Abstract
Neurons communicate through excitatory and inhibitory synapses. Both lines of communication are adjustable and allow the fine tuning of signal exchange required for learning processes in neural networks. Several distinct modes of plasticity modulate glutamatergic and GABAergic synapses in Purkinje cells of the cerebellar cortex to promote motor control and learning. In the present paper, we present evidence for a role of short-term ionic plasticity in the cerebellar circuit activity. This type of plasticity results from altered chloride driving forces at the synapses that molecular layer interneurons form on Purkinje cell dendrites. Previous studies have provided evidence for transiently diminished chloride gradients at these GABAergic synapses following climbing fiber activity. Electrical stimulation of climbing fibers in acute slices caused a decline of inhibitory postsynaptic currents recorded from Purkinje cells. Dendritic calcium-gated chloride channels of the type anoctamin 2 (ANO2) were proposed to mediate this short-term modulation of inhibition, but the significance of this process for motor control has not been established yet. Here, we report results of behavioral studies obtained from Ano2 −/− mice, a mouse line that was previously shown to lack this particular mode of ionic plasticity. The animals display motor coordination deficits that constitute a condition of mild ataxia. Moreover, motor learning is severely impaired in Ano2 −/− mice, suggesting cerebellar dysfunction. This reduced motor performance of Ano2 −/− mice highlights the significance of inhibitory control for cerebellar function and introduces calcium-dependent short-term ionic plasticity as an efficient control mechanism for neural inhibition.
Keywords: Purkinje cells, Inhibition, Plasticity, Calcium, Motor performance
Introduction
The cerebellum controls body movements and mediates the acquisition of procedural memory [1]. The output neurons of the cerebellar cortex, the Purkinje cells, receive inhibitory GABAergic synapses from a network of molecular layer interneurons (MLIs), which provide fast feed-forward inhibition and co-determine the activity of the Purkinje cells [2–6]. MLIs limit the time window for synaptic integration in Purkinje cells to 1–2 ms [2], a control function that shapes the output signal of the cerebellar cortex. The MLI-Purkinje cell synapses are also subject to modulatory mechanisms, which probably contribute to motor learning. In particular, Purkinje cell depolarization that results from the activation of climbing fibers causes a slow but persistent increase of GABAergic synaptic strength termed rebound potentiation [7–9]. In genetic experiments, ablation of the γ2 subunit of GABAA receptors at the MLI-Purkinje cell synapse impaired inter-limb coordination and motor memory consolidation [10, 11]. Moreover, pharmacologically boosted inhibition at this synapse severely disturbed motor coordination [12]. Thus, the inhibitory regulation of Purkinje cells by MLIs is clearly crucial for motor performance. Recent studies have indicated that the inhibition efficacy at the MLI-Purkinje cell synapse can be attenuated by a postsynaptic process which is triggered by activation of climbing fibers [13, 14]. Ca2+ signals that accompany climbing fiber-induced depolarization cause the opening of chloride channels of the type anoctamin 2 (ANO2, alias TMEM16B) [15]. It was proposed that these channels, in combination with increased Cl− uptake through the co-transporter NKCC1, cause the local intra-dendritic Cl− concentration to increase. Such postsynaptic Cl− accumulation would reduce the inward driving force for chloride ions at MLI-Purkinje cell synapses and, hence, induce a decline of inhibitory postsynaptic current (IPSC) amplitudes. This mechanism was termed depolarization-induced depression of inhibition (DDI) and was shown to attenuate the inhibitory input for several minutes after climbing fiber activation [13]. This mode of modulation was detected by recording from Purkinje cells in tissue slices of rats [13] and mice [14]. Climbing fiber activity triggered a rapid, transient decline of IPSCs at the MLI-Purkinje cell synapse, and no such effect was observed in Ano2 −/−mice. This finding left the question unanswered whether the lack of DDI would compromise motor coordination and learning in Ano2 −/−mice. Thus, in the present study, we looked for the relevance of DDI for motor control. We compared the motor performance of wild-type and Ano2 −/− mice in a variety of behavioral tasks designed to specifically reveal cerebellar dysfunction. We found that Ano2 −/− mice display deficiency in motor coordination and motor learning. Our results illustrate the behavioral significance of calcium-dependent modulation of inhibitory network activity through short-term ionic plasticity, a novel pathway for controlling network function in the brain.
Materials and Methods
Animals: Housing, General Health, and Behavior
C57BL/6N mice (Charles River Laboratories, Germany) and Ano2 −/− mice [16] (kindly provided by Thomas Jentsch, Leipniz-Institute for Molecular Pharmacology, Berlin) were kept in groups of 2–3 with ad libitum access to food and water. A standard 12-h light/dark cycle was provided (light on: 7 am to 7 pm) and temperature was maintained at ±22 °C at a relative humidity of 40–50%. All experiments were approved by the Regierungspräsidium Karlsruhe and were in agreement with national and international guidelines. For general health screening, a modified version of the SHIRPA test [17] was used for both genotypes. Spontaneous activity, anxiety, body strength, and muscle tone as well as several reflexes were tested according to the provided scoring system. General behavior was monitored using the LABORAS animal behavior observation system (Metris B.V., Netherlands) individually for six mice of each genotype for 72 h. Recorded activities included locomotion, climbing, rearing, grooming, eating, and drinking.
Experimental Design
Following a habitation time of 1 week, animals were handled for three consecutive days before the behavioral tests were started. Behavioral tests were performed by one female experimenter only in order to avoid unnecessary stress for the mice. Tests were run, if not indicated otherwise, from 1 to 4 pm. Non-automatic rated experiments (apart from the SHIRPA test) were analyzed in a blind fashion by two different persons. Only male, adult mice were used with a starting age of 8 weeks (cohort 3) and 10–12 weeks (cohorts 1 and 2) and tests were conducted for up to 10 weeks (cohorts 1 and 2) or 4 days (cohort 3). Numbers of animals tested were (wt/Ano2 −/−) general behavior 6/6 (cohort 1); rotarod 9/8 (cohort 1); open field, grip strength, parallel rod floor, gait analysis 9/10 (cohort 2); horizontal ladder 9/9 (cohort 2); and voluntary wheel running 4/5 (cohort 3).
Immunohistochemistry
To prepare cerebellar cryosections, animals were anesthetized by isoflurane (Baxter, Germany) inhalation and killed by an overdose of isoflurane. The cerebellum was removed and the fresh, unfixed tissue was cut on ice in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; 130 mM NaCl, 8.1 mM Na2HPO4, 1.9 mM NaH2PO4, pH 7.4).) using a vibratome (VT1000S, Leica Biosystems, Wetzlar, Germany). Sections of 180 µm thickness were fixed with 2% paraformaldehyde (PFA) for 30 min and washed 4× 10 min with PBS (for ANO2 immunostaining) or with PBST (PBS containing 0.5% Tween 20 (Carl Roth, Germany, 9127.1)) for calbindin immunostaining. For immunofluorescence staining, sections were incubated in blocking solution (goat serum, Sigma-Aldrich, Germany, G9023) for 2 h and then the primary antiserum (diluted in blocking solution) was applied overnight at room temperature. After washing 4× 10 min with PBS or PBST, the secondary antisera, conjugated with fluorescence tags, were incubated for 2 h and washed 4× 10 min with PBST. To visualize cell nuclei, the slices were incubated in 0.3 μM DAPI (Sigma-Aldrich, 32670) for 15 min, washed 4× 5 min in PBS, and then mounted on glass slides with Aqua-Poly/Mount (Polyscience, Germany, 18606). The sections were kept on a shaker for the whole staining process where they were gently shaken to improve the uptake of the antisera. The primary antibody against ANO2 (used at 1:200) was raised in guinea pig in our lab and was extensively characterized in various tissues including cerebellum [14, 18–20]. The primary antibody against calbindin (dilution 1:500) was raised in rabbit (Swant, Marly, Switzerland, CB-38a) and binds specifically to calbindin D-28k. The secondary antibodies were donkey anti-rabbit conjugated with Alexa Fluor 568 (Invitrogen, A10042, dilution 1:1000) and goat anti-guinea pig conjugated with Alexa Fluor 488 (Invitrogen, A11073).
Grip Strength Analysis
Forelimb strength of both mouse lines was tested using a grip strength meter (Ugo Basile, Gemonio, Italy, model 47106), which automatically measured the force needed for the mouse to release its grip. A mouse had to grasp a trapeze wire and would cling to it until the pulling force of the experimenter exceeded its own pulling strength. After an initial training session, the mice were tested in one session only, in which each mouse was tested in three consecutive runs. The runs were then averaged for each animal.
Open Field Test
Mice were individually placed in a wooden box (40 × 40 cm) for a period of 5 min and their movements were camera tracked. Only one run per mouse was conducted and the distance they walked was analyzed to evaluate their spontaneous activity.
Parallel Rod Floor Test
The open field setup was adjusted with an additional rod floor with parallel bars (3 mm in diameter, 6 mm space in between) to attain the mice’s locomotor activity at slightly aggravated conditions. On three consecutive days, mice were placed on the rod and their movement was video-tracked over a period of 5 min. One session per day was conducted per mouse. The maximal distance walked by all mice of a genotype was pooled for each session.
Horizontal Ladder Test
A horizontal ladder (40 cm in length, 43 rung positions) with a randomized and irregular rung pattern was used to assess skilled motor coordination of the animals. The metal rungs were 3 mm in diameter with a minimal space of 6 mm between two rods. Rods were placed in an irregular randomized pattern and their pattern was not altered during the testing phase. An initial training session was conducted to let the mice get acquired to the setup. While testing, mice were put at the beginning of the ladder individually and had to cross the ladder voluntarily. As a reward input, their home cage with the remaining housing companions was placed at the end of the ladder. Every walk was video-taped and the video was stopped as soon as the mice reached the end of the ladder. Each mouse did four trails so that each body side was taped twice. The videos were then analyzed by two different evaluators in a blinded manner. The time the mice needed to traverse the ladder was measured and the number of steps counted. Final results were obtained by averaging four runs of each mouse as well as the individual results of the two evaluators.
Gait Analysis
The animals’ gait patterns were analyzed using the CatWalk XT analysis apparatus (Noldus, Netherlands, version 10.6). The 1.3 m black corridor led over a glass plate that was illuminated by a green LED light. As soon as the mouse’s paw touched the glass slide, this green light was reflected and the resulting illuminated green footprint could then be captured by a high speed camera underneath the glass. As the mouse traversed the glass plate, its gait pattern was recorded and the resulting data automatically were analyzed by the CatWalk XT software according pre-set paradigms [21]. Evaluation parameters used for our analysis are specified in the legend to Table 1.
Table 1.
Parameter | Ano2 +/+ | Ano2 −/− | p value |
---|---|---|---|
Basic Locomotion | |||
Average running speed (cm/s) | 17.60 ± 1.14 | 11.84 ± 0.64 | <0.001 |
Number of steps per s | 12.35 ± 0.57 | 9.70 ± 0.39 | <0.001 |
Total number of steps | 84.23 ± 3.06 | 95.75 ± 2.19 | <0.01 |
Running duration (s) | 3.48 ± 0.26 | 4.89 ± 0.25 | <0.001 |
Step characteristics | |||
Stride length (cm)a | 5.28 ± 0.10 | 4.68 ± 0.07 | <0.001 |
Step cycle for each paw (s)b | 0.32 ± 0.009 | 0.42 ± 0.01 | <0.001 |
% of step cycle standing | 56.86 ± 0.53 | 61.18 ± 0.57 | <0.001 |
Swing speed in cycle (cm/s) | 45.58 ± 1.09 | 37.34 ± 0.75 | <0.001 |
Mean duration of standing (s)c | 0.20 ± 0.009 | 0.28 ± 0.007 | <0.001 |
Hind paws: single stance (s)d | 0.11 ± 0.003 | 0.13 ± 0.004 | <0.01 |
Hind paws: dual stance (s)e | 0.04 ± 0.004 | 0.07 ± 0.006 | <0.001 |
Distance HP-FP: left (cm)f | 1.10 ± 0.08 | 1.44 ± 0.12 | <0.05 |
Distance HP-FP: right (cm) | 0.99 ± 0.10 | 1.44 ± 0.07 | <0.001 |
Width between hind paws (cm) | 2.45 ± 0.04 | 2.59 ± 0.04 | <0.05 |
Support on three legs (%)g | 22.60 ± 1.94 | 31.12 ± 1.11 | <0.001 |
Support on diagonal legs (%)h | 60.81 ± 3.17 | 46.65 ± 2.5 | <0.001 |
Gait variability | |||
Run maximum variation (%)i | 55.00 ± 4.86 | 68.65 ± 3.89 | <0.05 |
Step sequence regularity (%)j | 88.80 ± 1.15 | 83.53 ± 1.74 | <0.05 |
Interpaw coordinationk | |||
RF->LH | 0.68 ± 0.03 | 0.54 ± 0.04 | <0.05 |
LF->RH | 0.73 ± 0.03 | 0.56 ± 0.03 | <0.001 |
LH->RH | 0.57 ± 0.03 | 0.40 ± 0.04 | <0.01 |
LF->RF | 0.80 ± 0.02 | 0.72 ± 0.02 | <0.01 |
RF->RH | 0.68 ± 0.03 | 0.53 ± 0.03 | <0.001 |
LF->LH | 0.68 ± 0.04 | 0.55 ± 0.03 | <0.01 |
aDistance between successive placements of the same paw
bTime between two consecutive initial placements of the same paw
cDuration of ground contact of paw
dDuration of ground contact of single hind paw
eDuration of simultaneous ground contact of two hind paws
fDistance between hind paw and ipsilateral front paw in the same step cycle
gPercentage of time with three paws on the ground
hPercentage of time with contralateral hind paw—front paw combinations on the ground
iVariation in running speed given as percentage of average running speed
jA measure of uniformity in the execution of stepping patterns (100% indicates a completely uniform running pattern)
kTemporal relationship between placement of the indicated paw pairs; the range of the indicated Cstat parameter R (strength of directedness) is between 0 and 1, with smaller values indicating larger variance of phase dispersion
Voluntary Wheel Running
To monitor voluntary wheel running activity, mice were placed individually in a home cage with a running wheel (diameter 9.2 cm, width 5.1 cm) to which they had free access. Water and food was provided, and the experiment was conducted at a 12 h light/dark cycle during four successive days and nights. Wheel revolutions per 15 min intervals were recorded by a sensor connected to a computer interface (Columbus Instruments, Columbus, OH, USA). Only mice that did run were taken into account.
Rotarod
Accelerated rotarod tests (4–40 rpm) were performed up to maximal 8 min to examine the animals’ motor coordination and motor learning skills. The rotarod apparatus (Ugo Basile, Gemonio, Italy, model 47600) had a rod of 3 cm in diameter and five lanes with a width of 5 cm each. The test is designed to measure the time mice are able to run on the accelerated rod. As soon as a mouse falls off the rod, the time is stopped automatically. After an initial training session, mice were tested for three consecutive weeks every 2–3 days. In each session, mice were tested in three trials with a recovery phase of 10 min in between. For analysis, the three trials were averaged for each mouse. After a test break of 3 weeks, mice were again tested for 3 weeks in the same pattern.
Results
Ano2 −/− mice that lacked DDI in acute tissue slices [14] were used for these experiments. The animals displayed normal cerebellar anatomy without conspicuous structural differences in the granule cell layer, the Purkinje cell layer, the molecular cell layer, and the deep cerebellar nuclei (wt: Fig. 1a–c; Ano2 −/−: Fig. 1f–h). In accordance with previous data [14], ANO2 expression was detected in Purkinje cells (Fig. 1d, i) but not in the deep cerebellar nuclei (Fig. 1e, j). During home cage observation, Ano2 −/− mice showed normal basal voluntary activities, including similar locomotion, climbing, rearing, grooming, eating, and drinking. Mean body weight increased during 6 weeks of experimentation from 25.9 ± 0.3 to 30.4 ± 0.6 g in wild-type mice and from 28.3 ± 0.7 to 31.1 ± 0.4 g in Ano2 −/−animals. In open field tests, they displayed similar explorative behavior and covered similar distances during the 5-min test period (wt 2.04 ± 0.15 m; Ano2 −/−1.79 ± 0.18 m, t test, p = 0.31). Furthermore, the grip strength test yielded similar results for muscle strength in the front limbs (wt 523 ± 32 mN; Ano2 −/− 574 ± 20 mN; n = 9, t test, p = 0.19), indicating normal muscle development. Ano2 −/− mice scored normally in SHIRPA tests for spontaneous activity, anxiety, body strength, muscle tone, and various reflexes, with the exception of the negative geotaxis test where they displayed a reduced tendency to climb upward on a vertical grid. Thus, Ano2 −/− mice appeared basically healthy and active.
To specifically examine motor coordination abilities, we first used the parallel rod floor test [22]. Mice walked spontaneously and voluntarily on a grid of steel bars while being video-taped. The distance that the animals covered in three 5-min sessions was significantly shorter in Ano2 −/− mice (Fig. 2a). In the related horizontal ladder test, mice had to walk over a series of 27 rungs, with 16 randomly positioned gaps. Ano2 −/− mice required significantly more time (Fig. 2b) and more steps (Fig. 2c) to perform this task. Although Ano2 −/− mice appeared insecure and cautious on the horizontal ladder, their forelimb and hindlimb placement was not significantly impaired. The number of slips (Fig. 2d) and corrective paw movements (Fig. 2e) was similar to wild-type animals. To closer inspect walking parameters for differences between wild-type and Ano2 −/− mice, we used the CatWalk XT gait analysis system [21]. In this voluntary walking assay, the position of each paw print was recorded and was analyzed with respect to walking patterns and leg coordination. Walking patterns of wild-type mice were typically regular and repetitive (Fig. 3a), while Ano2 −/− mice displayed a more irregular gait when walking along the test lane (Fig. 3b). Table 1 summarizes those parameters of the quantitative gait analysis that differed significantly between wild-type and Ano2 −/− mice. Briefly, the data set indicates that locomotion was slower in Ano2 −/− mice (basic locomotion in Table 1), that stepping patterns and stepping speed were different (step characteristics in Table 1), that Ano2 −/− mice displayed reduced gait regularity (gait variability in Table 1), and that the coordination of paw positioning was compromised (interpaw coordination in Table 1). Several of these differences point to cerebellar problems in Ano2 −/− mice. For example, the increased width between hindpaws is indicative of ataxia that does not depend on walking speed [24]. The increased gait variability is consistent with an earlier report on a cerebellar small-lesion model [25]. Taken together, the gait analysis produced indications of mild ataxia during spontaneous, voluntary walking in Ano2 −/− mice, a condition that appeared to enforce a slow, hesitant walking pattern.
To examine whether motor learning was also affected in the Ano2 −/− mice, each mouse was provided with a running wheel over a test period of 4 days and nights. Both wild-type and Ano2 −/− mice used the wheels during the night over a total period of 10–12 h with only short intermissions (Fig. 4a). Both mouse lines spent similar time on wheel running, displaying equally strong motivation for this activity (Fig. 4b). Ano2 −/− mice, however, reached 70% less rotations within the testing period (Fig. 4c). Moreover, while wild-type mice almost doubled their motor performance over four nights, Ano2 −/− mice were unable to increase the number of rotations (Fig. 4d). These results indicate impaired motor coordination and learning in Ano2 −/− mice in a voluntary locomotion task.
To find out whether the animals were able to learn in an enforced locomotion task, we challenged them with the accelerating rotarod test. Both wild-type and Ano2 −/− mice performed similarly well on the first day of testing (Fig. 5a). Ano2 −/− mice, however, did not significantly improve their motor skills over 3 weeks of rotarod training, while wild-type mice showed a steady learning curve and almost doubled their time on the rotarod (Fig. 5a). To test whether this learned motor skill represented consolidated procedural memory, we repeated the entire experiment after a 20-day interval. Wild-type mice regained their full rotarod performance already on the second day of rotarod training, while Ano2 −/− mice again did not improve significantly (Fig. 5b). These results demonstrate that Ano2 −/− mice have a reduced ability for motor learning in both the initial (Fig. 5c) and the repeated (Fig. 5d) period of the experiment, corroborating the evidence for cerebellar dysfunction in this mouse line.
Discussion
Cl− concentrations in dendrites are not uniform but can change locally and transiently [26–33]. The results presented here are consistent with the idea that ANO2 channels modulate Purkinje cell inhibition by MLIs and that this modulation plays a role in motor coordination and motor learning. The behavioral protocols described here are suitable for the detection of cerebellar dysfunctions. Several mouse models, mostly animals with defect Purkinje cells, display a motor phenotype in these tasks [11, 34, 35]. We thus interpret the motor problems of Ano2 −/− mice as indicative of a cerebellar problem. This view is supported by previous results from mice with genetically disabled MLI-Purkinje cell synapses. L7-Δγ2 mice do not express functional synaptic GABAA receptors in Purkinje cells [10]. These animals tend to use smaller steps on the Erasmus Ladder, a sophisticated version of the horizontal-grid task [11], and generally display only subtle changes in gait, even less conspicuous than the walking pattern observed in Ano2 −/− mice. In comparing these two mouse lines, one has to take into account that the effective removal of fast-forward inhibition in L7-Δγ2 mice constitutes a severe interference with cerebellar circuitry, which probably triggers developmental compensations to extenuate any phenotype [12]. In contrast, the removal of the modulatory pathway in Ano2 −/− mice appears to be less severe, and its effects on motor performance are not obliterated by developmental compensation.
Taken together, the lack of DDI reported previously [14] and the Ano2 −/− phenotype described here match the notion of dysregulation on the level of MLIs. ANO2 is not expressed in skeletal muscle, motoneurons, or other spinal cord neurons. Muscular dysfunction can, therefore, be ruled out as a cause for the Ano2 −/− phenotype, a conclusion supported by the normal grip strength of Ano2 −/− mice. Apart from the cerebellum, ANO2 channels are also expressed in the cilia of olfactory sensory neurons [16, 36, 37], in rod photoreceptor terminals [19, 38], in dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neurons [39], and in thalamocortical neurons [40]. Contribution to the Ano2 −/− phenotype from these structures still has to be examined. In olfaction, ANO2 ablation leads to a sensory phenotype that shows no altered sensitivity in operant conditioning experiments [16] but displays problems with the detection of novel odors [41]. Mice in our experiments were habituated to their olfactory surroundings and should, consequently, not be compromised in olfactory performance. No Ano2 −/− phenotype was reported so far for the visual system. ANO2 channels seem to be involved in the regulation of glutamate release from rod terminals under scotopic conditions [19], but the contribution of ANO2 to visual performance is not yet understood. Whether the loss of ANO2 in hippocampal neurons may contribute to the motor problems of the Ano2 −/− mice cannot be assessed at this stage. Hippocampal lesions tend to cause hyperactivity and do not reduce rotarod performance [42], whereas the Ano2 −/− mice present the opposite phenotype. Thalamocortical neurons transmit sensory information from the thalamus to the cortex. They show a distinct, ANO2-mediated type of self-inhibition that reduces their spike frequency during sensory stimulation [40]. Ano2 −/− mice respond more strongly to visceral—but not to acute—pain than wild-type mice, and it is conceivable that increased sensory perception may also affect motor performance. However, a possible link between thalamocortical self-inhibition and cerebellar function still needs to be investigated. A possible contribution to the Ano2 −/− phenotype may come from the striatum, as striatal low-threshold spiking, NPY-positive interneurons express ANO2 and appear to require the channels for maintaining membrane potential oscillations of 3–7 Hz [43]. The inhibitory interneuron circuits of the striatum serve to filter incoming signals, and their dysfunction may cause problems in motor coordination [44]. A striatal aspect of the Ano2 −/− phenotype must be examined in future when conditional ANO2 knockouts become available [45]. For the present, the global Ano2 −/− mouse provides a consistent set of results linking mild ataxia and reduced motor learning to the lack of DDI in cerebellar Purkinje cells.
The attenuation of MLI input through DDI limits GABAergic inhibition of Purkinje cells after climbing fiber stimulation, an effect that may last for several minutes [13]. Our results indicate that the loss of this process in Ano2 −/− mice causes problems in motor coordination as brought out by mildly ataxic behavior on horizontal grids, running wheels, and rotarods. However, motor memory consolidation also appeared compromised, as Ano2 −/− mice seemed to be unable to improve motor performance significantly and could not maintain the small progress they achieved in rotarod training over 2 weeks. Such impaired procedural memory may be related to the ablation of ANO2 inasmuch as the reduced disinhibition of Purkinje cells in Ano2 −/− mice attenuates the output signal that the cerebellar cortex conveys to the deep cerebellar nuclei. While this concept has not yet been tested experimentally, it is consistent with the notion that signal flow from Purkinje cells to cerebellar nuclei is necessary for memory consolidation [46]. As ANO2 appears not to be expressed in neurons of the cerebellar nuclei, the reduced motor learning ability of Ano2 −/− mice is more likely caused by impaired communication between cerebellar cortex and nuclei than by functional problems in the nuclei themselves. In any case, ANO2 channels contribute to motor coordination and motor learning in the cerebellum.
Conclusion
Behavioral experiments revealed mild ataxia and reduced motor learning in Ano2 −/− mice, which lack the calcium-dependent mode of ionic plasticity termed depolarization-induced depression of inhibition at the MLI-Purkinje cell synapses. This finding demonstrates that modulation of inhibitory input to Purkinje cells is an important component of signal processing in the cerebellar cortex. Modulation appears to protect the cells from excessive inhibition during and shortly after an excitatory signal is delivered by a climbing fiber. It precedes the long-lasting (>1 h) increase of inhibitory strength termed rebound potentiation. Thus, a biphasic modulation of synaptic strength appears to shape the GABAergic inhibition of cerebellar Purkinje cells.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (FR 937/17). We are grateful to Dr. Thomas Jentsch for providing the Ano2 -/- mouse line. We are grateful for the competent assistance of Barbara Kurpiers.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
All experiments were approved by the Regierungspräsidium Karlsruhe and were in agreement with national and international guidelines.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict interest.
References
- 1.Ito M. The cerebellum. Upper Saddle River: FT Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mittmann W, Koch U, Hausser M. Feed-forward inhibition shapes the spike output of cerebellar Purkinje cells. J Physiol. 2005;563:369–378. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.075028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dizon MJ, Khodakhah K. The role of interneurons in shaping Purkinje cell responses in the cerebellar cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31:10463–10473. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1350-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Park SM, Tara E, Khodakhah K. Efficient generation of reciprocal signals by inhibition. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107:2453–2462. doi: 10.1152/jn.00083.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hausser M, Clark BA. Tonic synaptic inhibition modulates neuronal output pattern and spatiotemporal synaptic integration. Neuron. 1997;19:665–678. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80379-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Blot A, de Solages C, Ostojic S, Szapiro G, Hakim V, Lena C. Time-invariant feed-forward inhibition of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar cortex in vivo. J Physiol. 2016;594:2729–2749. doi: 10.1113/JP271518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kano M, Fukunaga K, Konnerth A. Ca(2+)-induced rebound potentiation of gamma-aminobutyric acid-mediated currents requires activation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent kinase II. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 1996;93:13351–13356. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.23.13351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kano M, Rexhausen U, Dreessen J, Konnerth A. Synaptic excitation produces a long-lasting rebound potentiation of inhibitory synaptic signals in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature. 1992;356:601–604. doi: 10.1038/356601a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tanaka S, Kawaguchi SY, Shioi G, Hirano T. Long-term potentiation of inhibitory synaptic transmission onto cerebellar Purkinje neurons contributes to adaptation of vestibulo-ocular reflex. J Neurosci. 2013;33:17209–17220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0793-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wulff P, Schonewille M, Renzi M, Viltono L, Sassoe-Pognetto M, Badura A, Gao Z, Hoebeek FE, van Dorp S, Wisden W, et al. Synaptic inhibition of Purkinje cells mediates consolidation of vestibulo-cerebellar motor learning. Nature Neurosci. 2009;12:1042–1049. doi: 10.1038/nn.2348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Vinueza Veloz MF, Zhou K, Bosman LW, Potters JW, Negrello M, Seepers RM, Strydis C, Koekkoek SK, De Zeeuw CI. Cerebellar control of gait and interlimb coordination. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220:3513–3536. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0870-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wisden W, Murray AJ, McClure C, Wulff P. Studying cerebellar circuits by remote control of selected neuronal types with GABA(A) receptors. Front Mol Neurosci. 2009;2:29. doi: 10.3389/neuro.02.029.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Satoh H, Qu L, Suzuki H, Saitow F. Depolarization-induced depression of inhibitory transmission in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Physiol Rep. 2013;1:e00061. doi: 10.1002/phy2.61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zhang W, Schmelzeisen S, Parthier D, Frings S, Mohrlen F. Anoctamin calcium-activated chloride channels may modulate inhibitory transmission in the cerebellar cortex. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0142160. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schroeder BC, Cheng T, Jan YN, Jan LY. Expression cloning of TMEM16A as a calcium-activated chloride channel subunit. Cell. 2008;134:1019–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Billig GM, Pal B, Fidzinski P, Jentsch TJ. Ca2+-activated Cl- currents are dispensable for olfaction. Nature Neurosci. 2011;14:763–769. doi: 10.1038/nn.2821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rogers DC, Fisher EM, Brown SD, Peters J, Hunter AJ, Martin JE. Behavioral and functional analysis of mouse phenotype: SHIRPA, a proposed protocol for comprehensive phenotype assessment. Mammal Genome. 1997;8:711–713. doi: 10.1007/s003359900551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dauner K, Lissmann J, Jeridi S, Frings S, Mohrlen F. Expression patterns of anoctamin 1 and anoctamin 2 chloride channels in the mammalian nose. Cell Tiss Res. 2012;347:327–341. doi: 10.1007/s00441-012-1324-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dauner K, Mobus C, Frings S, Mohrlen F. Targeted expression of anoctamin calcium-activated chloride channels in rod photoreceptor terminals of the rodent retina. Invest Ophthal Vis Sci. 2013;54:3126–3136. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-11711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hengl T, Kaneko H, Dauner K, Vocke K, Frings S, Mohrlen F. Molecular components of signal amplification in olfactory sensory cilia. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:6052–6057. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909032107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hamers FP, Koopmans GC, Joosten EA. CatWalk-assisted gait analysis in the assessment of spinal cord injury. J Neurotrauma. 2006;23:537–548. doi: 10.1089/neu.2006.23.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kamens HM, Crabbe JC. The parallel rod floor test: a measure of ataxia in mice. Nature Prot. 2007;2:277–281. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Metz GA, Whishaw IQ. The ladder rung walking task: a scoring system and its practical application. J Visual Exp. 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 24.Batka RJ, Brown TJ, McMillan KP, Meadows RM, Jones KJ, Haulcomb MM. The need for speed in rodent locomotion analyses. Anat Rec. 2014;297:1839–1864. doi: 10.1002/ar.22955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Stroobants S, Gantois I, Pooters T, D'Hooge R. Increased gait variability in mice with small cerebellar cortex lesions and normal rotarod performance. Behav Brain Res. 2013;241:32–37. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Mohapatra N, Tonnesen J, Vlachos A, Kuner T, Deller T, Nagerl UV, Santamaria F, Jedlicka P. Spines slow down dendritic chloride diffusion and affect short-term ionic plasticity of GABAergic inhibition. Sci Rep. 2016;6:23196. doi: 10.1038/srep23196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kaila K, Price TJ, Payne JA, Puskarjov M, Voipio J. Cation-chloride cotransporters in neuronal development, plasticity and disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:637–654. doi: 10.1038/nrn3819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Glykys J, Dzhala V, Egawa K, Balena T, Saponjian Y, Kuchibhotla KV, Bacskai BJ, Kahle KT, Zeuthen T, Staley KJ. Local impermeant anions establish the neuronal chloride concentration. Science. 2014;343:670–675. doi: 10.1126/science.1245423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Delpire E, Staley KJ. Novel determinants of the neuronal Cl(-) concentration. J Physiol. 2014;592:4099–4114. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.275529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Alger BE, Nicoll RA. GABA-mediated biphasic inhibitory responses in hippocampus. Nature. 1979;281:315–317. doi: 10.1038/281315a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Staley KJ, Proctor WR. Modulation of mammalian dendritic GABA(A) receptor function by the kinetics of Cl- and HCO3- transport. J Physiol. 1999;519:693–712. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0693n.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Doyon N, Vinay L, Prescott SA, De Koninck Y. Chloride regulation: a dynamic equilibrium crucial for synaptic inhibition. Neuron. 2016;89:1157–1172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jedlicka P, Deller T, Gutkin BS, Backus KH. Activity-dependent intracellular chloride accumulation and diffusion controls GABA(A) receptor-mediated synaptic transmission. Hippocampus. 2011;21:885–898. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.van der Vaart T, van Woerden GM, Elgersma Y, de Zeeuw CI, Schonewille M. Motor deficits in neurofibromatosis type 1 mice: the role of the cerebellum. Genes Brain Behav. 2011;10:404–409. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cendelin J. From mice to men: lessons from mutant ataxic mice. Cereb Atax. 2014;1:4. doi: 10.1186/2053-8871-1-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Stephan AB, Shum EY, Hirsh S, Cygnar KD, Reisert J, Zhao H. ANO2 is the cilial calcium-activated chloride channel that may mediate olfactory amplification. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:11776–11781. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903304106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Li RC, Ben-Chaim Y, Yau KW, Lin CC. Cyclic-nucleotide-gated cation current and Ca2+−activated cl current elicited by odorant in vertebrate olfactory receptor neurons. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:11078–11087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1613891113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Stohr H, Heisig JB, Benz PM, Schoberl S, Milenkovic VM, Strauss O, Aartsen WM, Wijnholds J, Weber BHF, Schulz HL. TMEM16B, a novel protein with calcium-dependent chloride channel activity, associates with a presynaptic protein complex in photoreceptor terminals. J Neurosci. 2009;29:6809–6818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5546-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Huang WC, Xiao S, Huang F, Harfe BD, Jan YN, Jan LY. Calcium-activated chloride channels (CaCCs) regulate action potential and synaptic response in hippocampal neurons. Neuron. 2012;74:179–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ha GE, Lee J, Kwak H, Song K, Kwon J, Jung SY, Hong J, Chang GE, Hwang EM, Shin HS, et al. The Ca2+-activated chloride channel anoctamin-2 mediates spike-frequency adaptation and regulates sensory transmission in thalamocortical neurons. Nature Comm. 2016;7:13791. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pietra G, Dibattista M, Menini A, Reisert J, Boccaccio A. The Ca2+-activated Cl- channel TMEM16B regulates action potential firing and axonal targeting in olfactory sensory neurons. J Gen Physiol. 2016;148:293–311. doi: 10.1085/jgp.201611622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Goddyn H, Leo S, Meert T, D'Hooge R. Differences in behavioural test battery performance between mice with hippocampal and cerebellar lesions. Behav Brain Res. 2006;173:138–147. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Song SC, Beatty JA, Wilson CJ. The ionic mechanism of membrane potential oscillations and membrane resonance in striatal LTS interneurons. J Neurophysiol. 2016;116:1752–1764. doi: 10.1152/jn.00511.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gittis AH, Kreitzer AC. Striatal microcircuitry and movement disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:557–564. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Slugocka A, Wiaderkiewicz J, Barski JJ. Genetic targeting in cerebellar Purkinje cells: an update. Cerebellum. 2017;16:191–202. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0770-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.D'Angelo E, Mapelli L, Casellato C, Garrido JA, Luque N, Monaco J, Prestori F, Pedrocchi A, Ros E. Distributed circuit plasticity: new clues for the cerebellar mechanisms of learning. Cerebellum. 2016;15:139–151. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0711-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]