically, reduced apprehensiveness. Similar to memory suppression, it was associated with a top-down modulation originating from the right dlPFC that down-regulated activity in the hippocampus and vmPFC. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Gaesser B, Schacter DL. Episodic simulation and episodic memory can increase intentions to help others. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:4415–4420. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402461111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Gaesser B, Dodds H, Schacter DL. Effects of aging on the relation between episodic simulation and prosocial intentions. Memory. 2017 doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1288746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Altgassen M, Rendell PG, Bernhard A, Henry JD, Bailey PE, Phillips LH, Kliegel M. Future thinking improves prospective memory performance and plan enactment in older adults. Q J Exp Psychol. 2015;68:192–204. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.956127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Neroni MA, Gamboz N, Brandimonte MA. Does episodic future thinking improve prospective remembering? Conscious Cogn. 2014;23:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Platt B, Kamboj SK, Italiano T, Rendell PG, Curran HV. Prospective memory impairments in heavy social drinkers are partially overcome by future event simulation. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:499–506. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4145-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.D’Argembeau A, Lardi C, Van der Linden M. Exploring the identity function of thinking about the future. Memory. 2012;20:110–120. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.647697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Rathbone CJ, Conway MA, Moulin CJA. Remembering and imagining: The role of the self. Conscious Cogn. 2011;20:1175–1182. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Addis DR, Pan L, Musicaro R, Schacter DL. Divergent thinking and constructing episodic simulations. Memory. 2016;24:89–97. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.985591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Roberts RP, Wiebels K, Sumner RL, van Mulukom V, Grady CL, Schacter DL, Addis DR. An fMRI investigation of the relationship between future imagination and cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia. 2017;95:156–172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Schacter DL, Benoit RG, De Brigard F, Szpunar KK. Episodic future thinking and episodic counterfactual thinking: intersections between memory and decisions. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2015;17:114–211. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Madore KP, Schacter DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: selective effects of an episodic specificity induction on detail generation. Q J Exp Psychol. 2016;69:285–298. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.999097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Roy M, Shohamy D, Wager TD. Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:147–56. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Benoit RG, Szpunar KK, Schacter DL. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex supports affective future simulation by integrating distributed knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:16550–16555. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419274111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Lin W-J, Horner AJ, Bisby JA, Burgess N. Medial prefrontal cortex: adding value to imagined scenarios. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015;27:1957–1967. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Bertossi E, Aleo F, Braghittoni D, Ciaramelli E. Stuck in the here and now: construction of fictitious and future experiences following ventromedial prefrontal damage. Neuropsychologia. 2016;81:107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Bertossi E, Tesini C, Cappelli A, Ciaramelli E. Ventromedial prefrontal damage causes a pervasive impairment of episodic memory and future thinking. Neuropsychologia. 2016;90:12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Ghosh VE, Moscovitch M, Colella BM, Gilboa A. Schema representation in patients with ventromedial PFC lesions. J Neurosci. 2014;34:12057–12070. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0740-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Wikenheiser AM, Redish AD. Hippocampal theta sequences reflect current goals. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:289–294. doi: 10.1038/nn.3909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105•.Brown TI, Carr VA, LaRocque KF, Favila SE, Gordon AM, Bowles B, Bailenson JN, Wagner AD. Prospective representation of navigational goals in the human hippocampus. Science. 2016;352:1323–1326. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0784. In this fMRI study, the authors used a virtual reality procedure and multi-voxel pattern classification to show that the hippocampus and related cortical structures are involved in representing future goals during a spatial navigation task. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Horner AJ, Bisby JA, Zotow E, Bush D, Burgess N. Grid-like processing of imagined navigation. Curr Biol. 2015;26:842–847. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107•.Arnold AEGF, Iaria G, Ekstrom AD. Mental simulation of routes during navigation involves adaptive temporal compression. Cognition. 2016;157:14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.009. This study provides compelling behavioral evidence for the adaptive value of mental simulations of upcoming routes in a virtual navigation task. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]