Abstract
Working memory is central to human cognition, and intensive cognitive training has been shown to expand working memory capacity in a given domain. It remains unknown, however, how the neural systems that support working memory are altered through intensive training to enable the expansion of working memory capacity. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure plasticity in activations associated with complex working memory before and after 20 days of training. Healthy young adults were randomly assigned to train on either a dual n-back working memory task or a demanding visuospatial attention task. Training resulted in substantial and task-specific expansion of dual n-back abilities accompanied by changes in the relationship between working memory load and activation. Training differentially affected activations in two large-scale fronto-parietal networks thought to underlie working memory: the executive control network and the dorsal attention network. Activations in both networks linearly scaled with working memory load before training, but training dissociated the role of the two networks and eliminated this relationship in the executive control network. Load-dependent functional connectivity both within and between these two networks increased following training, and the magnitudes of increased connectivity were positively correlated with improvements in task performance. These results provide insight into the adaptive neural systems that underlie large gains in working memory capacity through training.
INTRODUCTION
Effectively using working memory (WM) allows humans to maintain and manipulate goal-relevant information in the face of interference (Baddeley, 1992). Working memory capacity (WMC), the amount of information that an individual can hold in WM, is associated with performance on a wide range of cognitive tasks, including reasoning, problem solving, and reading comprehension (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999) and also academic performance (e.g., Finn et al., 2014; Gathercole, Pickering, Knight, & Stegmann, 2004). Although WMC has traditionally been conceptualized as a trait fixed before young adulthood, there is evidence that WMC can be increased in young adults who undergo adaptive WM training (reviewed in Klingberg, 2010). Further, it has been suggested that gains in WM training might transfer to gains in broader reasoning abilities (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Schweizer, Grahn, Hampshire, Mobbs, & Dalgleish, 2013). Although evidence for such “far transfer” after WM training is mixed (Redick et al., 2013; Thompson et al., 2013), participants consistently display impressive gains on the training task itself, typically doubling or tripling pre-training levels of performance (Jaeggi et al., 2008; Kundu, Sutterer, Emrich, & Postle, 2013; Redick et al., 2013; Thompson et al., 2013). Further, these gains are largely sustained over 6 months without further training (Thompson et al., 2013). Despite these observations, the functional brain plasticity that supports such large and enduring task-specific improvements following intensive long-term training remains poorly understood.
Although cognitive training has provoked substantial interest, previous fMRI studies of brain plasticity associated with WM training have been limited in two important ways: absence of an active control condition, and a precise definition of which neural systems exhibit functional plasticity associated with expanded WM. Some studies have shown that short-term practice (Kelly & Garavan, 2005; Landau, Garavan, Schumacher, & D’Esposito, 2007; Landau, Schumacher, Garavan, Druzgal, & D’Esposito, 2004) or longer-term practice with a non-adaptive task (Hempel et al., 2004) can modify the relationship between WM demands and fronto-parietal activation. Short-term non-adaptive training, however, does not produce large or enduring growth in WMC (Klingberg, 2010). Longer-term and adaptive WM training studies have reported disparate results after training. An early finding reported that increased WMC was associated with increased fronto-parietal activation (Olesen, Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2004), whereas most subsequent training studies have observed decreased fronto-parietal activation (Schneiders et al., 2012; Schneiders, Opitz, Krick, & Mecklinger, 2011; Schweizer et al., 2013).
Critically, none of these fMRI studies included an active control condition in which a control group performed an alternative adaptive cognitive training, so it is unknown whether previously-reported training effects were specific to WM training or if the neural changes would have been seen with any intensive training task, regardless of cognitive domain. Electroencephelography (EEG) studies with active control conditions have found electrophysiological changes in electrodes over fronto-parietal regions after WM training, including increased network connectivity and increased power in theta bands during encoding (Kundu et al., 2013; Langer, von Bastian, Wirz, Oberauer, & Jäncke, 2013) but, relative to fMRI, these EEG measures offer limited neuroanatomical specificity regarding the location of those changes.
Indeed, little is known about the mapping between the activation changes that accompany WMC expansion and the specific neural systems that support WM. Substantial neuroimaging evidence indicates that both dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and posterior parietal cortex support WM through mechanisms of persistent activation (Cohen et al., 1997; Curtis & D’Esposito, 2003; E. Smith & Jonides, 1997). Activation in these regions parametrically scales with WM load, or the amount of information that must be maintained (Braver et al., 1997; reviewed in Owen, McMillan, Laird, & Bullmore, 2005; Wager & Smith, 2003). Load-dependent activation further correlates with individual differences in WMC (McNab & Klingberg, 2008).
WM load-dependent effects on the brain are, however, spatially expansive and likely engage multiple component subsystems (Owen et al., 2005; Schweizer et al., 2013). In particular, fronto-parietal association cortex contains two distinct large-scale networks associated with WM: the “executive control network” (ECN) (identified as the “frontoparietal network” in Yeo et al., 2011, but renamed here for clarity), comprising dorsolateral and dorsomedial frontal nodes and a parietal node centered around the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and the “dorsal attention network” (DAN), comprising the human frontal eye fields (FEF) and topographically-mapped areas in the superior parietal lobe (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Power et al., 2011; Vincent, Kahn, Snyder, Raichle, & Buckner, 2008; Yeo et al., 2011). It is unknown as to whether training-related plasticity specifically occurs in one or both of these fronto-parietal systems.
Here, we examined changes in neural function associated with large, enduring, and specific expansions of WMC in a randomized controlled trial (RCT). Two groups of young adults, matched for age, IQ, and gender, were randomly assigned to one of two adaptive training programs for 4 weeks (20 sessions). One group performed WM training on a dual n-back task (Jaeggi et al., 2008) and the other group (serving as an active control) performed a similarly intensive visuospatial training task involving multiple object tracking (MOT) (Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988). A third group (serving as a passive control) performed the n-back task with the same pre-post interval, but without any training. Both training groups exhibited large and enduring task-specific gains that did not transfer between the two training tasks, while the passive group showed no improvement on either task (Thompson et al., 2013).
We examined the functional changes that were associated with the improvement in WM performance, measured both across the whole brain and specifically within the two major fronto-parietal networks. Our analyses considered changes in both the magnitude of activation associated with working memory as well as changes in functional connectivity within and between the two large-scale cortical networks. Finally, we assessed how the observed neural changes correlated with changes in task performance.
METHODS
Participants, Recruitment, and Group Assignment
Participants were recruited through web advertisements, physical flyers, and e-mail to the Northeastern University and Tufts University mailing lists. They were required to be adults between the ages of 18 and 45, right-handed, in good health, and not taking psychoactive medication. All participants provided informed written consent before participation. This study was approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Institutional Review Board (PI: Leigh Firn).
After recruitment, participants underwent pre-training behavioral testing to determine group assignment. Participants were initially sorted into one of two active training groups. Each participant was paired with another participant based on age, gender, and score on a preselected set of 18 of the 36 problems in the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) (Raven, JC, Court, JH, & Raven, J, 1998), as described in (Thompson et al., 2013). Each member of that pair was then randomly assigned to either the N-Back or the MOT training group. MOT training was selected because the intensity and magnitude of task-specific learning were comparable to n-back training in a pilot study (Thompson, Gabrieli, & Alvarez, 2010), but gains in MOT performance did not transfer to other working memory or executive function measures.
In order to control for performance improvements due to simple test-retest practice, we recruited a third matched passive control group that was examined twice with the same behavioral and neuroimaging measures and with the same interval between sessions as the training groups, but without any training. This group was recruited separately, but in the same fashion, and matched to a training pair by gender and initial RAPM score. Here, we use this Passive Control group to define independent regions of interest for functional connectivity analyses, described below. The three groups did not differ significantly by gender [X2(1, N = 37) = .21, p > .65], RAPM scores [t(1,37) < 1, p > .48], or on the full IQ score from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999), administered as part of the pre-training battery [t(1,37) < 1, p > .97]. The Passive Control group averaged 1.8 years older than the two active training groups [F(2,55) = 3.37, p < .05] (Table 1).
Table 1.
Group | N (n female) | Age (STD) | IQ (STD) | RAPM (STD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
MOT | 19 (11) | 21.3 (2.3) | 120.7 (7.0) | 13.8 (2.3) |
N-Back | 20 (13) | 21.2 (2.0) | 120.9 (10.8) | 13.4 (2.1) |
Passive Control | 19 (12) | 23.1 (3.3) | 117.6 (7.4) | 13.3 (2.2) |
Notes: Age in years. IQ measure is the Full 4 IQ measure from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. RAPM measure reflects the number of problems solved on half of the Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices (see Thompson et al., 2013 for RAPM details).
The training-based analyses reported here come from the 39 active training participants. Of those additionally recruited, 19 participants were assigned to the Passive Control group, and 14 potential participants either dropped out of the study or were excluded after initial scanning was completed. Two participants assigned to the dual n-back condition voluntarily withdrew (one after 5 days of training, the other after 9 days); no other participant had begun training when they were excluded or withdrew. The remaining potential participants were excluded for various logistical reasons, including difficulties aligning schedules with the experimenters, claustrophobia or excessive movement in fMRI scanning sessions, or repeatedly skipping pre-training appointments.
Participants were paid $20 per training session, with an additional weekly $20 bonus for completing all five training sessions in that week. Participants were paid $20 per hour for the initial and final behavioral testing sessions (approximately 3 hours each) and $30 per hour for neuroimaging sessions (2 hours pre- and post-training of fMRI, 1 hour of electroencephalography). Total compensation for each participant completing the experiment was approximately $900.
Overall experiment design
After recruitment, participants completed baseline behavioral testing (described in Thompson et al., 2013) and a pre-training imaging session that included structural scans for anatomical registration and four runs of the dual n-back task described below. They then completed twenty sessions of adaptive training on either the n-back or MOT task while at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology campus. In the dual n-back task, successful performance increased the “n” in the n-back, whereas successful performance in the MOT training task increased the speed of the tracked objects, but did not affect the number of objects to be tracked. After training was completed, behavioral testing and post-training imaging were administered as quickly as possible. The average number of days between the last training session and post-training testing was 4.3 days, with a minimum of 0 days and maximum of 14 days. This time was not significantly different between the two training groups [t(37) = .2, p > .8]).
Dual n-back Functional Imaging Task Description
Implementation of the adaptive dual n-back training task followed Jaeggi et al., 2008. An auditory letter and a visual square were simultaneously presented for 500ms, followed by a 2500ms response period. Letters were chosen from the consonants B, F, H, J, M, Q, R, and W to maximize auditory discriminability between letters. Squares were presented at one of eight positions evenly spaced around the periphery of the screen. Participants responded when one or both of the current stimuli matched a stimulus presented n trials ago. In the “0-back” condition, participants were instructed to respond to a spatial target in the upper-right corner as a visual match, or to the letter “Q” as an auditory match. In order to ensure that each participant fully understood the task, at least one block of each difficulty level was practiced outside the scanner, and participants were allowed to repeat this practice task as necessary until they reported full understanding of the instructions. In addition to the task practice before both of the pre- and post-scanning sessions, participants had completed a behavioral dual n-back testing session in the days prior to each scan which lasted approximately an hour and measured baseline performance on dual n-back loads of 1–6. This process familiarized participants with the task before scanning, and ensured that the MOT group remained familiar with the task after the training period.
Each block in the imaging version of the task presented 10 trials, containing two auditory targets and two visual targets, with no trials where both auditory and visual stimuli matched. In order to ensure a consistent level of difficulty between blocks of a given load, trials that would have matched either the n+1 or n-1 stimulus (“lure trials”) were not included. During each block, the current load was indicated at a central fixation point along with additional text labels showing the mapping between the two response buttons and “Audio” or “Video” match types.
Responses were made using a scanner-compatible button-box that the participant held in the right hand. A press of the first button, under the participant’s index finger, indicated auditory matches, and a press of the second button, under the middle finger, indicated spatial matches. No response was required on trials that did not match the target. Participants were allowed the entire 3 seconds following the onset of stimulus presentation to make a response.
Each dual n-back run consisted of eight 30-second blocks of the dual n-back task, with 2 blocks of each load from 0-back to 3-back. The order of the blocks was counter-balanced across the four total runs. Each block was preceded by a 3-second instruction screen indicating the n-back load for the upcoming block and was followed by 16 seconds of rest in order to let the hemodynamic response return to baseline. The total acquisition time for each run was 6 minutes and 36 seconds. Each participant completed 4 runs of the dual n-back task before and after training. In sum, there were 10 trials per block and 8 blocks for each load, equating to 80 total stimulus presentations at each load. In 4 cases, scanning delays prevented acquisition of the last run. This occurred once in a pre-training N-Back participant, twice in pre-training MOT participants, and once in a post-training MOT participant.
MRI Data Acquisition
Whole-brain imaging was performed on a 3T Siemens Tim Trio MRI system using a 32-channel head coil. Functional images were obtained using a standard T2*-weighted echoplanar pulse sequence (TR = 2 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, 32 slices, 3.0 × 3.0 × 3.1 mm voxels, 20% slice gap, axial interleaved acquisition). Prospective adaptive motion correction was employed to minimize the effects of participant motion. For steady state magnetization, 8 seconds of dummy scans were collected at the beginning of each run, before the experimental paradigm began. Additionally, a whole-brain high-resolution T1-weighted multiecho MP-RAGE anatomical volume was acquired for purposes of cortical surface modeling, registration to common anatomical space, and across-run alignment (TR = 2.5s, TEs = 1.64ms, 3.5ms, 5.36ms, 7.22ms, 176 slices, 2x GRAPPA acceleration, FOV = 256mm). Visual stimuli were projected onto a screen at the back of the scanner and viewed through a mirror attached to the head coil.
Data Analysis
In-scanner dual n-back performance
In order to control for response biases between participants, performance was characterized as Hit Rate – False Alarm Rate within each level from 0-back to 3-back. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were evaluated on this dependent variable with training group as a between-subjects factor and task load and session (pre- or post-training) as within-subjects factors.
Functional Imaging Analysis Procedure
Functional imaging data were processed with a workflow of FSL (Smith et al., 2004) and Freesurfer (Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, 1999) tools implemented in Nipype (Gorgolewski et al., 2011). Each timeseries was first realigned to its middle volume using normalized correlation optimization and cubic spline interpolation. Next, a mask of brain voxels was estimated to constrain later procedures. Images with artifacts were automatically identified as those frames on which total displacement relative to the previous frame exceeded 1 mm or where the average intensity within the brain mask deviated from the run mean by greater than three standard deviations. The functional data were spatially smoothed with a 6 mm FWHM kernel using the SUSAN algorithm from FSL, which restricts smoothing to voxels of similar intensity (Smith & Brady, 1997). Finally, the timeseries data were high-pass filtered by fitting and removing gaussian-weighted running lines with an effective cycle cutoff of 160 seconds.
Separately, the T1-weighted anatomical volume was processed using Freesurfer to segment the grey-white matter boundary and construct tessellated meshes representing the cortical surface (Dale et al., 1999). Functional data from each run were then registered to the anatomical volume with a 6 degree-of-freedom rigid alignment optimizing a boundary-based cost function (Greve & Fischl, 2009). The anatomical image was separately normalized to MNI152 space using FSL’s nonlinear registration algorithm (Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich, & Smith, 2012). Following these steps, the linear functional-to-anatomical transformation matrix was combined with the nonlinear anatomical normalization parameters to derive a single transformation from native run space to MNI152 space.
Parametric Load Analysis
A linear model was fit separately to each functional timeseries using Gaussian least squares with local correction for temporal autocorrelation (Woolrich, Ripley, Brady, & Smith, 2001). A single task regressor with boxcar functions indicated the task load, with 0-back, 1-back, 2-back and 3-back blocks modeled with a −3, −1, 1, and 3 weights, respectively. A separate column of 1s modeled the main effect of task. Additionally, the instruction period before each block was modeled with a separate regressor. These regressors were then convolved with the canonical difference-of-gammas hemodynamic response function from the FSL software package (Jenkinson et al., 2012). In addition to the artifact indicator vectors described above, we included regressors for the six realignment parameters used during motion correction (i.e. translations along and rotations around the three main axes in native participant space) to account for residual noise variance introduced by participant motion. Following model fitting, the contrast effect-size and standard error images were normalized to group space using the transformation described above and resampled with trilinear interpolation. Contrast effects were then combined across runs using a subject-level fixed effects model.
Mixed-effects higher-level analyses were used to model the parametric effect of load within the MOT and dual N-Back groups independently for the pre- and post-training scans. The resulting contrast estimates were than entered into a higher-level random effects model to determine both the longitudinal effect of training within each group and the group by time interaction. Correction for multiple comparisons was accomplished by first thresholding resulting whole-brain maps at a z-score of 2.3 and then cluster-correcting to control the family-wise error rate at p < .05.
Analysis of Independent Load Contrasts
This model was identical to the parametric model described above, but replaced the single parametric task regressor with four independent regressors, one for each of the n-back difficulty levels, with boxcar functions indicating the task blocks.
Regions/Networks of Interest Analysis
In addition to the whole-brain univariate approach, we also performed focused analyses within the executive control network (ECN) and dorsal attention network (DAN) regions of interest (ROIs) derived from a population atlas of task-independent cortical networks (Yeo et al., 2011). As this atlas is defined in Freesurfer’s common surface space, region labels were first warped back to the individual participant surfaces by inverting the spherical normalization parameters obtained during cortical reconstruction. Vertex coordinates within each of these labels were then transformed into the native functional space by inverting the linear functional-to-anatomical transformation for the first run. Finally, voxels were identified for inclusion within each region’s ROI mask by projecting half the distance of the cortical thickness at each vertex and labeling the intersected voxels. This method produced ROIs that reflected the underlying two-dimensional topology of the cortex and minimized the inclusion of voxels lying outside of gray matter.
Performance-Weighted Load Analysis
We further explored how BOLD activation within the ECN and DAN related to WM load using a performance-weighted analysis. In this analysis, we regressed mean BOLD activation at each n-back level against a weighted n-back variable formed by multiplying the “n” of the current level by each participant’s behavioral performance (hit rate – false alarm rate) at that level, thus scaling “n” to an estimate of the actual working memory load. As in the parametric analysis described above, the resulting regression coefficient describes the relationship between WM load and BOLD activation. To test whether training influenced this relationship, and whether that influence differed in the two large-scale networks, we applied linear mixed effects models using the R package nlme (Pinheiro, Bates, DebRoy, Sarkar, & R Core Team, 2015).
Functional Connectivity Analysis
Functional connectivity within task-activated (task-positive) regions was assessed using seed-based timeseries correlations. Connectivity analyses can be particularly sensitive to misspecification of nodes (S. Smith et al., 2011) so we constrained analyses to regions that were reliably active before training. We used data from the Passive Control group’s first scanning session, which allowed us to define regions that were statistically independent from the main analysis and to avoid the concern that the selected ROIs might be biased toward one training group’s pre-training activations. Because the greatest training-related activation changes occurred in the 2-back condition (reported below), the Passive Control group’s 2-back vs. baseline contrast was used to obtain functional connectivity seeds. The positive activations from this contrast were thresholded at p < .001, then cluster-corrected at p < .05. The resulting clusters were then intersected with an anatomical mask from the Harvard-Oxford atlas that included lateral prefrontal and parietal cortex. This approach yielded 4 ROIs: one prefrontal ROI and one parietal ROI in both the right and the left hemisphere. Each ROI was then reverse-normalized into native space using the surface-based approach described above and intersected with the subject-specific DAN and ECN masks, thus yielding 12 ROIs in total per participant (4 whole cluster ROIs, 4 DAN ROIs and 4 ECN ROIs).
For each prefrontal ROI in each participant, the experimental timecourse was extracted using the mean activity of all voxels within the ROI at each timepoint. For a given n-back level, the portions of the timecourse corresponding to seconds 8–30 of each n-back block were preserved (the first 3 volumes were omitted to avoid artifactual correlations emerging from the rising hemodynamic response), while the remainder of the timecourse was set to 0.
A general linear model was then created for each ROI using its specific n-back timecourse as the regressor of interest and including traditional block regressors for the other 3 conditions, along with the artifact indicator vectors and motion parameter nuisance regressors described above. Finally, 6 additional nuisance regressors were included representing the first 6 principal components of the white matter and CSF timecourse, which were extracted using the subject-specific Freesurfer mask of white matter and CSF. Prior to extraction, this mask was eroded by 2 voxels to avoid contamination from partial voluming with gray matter voxels.
These models were then processed as described above through the fixed effects analysis for each subject, using FSL. Finally, for each of the prefrontal ROI models, the mean parameter estimate was extracted to measure functional connectivity between that prefrontal ROI and each of the parietal ROIs. Higher-level analyses were performed on these parameter estimates using a group x session mixed-effects ANOVA separately for the 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back levels. We specifically examined connectivity between the whole-cluster ROIs and, separately, connectivity both within and between the DAN and ECN ROIs. Analyses were corrected for multiple-comparisons using Bonferroni adjustments across pairs of ROIs and, where applicable, n-back levels.
Connectivity/Behavioral Correlations
After observing training-related changes in network connectivity, we assessed the relationship between those changes and training-related changes in performance on the in-scanner dual n-back task by calculating the Pearson correlation between the change in functional connectivity and the change in behavioral performance across participants.
RESULTS
Behavioral Results
Baseline performance
Before training, increasing dual n-back loads resulted in significantly decreasing accuracy (measured as hit rate – false alarm rate) [one-way ANOVA F(3,114) = 108.3, p < .001] and increasing reaction time [F(3,114) = 125.1, p < .001] (Figure 1). There were no significant baseline differences between the two training groups on either accuracy (p’s > .11) or reaction time (p’s > .14).
Changes in performance after training
Four weeks of adaptive dual n-back training selectively improved performance on the dual n-back task in the scanner (Figure 1A). A mixed-design ANOVA examining session x n-back load x group for accuracy revealed that there were significant main effects of load [F(3,137) = 85.9, p < .001], session [F(1,37) = 30.9, p < .001], and group [F(1,37) = 5.84, p = .021], and significant interactions of group x load [F(3,111) = 6.22, p < .001], group x session [F(1,37) = 11.8, p = .002], load x session [F(3,111) = 24.4, p < .001], and group x load x session [F(3,111) = 21.3, p < .0001]. Thus, performance decreased as load increased, improved after training, and was better in the N-Back group than in the MOT group. Critically, the improvements across sessions were specific to n-back training. In mixed session x load analyses within each training group, the N-Back group performed significantly better after training [main effect of session F(1,19) = 32.8, p < .001], whereas the MOT group did not improve significantly after training [main effect of session F(1,18) = 2.7, p > .11].
In order to better understand the results from the omnibus ANOVA, session x group ANOVAs were performed separately for each load. Mixed-design ANOVAs showed that the omnibus interaction was driven by significant group x session interactions only at the dual 2- and 3-back loads in which the N-Back group became more accurate after training than did the MOT training group. [0-back F(1,37) = 2.2, p = .14; 1-back F(1,37) = 1.6, p = .21; 2-back F(1,37) = 5.8, p = .02; 3-back F(1,37) = 28.7, p < .001].
Analyses of reaction times revealed a similar pattern to that seen with accuracies. After training, the N-Back group responded significantly faster than they had before training (Figure 1B) and improved their response times more than did the MOT group. A mixed-design ANOVA examining session x load x group showed significant main effects of load [F(3,137) = 101.8, p < .001] and session [F(1,37) = 44.3, p < .001] but no main effect of group [F(1,37) = 0.02, p = .88]. However, there were significant interactions of group x load [F(3,111) = 5.33, p = .002], group x session [F(1,37) = 21.3, p < .001], load x session [F(3,111) = 30.7, p < .001], and group x load x session [F(3,111) = 20.4, p < .0001]. In mixed session x load analyses within each training group, both the N-Back group [main effect of session F(1,19) = 39.0, p < .001], and the MOT group [main effect of session F(1,18) = 5.5, p = .03] responded significantly faster after training, although the gain in speed was significantly greater in the N-Back group than the MOT group as reflected in the group x session interaction.
The omnibus interaction was most influenced by faster reaction times in the N-Back group after training at the more difficult loads. Specifically, mixed-effects group x session ANOVAs run at each n-back load independently showed that the group x session x load interaction was driven by changes in reaction time at the dual 2-back and 3-back loads [0-back F(1,37) = 1.1, p = .30; 1-back F(1,37) = 1.15, p = .29; 2-back F(1,37) = 16.7, p < .001; 3-back F(1,37) = 48.8, p < .001].
Load-dependent activations before training
Across both training groups, activations increased as a function of WM load in many regions, including lateral and medial prefrontal regions, parietal regions, anterior insula and subcortical regions in the basal ganglia and thalamus (Figure 2 and Table 2). These load-dependent activations occurred bilaterally in both the ECN and DAN (Figure 3) as characterized by a group atlas of resting-state networks (Yeo et al., 2011).
Table 2.
Cluster | Peak Z-Value | X | Y | Z | Region |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Anterior | 9.76 | 32 | 22 | 2 | Insular cortex |
9.59 | −32 | 24 | 2 | Insular cortex | |
8.81 | 30 | 10 | 60 | Middle frontal gyrus | |
8.78 | 0 | 14 | 54 | Superior frontal gyrus | |
8.69 | −46 | 24 | 30 | Middle frontal gyrus | |
8.61 | 44 | 34 | 28 | Middle frontal gyrus | |
Posterior | 10.00 | 44 | −42 | 48 | Posterior supramarginal gyrus |
9.76 | −38 | −48 | 42 | Posterior supramarginal gyrus | |
8.53 | −10 | −68 | 60 | Superior lateral occipital cortex | |
8.16 | 32 | −70 | 52 | Superior lateral occipital cortex | |
3.28 | 20 | −56 | 24 | Precuneus cortex | |
2.42 | −30 | −84 | 6 | Inferior lateral occipital cortex |
Local maxima for the linear effect of WM load, collapsing across training groups at the pre-training scan (n = 39). Statistical thresholding identified two major clusters of activation; local maxima are reported for peaks within these clusters separated by a minimum distance of 30 mm. Coordinates are reported in the FSL MNI152 space.
Training-related changes in load-dependent activation
Regions exhibiting training-dependent changes in activation were identified in a repeated measures model using the session x group interaction. The N-Back group exhibited significantly reduced load-dependent responses after training in prefrontal, parietal, and insular cortical regions (Figure 4). Training-induced changes in BOLD activation were primarily observed at 2-back and 3-back loads and were most pronounced for the 2-back load (Figure 5).
Relation of training-related reductions of activation to ECN and DAN
ROI analyses confirmed that the N-Back group exhibited a greater reduction of the relationship between WM load and activation in the ECN than in the DAN (Figure 6). We extracted activation coefficients for each n-back load from the independently defined ECN and DAN (Yeo et al., 2011). A mixed-effect ANOVA on these values revealed a significant four-way load x network x session x group interaction [F(3,111) = 9.14, p = .003]. Before training, activation in both networks increased as a function of WM load. Dual n-back training-induced activation changes in the ECN were significantly greater than those in the DAN as measured by a load x network x session interaction [F(3,57) = 25.9, p < .001], with the ECN displaying substantially reduced activations at the 2-back and 3-back loads. Consistent with the prior analyses, there was no significant change in activation in either network for the MOT training group.
Performance-weighted analyses of the relationship with WM load
An alternate approach to identifying regions responsible for task-performance directly includes participant performance in the model. These results are largely consistent with the previous load-dependent analysis, but reveal additional nuances of the training-related changes (Figure 7). BOLD responses in both the ECN [F(1,59) = 101.5, p < 0.001] and DAN [F(1,59) = 70.7, p < 0.001] were significantly and positively related to participant performance at a given load before training. In both the ECN and the DAN, training reduced the relationship between activation and load [ECN load x session interaction F(1,137) = 41.8, p < 0.001; DAN load x session interaction F(1,137) = 13.3, p < .001], but the reduction of that relationship was significantly greater in the ECN [load by session by network interaction F(1,239) = 9.7, p = 0.002]. DAN activation, unlike ECN activation, remained significantly related to WM load in the post-training session [main effect of load in DAN F(1,59) = 24.2, p < 0.001; main effect of load for ECN F(1,59) = .16, p > .68].
Fronto-parietal functional connectivity
Changes with training
We examined training-related changes in functional connectivity (BOLD timeseries correlations) during task performance between left and right prefrontal and parietal ROIs defined using pre-training activations in the passive control group. For each pair of ROIs in the dual 1, 2, and 3-back conditions, a session x group interaction measure was calculated from a mixed-effects ANOVA. Significant n-back training-induced increases in functional connectivity were observed for all 4 pairings of prefrontal and parietal ROIs in the 2-back condition (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected across load and connection), but no changes were observed in the 1- or 3-back conditions (Figure 8A). We then divided each ROI into ECN and DAN components using the Yeo network ROIs. In contrast to the asymmetric findings of the univariate analysis, in which the ECN exhibited substantively larger changes than did the DAN, this analysis revealed significantly increased connectivity between nodes of the ECN, between nodes of the DAN, and also for between-network connections (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected across connection; Figure 8B–C).
Correlations between connectivity changes and in-scanner performance
Pre-post training accuracy improvements among participants in the N-Back group in the dual 2-back scanner task were positively correlated with their increases in functional connectivity within the four fronto-parietal pairings displayed in figure 7A (r = .50, p =.03; Figure 8D). Connectivity increases were not significantly correlated with changes in reaction times (r = .20, p > .39). There were no significant correlations between accuracy improvements and univariate activation changes.
Discussion
Functional brain plasticity associated with a large increase in dual n-back performance was characterized as highly specific in multiple ways. First, learning occurred selectively in the N-Back training group, who displayed marked gains on the trained task (Thompson et al., 2013), and not in the MOT training group, who showed neither behavioral improvements nor brain plasticity associated with the dual n-back task. This is the first fMRI evidence that such plasticity is specific to WM training and not a consequence of any intensive and adaptive training program. Second, the N-Back training group showed both behavioral gains and reduced activation selectively for the WM-demanding 2-back and 3-back conditions. Third, although both the executive control (ECN) and dorsal attention (DAN) fronto-parietal networks demonstrated load-dependent activation before training, training produced a dissociation between these networks and eliminated the relationship with WM load selectively within the ECN. Fourth, the ECN and DAN both exhibited training-selective increases in functional connectivity, which were correlated with corresponding improvements in behavioral performance. This finding indicates that brain plasticity resulting from intensive WM training occurs not only in terms of activation magnitudes, but also in relation to altered network functional connectivity.
Pre-training activation in dual n-back task
The pattern of load-dependent activation at pre-training was generally consistent with prior neuroimaging studies, which have typically reported activations in dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, frontal poles, lateral and medial premotor cortex, dorsal cingulate, and medial and lateral posterior parietal cortex (Owen et al., 2005). Specific patterns of activation have varied in relation to stimulus and task dimensions, and only three fMRI studies have examined the functional activations associated with a simultaneous visual-spatial and auditory-verbal dual-back task (Buschkuehl, Hernandez-Garcia, Jaeggi, Bernard, & Jonides, 2014; Jaeggi et al., 2007; Yoo, Paralkar, & Panych, 2004). The pattern of load-dependent activation observed here, including frontal, parietal, temporal, and subcortical activations, resembles those reported in the prior dual n-back studies.
Training-related activations in dual n-back task
Functional brain changes mirrored behavioral changes in performance after training. The MOT group exhibited no improvement in dual n-back capacity despite great improvement on the MOT task (Thompson et al., 2013), and also exhibited no difference across sessions in functional activation. The N-Back group showed no gain in performance for the 0-back and 1-back conditions, presumably performing at ceiling from the outset, and exhibited no difference across sessions in functional activation at those loads. The training did, however, substantially improve performance in the more demanding 2-back and 3-back conditions, which the N-Back group performed with the same ease (measured by accuracy and reaction time) as the 0-back and 1-back conditions after training. Correspondingly, there were significant and widespread reductions in activation, most notably in bilateral inferior and middle frontal gyri, insular cortex, and intraparietal sulci.
Frontal and parietal regions were divided into two independent systems, the executive control network and the dorsal attention network, based on a group atlas of resting-state networks (Yeo et al., 2011). Activation in both networks exhibited a strong relationship with WM load in the initial session, but training produced a dissociation between the two networks. After training, activation in the DAN remained significantly related to WM load, but was no longer related to WM load in the ECN. This dissociation provides a novel perspective on the distinct roles of those fronto-parietal systems. Although both networks are thought to play a role in exerting top-down control on cognitive processing, they have been associated with different functional roles. Regions within the executive control network are characterized by the flexibility of their representational content (Duncan, 2010) and ability to sustain distractor-resistant representations of information relevant for goal-directed processing (Miller, Erickson, & Desimone, 1996; Waskom, Kumaran, Gordon, Rissman, & Wagner, 2014). Dorsal attention regions, in contrast, appear to contain prioritized topographical maps of visual space (Silver & Kastner, 2009) that are used for internally directed visual attention (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002), a possible mechanism of visual working-memory maintenance (Sreenivasan, Curtis, & D’Esposito, 2014). Our results thus suggest that training reduces the demand on higher levels of a hierarchical system that supports the maintenance and updating of active working-memory traces.
WM training studies have reported multiple patterns of functional plasticity associated with gains in WM performance, including increased activations or decreased activations. The variety of changes in activation may reflect not only the variety of tasks, but also the duration of training (ranging from a single session of training to our study of a month of intensive training) and the initial and final levels of performance. In our study, participants were considerably above chance in all conditions at the outset, improved their performance substantially in the more difficult conditions (which were far below their dual n-back capacity from the month of intensive training), and exhibited large reductions of activation in the more demanding conditions.
Training-related changes in functional connectivity
Working memory training also increased fronto-parietal functional connectivity during task performance, although these changes were observed only in the dual 2-back condition. In contrast to the changes in univariate activation, changes in within-network functional connectivity did not significantly differ between the ECN and DAN, and between-network connectivity also increased. Further, connectivity increases correlated with improvements in behavioral performance. It is unclear why connectivity (and activation) changes were modest in the 3-back condition given the robust behavioral improvements in that condition for the N-Back group. The training-induced relations between brain plasticity as measured by connectivity and gains in WM performance are consistent with EEG studies examining the WM benefits of visual-perceptual (Berry et al., 2010; Mishra, Rolle, & Gazzaley, 2015) and distractor training (Mishra, de Villers-Sidani, Merzenich, & Gazzaley, 2014).
Conclusions
Interest in the dual n-back task arises from at least two sources. First, scientifically, the task is a complex working memory task that exercises each of the putative constructs in the suite of “executive functions” (Miyake et al., 2000) – monitoring and maintenance in the encoding of incoming stimuli, inhibition in the avoidance of lure trials, and switching in the requirement of encoding stimuli from two domains simultaneously. Second, perhaps owing to the many working memory processes engaged by this task, there has been some evidence that training on the dual n-back task transfers to other cognitive domains (Au et al., 2015; Jaeggi et al., 2008; Kundu et al., 2013). This transfer could have been the result of training-induced plasticity in a common neural substrate relevant to multiple cognitive domains (Dahlin, Neely, Larsson, Bäckman, & Nyberg, 2008; Jonides, 2004). Indeed, in the present study there were large and specific functional changes in both activation and connectivity of the DLPFC and parietal brain regions known to be associated with human intelligence (Jung & Haier, 2007). Nevertheless, the robust training-induced plasticity did not support transfer to any other domain of cognition (Thompson et al., 2013).
Dual n-back training did, however, enable remarkable learning on the trained task itself, with some participants becoming able to perform a dual 9-back after 20 sessions of training. By the end of training, participants could perform the originally highly demanding 2-back and 3-back conditions with same ease (measured by speed and accuracy) as the minimally demanding 0-back and 1-back conditions. This was accompanied by large reductions of activation, specifically in the fronto-parietal executive control network, that were, by the end of training, no greater for the originally highly demanding 2-back and 3-back conditions than the minimally demanding 0-back and 1-back conditions. Further, the task-specific growth of dual n-back working memory capacity was associated with increased functional connectivity within and between the executive control and dorsal attention networks. These findings, in the context of an active control training condition, reveal the anatomically specific nature of functional brain plasticity associated with the expansion of task-specific working memory capacity. Although these particular gains did not enable far transfer to untrained measures, they do serve as a valuable exemplar of the plastic capacity of the human brain as it develops a remarkable mastery of a quite complicated and challenging task‥
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Jorie Koster-Hale and Amy Skerry for their comments and input on early drafts of this manuscript, Susan Whitfield-Gabrieli and Satrajit Ghosh for advice on analyses, and staff at the Athinoula A. Martinos Imaging Center at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research, MIT. This work was supported by The Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency contract W911QY09C0066. The views, opinions, and/or findings contained in this article are those of the author and should not be interpreted as representing the official views or policies, either expressed or implied, of the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency or the Department of Defense. Todd Thompson was supported by a training grant from the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research (T90DA022759/R90DA023427), by the Department of Defense (DoD) through the National Defense Science and Engineering Graduate Fellowship (NDSEG) program, the Sheldon Razin (1959) Fellowship at MIT and the United States Intelligence Advanced Research Projects Agency SHARP program (contract 2014-13121700007). Michael Waskom was supported as an NSF IGERT Traineeship Recipient under Award 0801700. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- Au J, Sheehan E, Tsai N, Duncan GJ, Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2015;22(2):366–377. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. Working memory. Science. 1992;255(5044):556–559. doi: 10.1126/science.1736359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry AS, Zanto TP, Clapp WC, Hardy JL, Delahunt PB, Mahncke HW, Gazzaley A. The influence of perceptual training on working memory in older adults. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(7):1–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braver TS, Cohen JD, Nystrom LE, Jonides J, Smith E, Noll DC. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. NeuroImage. 1997;5(1):49–62. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschkuehl M, Hernandez-Garcia L, Jaeggi SM, Bernard JA, Jonides J. Neural effects of short-term training on working memory. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;14(1):147–60. doi: 10.3758/s13415-013-0244-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, Smith E. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature. 1997;386(6625):604–8. doi: 10.1038/386604a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2002;3(3):201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis CE, D’Esposito M. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7(9):415–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahlin E, Neely AS, Larsson A, Bäckman L, Nyberg L. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science (New York, N.Y.) 2008;320(5882):1510–1512. doi: 10.1126/science.1155466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1980;19(4):450–466. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan J. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14(4):172–179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway ARA. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 1999;128(3):309–331. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn AS, Kraft MA, West MR, Leonard JA, Bish CE, Martin RE, Gabrieli JDE. Cognitive skills, student achievement tests, and schools. Psychological Science. 2014;25(3):736–44. doi: 10.1177/0956797613516008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Knight C, Stegmann Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2004;18(1):1–16. doi: 10.1002/acp.934. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gorgolewski K, Burns CD, Madison C, Clark D, Halchenko YO, Waskom ML, Ghosh SS. Nipype: a flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in python. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2011 Aug;5:13. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage. 2009;48(1):63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hempel A, Giesel FL, Garcia Caraballo NM, Amann M, Meyer H, Wüstenberg T, Schröder J. Plasticity of cortical activation related to working memory during training. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2004 Apr;161:745–747. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Etienne A, Ozdoba C, Perrig WJ, Nirkko AC. On how high performers keep cool brains in situations of cognitive overload. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;7(2):75–89. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.2.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105(19):6829–33. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith S. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62(2):782–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonides J. How does practice makes perfect? Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(1):10–11. doi: 10.1038/nn0104-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung RE, Haier RJ. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2007;30(02):135–154. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly AMC, Garavan H. Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15(8):1089–1102. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14(7):317–24. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kundu B, Sutterer DW, Emrich SM, Postle BR. Strengthened effective connectivity underlies transfer of working memory training to tests of short-term memory and attention. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(20):8705–8715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5565-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau SM, Garavan H, Schumacher EH, D’Esposito M. Regional specificity and practice: dynamic changes in object and spatial working memory. Brain Research. 2007;1180:78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.08.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau SM, Schumacher EH, Garavan H, Druzgal TJ, D’Esposito M. A functional MRI study of the influence of practice on component processes of working memory. NeuroImage. 2004;22(1):211–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langer N, von Bastian CC, Wirz H, Oberauer K, Jäncke L. The effects of working memory training on functional brain network efficiency. Cortex. 2013;49:2424–2438. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.008. 9Smith. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNab F, Klingberg T. Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11(1):103–107. doi: 10.1038/nn2024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller EK, Erickson CA, Desimone R. Neural mechanisms of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1996;16(16):5154–5167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-16-05154.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishra J, de Villers-Sidani E, Merzenich M, Gazzaley A. Adaptive training diminishes distractibility in aging across species. Neuron. 2014;84(5):1091–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishra J, Rolle C, Gazzaley A. Neural plasticity underlying visual perceptual learning in aging. Brain Research. 2015;1612:140–151. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(1):75–79. doi: 10.1038/nn1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25(1):46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D R Core Team. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models 2015 [Google Scholar]
- Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Petersen SE. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 2011;72(4):665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pylyshyn ZW, Storm RW. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision. 1988;3(3):179–197. doi: 10.1163/156856888X00122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J. Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychological Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Redick TS, Shipstead Z, Harrison TL, Hicks KL, Fried DE, Hambrick DZ, Engle RW. No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2013;142(2):359–379. doi: 10.1037/a0029082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneiders JA, Opitz B, Krick CM, Mecklinger A. Separating intra-modal and across-modal training effects in visual working memory: An fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(11):2555–2564. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneiders JA, Opitz B, Tang H, Deng Y, Xie C, Li H, Mecklinger A. The impact of auditory working memory training on the fronto-parietal working memory network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:173. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweizer S, Grahn J, Hampshire A, Mobbs D, Dalgleish T. Training the emotional brain: Improving affective control through emotional working memory training. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(12):5301–5311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2593-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver MA, Kastner S. Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(11):488–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith E, Jonides J. Working memory: a view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology. 1997;33(1):5–42. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1997.0658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S, Brady J. SUSAN—a new approach to low level image processing. International Journal of Computer Vision. 1997;23(1):45–78. doi: 10.1023/A:1007963824710. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S, Miller KL, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Webster M, Beckmann CF, Nichols TE, Woolrich MW. Network modelling methods for FMRI. NeuroImage. 2011;54(2):875–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sreenivasan KK, Curtis CE, D’Esposito M. Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18:82–89. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson TW, Gabrieli JDE, Alvarez GA. Adaptive Training in Multiple Object Tracking Expands Attentional Capacity. Journal of Vision. 2010;10(7):308–308. doi: 10.1167/10.7.308. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson TW, Waskom ML, Garel KLa, Cardenas-Iniguez C, Reynolds GO, Winter R, Gabrieli JDE. Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PloS One. 2013;8(5):e63614. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2008;100(6):3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wager TD, Smith E. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3(4):255–74. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.4.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waskom ML, Kumaran D, Gordon AM, Rissman J, Wagner AD. Frontoparietal representations of task context support the flexible control of goal-directed cognition. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(32):10743–10755. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5282-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady M, Smith S. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. NeuroImage. 2001;14(6):1370–86. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Buckner RL. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;106(3):1125–65. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoo S-S, Paralkar G, Panych LP. Neural substrates associated with the concurrent performance of dual working memory tasks. The International Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;114:613–631. doi: 10.1080/00207450490430561. (August 2003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]