Abstract
Importance
Higher purpose in life is hypothesized to reduce the likelihood of developing weak grip strength and slow walking speed because purpose has been linked with a range of positive health behaviors and biological processes that are potentially protective against declining physical function. However, the association between purpose in life and objective physical function has not been examined.
Objective
To assess whether higher purpose in life among adequately functioning older adults is associated with lower risk of developing weak grip strength and slow walking speed over time.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Data for a longitudinal cohort study were collected in 2006 and again in 2010 from the Health and Retirement Study, a nationally representative study of US adults older than 50 years. Data analysis was conducted from November 23, 2016, to June 2, 2017.
Main Outcomes and Measures
The risk of developing weak grip strength (assessed as a binary yes or no outcome) or slow walking speed (yes or no) during the 4-year follow-up period. Grip strength was assessed using Smedley spring-type hand dynamometers, and walking speed was assessed by asking respondents to walk 2.5 m at their normal walking pace.
Results
In this study, 4486 adults had grip scores at baseline indicating adequate function (2665 women and 1821 men; mean [SD] age, 63.0 [8.2] years) and 1461 adults had walking scores at baseline indicating adequate function (801 women and 660 men; mean [SD] age, 70.8 [6.5] years). After controlling for sociodemographic factors, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with a 13% decreased risk (95% CI, 1%-23%) of developing weak grip strength and a 14% decreased risk (95% CI, 8%-20%) of developing slow walking speed. Associations with walking speed were maintained in all covariate models (fully adjusted model: risk ratio, 0.89; 95% CI, 0.83-0.95), but associations with grip strength did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance after additionally adjusting for relevant baseline health factors, depressive symptoms, and health behaviors (fully adjusted model: risk ratio, 0.91; 95% CI, 0.80-1.04).
Conclusions and Relevance
Purpose in life was prospectively associated with a decreased risk of developing weak grip strength and slow walking speed, although the findings were more robust for walking speed than for grip strength. These findings suggest that a sense of purpose in life, a modifiable factor, may play an important role in maintaining physical function among older adults.
This longitudinal cohort study uses data from the Health and Retirement Study to assess whether higher purpose in life among adequately functioning older adults is associated with lower risk of developing weak grip strength and slow walking speed over time.
Key Points
Question
Is higher purpose in life associated with lower likelihood of objectively measured declines in physical function?
Findings
In a longitudinal cohort study of adults older than 50 years who were adequately functioning at baseline, each 1-SD increase in purpose in life was associated with a 13% decreased risk of developing weak grip strength and 14% decreased risk of developing slow walking speeds 4 years later.
Meaning
A sense of purpose in life, a modifiable factor, may play an important role in maintaining physical function among older adults.
Introduction
The number of older adults is growing rapidly, and meeting the unique needs of this growing demographic is considered a global public health challenge. As populations age, identifying factors that facilitate maintenance of adequately functioning strength and mobility into late life is crucial; decline in these functional capacities is associated with increased risk of disability, morbidity, institutionalization, and mortality. Furthermore, capacity to maintain physical function is an important component of aging well and thriving. However, in the United States, almost 1 in 3 adults (31.7%) 65 years or older report difficulty walking 3 city blocks. Although much effort has been directed toward identifying risk factors for disease onset, investigators are increasingly seeking protective factors, that is, attributes that enhance a person’s ability to maintain health.
Recent research finds that having a higher sense of purpose in life is associated with greater likelihood of engaging in healthier behaviors (eg, higher physical activity, use of preventive health screening), better biological functioning (eg, reduced allostatic load), reduced risk of disease (eg, lower risk of cardiovascular disease and cognitive impairment), and mortality. Purpose in life, often viewed as a central component of well-being, refers to the extent to which people see their lives as having meaning, a sense of direction, and goals. It is shaped by social structural factors and changing life circumstances; moreover, several studies show that purpose in life can be modified. Because purpose appears to promote healthier behaviors and protect against disease, investigators speculate that it contributes to people’s capacity to maintain independent physical function. However, we know of no studies directly evaluating whether purpose is associated with objective performance-related measures.
In this study, we tested whether higher purpose was associated with decreased risk of developing weak grip strength (an indicator of strength) and slow walking speed (an indicator of mobility) after 4 years of follow-up. Prior work shows that these specific indicators of physical function are associated with major health end points, including morbidity, institutionalization, and mortality. Identifying factors that contribute to maintaining these aspects of physical function may provide insight into strategies for enhancing healthy aging. Based on results of prior research, we identified relevant covariates to consider.
Methods
Study Population
Data used in this study are from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), an ongoing nationally representative panel study of US adults older than 50 years that surveys people every 2 years. Starting in 2006, a random 50% of the HRS longitudinal panel was selected for an enhanced face-to-face interview. In 2006 and again in 2010, physical function assessments were obtained from the same respondents by trained interviewers. After the interview, respondents were given a self-report psychological questionnaire, which they completed and returned by mail to the University of Michigan. Because physical function and psychological measures were first assessed in 2006, we considered this wave to be the baseline for the current study. Although the HRS interviews all couples in a household, HRS survey sampling weights were developed only for respondents older than 50 years; hence, only those respondents are included in the sample for the present study. These weights account for the complex multistage probability survey design; they also use propensity models to adjust for nonresponse in the face-to-face interview and leave-behind psychosocial questionnaire. The HRS website provides extensive documentation about the protocol, instrumentation, and complex sampling strategy (http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/). Because the study used deidentified, publicly available data, the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health Institutional Review Board exempted it from review; in addition, all HRS respondents provided written informed consent.
Analyses were conducted using 2 different samples owing to differing inclusion criteria for participants when considering grip strength (n = 4486) or walking speed (n = 1461). eFigures 1 and 2 in the Supplement provide detailed inclusion criteria for each sample.
Measures
Purpose in Life
Purpose in life was assessed in 2006 using the 7-item Purpose in Life subscale of the Ryff Psychological Well-being Scales, previously validated in a nationally representative sample of adults. On a 6-point Likert scale, respondents rated the degree to which they agreed with each item. The mean of all items was taken to create a scale. Scores ranged from 1 to 6, where higher scores reflected higher purpose (Cronbach α = 0.75); we then standardized these scores so that all binary results can be interpreted as the change in risk of developing a slow walking speed or weak grip strength as a function of a 1-SD increase in purpose in life. To examine the possibility of threshold effects, we created tertiles of purpose based on the baseline distribution of purpose scores in the sample (eTables 1 and 2 in the Supplement). Additional details about the exposure and outcome variables are in the eMethods in the Supplement.
Grip Strength
In each wave (2006 and 2010), a respondent’s grip strength was measured 4 times by HRS interviewers who followed a standard protocol and used Smedley spring-type hand dynamometers; 2 measurements were taken with each hand, alternating hands during measurement. Grip strength was considered as both a continuous and binary variable. A change score was calculated by subtracting baseline grip strength scores from follow-up scores (scores ranged from −34.5 kg to 19.75 kg; mean change, −2.31 kg). The binary grip strength score used cut points identified in previous research; scores less than 26 kg for men and less than 16 kg for women were categorized as weak grip strength.
Walking Speed
In each wave (2006 and 2010), a respondent’s walking speed was measured twice by HRS interviewers who followed a standard walking task protocol. Walking speed was considered as both a continuous and binary variable. Change scores ranged from −0.89 m/s to 0.71 m/s, with a mean change of −0.15 m/s. Following previous research, binary walking speed scores categorized scores less than 0.8 m/s as slow walking speed.
Covariates
All covariates were assessed by self-report in 2006 and included sociodemographic characteristics, baseline health, and depressive symptoms. Sociodemographic characteristics included age, sex, race/ethnicity (white, African American, Hispanic, or other), marital status (married or not married), educational level (no degree, General Educational Development certificate or high school diploma, or college degree or higher), and total wealth (based on quintiles of the score distribution). Baseline health was assessed by summing the number of major chronic illnesses (heart problems, stroke, cancer, high blood pressure, diabetes, lung disease, or arthritis or rheumatism) for which participants reported a physician’s diagnosis, a measure that was validated in previous HRS work. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the validated 8-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Cronbach α = 0.91). Health behaviors included smoking status (never, former, or current), frequency of exercise (never, 1-4 times per month, or more than once a week), and frequency of alcohol consumption (abstinent, <1-2 days per month, 1-2 days per week, and >3 days per week).
Statistical Analysis
Data analysis was conducted from November 23, 2016, to June 2, 2017. Given that physical function decline is common (>10% in our sample), we used Poisson regression with robust error variance to evaluate the likelihood of developing weak grip strength (yes or no) or slow walking speed (yes or no) at follow-up among participants who were adequately functioning at baseline. Furthermore, we used ordinary least squares regression to assess the magnitude of physical function change (measured continuously) during the 4-year follow-up. For all analyses, 5 models were evaluated. Model 1 was unadjusted. Model 2 was minimally adjusted for age, sex, and race/ethnicity. Model 3 (core model) adjusted for the additional sociodemographics of marital status, educational level, and total wealth. We then considered the outcome of sequentially adding other covariates, including baseline health and depressive symptoms (model 4) and then health behaviors (model 5).
For more easily interpretable results, coefficients created by Poisson models were exponentiated, thereby providing risk ratios. We assessed whether the data met assumptions of ordinary least squares regression and found nonnormality near the tails of the change scores for both grip and speed; thus, we winsorized outcome data, using 95% winsorization (the least amount needed to alleviate nonnormal residuals). All analyses were conducted in Stata, version 14.1 (StataCorp). P < .05 (2-sided) was considered significant.
Additional Analyses
First, to examine the possibility that better physical function might lead to higher purpose, we tested whether higher baseline physical function was associated with increased purpose during the follow-up period (calculated as the change in purpose between baseline and follow-up). Second, precise cut points are being debated in the literature; hence, to evaluate the robustness of our results we examined commonly agreed-on alternative cut points for grip strength (<32 kg for men and <20 kg for women) and walking speed (<0.6 m/s; see eTables 3-6 in the Supplement). Third, owing to past research showing associations between low cognitive function and declines in physical function, we tested the outcome that cognitive impairment might have on our primary association. Fourth, we examined cross-sectional analyses in the full sample.
Missing Data
Among respondents in the final analytic sample, the overall item nonresponse rate was 0.9% for the grip strength sample and 5.9% for the walking speed sample. Because missing data were distributed across variables, complete-case analyses resulted in a loss of 9.1% (n = 408) of the grip strength respondents and 13.1% (n = 191) of the walking speed respondents. Hence, we imputed missing data on all covariates and outcomes using a multivariate normal multiple imputation procedure because it often provides a more accurate estimate of association than other methods of handling missing data; therefore, we present results with multiple imputation for all analyses.
Results
At study baseline, respondents in the grip strength sample were a mean (SD) 63.0 (8.2) years of age and respondents in the walking speed sample were 70.8 (6.5) years of age. Among 4486 people with adequately functioning grip strength at baseline, 426 respondents (9.5%) developed weak grip strength at follow-up. Among 1461 people with adequately functioning walking speed at baseline, 687 respondents (47.0%) developed slow walking speed at follow-up. Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of both samples.
Table 1. Characteristics of Study Participants at Baseline.
Characteristic | Valuea | |
---|---|---|
Grip Strength Participants (N = 4486) |
Walking Speed Participants (N = 1461) |
|
Age, mean (SD), y | 63.0 (8.2) | 70.8 (6.5) |
Female | 2665 (59.4) | 801 (54.8) |
Married | 3071 (68.5) | 1011 (69.2) |
Race/ethnicity | ||
White | 3399 (75.8) | 1216 (83.2) |
African American | 664 (14.8) | 128 (8.8) |
Hispanic | 349 (7.8) | 87 (6.0) |
Other | 74 (1.6) | 30 (2.1) |
Educational level | ||
<High school | 685 (15.3) | 241 (16.5) |
High school | 2439 (54.4) | 859 (58.8) |
≥College | 1362 (30.4) | 361 (24.7) |
Total wealth | ||
1st Quintile (≤$24 000) | 735 (16.4) | 173 (11.8) |
2nd Quintile ($24 030-$116 500) | 861 (19.2) | 163 (11.2) |
3rd Quintile ($117 000-$270 000) | 923 (20.6) | 281 (19.2) |
4th Quintile ($270 500-$598 000) | 979 (21.8) | 349 (23.9) |
5th Quintile (≥$599 647) | 988 (22.0) | 495 (33.9) |
No. of illnesses, mean (SD) | 1.6 (1.2) | 1.9 (1.3) |
Smoking status | ||
Never | 2020 (45.0) | 617 (42.2) |
Former smoker | 1787 (39.8) | 710 (48.6) |
Current smoker | 679 (15.1) | 134 (9.2) |
Exercise frequency | ||
Never | 620 (13.8) | 156 (10.7) |
1-4 Times/mo | 1013 (22.6) | 291 (19.9) |
>1 Time/wk | 2854 (63.6) | 1014 (69.4) |
Alcohol frequency, d/wk | ||
Never | 2015 (44.9) | 616 (42.1) |
<1 | 836 (18.6) | 257 (17.6) |
1-2 | 825 (18.4) | 257 (17.6) |
≥3 | 811 (18.1) | 332 (22.7) |
Data are presented as number (percentage) of respondents unless otherwise indicated.
Purpose in Life and Grip Strength
We observed inverse associations between higher purpose and lower risk of developing weak grip strength (Table 2). For example, in the core model adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with a 13% decreased risk of developing weak grip strength levels (95% CI, 1%-23%). After further controlling for all covariates, results did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance (risk ratio, 0.91; 95% CI, 0.80-1.04). The association between purpose and 4-year change in grip strength was consistently positive in all covariate models, with higher purpose associated with more grip strength improvement (Table 3). For example, after adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with a modest increase in grip strength (β = 0.27; 95% CI, 0.07-0.47); this effect size is approximately equivalent to being 3 years younger.
Table 2. Risk Ratios for the Association Between 1-SD Increase in Baseline Purpose in Life and Risk of Developing Weak Grip Strength or Slow Walking Speed 4 Years Later.
Characteristic | Adjusted Risk Ratio (95% CI) | P Value |
---|---|---|
Grip Strength (n = 4486)a | ||
Model | ||
Unadjusted | 0.76 (0.67-0.86) | <.001 |
Age, sex, and race/ethnicity | 0.85 (0.75-0.96) | .01 |
Demographicsb | 0.87 (0.77-0.99) | .03 |
Demographics,b baseline health,c and depressive symptoms | 0.90 (0.79-1.02) | .11 |
Demographics,b baseline health,c depressive symptoms, and health behaviorsd | 0.91 (0.80-1.04) | .15 |
Walking Speed (n = 1461)e | ||
Model | ||
Unadjusted | 0.80 (0.75-0.85) | <.001 |
Age, sex, and race/ethnicity | 0.83 (0.78-0.89) | <.001 |
Demographicsb | 0.86 (0.80-0.92) | <.001 |
Demographics,b baseline health,c and depressive symptoms | 0.89 (0.83-0.95) | .002 |
Demographics,b baseline health,c depressive symptoms, and health behaviorsd | 0.89 (0.83-0.95) | .001 |
Grip strength cut point, 26 kg for men and 16 kg for women.
Demographic model adjusted for age, sex, race/ethnicity, marital status, educational level, and total wealth.
Baseline health model adjusted for a history of myocardial infarction, coronary heart disease, angina, congestive heart failure, other heart problems, stroke, cancer or malignant tumor of any kind (excluding minor skin cancer), high blood pressure, diabetes, lung disease, and arthritis or rheumatism.
Health behavior model adjusted for smoking, exercise, and alcohol frequency.
Walking speed cut point, 0.8 m/s.
Table 3. Linear Regressions Modeling Mean Changes in Physical Function (Assessed in Waves 2006 and 2010) as a Function of a 1-SD Increase in Baseline Purpose in Life.
Characteristic | Adjusted Regression Coefficients (95% CI) | P Value |
---|---|---|
Grip Strength (n = 4486) | ||
Model | ||
Unadjusted | 0.38 (0.17-0.58) | .001 |
Age, sex, and race/ethnicity | 0.32 (0.11-0.52) | .003 |
Demographicsa | 0.27 (0.07-0.47) | .009 |
Demographics,a baseline health,b and depressive symptoms | 0.25 (0.06-0.45) | .01 |
Demographics,a baseline health,b depressive symptoms, and health behaviorsc | 0.24 (0.04-0.44) | .02 |
Walking Speed (n = 1461) | ||
Model | ||
Unadjusted | 0.03 (0.02-0.05) | <.001 |
Age, sex, and race/ethnicity | 0.02 (0.01-0.04) | .003 |
Demographicsa | 0.02 (0.00-0.03) | .02 |
Demographics,a baseline health,b and depressive symptoms | 0.01 (0.00-0.03) | .08 |
Demographics,a baseline health,b depressive symptoms, and health behaviorsc | 0.01 (0.00-0.03) | .05 |
Demographic factors were age, sex, race/ethnicity, marital status, educational level, and total wealth.
Baseline health considered whether there existed a history of myocardial infarction, coronary heart disease, angina, congestive heart failure, other heart problems, stroke, cancer or malignant tumor of any kind (excluding minor skin cancer), high blood pressure, diabetes, lung disease, and arthritis or rheumatism.
Health behaviors consisted of smoking, exercise, and alcohol frequency.
Purpose in Life and Walking Speed
Across all covariate models, we observed strong inverse associations between higher purpose in life and lower risk of developing slow walking speed (Table 2). For example, in the core model, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with a 14% decreased risk of developing slow walking speed (95% CI, 8%-20%). In the core model, when the association between purpose and change in walking speed was examined, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with a modest increase in walking speed (β = 0.02; 95% CI, 0.00-0.03); this effect size is approximately equivalent to being 2.5 years younger (Table 3). After adjusting for additional covariates, the primary associations were consistently positive but did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance. When considering purpose tertiles, however, there was evidence of a threshold effect whereby only people with high purpose displayed increased walking speeds (β = 0.05high; 95% CI, 0.02-0.09; eTable 2 in the Supplement).
Additional Analyses
No associations were evident between baseline grip strength (β = 0.00; 95% CI, −0.01 to 0.01) or walking speed (β = –0.02; 95% CI, −0.22 to 0.17) and changes in purpose 4 years later. When alternate physical function cut points were considered, associations were similar in all covariate models; for example, in demographic-adjusted models, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with an 8% decreased risk of developing weak grip strength (95% CI, 1%-14%) (eTable 3 in the Supplement) and a 21% decreased risk of developing slow walking speeds (95% CI, 13%-28%; eTable 3 in the Supplement). When cognitive impairment was added to the demographic characteristic–adjusted model, each 1-SD increase in purpose was associated with a marginally decreased risk of developing weak grip strength (risk ratio, 0.93; 95% CI, 0.85-1.03) and a significantly reduced risk of developing slow walking speeds (risk ratio, 0.89; 95% CI, 0.83-0.96). In demographic characteristic–adjusted cross-sectional models, the association between purpose and walking speed was positive and significant (β = 0.02; 95% CI, 0.01-0.03) and the association between purpose and grip strength was marginally significant (β = 0.21; 95% CI, −0.01 to 0.43).
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to evaluate the association between purpose in life and objectively assessed physical function. In a prospective and nationally representative sample of older US adults who had adequately functioning levels of physical function at baseline, higher baseline purpose in life was associated with decreased risk of developing slow walking speed during 4 years of follow-up and small increases in walking speed among people with high purpose. Higher purpose was also associated with increases in grip strength but was less robustly associated with decreased risk of developing weak grip strength. In models evaluating risk of declining into slow walking speeds, findings were maintained after careful control for potential confounders, including sociodemographic characteristics, baseline health, depressive symptoms, and cognitive impairment. Reduced risk of declining into weak grip strength was observed in some models, but was less strongly apparent when additionally adjusting for baseline health, depressive symptoms, or cognitive impairment. The somewhat divergent findings in our 2 outcomes may be explained by the fact that they reflect unique aspects of physical function, as reflected by their low correlation in our sample (0.25). Associations were also maintained when alternate cut points for physical function were tested. Furthermore, we did not find convincing evidence that baseline physical function was associated with subsequent changes in levels of purpose, although it is plausible that bidirectional associations exist. Inclusion of behavioral variables in the fully adjusted model likely overadjusts our results because many hypothesize behaviors as mechanisms between purpose and physical function. However, adjustment for behaviors did not substantially alter our findings, indicating there may be other mechanisms that need to be identified.
Although mechanisms that explain the potential health effects of purpose have not yet been clearly defined, there are likely indirect (eg, other health-related behaviors) and/or direct effects (eg, altered biological function). For example, people with higher purpose are more proactive in taking care of their health, have better impulse control, and engage in healthier activities. Given that associations between purpose and physical function were only modestly attenuated after adjusting for standard health behaviors, the range of health behaviors considered may need to be broadened, the measurement instruments used to capture health behaviors might be imprecise, or other mechanisms may be factors. Other work evaluating direct effects shows that people with higher purpose display enhanced regulation of physiological systems associated with changes in physical function (eg, reduced inflammatory markers). Hence, the association of purpose in life with physical function may be additionally explained by a direct effect on biological function. Future work should continue examining potential mechanisms.
Our findings are consistent with those of 3 previous studies evaluating the association between purpose and disability, which is an important correlate of physical function. One study observed 970 older adults recruited from senior housing facilities in the Chicago area and observed them for approximately 5 years. Results showed that higher baseline purpose was associated with reduced risk of disability over time. A second study observed 1475 older adults from South Australia across 18 years and found that higher purpose was associated with lower levels of disability, but not with slower declines in disability. A third study evaluated whether purpose was associated with limitations in activities of daily living. This cross-sectional study found that higher purpose was associated with fewer physical functional limitations among women with breast cancer, but not among community-based middle-aged women.
Our study provides an important contribution by (1) evaluating objective measures of physical function (while these studies used a self-reported physical function measure that may be subject to self-report bias), (2) using a reasonably representative sample of older US adults, and (3) focusing on a broader range of physical function than has previously been considered. Previous studies focused on physical disability and deficits in functioning, which restricts the range of data on physical function that people without disability can provide. Our study also converges with evidence evaluating the association between related but distinct psychological factors (eg, mastery and optimism) and enhanced physical function on objective measures.
Strengths and Limitations
Our study has several limitations. Unmeasured confounding is always plausible in observational studies; however, we carefully controlled for a range of potential confounders. Reverse causality is another limitation, but analyses were conducted to address this issue. Furthermore, it is conservative to remove people with suboptimal physical function at baseline because the baseline association incorporates the lifelong forward association between purpose and physical function; by removing this population, we essentially focus on short-term changes in declining function. This method also reduces the likelihood of reverse causality. Although several analyses attempted to reduce the possibility of reverse causality, it is still a likelihood and an area that should be further evaluated. Biased estimates due to attrition may be a problem in studies of older adults because participants with worsening physical function may be more likely to drop out. However, we used a multiple imputation method to alleviate this concern. In addition, the follow-up period of 4 years was relatively short; future studies should examine these associations using longer follow-up periods and repeated measures, which would suggest that findings hold over longer durations of time and multiple measures. Although specific cut points of physical function are the subject of ongoing debate, our findings were robust to the most widely used cut points, as well as alternative cut points, which suggests that effects of purpose are not evident only at arbitrary points in the spectrum of functioning.
Our study also had several strengths. We drew from a large, diverse, prospective and national sample of older US adults. Furthermore, objective measures of physical function were used, reducing concerns about self-report bias and common method variance (eg, self-reported purpose and physical function).
Conclusions
Recent large-scale studies show that grip strength and walking speed are robust factors associated with survival; identifying factors that contribute to maintaining these aspects of physical function may provide insight into strategies for increasing the likelihood of healthy aging. Considering the rapidly aging population, there is an escalating need to identify factors that can lengthen the time that people maintain physical function. Given the many studies showing that purpose can be improved, our study suggests that purpose in life may be one promising and novel upstream factor that serves as a target for improving not only mental health but physical function as well.
References
- 1.Suzman R, Beard JR, Boerma T, Chatterji S. Health in an ageing world—what do we know? Lancet. 2015;385(9967):484-486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fried LP, Guralnik JM. Disability in older adults: evidence regarding significance, etiology, and risk. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1997;45(1):92-100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Leong DP, Teo KK, Rangarajan S, et al. ; Prospective Urban Rural Epidemiology (PURE) Study investigators . Prognostic value of grip strength: findings from the Prospective Urban Rural Epidemiology (PURE) study. Lancet. 2015;386(9990):266-273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Studenski S, Perera S, Patel K, et al. . Gait speed and survival in older adults. JAMA. 2011;305(1):50-58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cawthon PM, Fox KM, Gandra SR, et al. ; Health, Aging and Body Composition Study . Do muscle mass, muscle density, strength, and physical function similarly influence risk of hospitalization in older adults? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57(8):1411-1419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Vermeulen J, Neyens JC, van Rossum E, Spreeuwenberg MD, de Witte LP. Predicting ADL disability in community-dwelling elderly people using physical frailty indicators: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2011;11:33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hardy SE, Kang Y, Studenski SA, Degenholtz HB. Ability to walk 1/4 mile predicts subsequent disability, mortality, and health care costs. J Gen Intern Med. 2011;26(2):130-135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Pavasini R, Guralnik J, Brown JC, et al. . Short Physical Performance Battery and all-cause mortality: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med. 2016;14(1):215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cooper R, Kuh D, Hardy R; Mortality Review Group; FALCon and HALCyon Study Teams . Objectively measured physical capability levels and mortality: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2010;341:c4467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cooper R, Kuh D, Cooper C, et al. ; FALCon and HALCyon Study Teams . Objective measures of physical capability and subsequent health: a systematic review. Age Ageing. 2011;40(1):14-23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.den Ouden MEM, Schuurmans MJ, Arts IEMA, van der Schouw YT. Physical performance characteristics related to disability in older persons: a systematic review. Maturitas. 2011;69(3):208-219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rijk JM, Roos PR, Deckx L, van den Akker M, Buntinx F. Prognostic value of handgrip strength in people aged 60 years and older: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2016;16(1):5-20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Woods NF, Rillamas-Sun E, Cochrane BB, et al. . Aging well: observations from the Women’s Health Initiative study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71(suppl 1):S3-S12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Prevalence and most common causes of disability among adults—United States, 2005. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2009;58(16):421-426. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Boat TF. Improving lifetime health by promoting behavioral health in children. JAMA. 2015;313(15):1509-1510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lloyd-Jones DM. Improving the cardiovascular health of the US population. JAMA. 2012;307(12):1314-1316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Seligman MEP. Positive health. Appl Psychol. 2008;57:3-18. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00351.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hooker SA, Masters KS. Purpose in life is associated with physical activity measured by accelerometer. J Health Psychol. 2016;21(6):962-971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kim ES, Hershner SD, Strecher VJ. Purpose in life and incidence of sleep disturbances. J Behav Med. 2015;38(3):590-597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kim ES, Strecher VJ, Ryff CD. Purpose in life and use of preventive health care services. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(46):16331-16336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Zilioli S, Slatcher RB, Ong AD, Gruenewald TL. Purpose in life predicts allostatic load ten years later. J Psychosom Res. 2015;79(5):451-457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Boylan JM, Tsenkova VK, Miyamoto Y, Ryff CD. Psychological resources and glucoregulation in Japanese adults: findings from MIDJA. Health Psychol. 2017;36(5):449-457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kim ES, Sun JK, Park N, Peterson C. Purpose in life and reduced incidence of stroke in older adults: “The Health and Retirement Study”. J Psychosom Res. 2013;74(5):427-432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kim ES, Sun JK, Park N, Kubzansky LD, Peterson C. Purpose in life and reduced risk of myocardial infarction among older US adults with coronary heart disease: a two-year follow-up. J Behav Med. 2013;36(2):124-133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lewis NA, Turiano NA, Payne BR, Hill PL. Purpose in life and cognitive functioning in adulthood. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2016;7:1-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Effect of a purpose in life on risk of incident Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older persons. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):304-310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Cohen R, Bavishi C, Rozanski A. Purpose in life and its relationship to all-cause mortality and cardiovascular events: a meta-analysis. Psychosom Med. 2016;78(2):122-133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hill PL, Turiano NA. Purpose in life as a predictor of mortality across adulthood. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(7):1482-1486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Boyle PA, Barnes LL, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with mortality among community-dwelling older persons. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(5):574-579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Windsor TD, Curtis RG, Luszcz MA. Sense of purpose as a psychological resource for aging well. Dev Psychol. 2015;51(7):975-986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. 2015;385(9968):640-648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Steger MF, Frazier P, Oishi S, Kaler M. The Meaning in Life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J Couns Psychol. 2006;53:80-93. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ryff CD. Psychological well-being revisited: advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychother Psychosom. 2014;83(1):10-28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.McKnight PE, Kashdan TB. Purpose in life as a system that creates and sustains health and well-being: an integrative, testable theory. Rev Gen Psychol. 2009;13:242-251. doi: 10.1037/a0017152 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Frankl VE. Man’s Search for Meaning. Boston, MA: Beacon Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Weiss LA, Westerhof GJ, Bohlmeijer ET. Can we increase psychological well-being? the effects of interventions on psychological well-being: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS One. 2016;11(6):e0158092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Brown CJ, Flood KL. Mobility limitation in the older patient: a clinical review. JAMA. 2013;310(11):1168-1177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Demakakos P, Cooper R, Hamer M, de Oliveira C, Hardy R, Breeze E. The bidirectional association between depressive symptoms and gait speed: evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e68632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Stuck AE, Walthert JM, Nikolaus T, Büla CJ, Hohmann C, Beck JC. Risk factors for functional status decline in community-living elderly people: a systematic literature review. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(4):445-469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sonnega A, Faul JD, Ofstedal MB, Langa KM, Phillips JWR, Weir DR. Cohort profile: the Health and Retirement Study (HRS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(2):576-585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Smith J, Fisher G, Ryan L, Clarke P, House J, Weir D Psychosocial and lifestyle questionnaire: 2006-2010. http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/HRS2006-2010SAQdoc.pdf. Published February 2013. Accessed January 5, 2017.
- 42.Ryff CD, Keyes CLM. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(4):719-727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Crimmins E, Guyer H, Langa K, Ofstedal MB, Wallace R, Weir D Documentation of physical measures, anthropometrics and blood pressure in the Health and Retirement Study. http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-011.pdf. Published February 2008. Accessed January 5, 2017.
- 44.Alley DE, Shardell MD, Peters KW, et al. . Grip strength cutpoints for the identification of clinically relevant weakness. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(5):559-566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Studenski SA, Peters KW, Alley DE, et al. . The FNIH sarcopenia project: rationale, study description, conference recommendations, and final estimates. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(5):547-558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Abellan van Kan G, Rolland Y, Andrieu S, et al. . Gait speed at usual pace as a predictor of adverse outcomes in community-dwelling older people: an International Academy on Nutrition and Aging (IANA) Task Force. J Nutr Health Aging. 2009;13(10):881-889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Fisher GG, Faul JD, Weir DR, Wallace RB Documentation of chronic disease measures in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS/AHEAD). http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-009.pdf. Updated February 10, 2005. Accessed December 3, 2016.
- 48.Wallace RB, Herzog AR, Ofstedal MB, Steffick D, Fonda S, Langa K Documentation of affective functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study. http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdf. Accessed December 3, 2016.
- 49.Zou G. A modified Poisson regression approach to prospective studies with binary data. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(7):702-706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wilcox RR. Introduction to Robust Estimation and Hypothesis Testing. 3rd ed Waltham, MA: Academic Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Duchowny KA, Peterson MD, Clarke PJ. Cut points for clinical muscle weakness among older Americans [published online February 9, 2017]. Am J Prev Med. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Clouston SAP, Brewster P, Kuh D, et al. . The dynamic relationship between physical function and cognition in longitudinal aging cohorts. Epidemiol Rev. 2013;35(1):33-50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Asendorpf JB, van de Schoot R, Denissen JJA, Hutteman R. Reducing bias due to systematic attrition in longitudinal studies: the benefits of multiple imputation. Int J Behav Dev. 2014;38(5):453-460. doi: 10.1177/0165025414542713 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Rawlings AM, Sang Y, Sharrett AR, et al. . Multiple imputation of cognitive performance as a repeatedly measured outcome. Eur J Epidemiol. 2017;32(1):55-66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Peeters M, Zondervan-Zwijnenburg M, Vink G, van de Schoot R. How to handle missing data: a comparison of different approaches. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2015;12(4):377-394. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2015.1049526 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cumming JJ, Goldstein H. Handling attrition and non-response in longitudinal data with an application to a study of Australian youth. Longit Life Course Stud. 2016;7(1):53-63. doi: 10.14301/llcs.v7i1.342 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Jones M, Mishra GD, Dobson A. Analytical results in longitudinal studies depended on target of inference and assumed mechanism of attrition. J Clin Epidemiol. 2015;68(10):1165-1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Boehm JK, Kubzansky LD. The heart’s content: the association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychol Bull. 2012;138(4):655-691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Burrow AL, Spreng RN. Waiting with purpose: a reliable but small association between purpose in life and impulsivity. Pers Individ Dif. 2016;90:187-189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Friedman EM, Hayney M, Love GD, Singer BH, Ryff CD. Plasma interleukin-6 and soluble IL-6 receptors are associated with psychological well-being in aging women. Health Psychol. 2007;26(3):305-313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Morozink JA, Friedman EM, Coe CL, Ryff CD. Socioeconomic and psychosocial predictors of interleukin-6 in the MIDUS national sample. Health Psychol. 2010;29(6):626-635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Brinkley TE, Leng X, Miller ME, et al. . Chronic inflammation is associated with low physical function in older adults across multiple comorbidities. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(4):455-461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with a reduced risk of incident disability among community-dwelling older persons. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(12):1093-1102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Scheier MF, Wrosch C, Baum A, et al. . The Life Engagement Test: assessing purpose in life. J Behav Med. 2006;29(3):291-298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Milaneschi Y, Bandinelli S, Corsi AM, et al. . Personal mastery and lower body mobility in community-dwelling older persons: the Invecchiare in Chianti study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(1):98-103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Brenes GA, Rapp SR, Rejeski WJ, Miller ME. Do optimism and pessimism predict physical functioning? J Behav Med. 2002;25(3):219-231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.