Abstract
Chemokines are small secreted signaling proteins produced by a broad range of cells, including immune cells. Several studies have recently suggested potential roles of chemokines and their receptors in the pathophysiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). SAM3 is a novel brain-specific chemokine-like molecule with an unknown physiological function. We explored the relevance of chemokines in the development of ASD in mice, with a focus on SAM3. We generated Sam3 gene knockout (KO) mice and characterized their behavioral phenotypes, with a focus on those relevant to ASD. Sam3-deficient mice displayed all three core phenotypes of ASD: impaired responses to social novelty, defects in social communication, and increased repetitive behavior. In addition, they showed increased anxiety. Interestingly, gender differences were identified for several behaviors: only male Sam3 KO mice exhibited increased anxiety and increased repetitive behaviors. Sam3 KO mice did not exhibit changes in other behaviors, including locomotor activities, fear learning and memory, and object recognition memory. These findings indicate that a deficiency of SAM3, a novel brain-specific chemokine-like molecule, may lead to the pathogenesis of ASDs and suggest the possibility that SAM3, a soluble factor, could be a novel therapeutic target for ASD treatment.
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are a neurodevelopmental disorder that are mainly characterized by symptoms in social interaction, social communication and repetitive behaviors1. In 2013, in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-V, a new guideline for the diagnosis of ASD was defined that integrates previous autistic disorders, such as Asperger’s disorder, childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD), and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) into ASD due to their related signs and symptoms in patients1. Moreover, according to the DSM-V, social communication disorder (SCD) was included as a new separate category for patients with disabilities in social communication without the presence of repetitive behaviors1–3. Thus, it is critical to identify an appropriate animal model that meets the criteria reflecting the key aspects of human symptoms.
Genetic factors are the most commonly proposed etiology of ASD4–6. Despite the numerous studies of gene variations in ASD, no single gene has been identified to confer a risk of autism, which indicates that genetic background may contribute only to the vulnerability of developing brains. Thus, exposing the sensitized brain to environmental insults, such as infection or inflammation during or after pregnancy, may contribute to the development of ASD7. Immune dysregulation has recently been reported among children with ASD and their mothers, which suggests potential roles of the immune system in the pathophysiology of the disorder8. However, the mechanisms responsible for immune dysfunction and how it is related to ASD development are not understood.
Chemokines, a family of small cytokines, are proteins that regulate the processes of the immune system9,10. Several chemokines have recently been implicated in the pathophysiology of ASD and have been suggested as potential biomarkers for ASD11,12. In human studies, the plasma levels of macrophage inflammatory protein 1 (MIP-1) alpha, MIP-1 beta and IP-10 have been reported to be highly associated with social behaviors in autistic patients12,13. In animal studies, genetic studies using KO mice have also suggested potential roles of chemokines in ASD. For example, mice deficient in CC chemokine receptor 6 (CCR6) displayed higher locomotion, lower anxiety and a reduced preference for social novelty14. These studies suggest that chemokines and their receptors may modulate cognition and behaviors related to ASD symptoms.
In this study, our research group first isolated novel functional genes in zebrafish using insertional mutagenesis. Eight members of the zebrafish chemokine-like gene family were identified, which we named the Samdori (Sam) family. We determined that their sequences are highly conserved with respect to the 5 sequences referred to as the FAM19A (TAFA) family sequences in the mouse and human, which have unknown functions. Importantly, these genes are exclusively expressed in the central nervous system with distinct expression patterns in the mouse15. Among these genes in the mouse, one gene, Sam3, is predominantly expressed in specific brain regions, including the hippocampus, the medial habenular nucleus, the posterior part of the thalamus, and the pars tuberalis16. Sam3 is distantly related to MIP-1 alpha in sequence, a key inflammatory molecule considered an ASD risk factor15,16. Furthermore, a genome-wide analysis of the copy number variation (CNV) identified a SAM3 gene deletion in an autism patient17,18, which raised the possibility that a lack of SAM3 may play a role in ASD development. To validate the human deletion study and elucidate the role of SAM3 in ASD pathophysiology, we generated Sam3 KO mice using the transcription activator-like effector nucleases (TALEN) method. To determine whether SAM3 is involved in ASDs, we performed several behavior assays related to the symptoms of ASDs. Sam3 KO mice exhibited all three core symptoms that constitute the criteria for ASD diagnosis, which suggests that SAM3 plays a preventive role in ASD development.
Results
Generation of Sam3 knockout (KO) mice
A TALEN-mediated KO for the SAM3 gene was produced by targeting exon 4, which contains the initiation codon ATG. A 43-bp deletion from this genomic region of SAM3 was confirmed by sequencing, which resulted in a frameshift mutation with a premature stop codon (Fig. 1A). We confirmed the deletion in genomic DNA via PCR using the primer pair as described in the Materials and Methods: wild-type (323 bp), heterozygous (323 bp, 280 bp) and homozygous KO mice (280 bp) (Fig. 1B). The mRNA expression was subsequently assessed via RT-PCR of whole brain lysates obtained from the mice. Two primer pairs were used for RT-PCR as described in the Materials and Methods. One pair was designed to assess the truncated mRNA, which amplified 209 bp for the wild-type and 166 bp for the mutant. The heterozygous mutant showed both bands (Fig. 1C). The other pairs were designed to confirm the sequence deletion in exon 4; thus, the forward primer was located in the deletion site. No amplified mRNA was detected in both the whole brain lysates without the hippocampus and the hippocampus only lysates of the Sam3 KO mice (Sam3 −/−), thus confirming the genomic deletion in exon 4 (Fig. 1D). Furthermore, reduced amplification of the transcripts was identified in the heterozygous Sam3 +/− mice (Fig. 1D). We also validated the genomic deletion in female mouse brain (Supplementary Fig. S2). These results confirmed the 43-bp deletion of Sam3 in both genomic DNA and mRNA. In addition, we tested compensatory effects of other Sam family in Sam3 KO mice. To test that, we performed a quantitative RT-PCR (qRT-PCR). The results showed that there are no compensatory effects of other Sam family in Sam3 KO mice at the transcript level (Supplementary Fig. S3).
No gross defect in Sam3−/− mice
Sam3 homozygous pups were born at approximately 25%, the expected Mendelian frequency, and showed no visible morphological defects. Prior to various behavior experiments, we initially measured the body weight of the mice at 8 weeks. There was no significant difference in the body weight among the three genotypes, Sam3 +/+, Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/−, in males (Fig. 2A; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,40) = 0.9056, p = 0.4124) and females (Fig. 2B; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,46) = 2.456, p = 0.0969).
Increased anxiety and normal locomotion: open field test and elevated plus maze
We initially used the open field test to examine the locomotor activity of the mutants. No significant difference in the locomotor activity was identified between the mutants and the wild types in males (Fig. 2C; two-way ANOVA, F(2,245) = 1.62, p = 0.2087). We did not observe any difference in the locomotor activity in the open field assay among the males of the three genotypes (Sam3 +/+, Sam3 −/−, and Sam3 +/−). In females, however, Sam3 +/− showed slightly increased locomotor activity limited to the 10–15 min period of the 30 min test compared to the other two genotypes, Sam3 +/+ and Sam3 −/− (Fig. 2D; two-way ANOVA, F(2,240) = 3.26, *p = 0.0470). Further studies may be required to clarify the biological significance of this finding. We did not identify this discrepancy between Sam3 +/− male mice and Sam3 −/− male mice. We subsequently used the elevated plus maze to measure the anxiety-like behavior of the mutant mice. The male Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− mice spent significantly less time in the open arms than the wild-type mice (Fig. 2E; one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,46) = 4.076, *p = 0.0235), which is also depicted in the representative trace of male mice in the maze (Fig. 2G,H). In contrast, there was no significant difference in the percentage of time that female mice spent in the open arms (Fig. 2F; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,49) = 0.8294, p = 0.4424) across all genotypes, which indicates a sex difference in this anxiety phenotype. In summary, both male and female Sam3 −/− mice exhibited normal locomotion, whereas only male KO mice exhibited increased anxiety.
Normal learning and memory: fear conditioning
To characterize the capacity for learning and memory, we assessed fear conditioning followed by contextual or auditory recall. During the training day, the male mice showed similar learning curves in the tone and shock paired conditioning test (Fig. 3A; two-way ANOVA, F(2,126) = 0.16, p = 0.8563). On the retrieval day, the context- or cue-dependent fear memory was tested. There was no significant difference in the freezing response level to the conditioned context or the cue, a tone. All mice elicited normal high-level freezing responses similar to the wild-type mice (Fig. 3B; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,43) = 0.8970, p = 0.4153, 3 C; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,41) = 1.654, p = 0.2038). Moreover, the female mutants showed similar levels of freezing during fear conditioning (Fig. 3D; two-way ANOVA, F(2,117) = 0.54, p = 0.5872), contextual retrieval (Fig. 3E; one-way ANOVA, F(2,39) = 1.070, p = 0.3528) and auditory retrieval tests (Fig. 3F; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,39) = 1.634, p = 0.2083). In summary, both male and female Sam3-deficient mice exhibited normal contextual and auditory fear learning and memory.
Normal sociability and impaired social novelty behavior: three-chamber test
Impaired social behavior is one of the major characteristics of autism patients. We used the 3-chamber test to explore the social behavior of Sam3-deficient mice. In the sociability test, all three groups of male and female mice spent significantly more time in the chamber with a stranger mouse than with a nonsocial novel object, which indicates normal sociability (Fig. 4A; male Sam3 +/+, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,30) = 64.75, ***p = 0.0004; male Sam3 +/−, F(2,51) = 112.8, **p = 0.0067; male Sam3 −/−, F(2,48) = 64.80, ***p = 0.0002; 4 C, female Sam3 +/+, F(2,48) = 55.33, *p = 0.02601; female Sam3 +/−, F(2,51) = 27.89, **p = 0.0023; female Sam3 −/−, F(2,36) = 84.07, **p = 0.0048). However, in the social novelty test, the male Sam3 −/− mice showed no preference for stranger 2 over stranger 1, whereas the Sam3 +/+ and Sam3 +/− male mice spent more time in the novel mouse chamber (stranger 2) than in the familiar mouse chamber (stranger 1) (Fig. 4B; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, Sam3 +/+, F(2,30) = 23.59, *p = 0.0135; Sam3 +/−, F(2,53) = 46.64, *p = 0.0263; Sam3 −/−, F(2,48) = 28.03, p > 0.9999). The female Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− mice did not show a preference for stranger 2 over stranger 1, whereas the Sam3 +/+ females spent more time in the chamber with stranger 2 than in the chamber with stranger 1 (Fig. 4D; one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, Sam3 +/+, F(2,48) = 63.10, ***p = 0.0003; Sam3 +/−, F(2,51) = 27.36, p = 0.7208; Sam3 −/−, F(2,36) = 22.19, p > 0.9999). In summary, both male and female Sam3-deficient mice exhibited impaired social novelty preference or social memory.
Normal novelty recognition and memory: novel object recognition test
To ensure that the social novelty deficit was not a result of a lack of novelty recognition, we tested the novel object recognition assay. After 10 min of the familiarization phase, all groups of male mice showed a significant preference for the novel object in the test (Fig. 4E; two-tailed t-test, Sam3 +/+, **p = 0.0076; Sam3 +/−, *p = 0.0150; Sam3 −/−, **p = 0.0017). Similar to males, all groups of female mice showed normal recognition memory (Fig. 4F; two-tailed t-test, Sam3 +/+, ***p = 0.0001; Sam3 +/−, **p = 0.0016; Sam3 −/−, *p = 0.0284). These results suggest that the impairment of the mutants in the social novelty test was not a result of a lack of novelty recognition.
Normal empathy: observational fear learning
Individuals with autism often display deficits in empathy and social skills19. Observational fear learning (OFL) behavior is a rodent model of emphatic responses. To examine the empathic fear response of Sam3 KO mice, we measured the freezing level of the observer mice, while the demonstrator mice received repetitive foot shocks. There was no significant difference among the Sam3 +/+, Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− male mice in the OFL conditioning (Fig. 4G; two-way ANOVA, F(2,248) = 0.71, p = 0.4990). In the retrieval test, there was no significant difference among the genotypes (Fig. 4H; one-way ANOVA, F(2,33) = 1.162, p = 0.3253). Similarly, the Sam3 +/+, Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− female mice did not exhibit a significant difference in the OFL conditioning (Fig. 4I; two-way ANOVA, F(2,232) = 0.65, p = 0.5293) or retrieval (Fig. 4J; one-way ANOVA, F(2,29) = 0.3073, p = 0.7378). In summary, both male and female mice showed normal empathy.
Impaired social communication: scent marking
Mice communicate via olfactory20–22 and acoustic signals23,24. Based on several reports regarding the low level of vocalization in C57BL/6N25,26 mice, the genetic background of the mutation, we focused on scent marking behavior in Sam3 KO mice instead of ultrasonic vocalization. A female urine-elicited scent marking is a type of chemical communication in social groups21,22. Because we utilized female urine as the scent, we tested only male mice for this specific behavior. During the 5-min test session, the total number of scent marks was counted. As shown in Fig. 5A, the male Sam3 −/− mice made fewer scent marks than the Sam3 +/+ and Sam3 +/− mice in the open field box (one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,30) = 3.846, *p = 0.0326). This result indicated that Sam3 −/− mice showed reduced social communication. The number of male urine marks 10 cm around the female urine tended to decrease in both the Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− mice; however, the data were not statistically significant (Fig. 5B; one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,30) = 2.017, p = 0.1507). Overall, the total number of scent marks was significantly reduced in the Sam3 −/− male mice.
Increased repetitive behaviors: marble burying test and self-grooming
To investigate whether mutant mice show repetitive behaviors, we analyzed the performance of mice in the marble burying test and self-grooming test. In the marble burying test, the male Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− mice buried significantly more marbles than the Sam3 +/+ mice (Fig. 6A; one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,44) = 4.621, *p = 0.0151). For the females, there was no significant difference in this behavior among the three groups of mice (Fig. 6B; one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,49) = 0.5950, p = 0.5555).
Repetitive behavior was subsequently evaluated by scoring the time the mice spent grooming during 10 min after a habituation session. Consistent with the marble burying result, the Sam3 mutant mice exhibited unusually high spontaneous repetitive grooming behavior. In the self-grooming test, the male Sam3 +/− and Sam3 −/− mice spent a significantly longer time grooming than the Sam3 +/+ mice (Fig. 6C; one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,30) = 3.846, *p = 0.0523). For females, the Sam3 −/− mice also displayed a longer duration of self-grooming than the Sam3 +/− and Sam3 +/+ mice (Fig. 6D; one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,28) = 4.318, *p = 0.0232). Although only the male Sam3 KO showed a significant increase in marble burying as previously described, both the male and female Sam3 KO mice showed increased levels of self-grooming behavior compared to the wild-type mice.
Discussion
In this study, we first generated Sam3 KO mice and assessed behavioral phenotypes that are relevant to the symptoms of human ASDs. Our data clearly showed that Sam3-deficient mice exhibited behavioral impairments in social interactions and social communication and increased repetitive behaviors, which are the core symptoms required for an ASD diagnosis in humans.
In our study, Sam3 KO mice showed increased anxiety as well as impairments in the three core domains of ASD (Fig. 2E). Autistic children exhibit various symptoms in addition to the three core symptoms27–29. Gene-based association studies have identified many genes associated with ASD. However, there are very few genetic mouse models that meet the criteria for the three core symptoms of autism. Three representative mouse models have been reported to date that meet the criteria: Cntnap2 KO mice, ProSAP1/shank2 KO mice and Shank3A KO mice30,31,32. Although these three genetic mouse models show reduced social interaction, reduced social communication and increased repetitive behaviors, they also present unique impairments in other behaviors. For example, Cntnap2 KO mice exhibit hyperactivity and epileptic seizures31,33. ProSAP1/shank2 KO mice show hyperactivity and impaired spatial learning and memory30,34. Shank3A KO mice show impaired learning and memory as well as motor coordination32,35. Shank3 KO mice with a non-sense mutation at arginine 1117 (R1117X) also show impaired prepulse inhibition36. Thus, the development of additional mouse models with these various symptoms is necessary to understand individual dysfunctions.
Interestingly, Sam3 KO did not affect sociability; however, it selectively impaired social novelty preference. Sociability is one of the most direct phenotypes used to measure social interaction; however, accumulating evidence from human patients suggests that an impaired social novelty preference is also a critical parameter of ASD. As previously reported, some individuals with ASD are unable to differentiate between familiar and unfamiliar acquaintances because of their difficulty in processing facial configurations37,38. fMRI studies have indicated that individuals with ASD showed a lack of fusiform gyrus activation when viewing unfamiliar faces. The fusiform gyrus is involved in discriminating between familiar and unfamiliar faces. Another study showed that children with ASD had significantly higher levels of cortisol, the primary stress hormone, in response to novel social interactions than typical peer control children, which resulted in an inappropriate approach or reaction to unfamiliar peers39. Difficulties in facial discrimination or feelings of stress during novel or unfamiliar social situations affect the social interactions of ASD patients. There are several mouse models that show a lack of social novelty preference but not sociability: integrin β3 KO mice40, Shank 3B KO mice35, ProSAP1/Shank2 KO mice34 and mice with oxytocin receptor (OXTR) knockdown in the lateral septum41. However, whether Sam3 and these related genes share common molecular pathways or brain circuits regarding the mechanism of selectively impaired social novelty preference but not impaired sociability remain unclear. We confirmed that the impairment in the social novelty test is not a result of a lack of novel object recognition memory (Fig. 4B,D).
In humans, the prevalence of ASD is 4-fold higher in boys than in girls, which supports the existence of gender differences in ASD development42,43,44. More interestingly, several previous studies have suggested that boys with ASD present more repetitive and restricted behavior than girls with ASD45,46. The marble burying test and the self-grooming test are well-established models used to measure repetitive behaviors in rodents. In our study, we identified impairments in both behaviors only in Sam3 KO male mice (Fig. 6A,C), whereas female Sam3 KO mice exhibited normal burying behavior in the marble burying test (Fig. 6B). The low number of buried marbles of WT males was a result of the type of bedding, which included a thick and heavy type of bedding selected to maximize the difference between WT and MT mice (Fig. 6A). Initially, with light bedding, WT male mice buried an average of 15 marbles (data not shown). Thus, it was difficult to observe the difference from the male MT mice. In this condition, we identified increased repetitive behaviors only in male KO mice. Another issue that occurred was that female wild-type mice (5.55 ± 1.014) (Fig. 6B) buried more marbles than male wild-type mice (2.267±0.4079) (Fig. 6A). This difference has also been reported in other studies47,48; however, the gender difference in repetitive behaviors remains unclear. For self-grooming, although the female KO group showed increased self-grooming, the female heterozygous (Het) group did not exhibit increased self-grooming (Fig. 6D). Considering the haploinsufficiency of risk genes related to autism in patients, the significant increase in self-grooming only in the male Het group suggests the possibility that Sam3 gene dysfunction may be associated with gender differences in autism patients. In addition to the effects of gender on repetitive behaviors, we also identified increased anxiety only in male Sam3 HET and KO mice (Fig. 2E). Recent studies have demonstrated that anxious children with ASD exhibited more repetitive behaviors than non-anxious children7,48,49. Given that SAM3 expression is high in the medial habenular nucleus (MHN)15, which is a thoroughly investigated brain region in anxiety disorders, it is possible that the Sam3 gene deletion in the MHN may be associated with the increased anxiety. Regardless of the assumption that the Sam3 deletion in the MHN is critical for an anxiogenic phenotype, it remains unclear how the sex difference in anxiety occurs and how anxiety and repetitive behaviors are associated in Sam3 KO male mice. Although research on sex-bias in ASD appears to be increasing, there is limited knowledge regarding the underlying mechanisms. Thus, understanding the underlying mechanisms of Sam3 deficiency may provide clues to answer questions regarding sex differences in ASD as well as ASD pathophysiology.
Chemokines, which are immune-related proteins, have been implicated in ASDs11,51,52. In humans, several types of plasma chemokines, including MIP-1 alpha, have been reported to be significantly lower in ASD children than their siblings without ASD13,53. A sequence homology study indicated that the SAM3 protein is distantly related to MIP-1 alpha, the CC chemokine family15,16. A recent study has suggested that Sam3 may also play a role in middle cerebral artery occlusion (MCAO)-mediated immune responses by maintaining M2 polarization of microglia54. M2 polarized microglia express cytokines and receptors that inhibit inflammation and subsequently exert beneficial roles in homeostasis and cell survival. Therefore, one potential explanation for the behavioral abnormalities identified in Sam3 KO mice is that a lack of Sam3 release may lead to the loss of the homeostatic control of inflammation, which may ultimately result in the abnormal brain development or function present in ASD patients51. Another potential underlying mechanism is that Sam3 may directly act on neurons. As previously discussed, a deletion of Cntnap2, Shank2 or Shank3, respectively, has been reported to induce three core phenotypes of ASD. Whether these molecules and SAM3 are directly associated is unknown; however, a better understanding of the general roles of each gene in the brain is required. Considering the role of Cntnap2 in the neuronal migration of cortical projection neurons, alterations in connectivity across diverse brain regions may be important for ASD development, and its specific expression in the brain may be directly associated with extra phenotypes, such as hyperactivity and defects in spatial learning and memory. Shank2 and Shank3 are multidomain scaffolding proteins that anchor NMDA, AMPA and metabotropic glutamate receptors in the postsynaptic membrane, assemble signaling molecules and G-protein coupled receptors, and regulate calcium homeostasis and synaptic plasticity30,32. Defects in synaptic transmission and synaptic plasticity are well-accepted molecular and cellular mechanisms for ASD pathophysiology. Importantly, accumulating evidence has indicated that chemokines couple with a subset of glutamate receptors and modulate synaptic activity27,42. Future studies are required to ascertain the specific cell types that express SAM3 and identify its specific receptors to elucidate the underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms of Sam3 deficiency-mediated ASD phenotypes.
In conclusion, we report experimental results that show a deficiency in the brain-specific chemokine Sam3 results in the development of ASDs in mice and suggest that Sam3, a soluble factor with a preventive function in ASD pathogenesis, may comprise a target for ASD treatment.
Materials and Methods
Animals
The C57BL6/NTac strain was used for all experiments. Mice were provided with free access to food and water under a 12 hr light/dark cycle with the light cycle beginning at 8:00 am. The mice were 8–17 weeks of age at the time of behavioral testing, which was performed during the light phase. All animal experiments were performed in accordance with a protocol approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST -site of the experiment) and the Institute for Basic Science (IBS), Korea. All methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Generation of targeted mouse Sam3 KO mutant and genotyping
The TALEN vectors that targeted exon 4 of Sam3 were designed and constructed by ToolGen (Seoul, Korea). The synthesis and microinjection of TALEN mRNAs into the cytoplasm of fertilized eggs obtained from C57BL/6NTac breeding females were performed as previously described55. TALEN-mediated Sam3 F0 mice were screened via T7E1 assay as previously described56. For the assay, genomic DNA was prepared from the tail and amplified using TALEN target site primers. The primers used for genotyping included mouse SAM3 forward (5′-GCATAGAGAAGGGGCTGA-3′) and mouse SAM3 reverse (5′- GAGGAGTCACATCTGCAG-3′). The founder line of a Sam3 heterozygous mouse (F0) was crossed to and maintained in the C57BL/6NTac background. Heterozygous breeding pairs were used to generate Sam3 homozygous mice (Sam3 −/−) and wild-type littermates.
RNA preparation and reverse transcription PCR (RT-PCR)
Total RNAs were extracted from whole brain tissues of adult Sam3 −/−, Sam3 +/−, and wild-type littermate mice using GeneAll Hybrid-R (GeneAll Biotechnology, Korea). cDNA was synthesized following the manufacturer’s protocols (SuperScript IV VILO Master Mix, Invitrogen). Two sets of primers were used: a forward primer (5′-ATG GAG AGG CCC ACC AG-3′) and a reverse primer (5′-CAG GAG CAT TTG ACC GTC TG-3′) to determine the truncated mRNA and a forward primer (5′-TGG CTG TGG ACG TGT CCG-3′) and a reverse primer (5′-TTA CCG TGT GAC CTT GGT G-3′) to confirm the absence of the deletion site. The glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase (GAPDH) gene was used as an internal control as previously described57.
Behavioral tests
Prior to the behavioral tests, all mice were placed in the behavior room for 1 hr for room habituation with white noise (65 dB). Every behavior test was performed in a sound-proof chamber with white noise (65 dB) and dim light (10 lux), except for the elevated plus maze (5 lux). Between each test session, the chambers and objects were cleaned with 70% ethanol. After the test, all mice were returned to their home cages.
Open field test (OFT)
The OFT was conducted as previously reported58. Briefly, each mouse was placed in a white acryl chamber (40 × 40 × 40 cm) for 30 mins to measure its exploratory activity. At the beginning of the test, the mouse was placed in the corner of the arena. Spontaneous movement was video recorded and automatically analyzed with EthoVision XT software, version 9 (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands).
Elevated plus maze (EPM) test
The EPM was performed as previously described58, with minor modifications. Briefly, each mouse was placed in the elevated plus maze and allowed to freely move within the maze for 5 mins. The mouse was first placed in the center (8 × 8 cm) facing the open arms. The light intensity in the chamber was 5 lux. The amount of time spent in each arm was video recorded and automatically analyzed using EthoVision XT software. The time spent in the open arms was reported as the percentage of time spent in the open arms (time in open arms/total time (open arm + closed arm) × 100).
Fear conditioning, contextual and auditory recall
Conventional Pavlovian fear conditioning was performed as previously described58, with minor modifications. One day before the conditioning day, each mouse was habituated in a cued recall box for 20 min. On the conditioning day, the mouse was placed in the conditioning chamber (Coulbourn Instruments). After 3 min of exploration, a 30-sec (86 dB, 3000 Hz) auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) was delivered. In the last 2 sec of the CS, an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US, 1 sec foot shock at 0.7 mA) was delivered. For the conditioning, the mice underwent four CS-US pairs separated by 150-sec intervals. Twenty-four hours after training, the contextual fear memory was tested in the same chamber for 5 min in the absence of the auditory stimulus and shock. After 3 hrs, the auditory fear memory was tested in a cued recall box, which differed from the conditioning chamber. After 10 min of exploration time, the 30-sec auditory CS was delivered. The freezing behavior of the mouse was recorded and automatically analyzed with FreezeFrame software (Actimetrics) using the significant motion pixels (SMP) algorithm.
Three-chamber test
The three-chamber test was conducted as previously described58, with minor modifications. The subject mouse was habituated to the empty chamber for 10 min before the sociability session. In the sociability session, the subject mouse was allowed to explore three chambers (object, center, and stranger 1) for 10 min. The chamber location for the object and stranger 1 was randomly assigned for each animal. In the social novelty session, the subject mouse was allowed to explore three chambers (stranger 2, center, and stranger 1) for an additional 10 min. In this session, a novel, unfamiliar, stranger 2 mouse was placed in the empty cylinder in the sociability session. The amount of time spent in each chamber was video recorded and automatically analyzed using EthoVision XT software, version 9 (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands).
Novel object recognition test
The novel object recognition test was administered using previously described methods59, with minor modifications. Each mouse was placed in the chamber used in the open field test. Because the mouse had experienced the chamber in previous tests, the habituation session was omitted. In the familiarization session, two objects (small soccer balls, 7 cm in diameter) were placed 5 cm away from the walls in a symmetric position from the center of the chamber. During 10 min, the mouse freely explored two identical objects. Between the familiarization session and test session, there was a 10 min intersession interval (ISI). During the ISI, one object was randomly replaced with a novel object (black pyramid, 8 × 7.5 × 6 cm) in the same location. During the test session, the mouse was allowed to freely explore the two objects (familiar vs novel objects) for 10 min. The object exploration time was automatically measured and analyzed using EthoVision XT software, version 9 (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands).
OFL test
The OFL test was performed as previously described60. Each mouse was placed in a modified passive avoidance cage (Coulbourn Instruments, Whitehall, PA, USA) for OFL conditioning and retrieval. The freezing behavior of the observer mouse was recorded and automatically analyzed with FreezeFrame software (Actimetrics).
Scent marking test
The scent marking test was conducted as previously reported61,62, with minor modifications. Before the test, urine was collected from C57BL6/NTac females on the day of the experiment. Clean paper (Strathmore Drawing Paper Premium, 400 series; Strathmore Artist Papers, Neenah, WI, USA) in the chamber (40 × 40 × 40 cm) was used for the open field test, and 15 µl fresh female urine were pipetted onto the center of the paper. In the test session, a male mouse freely explored the chamber with the female urine. After the 5-min test, the mouse was returned to its home cage. The marked sheets of Strathmore paper were treated with ninhydrin spray (LC-NIN-16; TritechForensics Inc., Southport, NC, USA) and dried for 24 hrs, which allowed visualization of the urine marks as purple spots. To analyze the urine marks, the number of scent marks was measured by placing a transparent grid (each 1 × 1 cm) over the dried substrate paper. The total number of grids that contained scent marks was counted for each genotype. The number of scent marks within an area of 10 × 10 cm around the female urine spot was also counted. This analysis was confirmed by a second experimenter.
Marble burying test
The test was performed as previously described63. Each mouse was placed in the center of a cage (20 × 30 × 15 cm) with 3-cm deep clean bedding. Twenty navy glass marbles (14 mm diameter) were gently arranged in an equidistant 4 × 5 array on top of the bedding. Following the 30-min testing session, the marbles covered with bedding more than two-thirds deep were counted as buried marbles. This analysis was confirmed by a second experimenter.
Self-grooming test
The self-grooming test was performed as described64,65. Briefly, an individual mouse was placed in a clean, transparent cage (20 × 30 × 15 cm) with no bedding. Each mouse was allowed a 10-min habituation session, and self-grooming was subsequently measured for an additional 10 min to assess the cumulative time spent grooming all body regions. The experimenter sat at a distance approximately 2 m from the test cage and recorded the cumulative time spent spontaneously grooming using a stopwatch program. The test was video recorded using EthoVision XT software, version 9 (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands), and the analysis was confirmed by a second experimenter.
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed using GraphPad Prism 7.03 (GraphPad Software Inc., California). We performed one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons post hoc test to evaluate the differences among the three groups. We also performed two-way ANOVA (Group × Time interaction) with Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons post hoc test. All data are presented as the mean ± SEM.
Availability of materials and data
All materials and data in this manuscript are available to Editorial Board Members and referees.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by the Institute for Basic Science (IBS-R001-D1), Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (MSIP) (2017R1A2B2010696).
Author Contributions
S. Kim, B. Lee and H.-S. Shin designed the study. C.-H. Kim and J.-H. Choi generated the Sam3 KO mouse line. S. Kim and B. Lee performed the molecular and behavior experiments. J.-H. Kim contributed molecular studies. S. Kim performed the statistical analyses. S. Kim, B. Lee and H.-S. Shin wrote the manuscript.
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-017-16769-5.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Cheol-Hee Kim, Email: zebrakim@cnu.ac.kr.
Hee-Sup Shin, Email: shin@ibs.re.kr.
References
- 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
- 2.Gibson J, Adams C, Lockton E, Green J. Social communication disorder outside autism? A diagnostic classification approach to delineating pragmatic language impairment, high functioning autism and specific language impairment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2013;54:1186–1197. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.McPartland JC, Reichow B, Volkmar FR. Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;51:368–383. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Frazier TW, et al. A Twin Study of Heritable and Shared Environmental Contributions to Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014;44:2013–2025. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2081-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pinto D, et al. Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2010;466:368–372. doi: 10.1038/nature09146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B. Genetics of austim: complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2001;2:943–955. doi: 10.1038/35103559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mahmood, U. et al. Dendritic spine anomalies and PTEN alterations in a mouse model of VPA-induced autism spectrum disorder. Pharmacol. Res. 10.1016/j.phrs.2017.08.006 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 8.Malkova NV, Yu CZ, Hsiao EY, Moore MJ, Patterson PH. Maternal immune activation yields offspring displaying mouse versions of the three core symptoms of autism. Brain. Behav. Immun. 2012;26:607–616. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cho C, Miller RJ. Chemokine Receptors and Neural Function. J. Neurovirol. 2002;8:573–584. doi: 10.1080/13550280290101003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rossi D, Zlotnik A. The biology of chemokines and their receptors. Annu. Rev. Immunol. 2000;18:217–242. doi: 10.1146/annurev.immunol.18.1.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Abdallah MW, et al. Neonatal chemokine levels and risk of autism spectrum disorders: Findings from a Danish historic birth cohort follow-up study. Cytokine. 2013;61:370–376. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2012.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ashwood P, et al. Elevated plasma cytokines in autism spectrum disorders provide evidence of immune dysfunction and are associated with impaired behavioral outcome. Brain. Behav. Immun. 2011;25:40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ashwood P, et al. Associations of impaired behaviors with elevated plasma chemokines in autism spectrum disorders. J. Neuroimmunol. 2011;232:196–199. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2010.10.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jaehne EJ, Baune BT. Effects of chemokine receptor signalling on cognition-like, emotion-like and sociability behaviours of CCR6 and CCR7 knockout mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2014;261:31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Tang YT, et al. TAFA: a novel secreted family with conserved cysteine residues and restricted expression in the brain. Genomics. 2004;83:727–734. doi: 10.1016/j.ygeno.2003.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fischer C, Christ E, Korf H-W, von Gall C. Tafa-3 encoding for a secretory peptide is expressed in the mouse pars tuberalis and is affected by melatonin 1 receptor deficiency. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2012;177:98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Krumm N, et al. Transmission Disequilibrium of Small CNVs in Simplex Autism. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2013;93:595–606. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2013.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Krumm N, et al. Excess of rare, inherited truncating mutations in autism. Nat. Genet. 2015;47:582–588. doi: 10.1038/ng.3303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Di Tore, P. A., Giuseppe, T. D. & Corona, F. Autism Spectrum as an Empathy Disorder. Autism-Open Access07 (2017).
- 20.Wyatt, T. D. Pheromones and Animal Behaviour. Communication by Taste and Smell. Camb. Univ. Press Camb (2003).
- 21.Arakawa H, Blanchard DC, Arakawa K, Dunlap C, Blanchard RJ. Scent marking behavior as an odorant communication in mice. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008;32:1236–1248. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ferkin MH. The response of rodents to scent marks: Four broad hypotheses. Horm. Behav. 2015;68:43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Portfors CV. Types and functions of ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats and mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2007;46:28–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jamain S, et al. Reduced social interaction and ultrasonic communication in a mouse model of monogenic heritable autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2008;105:1710–1715. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711555105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wöhr M, et al. Effects of Genetic Background, Gender, and Early Environmental Factors on Isolation-Induced Ultrasonic Calling in Mouse Pups: An Embryo-Transfer Study. Behav. Genet. 2008;38:579–595. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9221-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wöhr M, Schwarting RKW. Rodent ultrasonic communication and its relevance for models of neuropsychiatric disorders. E-Neuroforum. 2010;1:71–80. [Google Scholar]
- 27.de la Torre-Ubieta L, Won H, Stein JL, Geschwind DH. Advancing the understanding of autism disease mechanisms through genetics. Nat. Med. 2016;22:345–361. doi: 10.1038/nm.4071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Huguet G, Ey E, Bourgeron T. The Genetic Landscapes of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Annu. Rev. Genomics Hum. Genet. 2013;14:191–213. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genom-091212-153431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Won, H., Mah, W. & Kim, E. Autism spectrum disorder causes, mechanisms, and treatments: focus on neuronal synapses. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 6 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30.Won H, et al. Autistic-like social behaviour in Shank2-mutant mice improved by restoring NMDA receptor function. Nature. 2012;486:261–265. doi: 10.1038/nature11208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Peñagarikano O, et al. Absence of CNTNAP2 Leads to Epilepsy, Neuronal Migration Abnormalities, and Core Autism-Related Deficits. Cell. 2011;147:235–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.08.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wang X, et al. Synaptic dysfunction and abnormal behaviors in mice lacking major isoforms of Shank3. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2011;20:3093–3108. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddr212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Peñagarikano O, et al. Exogenous and evoked oxytocin restores social behavior in the Cntnap2 mouse model of autism. Sci. Transl. Med. 2015;7:271ra8–271ra8. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3010257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schmeisser, M. J. et al. Autistic-like behaviours and hyperactivity in mice lacking ProSAP1/Shank2. Nature (2012). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Peça J, et al. Shank3 mutant mice display autistic-like behaviours and striatal dysfunction. Nature. 2011;472:437–442. doi: 10.1038/nature09965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Zhou Y, et al. Mice with Shank3 Mutations Associated with ASD and Schizophrenia Display Both Shared and Distinct Defects. Neuron. 2016;89:147–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dawson G, et al. Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Child Dev. 2002;73:700–717. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Klin A, et al. A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1999;29:499–508. doi: 10.1023/A:1022299920240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Corbett, B. A., Muscatello, R. A. & Blain, S. D. Impact of Sensory Sensitivity on Physiological Stress Response and Novel Peer Interaction in Children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Neurosci. 10 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 40.Carter MD, et al. Absence of preference for social novelty and increased grooming in integrin β3 knockout mice: Initial studies and future directions. Autism Res. 2011;4:57–67. doi: 10.1002/aur.180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tops, M. et al. The role of oxytocin in familiarization-habituation responses to social novelty. Front. Psychol. 4, (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 42.Chahrour M, et al. Current Perspectives in Autism Spectrum Disorder: From Genes to Therapy. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:11402–11410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2335-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Werling DM, Geschwind DH. Understanding sex bias in autism spectrum disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2013;110:4868–4869. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301602110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Dworzynski K, Ronald A, Bolton P, Happé F. How different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold for autism spectrum disorders? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;51:788–797. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Solomon M, Miller M, Taylor SL, Hinshaw SP, Carter CS. Autism Symptoms and Internalizing Psychopathology in Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012;42:48–59. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1215-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child. 1943;2:217–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Wüstefeld L, et al. Selective expression of a constitutively active erythropoietin receptor in GABAergic neurons alters hippocampal network properties without affecting cognition. J. Neurochem. 2016;136:698–705. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Yin J, Chen W, Yang H, Xue M, Schaaf CP. Chrna7 deficient mice manifest no consistent neuropsychiatric and behavioural phenotypes. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:39941. doi: 10.1038/srep39941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Lidstone J, et al. Relations among restricted and repetitive behaviors, anxiety and sensory features in children with autism spectrum disorders. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2014;8:82–92. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.10.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rodgers J, Glod M, Connolly B, McConachie H. The Relationship Between Anxiety and Repetitive Behaviours in Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012;42:2404–2409. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1531-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Vargas DL, Nascimbene C, Krishnan C, Zimmerman AW, Pardo CA. Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann. Neurol. 2005;57:67–81. doi: 10.1002/ana.20315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Shen Y, et al. Altered plasma levels of chemokines in autism and their association with social behaviors. Psychiatry Res. 2016;244:300–305. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.07.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Manzardo AM, Henkhaus R, Dhillon S, Butler MG. Plasma cytokine levels in children with autistic disorder and unrelated siblings. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2012;30:121–127. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Shao Y, Deng T, Zhang T, Li P, Wang Y. FAM19A3, a novel secreted protein, modulates the microglia/macrophage polarization dynamics and ameliorates cerebral ischemia. FEBS Lett. 2015;589:467–475. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2015.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.May M, et al. ZC4H2, an XLID gene, is required for the generation of a specific subset of CNS interneurons. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2015;24:4848–4861. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddv208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Sung YH, et al. Knockout mice created by TALEN. Nat. Biotechnol. 2013;31:23–24. doi: 10.1038/nbt.2477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Lee SE, et al. Rebound burst firing in the reticular thalamus is not essential for pharmacological absence seizures in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2014;111:11828–11833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408609111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Keum S, et al. Variability in empathic fear response among 11 inbred strains of mice: Variability in empathic fear response. Genes Brain Behav. 2016;15:231–242. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Leger M, et al. Object recognition test in mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:2531–2537. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Jeon D, et al. Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:482–488. doi: 10.1038/nn.2504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Arakawa H, Arakawa K, Blanchard D, Blanchard R. Scent marking behavior in male C57BL/6J mice: Sexual and developmental determination. Behav. Brain Res. 2007;182:73–79. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wöhr M, Roullet FI, Crawley JN. Reduced scent marking and ultrasonic vocalizations in the BTBR T+tf/J mouse model of autism. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:35–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00582.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Thomas A, et al. Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2009;204:361–373. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1466-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Yang M, Zhodzishsky V, Crawley JN. Social deficits in BTBR T+tf/J mice are unchanged by cross-fostering with C57BL/6J mothers. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2007;25:515–521. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.McFarlane HG, et al. Autism-like behavioral phenotypes in BTBR T+tf/J mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7:152–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00330.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.