Abstract
The ability to integrate experiential information and recall it in the form of memory is observed in a wide range of taxa, and is a hallmark of highly derived nervous systems. Storage of past experiences is critical for adaptive behaviors that anticipate both adverse and positive environmental factors. The process of memory formation and consolidation involve many synchronized biological events including gene transcription, protein modification, and intracellular trafficking: However, many of these molecular mechanisms remain illusive. With Drosophila as a model system we use a nonassociative memory paradigm and a systems level approach to uncover novel transcriptional patterns. RNA sequencing of Drosophila heads during and after memory formation identified a number of novel memory genes. Tracking the dynamic expression of these genes over time revealed complex gene networks involved in long term memory. In particular, this study focuses on two functional gene clusters of signal peptides and proteases. Bioinformatics network analysis and prediction in combination with high-throughput RNA sequencing identified previously unknown memory genes, which when genetically knocked down resulted in behaviorally validated memory defects.
Author summary
Long term memory formation is a complex process, and at different stages, requires regulation of transcription and protein synthesis. In a novel learning and memory paradigm, we examined transcriptional changes in the fly brain during and after memory formation. With RNA sequencing, we captured transcriptional waves of numerous genes previously not associated with learning and memory. Genes in the functional groups of proteases, signal peptides, and immunity, were selectively tested and behaviorally validated as memory genes. Placed into a large context and with computational methods, this work presents novel gene networks that may be linked to the learning and memory process.
Introduction
The ability to form a memory is one of the hallmarks of the advanced nervous system. This capacity to learn from, and remember, past experiences is a critical attribute to many eukaryotes. It is this function that allows organisms to meet the various demands and challenges of a changing and stochastic world: The interruption of these processes is associated with devastating illnesses, such as Alzheimer’s Disease and Huntington’s Disease, amongst others. Given the importance of this facility, it is vital for us to understand the basic biological mechanisms behind the complex process of learning and memory.
A traditional view of learning and memory involves two phases, acquisition and retention. However, decades of investigation have revealed this to be an oversimplification, and instead, a variety of studies point to the existence of distinct features for learning, short term memory (STM), long term memory (LTM), memory consolidation, and memory retrieval [1–4]. Differences in mechanism and neural circuitry for these functions point to a degree of autonomy for these related processes [5,6]. For instance, it has been suggested that disruption in memory retrieval may be a pathology distinct from other memory impairments [7]. This notion is further supported by the identification of discrete waves of transcriptional activity and protein synthesis associating spatially and temporally with the various learning and memory processes [8,9].
One convention in the field is the distinction between short term and long term memory. Intrinsic to the process of STM is the transient nature of the memory. Mechanistically it relies predominantly on reversible processes, such as protein modifications that alter synaptic function [3]. Altered function of the synapse is not persistent in STM, lasting from minutes to several hours. Additionally, it is functionally unique in that STM can respond rapidly to stimulus and is generally characterized by being independent of de novo protein synthesis [10]. Alternatively, LTM relies on the time and energy consuming processes of transcription and translation. Formation of LTM creates persistent and stable alterations of the synapse including both changes in synaptic connections, as well as synaptic potentiation. These changes can last for hours to weeks and are a combined result of transcription factor activation, protein synthesis, and synaptic protein reorganization [11,12].
Dynamic gene transcription is central to synaptic plasticity, with distinct waves of gene expression defining the various phases of LTM formation [2,13,14]. Immediate early gene (IEG) transcription occurs rapidly in response to neuronal activity. Within minutes, key IEGs are up regulate. This rapid response to stimulus is made possible by chromatin accessibility and de novo protein synthesis independence [15,16]. In mammalian systems IEGs are enriched for transcription factors and are required for triggering later transcriptional waves [17,18]. In Drosophila, these activity-regulated genes are less well characterized; although studies indicate that the general dynamics of expression persist these ARGs appear to include a more functionally diverse set of genes [19]. Later waves of gene transcription encode key memory genes responsible for neural plasticity and memory establishment [20]. In general, the specific activity-related and subsequent target genes that change in expression seem to be paradigm specific [18,21]. In addition, these subsequent transcriptional waves are classified as either cycloheximide sensitive or cycloheximide insensitive, pointing to multiple mechanisms of transcriptional regulation that are distinguished by requiring new protein synthesis [22,23].
The identification of individual memory genes, such as Fragile-X mental retardation gene 1 (FMR1) and rutabaga (adenylate cyclase), has enhanced our knowledge of specific molecular processes in the neurons [24,25]. However, genes are likely acting in complex networks rather than having a singular effect on neuron function. In this regard, much remains unclear and a global picture of gene expression networks has not yet been established. In part, progress has been hindered by the technical challenges associated with traditional learning and memory paradigms. In particular, the high degree of variability between individuals and tissue types has created roadblocks to the systems level analysis common in other fields.
Classical studies involving associative learning paradigms and courtship rejection have identified critical learning and memory circuits in the Drosophila brain. The mushroom body (MB), considered the learning and memory center in Drosophila, consolidates the multiple sensory inputs critical to learning and memory formation [1,26–28]. This study utilizes an alternative approach and recently developed Drosophila LTM paradigm involving the endoparasitoid wasp, Leptopilina heterotoma. These wasps are a natural predator of Drosophila larvae. Although adult flies are not at risk of parasitism, female flies have an innate response to the presence of these wasps, resulting in a cascade of behavioral changes dependent on LTM formation [29,30]. Advantages to this paradigm include the robust response to the predator, and the persistence of this response across several days. We use this system to explore global changes in transcription over the course of memory formation. With a systems level approach of combining molecular, behavioral and computational methods allowed us to discover several previously unidentified LTM genes. In addition, by using a bioinformatics methodology we begin to place these genes in a larger context that distinguishes LTM formation from LTM maintenance.
Results
Memory formation
It has been previously observed that when Drosophila melanogaster are cohabitated with the parasitic wasp Leptopilina heterotoma (LH14), flies will seek out ethanol containing food [29,30]. Further, when wasps are removed from the environment, female flies continue to favor ethanol-containing food as an oviposition substrate, a behavior that persists through the process of long-term memory formation. Using a similar experimental design we were able to replicate these previous findings (Fig 1A). Eggs were counted at the end of a 24-hour period, the proportion of eggs laid on ethanol food was calculated for each cage, giving an ethanol preference index; where a proportion of 0.5, or 50%, would indicate indifference to the presence of ethanol food. Under our environmental conditions, the baseline of ethanol preference in unexposed flies was approximately 20–30% (Fig 1D and 1E), signifying an avoidance of ethanol food. However, in the presence of wasps this ethanol preference increases to 93% (p-value 1.08E-5) in wild type Canton S (CS) flies (Fig 1D), illustrating a strong affinity for ethanol food under these conditions. In wild type flies, the ethanol preference is maintained following wasp exposure period, with female flies displaying an ethanol preference of 94% in the absence of wasps, which is significantly increased when compared to 32% unexposed (p-value 1.8E-4) (Fig 1E). These findings illustrate a behavioral switch perpetuated by the memory formed during wasp exposure. This ethanol seeking behavior has been shown to be robust across genetic backgrounds, and the maintenance of the ethanol seeking behavior is reliant on long-term memory formation. As expected, when we tested the classic memory mutants dnc1 and Orb2ΔQ, flies were able to respond to wasps, increasing from 19% and 19% to 96% and 94% ethanol preference respectively (p-values 1.5E-4, 1.7E-4). However, these mutants were not able to form memory, showing no significant increase in ethanol-seeking behavior after the wasp exposure period (p-values 0.96 dnc1, 0.42 Orb2ΔQ) (Fig 1D and 1E). These data support the previous finding that ethanol seeking post-wasp exposure is dependent on long-term memory formation.
Gene expression
To explore gene expression changes that correspond to this long-term memory formation, we sequenced four-day wasp exposed and paired unexposed head samples. A total of 165 genes had at least a log2 fold increase of 2, and 14 genes were decreased by log2 fold of 2 or greater, with a false discovery rate (FDR) of 0.05 or less (Fig 2A, S1 Table). Of these differentially expressed genes, six functional gene clusters were identified as enriched in a DAVID analysis with enrichment greater than three. In order from highest to lowest enrichment clusters are as follows; chitin binding and extracellular region, signal peptides, attacin-related, proteases, glycosidase and sucrose metabolism, and defense response to fungus (S1 Fig, S2 Table).
Similar results were observed when the fold change restriction was removed, and all genes with significant FDR were used as input. This analysis identified five functional clusters with enrichment greater than three. In descending order, these clusters are annotated as chitin binding, signal peptides, DM9 domain proteins, sugar metabolism, and proteases (S3 Table).
The DAVID analysis generated from the gene list with both log2 fold change cutoff and significant FDR returned two groups of particular interest: A protease cluster with enrichment of 4.9, and a signal peptide cluster with enrichment of 8.1 (Fig 2B, S2 & S3 Figs). Given the potential biological relevance of these groups, genes from these clusters with a minimum log2 fold change of 2 were used to generate interaction networks through Integrative Multi-species Prediction (IMP). IMP predicts and graphically displays interaction between the genes as well as key genes predicted to be part of the network (Fig 2C and 2D). It is noteworthy that the trypsin genes are cross-listed in both protease and signal peptide clusters, and this fact is partly responsible for some overlap between the two interaction networks. It is reasonable to assume that these clusters are integrated into larger networks and pathways, and possibly interact with one another. One observation in support of this possibility is the presence of Jon65Aiii, a member of the protease cluster, as a predicted node of the signal peptide network. Additional genes with overlap between the clusters are the predicted node of Tsp2A as well as κTry. Of interest, κTry is the gene with the greatest number of interactions in both clusters totaling 13 edges in signal peptide network, and 18 edges in protease network. Overall, the protease cluster was more highly connected and this necessitated the use of more stringent prediction parameters for this cluster to focus the analysis. In this network, after κTry the genes with the greatest number of interactions are CG10911 (17 edges), CG3868 (15 edges), and αTry (14 edges) (Fig 2D).
The most connected genes in the signal peptide cluster are Amy-d, βTry, Tsp2A with 12 edges each (Fig 2C). A subset of genes identified by sequencing was further confirmed by quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR) and validated as significantly differentially expressed (Fig 2E, S4 Table). PGRP-sb2 was used as a negative control, as it was not identified as differentially regulated in the sequencing data. P-values for all tests can be found in S5 Table.
Classical memory genes were noticeably absent for the list of differentially expressed genes; many classical memory genes, such as rut, dnc, and amn, were identified through mutagenesis screens rather than differential gene expression [31–33]. In addition, more recent memory studies have not observed these traditional memory genes to be differentially expressed during memory formation on a global scale [34,35]. Therefore, the absence of these genes in our differentially expressed gene list is not surprising. However, two previously identified memory genes were present in the sequencing list with significant FDR: RYa-R, a neuropeptide receptor (log2 fold change 3.4, FDR 1.25e-7) and scb, a member of the integrin alpha chain family, (log2 fold change 0.57, FDR 1.37e-4) [36,37]. These findings function as a partial positive control within the data set.
Dynamic gene expression across time
The process of memory formation is not instantaneous. In most learning paradigms, the animal requires repeated training sessions or extended duration of exposure to the stimulus for memory formation to occur. To capture transcriptional changes involved in the early stages of memory consolidation, we titrated the length of exposure to identify a wasp-exposure interval that did not confer the ethanol seeking behavior. Time points of 2.5, 7, and 14 hours were tested. We observed that neither 2.5 hours nor 7 hours of exposure is sufficient to trigger a behavioral switch in these flies. However, by 14 hours memory has been formed, as seen by the ethanol preference of 95% in the exposed group compared to 18% in the unexposed group (p-value 1.6E-4) (Fig 3A). Given these data, we hypothesized that unique memory-related genes not detected in the 4-day wasp-exposed samples, may be differentially regulated at these earlier time points.
To explore this possibility, sequencing data was generated from female fly heads with 2.5 and 7 hours of exposure (Fig 3B and 3C). The 2.5-hour time point revealed minimal differential gene expression (Fig 3B, Fig 4A and 4B). Two genes, Bsg25A and Elba3, were identified as having both a log2 fold change magnitude of 2 and significant FDR (FDR = <0.05). No genes with significant FDR were identified as overlapping with the 4-day differentially expressed gene list (Fig 4A).
At the 7-hour time point, 1693 genes were differentially expressed with a significant FDR, 93 of which had a minimum fold change magnitude of 4. Down regulated genes constituted the bulk of this gene list, with 79 genes down regulated compared to only 14 genes up regulated (S6 Table). When compared to the 4-day sequencing data, 10 genes were found to overlap between the two gene sets. Of these shared genes, only two genes are differentially regulated in the same direction; Jon65Aiii and Jon65Aiv are both up regulated (Fig 4C, Fig 5A, S7 Table). Interestingly, Bsg25A and Elba3 are also down regulated at the 7-hour time point, resulting in complete overlap of the 2.5-hour gene list (Fig 4B).
We further examined the temporal dynamics of gene expression by measuring RNA levels following a 14-hour wasp exposure (Fig 5, S4 Table). Considerable overlap exists between the 4-day and 14-hour time point: For instance, αTry, βTry, yip7, Jon65AiV, and Jon65Aiii were up-regulated at 14 hours, but to a lesser degree than the 4-day time point. Alternatively, IM18 and CecA1 had similar expression levels at the two time points. These differences in gene expression patterns hint at multiple regulatory pathways governing gene expression. Certain genes may reach their maximum induction quickly, or have their mRNA strictly regulated, resulting in plateaued RNA levels between the 14-hour and 4-day time points. Other genes may maintain increasing mRNA production as there is continuing memory formation or prolonged exposure to stress conditions.
In addition to dynamics of a specific gene across time, gene-gene interactions may be key to the memory formation process as well. Although the gene sets show limited overlap in differentially expressed genes, it is noteworthy that signal peptides were found to be enriched in a DAVID analysis of the 7-hour data set (S8 Table). Many of these genes are different from those in the 4-day signal peptide cluster; however, IMP network prediction suggests possible interactions between genes in the two clusters (Fig 5B). Further, the network analysis predicted nodes within the 4-day signal peptide cluster that are differentially regulated in the 7-hour samples. In other words, genes that are predicted to interact with the 4-day signal peptide cluster are differentially regulated at an earlier time. Specifically, CecA1, Dpt and Dro are up regulated in the 4-day exposed samples; these genes are known to interact with CecA2, DptB, and AttA (Fig 2C). These three predicted nodes of CecA2, DptB, and AttA are differentially regulated with significant FDR, although only CecA2 meets the log2 fold change threshold; with log2 fold change values of -2.86, -1.3, and -1.85 respectively.
To explore the role of these genes following memory formation, we measured transcript abundance in flies after a 24-hour recovery period following wasp removal. Although several genes exhibited a trend towards up or down regulation, none of the genes assayed had statistically significant differential gene expression, likely due to the unusually high variance in these samples (Fig 5A, S4 Table).
Behavioral validation
To evaluate the functional significance of the differentially expressed genes and their possible role in memory formation, we conducted behavioral experiments paired with gene knock down. Initial experiments were preformed with the Elav-Gal4 switch driver, an inducible pan neuronal driver, with the specific advantage of the Gal4 transcription factor being active only when the RU486 ligand is present. In this system the RU486 must be externally provided, thus allowing for temporal control of the RNAi expression (Fig 6A and 6B). Elav-Gal4 switch lines were crossed to UAS-RNAi lines for a subset of differentially regulated genes; βTry, Dpt, Kaz-M1, Jon65Aiii, IM18, yip7, Jon65Aiv, αTry, MalA1. κTry and Tsp2A were additionally tested as both were predicted to be an interacting gene in the IMP analysis although not differentially expressed. Expression level of κTry was confirmed with qPCR as not differentially expressed following wasp exposure (S4 Table). All of these genotypes had significant ethanol preference in the acute assays for vehicle only as well as with RU486 feeding, indicating that vehicle or RNAi depletion of each gene did not cause any defects in animals' ability to perceive and respond to the presence of the wasp predator (Fig 6C and 6D). All 10 genotypes had memory formation following wasp exposure with vehicle only feeding, indicating that ingestion of vehicle (5% methanol) did not disrupt memory formation (Fig 6E). However, after wasp removal Kaz-M1, Jon65Aiii, IM18, yip7, αTry, MalA1, κTry and Tsp2A displayed memory defects upon RNAi knock down (Fig 6F). These data indicate that each genotype is able to respond to wasps and form memory when the RNA-hairpin is not expressed. In addition, these results show that the RNA-hairpin expression does not inhibit the ethanol-seeking wasp response in the presence of wasps, but rather after wasp removal, RNAi depletion of certain genes interrupts memory formation or maintenance. Several attractive candidate genes did not result in memory defects, and we offer two conceivable explanations beyond the hypothesis that they are not relevant memory genes. First, it is possible that we had insufficient knock down of the gene to illicit a phenotype. Secondly, it is also reasonable to consider that, as the sequencing was performed on whole heads, some of the differentially expressed genes may be in non-neuronal tissues. In such a case our pan-neuronal driver would not express the RNA-hairpin in the relevant cell type.
To understand where in the nervous system these genes function, we used a more specific Gal4 driver line, which expresses in the learning and memory center of the Drosophila brain, known as the mushroom body (MB). Genes that yielded memory defects in the previous experiments were tested with this more specific MB-Gal4 switch driver. All genotypes tested had ethanol seeking behavior in the presence of wasps (Fig 7A and 7B). Additionally, memory formation of the genotypes was not disabled when treated with vehicle only (Fig 7C). However, IM18, Jon65Aiii, αTry, and ĸTry knock down in the MB caused memory defects. We considered the possibility that RNAi depletion of any essential gene in MB neurons could damage or kill cells important for memory consolidation or maintenance. In this scenario such genes would not be essential for memory per se, but instead they could simply be required for cell survival or neuronal activity. To test this possibility flies were treated with RU486 to induce RNAi depletion, as preformed previously, and then allowed to ‘recover’ for 4 days without RU486 feeding. Subsequently, they were tested for memory formation after exposure to predatory wasp. We found that RNAi knock down of IM18 and alpha Try before wasp exposure did not lead to disruption of memory (Fig 8). These observations suggest that knock down of these genes does not permanently damage the neurons of the mushroom body, which would prevent memory formation as an indirect consequence of gross neurological defect. Therefore, we conclude that IM18 and αTry are likely to be required for memory formation or maintenance as opposed to indirectly disrupting memory by affecting general neuronal processes and survival.
Of the validated genes resulting in memory defects in the signal peptide annotation cluster, little is known about functional processes, making speculation about their mechanistic role in the brain challenging. For instance, IM18 has no identified protein similarity nor does it have a described function [38]. Kaz-m1 has been studied in greater detail, located in the E(spl)-C locus it is a Kazal family protease inhibitor [39]: Yet, basic information on binding partners and localization remain unknown, making specific predictions impractical. However, general trends may point to larger scale pathways and processes. Jak/Stat signaling has been observed in the Kenyon cells of the mushroom body, leading to the hypotheses that immune signaling is triggering actin cytoskeleton arrangement or chromatin remodeling as part of memory formation [40]. Alternatively, it is possible that the activation of immune pathways is indicative of synaptic pruning similar to what is observed during nervous system development [41].
The protease annotation cluster has few well-characterized genes, but predicted functions of these genes and expression patterns provide useful information nonetheless. Yip7 RNA expression has been observed in Drosophila surface glia, although its role in these cells remains unknown [42]. More generally, serine proteases are suggested to interact with protease activated receptors. Further, Trypsin-like proteins have been observed at presynaptic terminals in mammalian models, leading to speculation that such genes are involved in synaptic plasticity and long term potentiation [43,44]. It is possible that similar processes are occurring in the Drosophila nervous system and that the up regulation of these genes during memory formation is part of synaptic remodeling.
Discussion
Memory formation, maintenance, and retrieval occur through an intricate system where information from new sensory inputs and existing neurocircuitry is consolidated. This process is massively complex and to date we lack a clear, global understanding of it. In the case of LTM, memory formation relies on basic functions, such as gene expression and protein synthesis. Dissecting these mechanisms and their dynamics brings the field closer to this large-scale view of learning and memory. In this study we approached the question of gene expression dynamics during memory formation in a novel non-associative LTM paradigm. We confirm previous findings that wasp exposure triggers a LTM dependent behavioral change resulting in female flies preferring ethanol-containing food as an oviposition substrate; and report RNA sequencing data specific to this wasp induced LTM.
It is well established that dynamic gene expression is necessary for persistent memory [2]. In this sense the results presented in this paper may not be surprising, where we identified 179 genes that were differentially regulated following a four day exposure to wasps. It is noteworthy, however, that more than 90% of these genes were up regulated. Previous gene expression studies have shown conflicting results in this regard. The distribution of differentially expressed genes is varied and seems to depend on the neuron type, paradigm, and timing of collection [18,21,45]. Given that samples were collected immediately following the removal of the wasps, it is possible that a number of these genes are activity regulated genes (ARGs), which are typically up regulated following neuronal firing [16,17]; although our sequencing data from earlier time points does not provide strong support for this hypothesis. Alternatively, this observation may be a behavioral paradigm specific phenomenon, possibly unique to non-associative learning.
Based on this data we identified six enriched functional gene clusters from our DAVID analysis. The gene cluster with highest enrichment related to chitin binding. A previous study has implicated peptidoglycans in the behavioral changes resulting from bacterial infection [46]. Enrichment of genes with these functional annotations may hint at a similar role for them in the modification of other defense related behaviors. In addition to this, two functional clusters, signal peptides and proteases, were of particular interest based on biological inference. These experiments have therefore generated a substantial candidate list of novel up regulated genes, some of which may be important for memory.
The signal peptide cluster was of particular interest, as it contained a number of immune associated genes. In particular, immune deficiency pathway (IMD) components appeared in the sequencing, in addition to two immune inducible genes IM18 and AttB. The IMD genes identified included CecA1, CecA2, Dro, AttA, Dpt, and DptB and were differentially regulated at various time points. These findings raise questions about the role of immune genes in learning and memory. The immune system in Drosophila has long been linked to inflammation and neurodegeneration [47–50]: Yet this would be a surprising discovery in a LTM paradigm. We suggest that a more likely scenario relates to recent studies that have been revealing a larger role for immune peptides in the nervous system, such as their participation in neuronal differentiation [51–53]. It is becoming clear that immune genes, and the IMD pathway, have non-canonical functions in the nervous system. Sleep regulation is one key area in which these genes are being examined. In particular Dro and AttB increase in expression with sleep deprivation, and more generally IMD genes are involved in sleep regulation [54,55]. Given the profound effect of sleep on learning and memory, we speculate that these immune genes are contributing to neuronal function in some way. Another tantalizing observation is that a STAT92E isoform is up regulated following courtship rejection training, perhaps implicating immune regulatory networks outside of the IMD pathway [35]. The precise mechanism underlying the importance of immune genes in the brain currently remains unclear. However, it has been speculated that perhaps immune signaling pathways are used as communication between neuron and non-neuronal tissues, such as the fat body [55,56]. Additional hypotheses have been put forth that focus on post-transcriptional actions, for instance NF-ĸB, the upstream activator of several immune pathways, has been implicated in affecting receptor density and synaptic stability [57,58].
Although this and other papers have presented plausible evidence for the role of immune genes in neurons; it is nonetheless important to consider indirect, system wide effects of the immune system and related processes. It has been shown that long term memory formation is energy demanding, as it requires protein synthesis and synaptic remodeling. These energy demands have measurable phenotypic outcomes, for instance one study found that Drosophila with enhanced memory in turn had reduced resiliency to starvation and dehydration [59]. Conversely, fruit flies under starvation conditions display impaired memory [60]. Such findings reinforce the notion that neurological processes such as long-term memory formation require tradeoffs. We would therefore be remiss to not consider the energy demands of the immune system. It is possible that these genes are negative regulators of the immune system, and by up-regulating these genes, energy is redirected from immunity to the brain. Alternatively, such genes may be protective against oxidative and immune related stress damage on the nervous system. Given the complexity of memory formation at the organism level, such hypotheses will need to be rigorously addressed in future works.
The second gene cluster explored was the protease enrichment group. Two genes identified showed particularly interesting trend: Jon65Aiii and Jon65AiV were the only two genes up-regulated at both the 7-hour and four-day time points. Their increased gene expression at 7 hours, before memory formation has occurred may indicate a role for them in memory initiation or possibly unidentified ARGs. Of further note, Jon65Aiii is cross-listed between the predicted gene networks for the signal peptide and protease clusters. Although not experimentally verified, the overlap between networks illustrates the complex interactions that may be at play in LTM. The predicted gene nodes of κTry and Tsp2A, again shared between both networks, add additional emphasis to complex interactions between gene networks.
Of the candidate genes, several were experimentally validated as LTM genes. Using a pan-neuronal conditional RNAi, we were able to show that Kaz-M1, Jon65Aii, IM18, yip7, αTry, and MalA1 are essential genes for this memory paradigm. In addition, we used network prediction to identify genes possibly important to the memory formation process that are not differentially regulated in our data sets. This computational method produced κTry and Tsp2A as candidate memory genes; remarkably, RNAi knock down of these genes yielded a memory phenotype, further validating such predictive tools. It is important to emphasize that the induction of double-stranded RNA and RNAi depletion of any of these genes did not inhibit wasp perception or the behavioral response; instead the memory formation itself was interrupted.
Additional nuances are being introduced to the field of study as the distinctions between LTM and memory consolidation are established [61]. The reinforcement and establishment of a memory outside of the traditional learning and memory centers of the brain, such as the hippocampus in mammalian systems, may be viewed as fundamentally different from LTM [62,63]. Inhibition of certain waves of protein synthesis appears to affect memory persistence, instead of memory formation, in some model systems [5,7]. However, the line separating these two processes is not well defined and the role of previously identified LTM genes may need to be evaluated in the memory consolidation process to truly understand their functional significance. Of the seven genes tested, IM18, αTry, Jon65Aiii, and κTry showed memory defects, indicating that these genes have essential functions in the learning and memory center of the Drosophila brain.
Identifying genes acting specifically within the MB was initially unexpected, as the expression data was generated from whole heads. It is possible that these genes have multiple functions within the brain, or possibly that they are highly expressed in enough cells to overcome this whole head dilution. Also surprising was the lack of differential gene expression at 2.5 hours. Previous data would suggest that ARGs should be activated within this time window. However, data also suggests that the shared number of ARGs across neuron type is limited, this may restrict the genes found due to the pooling effect caused by full head samples [19]. Future approaches that can detect single cell changes in gene expression will be particularly useful in spatially mapping how and where changes in gene regulation contribute to learning and memory.
Overall, this study has identified novel genes involved in non-associative LTM, and we have attempted to place these genes into a larger network context. Three of the genes identified have critical roles in the MB, while the remaining memory genes are likely acting either up stream or down stream of the learning and memory center. Although the behavioral experimentation suggests important roles for these genes, their specific functions are not known and require additional experimentation to elucidate mechanism. These data also support the existing literature that point to the IMD pathway as an important player in learning and memory. With continued research and the use of bioinformatics tools we hope these data complement and inform future studies into the process of LTM formation, and in combination with a robust non-associative learning and memory approach, we propose that gene function can be further dissected into learning, memory consolidation, and memory maintenance activities.
Methods
Fly husbandry and behavioral experiments
Stocks were maintained on standard Drosophila cornmeal-molasses media at room temperature (S9 Table). Memory formation experiments were conducted in vials (9.5 by 2.5 cm) containing 40 female flies and 10 male flies. The wasp-exposed group had the addition of 20 female Lh14 wasps, as previously described [30]. Wasp exposures were maintained for 2.5, 7, 14, and 96 hours (4 days). Memory formation was determined by ethanol preference, measured by a food choice assay. Briefly, one male and five female flies were placed into cages with two food sources, one with 0% ethanol and the other 6% ethanol food. Flies remain in the cages for 24 hours, at which point the food plates are collected and the number of eggs laid on each food source is counted in a blinded manner, such that the counter is not aware of genotype or treatment. As noted previously, the baseline ethanol preference/avoidance is sensitive to both temperature and humidity; all experiments reported were performed in a room with over head lighting and maintained at 25° with 30% humidity. All memory experiments measured ethanol-seeking behavior immediately following the removal of the wasps.
Acute response experiments were conducted to determine the ability of the flies to respond to wasps irrespective of memory formation. These experiments were completed in similar fashion to the memory experiments, but with the addition of three female wasps in the cages of the exposed group during the food choice assay.
Knock down experiments were performed using the UAS-Gal4 Switch system, where the Gal4 transcription factor becomes active only in the presence of RU486 [64,65]. Instant food impregnated with either the drug or vehicle only was used as the delivery system for this method of genetic manipulation. Two grams of instant food was hydrated with 8 mL of either RU486 (0.22 mg/mL) in 5% methanol, or vehicle only (5% methanol). Flies were transferred to new food each day. Memory experiments consisted of four days of feeding concurrent with wasp exposure. Ethanol choice assays immediately followed the removal of wasp and RU486/methanol food. Alternatively, the acute response experiments were comprised of three days of feeding concurrent with wasp exposure, followed by the acute ethanol choice assay. In these acute response experiments the feeding protocol of drug or vehicle only was maintained during the ethanol choice assay.
For the above-mentioned experiments, each is comprised of 10 cages per group unless otherwise noted. The data presented from these experiments is shown as a proportion of eggs laid on ethanol food compared to the total egg number from the cage and plotted as an average of the 10 replicates. Error bars were generated through bootstrapping the mean with 95% confidence. P-values were calculated from the Mann-Whitney U test. All statistics were calculated in R (version 3.0.2 “Frisbee Sailing”), p-values for all tests can be found in S5 Table.
RNA isolation and sequencing
Female flies were collected in 15 mL conical tubes, frozen in liquid nitrogen and briefly vortexed. Heads were separated using stackable steel sieves with pore size 710, 425, and 125 μm: Approximately 100 heads were collected for each replicate. Samples were maintained in Trizol at -80° until RNA isolation was performed using the miRNeasy Kit (Qiagen) with on-column DNase treatment. Four samples of each group were sent for sequencing on the illumina platform. Samples underwent rRNA reduction followed by random priming and were sequenced with a depth of 40 million reads. The adapters of short reads were trimmed by trimmomatic (version 0.33) [66]. The short reads were then mapped to Drosophila melanogaster reference genome (release 6.02) using STAR [67]. PCR duplicated short reads were removed by samtools (version 0.1.19) [68]. Read counts per gene were calculated via bedtools [69]. The differentially expressed gene analysis was performed with edgeR [70,71]. Expression values for the four-day wasp exposure experiment were calculated by normalizing the exposed group to the paired control samples. Additional time course sequencing experiments for hours 0, 2.5, and 7; expression values were determined by comparing to the 0 hour time point samples. Heat maps were generated using hierarchical clustering and the R package pheatmap. The sequencing heat map included every gene that had a log2 fold change magnitude of 2 or more and significant FDR at any single time point.
DAVID analysis & Integrative Multi-species Prediction (IMP)
Genes with significant FDR (FDR = < 0.05) and log2 fold change of 1 were used in the primary DAVID enrichment analysis [72,73]. Generation of the graphical illustration of the DAVID network required integration of the gene clusters and the corresponding fold changes from edgeR using a customized Perl script. The DAVID plot was subsequently created using a customized R script using package plotrix (Version 3.6–1) and iGraph (Version 1.0.1) with a Fruchterman and Reingold layout [74,75]. This code can be found at https://github.com/chenhao392/flyMemoryProject. Secondary DAVID enrichment analysis was conducted using all genes with significant FDR (FDR = <0.05) regardless of the fold change value.
Genes from these groups with at least log2 fold change of 2 were used to generate interaction networks in IMP (http://imp.princeton.edu) [76]. The clusters identified by DAVID were analyzed independently. The signal peptide cluster analysis used a 0.13 minimum prediction threshold and 10 additional gene nodes limit for the generation of the network. Given the increased number of interactions within the protease cluster, a more stringent threshold of 0.2 minimum prediction score and 8 additional gene nodes limit.
The network analysis for both the 7-hour and 4-day signal peptide clusters used all 9 genes from the 7-hour time-point with at least a log2 fold change of 2. The genes Amy-P and Yip7 from the 4-day cluster we used as input genes in the analysis; other genes from this cluster were identified by IMP as predicted interacting genes.
qPCR
cDNA was generated from RNA samples using the QuantiTect Reverse Transcription Kit (Qiagen). The iTaq Universal SYBR Green (BioRad) was used for the PCR reaction, and all primers were validated with standard curve before use (S10 Table). Gene specific data was normalized to actin and log2 fold change was calculated using the delta-delta CT method. An additional time point was collected post-memory formation: Following the 4 days of exposure, flies were separated from wasps and allowed to recover for 24 hours in a new vial before collection. All other time-points used RNA from the sequencing samples. Significance was determined by a two-tailed t-test. Statistical calculations were preformed in R (version 3.0.2 “Frisbee Sailing”).
Supporting information
Data Availability
All relevant data is contained with in the paper and supporting information files, with the exception of the raw sequencing files, which are on NCBI Sequencing Read Archive, with the corresponding BioProject ID PRJNA414223.
Funding Statement
This research was funded by the National Institute of Health, Pioneer grant 1DP1MH110234, (GB); the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency HR0011-15-1-0002 (GB); the National Institute of Health R01HD049116 (WET); National Institute of Health P01HD078253 (WET); and the National Institue of Health Training Grant T32-GM008704 (JB). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Madalan A, Yang X, Ferris J, Zhang S, Roman G. G(o) activation is required for both appetitive and aversive memory acquisition in Drosophila. Learn Mem. 2011;19: 26–34. doi: 10.1101/lm.024802.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Stork O, Welzl H. Memory formation and the regulation of gene expression. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences CMLS. 1999;55: 575–592. doi: 10.1007/s000180050316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bailey CH, Bartsch D, Kandel ER. Toward a molecular definition of long-term memory storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93: 13445–13452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dubnau J, Grady L, Kitamoto T, Tully T. Disruption of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom body blocks retrieval but not acquisition of memory. Nature. 2001;411: 476–480. doi: 10.1038/35078077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Igaz LM, Bevilaqua LR, Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Persistence of long-term memory storage requires a late protein synthesis-and BDNF-dependent phase in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2007;53: 261–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Aso Y, Herb A, Ogueta M, Siwanowicz I, Templier T, Friedrich AB, et al. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genet. 2012;8: e1002768 doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bekinschtein P, Katche C, Slipczuk L, Gonzalez C, Dorman G, Cammarota M, et al. Persistence of long-term memory storage: new insights into its molecular signatures in the hippocampus and related structures. Neurotoxicity research. 2010;18: 377–385. doi: 10.1007/s12640-010-9155-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Igaz LM, Bekinschtein P, Vianna MM, Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Gene expression during memory formation. Neurotoxicity research. 2004;6: 189–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Paratore S, Alessi E, Coffa S, Torrisi A, Mastrobuono F, Cavallaro S. Early genomics of learning and memory: a review. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2006;5: 209–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Barondes SH, Cohen HD. Arousal and the conversion of "short-term" to "long-term" memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1968;61: 923–929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McGaugh JL. Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science. 1966;153: 1351–1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Martin KC, Casadio A, Zhu H, Yaping E, Rose JC, Chen M, et al. Synapse-specific, long-term facilitation of aplysia sensory to motor synapses: a function for local protein synthesis in memory storage. Cell. 1997;91: 927–938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cavallaro S, D'Agata V, Manickam P, Dufour F, Alkon DL. Memory-specific temporal profiles of gene expression in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99: 16279–16284. doi: 10.1073/pnas.242597199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hirano Y, Ihara K, Masuda T, Yamamoto T, Iwata I, Takahashi A, et al. Shifting transcriptional machinery is required for long-term memory maintenance and modification in Drosophila mushroom bodies. Nat Commun. 2016;7: 13471 doi: 10.1038/ncomms13471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bepari AK, Sano H, Tamamaki N, Nambu A, Tanaka KF, Takebayashi H. Identification of optogenetically activated striatal medium spiny neurons by Npas4 expression. PloS one. 2012;7: e52783 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052783 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fowler T, Sen R, Roy AL. Regulation of primary response genes. Mol Cell. 2011;44: 348–360. doi: 10.1016/j.molcel.2011.09.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Herschman HR. Primary response genes induced by growth factors and tumor promoters. Annu Rev Biochem. 1991;60: 281–319. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bi.60.070191.001433 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Spiegel I, Mardinly AR, Gabel HW, Bazinet JE, Couch CH, Tzeng CP, et al. Npas4 regulates excitatory-inhibitory balance within neural circuits through cell-type-specific gene programs. Cell. 2014;157: 1216–1229. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Chen X, Rahman R, Guo F, Rosbash M. Genome-wide identification of neuronal activity-regulated genes in Drosophila. eLife. 2016;5: e19942 doi: 10.7554/eLife.19942 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Igaz LM, Vianna MR, Medina JH, Izquierdo I. Two time periods of hippocampal mRNA synthesis are required for memory consolidation of fear-motivated learning. J Neurosci. 2002;22: 6781–6789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.West AE, Greenberg ME. Neuronal activity-regulated gene transcription in synapse development and cognitive function. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2011;3: doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a005744 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lagasse F, Devaud JM, Mery F. A switch from cycloheximide-resistant consolidated memory to cycloheximide-sensitive reconsolidation and extinction in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2009;29: 2225–2230. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tucker A, Gibbs ME. Saccharin aversion memory in rats: Inhibition of cycloheximide-resistant memory by ouabain. Physiol Behav. 1979;23: 341–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Han P, Levin LR, Reed RR, Davis RL. Preferential expression of the Drosophila rutabaga gene in mushroom bodies, neural centers for learning in insects. Neuron. 1992;9: 619–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bolduc FV, Bell K, Cox H, Broadie KS, Tully T. Excess protein synthesis in Drosophila fragile X mutants impairs long-term memory. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11: 1143–1145. doi: 10.1038/nn.2175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guerin G, et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife. 2014;3: e04580 doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo TT, et al. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife. 2014;3: e04577 doi: 10.7554/eLife.04577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Perisse E, Yin Y, Lin AC, Lin S, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Different kenyon cell populations drive learned approach and avoidance in Drosophila. Neuron. 2013;79: 945–956. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kacsoh BZ, Lynch ZR, Mortimer NT, Schlenke TA. Fruit flies medicate offspring after seeing parasites. Science. 2013;339: 947–950. doi: 10.1126/science.1229625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kacsoh BZ, Bozler J, Hodge S, Ramaswami M, Bosco G. A novel paradigm for nonassociative long-term memory in Drosophila: predator-induced changes in oviposition behavior. Genetics. 2015;199: 1143–1157. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.172221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dudai Y, Jan YN, Byers D, Quinn WG, Benzer S. dunce, a mutant of Drosophila deficient in learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1976;73: 1684–1688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Livingstone MS, Sziber PP, Quinn WG. Loss of calcium/calmodulin responsiveness in adenylate cyclase of rutabaga, a Drosophila learning mutant. Cell. 1984;37: 205–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Feany MB, Quinn WG. A neuropeptide gene defined by the Drosophila memory mutant amnesiac. Science. 1995;268: 869 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dubnau J, Chiang A, Grady L, Barditch J, Gossweiler S, McNeil J, et al. The staufen/pumilio pathway is involved in Drosophila long-term memory. Current Biology. 2003;13: 286–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Winbush A, Reed D, Chang PL, Nuzhdin SV, Lyons LC, Arbeitman MN. Identification of gene expression changes associated with long-term memory of courtship rejection in Drosophila males. G3 (Bethesda). 2012;2: 1437–1445. doi: 10.1534/g3.112.004119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Grotewiel MS, Beck CD, Wu KH, Zhu X, Davis RL. Integrin-mediated short-term memory in Drosophila. Nature. 1998;391: 455–460. doi: 10.1038/35079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Walkinshaw E, Gai Y, Farkas C, Richter D, Nicholas E, Keleman K, et al. Identification of genes that promote or inhibit olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Genetics. 2015;199: 1173–1182. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.173575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Levy F, Rabel D, Charlet M, Bulet P, Hoffmann JA, Ehret-Sabatier L. Peptidomic and proteomic analyses of the systemic immune response of Drosophila. Biochimie. 2004;86: 607–616. doi: 10.1016/j.biochi.2004.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lai EC, Bodner R, Posakony JW. The enhancer of split complex of Drosophila includes four Notch-regulated members of the bearded gene family. Development. 2000;127: 3441–3455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Copf T, Goguel V, Lampin-Saint-Amaux A, Scaplehorn N, Preat T. Cytokine signaling through the JAK/STAT pathway is required for long-term memory in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108: 8059–8064. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012919108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Corty MM, Freeman MR. Cell biology in neuroscience: Architects in neural circuit design: glia control neuron numbers and connectivity. J Cell Biol. 2013;203: 395–405. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201306099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.DeSalvo MK, Hindle SJ, Rusan ZM, Orng S, Eddison M, Halliwill K, et al. The Drosophila surface glia transcriptome: evolutionary conserved blood-brain barrier processes. Front Neurosci. 2014;8: 346 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Frischknecht R, Fejtova A, Viesti M, Stephan A, Sonderegger P. Activity-induced synaptic capture and exocytosis of the neuronal serine protease neurotrypsin. J Neurosci. 2008;28: 1568–1579. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3398-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Almonte AG, Sweatt JD. Serine proteases, serine protease inhibitors, and protease-activated receptors: roles in synaptic function and behavior. Brain Res. 2011;1407: 107–122. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Minatohara K, Akiyoshi M, Okuno H. Role of immediate-early genes in synaptic plasticity and neuronal ensembles underlying the memory trace. Frontiers in molecular neuroscience. 2015;8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kurz CL, Charroux B, Chaduli D, Viallat-Lieutaud A, Royet J. Peptidoglycan sensing by octopaminergic neurons modulates Drosophila oviposition. Elife. 2017;6: doi: 10.7554/eLife.21937 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cao Y, Chtarbanova S, Petersen AJ, Ganetzky B. Dnr1 mutations cause neurodegeneration in Drosophila by activating the innate immune response in the brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110: E1752–60. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306220110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zhan L, Xie Q, Tibbetts RS. Opposing roles of p38 and JNK in a Drosophila model of TDP-43 proteinopathy reveal oxidative stress and innate immunity as pathogenic components of neurodegeneration. Hum Mol Genet. 2015;24: 757–772. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu493 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lucin KM, Wyss-Coray T. Immune activation in brain aging and neurodegeneration: too much or too little? Neuron. 2009;64: 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Mattson MP, Camandola S. NF-kappaB in neuronal plasticity and neurodegenerative disorders. J Clin Invest. 2001;107: 247–254. doi: 10.1172/JCI11916 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Zhang Y, Hu W. NFκB signaling regulates embryonic and adult neurogenesis. Frontiers in biology. 2012;7: 277–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ayyar S, Pistillo D, Calleja M, Brookfield A, Gittins K, Goldstone C, et al. NF-κB/Rel-mediated regulation of the neural fate in Drosophila. PLoS One. 2007;2: e1178 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Boersma MC, Meffert MK. Novel Roles for the NF-kappa B Signaling Pathway in Regulating Neuronal Function. Science Signaling. 2008;1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Dissel S, Angadi V, Kirszenblat L, Suzuki Y, Donlea J, Klose M, et al. Sleep restores behavioral plasticity to Drosophila mutants. Current Biology. 2015;25: 1270–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Williams JA, Sathyanarayanan S, Hendricks JC, Sehgal A. Interaction between sleep and the immune response in Drosophila: a role for the NFkappaB relish. Sleep. 2007;30: 389–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dissel S, Seugnet L, Thimgan MS, Silverman N, Angadi V, Thacher PV, et al. Differential activation of immune factors in neurons and glia contribute to individual differences in resilience/vulnerability to sleep disruption. Brain Behav Immun. 2015;47: 75–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.09.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Albensi BC, Mattson MP. Evidence for the involvement of TNF and NF-kB in hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Synapse-New York. 2000;35: 151–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Heckscher ES, Fetter RD, Marek KW, Albin SD, Davis GW. NF-κB, IκB, and IRAK control glutamate receptor density at the Drosophila NMJ. Neuron. 2007;55: 859–873. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Mery F, Kawecki TJ. A cost of long-term memory in Drosophila. Science. 2005;308: 1148 308/5725/1148 [pii]. doi: 10.1126/science.1111331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Placais PY, Preat T. To favor survival under food shortage, the brain disables costly memory. Science. 2013;339: 440–442. doi: 10.1126/science.1226018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Nader K, Schafe GE, Le Doux JE. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature. 2000;406: 722–726. doi: 10.1038/35021052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kitamura T, Saitoh Y, Takashima N, Murayama A, Niibori Y, Ageta H, et al. Adult neurogenesis modulates the hippocampus-dependent period of associative fear memory. Cell. 2009;139: 814–827. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.10.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Sekeres M. Memory consolidation or transformation: context manipulation and hippocampal representations of memory. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10: 555–557. doi: 10.1038/nn1880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Osterwalder T, Yoon KS, White BH, Keshishian H. A conditional tissue-specific transgene expression system using inducible GAL4. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98: 12596–12601. doi: 10.1073/pnas.221303298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Roman G, Endo K, Zong L, Davis RL. P[Switch], a system for spatial and temporal control of gene expression in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98: 12602–12607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.221303998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Bolger AM, Lohse M, Usadel B. Trimmomatic: a flexible trimmer for Illumina sequence data. Bioinformatics. 2014;30: 2114–2120. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.dos Santos G, Schroeder AJ, Goodman JL, Strelets VB, Crosby MA, Thurmond J, et al. FlyBase: introduction of the Drosophila melanogaster Release 6 reference genome assembly and large-scale migration of genome annotations. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43: D690–7. doi: 10.1093/nar/gku1099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Li H, Handsaker B, Wysoker A, Fennell T, Ruan J, Homer N, et al. The Sequence Alignment/Map format and SAMtools. Bioinformatics. 2009;25: 2078–2079. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Quinlan AR, Hall IM. BEDTools: a flexible suite of utilities for comparing genomic features. Bioinformatics. 2010;26: 841–842. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Robinson MD, McCarthy DJ, Smyth GK. edgeR: a Bioconductor package for differential expression analysis of digital gene expression data. Bioinformatics. 2010;26: 139–140. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.McCarthy DJ, Chen Y, Smyth GK. Differential expression analysis of multifactor RNA-Seq experiments with respect to biological variation. Nucleic Acids Res. 2012;40: 4288–4297. doi: 10.1093/nar/gks042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Huang da W, Sherman BT, Lempicki RA. Bioinformatics enrichment tools: paths toward the comprehensive functional analysis of large gene lists. Nucleic Acids Res. 2009;37: 1–13. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkn923 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Huang DW, Sherman BT, Lempicki RA. Systematic and integrative analysis of large gene lists using DAVID bioinformatics resources. Nature protocols. 2009;4: 44–57. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2008.211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Lemon J. Plotrix: a package in the red light district of R.R-News 6: 8–12. 2006.
- 75.Nepusz G, Csárdi G. The igraph software package for complex network research. Complex Systems. 2006;1695. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Wong AK, Krishnan A, Yao V, Tadych A, Troyanskaya OG. IMP 2.0: a multi-species functional genomics portal for integration, visualization and prediction of protein functions and networks. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43: W128–33. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data is contained with in the paper and supporting information files, with the exception of the raw sequencing files, which are on NCBI Sequencing Read Archive, with the corresponding BioProject ID PRJNA414223.