Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2017 Dec 13;11:609. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00609

Characteristics of Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Intractable Unilateral Temporal Lobe Epilepsy Patients with Impaired Executive Control Function

Chao Zhang 1,2,, Hongyu Yang 1,2,, Wen Qin 3, Chang Liu 4, Zhigang Qi 1,2, Nan Chen 1,2,*, Kuncheng Li 1,2,*
PMCID: PMC5770650  PMID: 29375338

Abstract

Executive control function (ECF) deficit is a common complication of temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). Characteristics of brain network connectivity in TLE with ECF dysfunction are still unknown. The aim of this study was to investigate resting-state functional connectivity (FC) changes in patients with unilateral intractable TLE with impaired ECF. Forty right-handed patients with left TLE confirmed by comprehensive preoperative evaluation and postoperative pathological findings were enrolled. The patients were divided into normal ECF (G1) and decreased ECF (G2) groups according to whether they showed ECF impairment on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Twenty-three healthy volunteers were recruited as the healthy control (HC) group. All subjects underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI). Group-information-guided independent component analysis (GIG-ICA) was performed to estimate resting-state networks (RSNs) for all subjects. General linear model (GLM) was employed to analyze intra-network FC (p < 0.05, false discovery rate, FDR correction) and inter-network FC (p < 0.05, Bonferroni correction) of RSN among three groups. Pearson correlations between FC and neuropsychological tests were also determined through partial correlation analysis (p < 0.05). Eleven meaningful RSNs were identified from 40 left TLE and 23 HC subjects. Comparison of intra-network FC of all 11 meaningful RSNs did not reveal significant difference among the three groups (p > 0.05, FDR correction). For inter-network analysis, G2 exhibited decreased FC between the executive control network (ECN) and default-mode network (DMN) when compared with G1 (p = 0.000, Bonferroni correction) and HC (p = 0.000, Bonferroni correction). G1 showed no significant difference of FC between ECN and DMN when compared with HC. Furthermore, FC between ECN and DMN had significant negative correlation with perseverative responses (RP), response errors (RE) and perseverative errors (RPE) and had significant positive correlation categories completed (CC) in both G1 and G2 (p < 0.05). No significant difference of Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was found between G1 and G2, while intelligence quotient (IQ) testing showed significant difference between G1and G2.There was no correlation between FC and either MoCA or IQ performance. Our findings suggest that ECF impairment in unilateral TLE is not confined to the diseased temporal lobe. Decreased FC between DMN and ECN may be an important characteristic of RSN in intractable unilateral TLE.

Keywords: executive control function, functional connectivity, resting-state networks, temporal lobe epilepsy, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, independent component analysis

Introduction

Cognitive impairment is a common complication of epilepsy that can decrease quality of life and increase condition-associated costs (Bell et al., 2011; Keezer et al., 2016). Executive control function (ECF) has been increasingly reported as a component separate from memory, language and other cognitive abilities traditionally used for evaluating cognitive function (Royall et al., 2002). ECF deficits can lead to complex clinical symptoms in temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) patients, with devastating effects on the patient’s ability to maintain activities of daily life and possibly increasing the risk of suicide (Lin et al., 2012; Keezer et al., 2016). Early detection of ECF impairment would be useful for prompting intervention and delaying or halting the progression of functional damage (Grafman and Litvan, 1999; Lin et al., 2012; Fazel et al., 2013).

The pathogenetic mechanism of TLE with ECF impairment remains unclear (Royall et al., 2002). Both morphological and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) researches on ECF in TLE patients had conflicting results. Such as, early studies reported that the hippocampus may be a major contributor to ECF in TLE (Corcoran and Upton, 1993; Giovagnoli, 2001); however, another study believed that prefrontal cortex (PFC) was a key structure (Keller et al., 2009). Similarly, this contradiction was also found in DTI studies (Wang et al., 2007; Kucukboyaci et al., 2012; Widjaja et al., 2013). According to recent reports, the most likely explanation of ECF was that the critical determinants of ECF impairment in patients with TLE were most related to frontal lobe system abnormalities and damage to pathways linking related brain structures rather than associating with a single structure dysfunction (Martin et al., 2000; Kim et al., 2007; Tuchscherer et al., 2010; Widjaja et al., 2013). But it is still awaited to be elucidated.

A growing number of resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) studies were performed to assess resting-state networks (RSNs) in TLE patients with cognitive impairment. Cognitive function alterations in MTLE could be reflected by correlated RSN abnormalities through intra-network functional connectivity (FC) analysis, including impaired perceptual function and working memory (Zhang et al., 2009; Ibrahim et al., 2014). The result may help us understanding the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying cognitive impairments in mTLE. In addition, Vollmar et al. (2011) found the underlying mechanism of myoclonic jerks in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy was correlated with multiple structures based on functional integration analysis of RSNs. FMRI studies provided us new insight in studying the mechanism of epilepsy. To our knowledge, no study has investigated FC alteration within and between RSNs in patients with ECF impairment.

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is recognized as a “gold standard” for ECF testing and is probably the most effective measurement tool (Royall et al., 2002). The WCST consists of 13 test items to comprehensively evaluate ECF. Therefore, WCST was employed as a standard for judging ECF performance and ECF testing results by WCST was used as the grouping standard in our study (Martin et al., 2000; Royall et al., 2002; Kim et al., 2007). Based on this standard, we investigated resting-state FC changes in unilateral intractable TLE patients with and without impaired ECF.

Rs-fMRI can detect FC changes between RSNs by correlating fluctuations in blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal (Biswal et al., 1995; Power et al., 2014). Previous studies reported that resting-state FC was not only able to reflect the brain network features but also provided a promising tool for exploring pathophysiological mechanism of disease (Stam et al., 2009; Baggio et al., 2015a; Liu et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2015), furthermore, FC changes was demonstrated closely relating to cognitive impairment (Baggio et al., 2015b; Browndyke et al., 2017; Pereira et al., 2017). Importantly, understanding interactions among the whole brain network is considered to be more valuable than studying a single RSN when exploring cognitive mechanisms (Petersen and Sporns, 2015). Group-information-guided independent component analysis (GIG-ICA) can identify subject-specific independent components (ICs) across different groups with stronger independence and better spatial correspondence, making it possible to compare FC between large-scale networks among different groups (Du and Fan, 2013; Du et al., 2015). On the other hand, RSNs were demonstrated to be associated with regional blood supply and electroencephalography (EEG) patterns (Seeley et al., 2007; Liang et al., 2013; Ma et al., 2016). In the present study, we employed GIG-ICA to investigate FC alterations within and between RSNs in TLE patients with and without ECF impairment to observe the characteristics of brain network connectivity in TLE patients with ECF dysfunction and to further explore the pathogenesis of this condition.

Materials and Methods

The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Forty patients with left intractable TLE proved by comprehensive preoperative evaluation and postoperative pathological findings were enrolled (Table 1). All the patients underwent a comprehensive assessment including neuropsychological and clinical symptoms and findings from MRI, magnetoencephalography, EEG and stereo-EEG examinations. ECF performances were evaluated based on WCST results (Kim et al., 2007), including response errors (RE), perseverative responses (RP), perseverative errors (RPE), nonperseverative errors (NRPE) and categories completed (CC). Normal WCST scores for all five parameters indicated good ECF, those with scores outside acceptable ranges were considered to have ECF impairment. The acceptable range was defined based on the T-score of RP through all subjects, and the final threshold of original RP judging ECF at normal level was less than 30 (Kim et al., 2007). These findings were used to divide patients into two subgroups: normal ECF was designated as subgroup 1 (G1: 18, 12 male and 6 female; mean age: 25.3 years; range: 16–38 years; mean education: 12.6 years; range: 6–19 years) and patients with impaired ECF were in subgroup 2 (G2: 22, 10 male and 12 female; mean age: 25.7 years; range: 13–35 years; mean education: 11.5 years; range: 6–19 years). All the patients underwent routine medication, none of them have received any other relevant interventions, no significant difference of age at seizure onset and seizure duration between G1 and G2 group (Table 1). None of the patients had extra-temporal lobe brain lesions. Thirty-one patients were pathologically identified as having hippocampal sclerosis (HS). The other nine patients had neocortical TLE confirmed by postoperative pathological studies. Twenty-three healthy volunteers (13 male and 10 female; mean age: 26.7 years; range: 23–35 years; mean education: 12.9 years; range: 6–19 years) were recruited as a healthy control (HC) group and completed the same neuropsychological tests. There was no significant group difference in age, sex, or education between the HC and patient groups (Table 1).

Table 1.

Group demographics and clinical information of all subjects.

Variable G1 (N = 18) G2 (N = 22) HC (N = 23) Between-group comparisons (p values)
G1 vs. G2 G1 vs. HC G2 vs. HC
Gender (M/F) 12/6 12/10 13/10 0.725*
Age (years) 16–38 13–35 23–35 0.695*
25.3 ± 6.9 25.7 ± 5.9 26.7 ± 3.0
Education (years) 6–19 6–19 6–19 0.611*
12.6 ± 3.9 11.5 ± 4.1 12.9 ± 3.9
MoCA 18–27 15–28 19–28 0.088 0.068 0.000
23.2 ± 2.3 21.5 ± 2.7 24.8 ± 2.0
IQ 81–110 72–103 85–111 0.047 1.000 0.002
93.4 ± 2.3 87.1 ± 9.2 95.5 ± 6.4
Age at seizure onset 5–30 3–24 N/A 0.326 N/A N/A
12.4 ± 7.1 10.4 ± 6.2 N/A
Duration of TLE 4–23 3–30 N/A 0.300 N/A N/A
13.1 ± 6.1 15.2 ± 6.8 N/A

M, male; F, female; Edu, education; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; IQ, intelligence quotient; G1, subgroup 1; G2, subgroup 2; HC, healthy controls; N/A, not applicable. Data are presented as range and mean ± SD. *The value represent the comparison between three groups.

To avoid the influence of confounding factors that may also affect executive function, the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) for cognitive performance and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) to measure the intelligence quotient (IQ) were also administered.

MRI Data Acquisition

All participants were scanned on a 3.0T Magneton Trio Tim MRI scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel phase-array head coil. Conventional brain axial fluid-attenuated inverse recovery (FLAIR) sequence scanning was performed to exclude other brain abnormalities before rs-fMRI. Tight but comfortable foam padding was used to minimize head motion, and the subjects wore earplugs to reduce imaging noise. The participants were asked to stay calm, close their eyes, breath smoothly and avoid thinking of anything specific. Resting BOLD images were acquired using an echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: time of repetition/time of echo = 2000/30 ms, field of view = 220 mm × 220 mm, GRAPPA (PE; GeneRalized Autocalibrating Partial Parallel Acquisition phase encoding) 2, slice thickness = 3 mm, voxel size = 3.4 mm × 3.4 mm × 3.0 mm, 35 slices, flip angle = 90°, and total acquisition time = 6:08 min and 180 volumes. T1-weighted images were also acquired using a 3D-MP-RAGE (three-dimensional three-dimension magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo) providing isotropic voxels of 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm.

Data Preprocessing

The rs-fMRI data for all subjects were preprocessed using data Processing and Analysis for (Resting-State) Brain Imaging (DPABI; Yan et al., 2016). The first 10 volumes of each functional time series were removed since the signals had not yet reached equilibrium. Slice timing was performed to correct differences between all slices, and the other volumes were then realigned with the first. The Friston-24 model was used for individual-level head motion correction for all subjects, and any subject who had a maximum displacement >2 mm, a maximum rotation >2.0°, or mean framewise displacement (FD) >0.3 was excluded from the group. In addition, we set mean FD as a covariate for the group-level statistics of our study to minimize the impact of potential head movements (Yan et al., 2013). The fMRI data were then normalized to the standard echo planar imaging template and resampled to 3-mm cubic voxels. Finally, fMRI data were smoothed (6-mm full width at half maximum). As described in previous studies, we did not choose global signal regression for our data (Scholvinck et al., 2010; Chai et al., 2012; Saad et al., 2012; Yang et al., 2014).

Independent Component Analysis

After data preprocessing, ICs for all subjects were analyzed through GIG-ICA using GIFT software (version 2.0a)1. Three steps for fMRI data decomposition were based on this toolbox: (i) data reduction; (ii) applying the ICA algorithm; and (iii) back-reconstruction for individual-level components. Finally, 24 ICs were auto-estimated though MELODIC criteria and 100 times with GICA. Eleven meaningful ICs were identified as functionally related RSNs via visual inspection and previous reports related to RSNs. The subject-specific spatial maps were transformed to z scores.

Statistical Analysis of Neuropsychological Tests

To eliminate the potential impact of other variables as much as possible, we employed a general linear model (GLM) analysis of neuropsychological tests with age, sex and education as covariates to identify significant differences between the HC and patient groups (Table 1). Pearson correlations between FC, MoCA and IQ tests were also calculated though partial correlation analysis with age, sex and education as covariates (p < 0.05).

Functional Network Connectivity Analysis

First, we removed head motion parameters, BOLD signals of the ventricles, white matter, and the whole brain as nuisance covariates from the datasets. Then, a random-effect one-sample t-test was performed on each meaningful IC in statistical parametric mapping (SPM) 8 using a false discovery rate (FDR) correction (p < 0.05 and T = 8) and cluster size of 100 voxels to create a sample-specific component map (Supplementary Figure S1; Song et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2014).

For each individual, the mean time course of each RSN was obtained by averaging the time course of all voxels within each sample-specific RSN mask. Pearson correlation coefficients of the mean time courses between each pair of RSNs of individuals were calculated and then converted to z values though Fisher’s r-to-z transformation to improve normality. The next step, a random effect one-sample t-test was used to test individuals’ z values to determine whether a correlation between each pair of RSNs of each group was statistically significant (p < 0.05). Intergroup comparisons were only performed on the condition that inter-network FC reached statistical significance in each group. A GLM with age, sex, education, IQ and mean FD as covariates was used to analyze which pairs of inter-network FC components were significantly different between G1, G2 and HC, and post hoc contrasts were tested to observe inter-network FC differences between each pair of groups (p < 0.05, Bonferroni correction). Pearson correlations between FC and each WCST parameter were calculated though partial correlation with age, sex, education, IQ and mean FD as covariates (p < 0.05). Statistical tests were carried out in SPSS version 16 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Intra-Network FC of RSNs Analysis

Intra-network FC differences of all RSNs extracted in this study were also tested using a GLM analysis with age, sex, education, IQ and mean FD as covariates in a voxel-wise manner by SPM. Multiple comparisons were corrected using family-wise error correction (FDR correction; p < 0.05) through SPM.

Results

RSNs

A total of 24 components were extracted by GIG-ICA. The 11 meaningful RSNs across all subjects were the dorsal sensorimotor-related network (dSMN), executive control network (ECN), occipital pole visual network (pVN), medial VN (mVN), anterior default-mode network (aDMN), left frontoparietal network (lFPN), right FPN (rFPN), visuospatial network (VSN), ventral SMN (vSMN), DMN and posterior part of DMN (pDMN), that all of the RSNs were consistent with previous reports (Supplementary Figure S1; Mantini et al., 2007; Seeley et al., 2007; Yeo et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2014; Du et al., 2015; Raichle, 2015; de Campos et al., 2016; Ma et al., 2016).

FC Changes

Comparison of intra-network FC of all 11 meaningful RSNs showed no significant FC difference in each RSNs among the three groups (FDR correction, p > 0.05).

As for inter-network comparison, we only found a significant FC difference between the DMN and ECN (p = 0.000) among G1, G2 and HC. G2 showed significantly decreased FC between the DMN and ECN when compared with G1 and HC (Bonferroni corrected, both p < 0.05). There was no significant difference between G1 and HC (p = 1.000; Figure 1). There was no significant difference of FC between any other RSN pair extracted in the present study.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Comparisons of inter-network FC alterations between the ECN and DMN among HC, G1 and G2. G2 exhibited decreased FC between the ECN and DMN compared with G1 and HC (both p = 0.000, Bonferroni corrected); G1 showed no significant difference of FC between the ECN and DMN compared with HC. DMN, default-mode network; ECN, executive control network; FC, functional connectivity; HC, healthy control; G1, subgroup 1; G2, subgroup 2.

Neuropsychological Test Score Correlations

The mean MoCA score of HC was 24.8 ± 2.0 and the range was 19–28; and the score of G1 and G2 was 23.2 ± 2.3 and 21.5 ± 2.7, the range was 18–27 and 15–28, respectively.

A GLM analysis for MoCA and IQ with age, sex, and education as covariates showed significant differences in MoCA scores between G2 and HC, however, there was no significant difference between G1 and HC or G1 and G2 (Table 1). For IQ analysis, significant differences were found between G2 and HC as well as between G1 and G2, and there was no significant difference between G1 and HC (Table 1). Additionally, we did not find any significant correlation between FC and MoCA (p = 0.444, r = −0.214 and p = 0.974, r = 0.008) or IQ (p = 0.401, r = 0.234 and p = 0.156, r = −0.339) when comparing G1 and G2 (Figure 2). Significant difference of IQ may effect on the ECF performance (Sherman et al., 2014). Therefore, IQ effect was included as a covariate in statistical analysis of FC.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Correlations of FC between ECN and DMN with Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) scores in all subjects. FC between the ECN and DMN was significantly negatively correlated with perseverative RE (G1: p = 0.034, r = −0.501; G2: p = 0.006, r = −0.568; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = −0.727; A), RPE (G1: p = 0.013, r = −0.575; G2: p = 0.012, r = −0.527; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = −0.659; B), RP (G1: p = 0.049, r = −0.470; G2: p = 0.037, r = −0.447; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = −0.688; C) and had significant positive correlation with CC (G1: p = 0.034, r = 0.503; G2: p = 0.009, r = 0.543; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = 0.719; D). For HC analysis, no significant correlation was found between FC with WCST scores (p > 0.05; A–D). CC, categories completed; DMN, default-mode network; ECN, executive control network; FC, functional connectivity; HC, healthy controls; G1, subgroup 1; G2, subgroup 2; RE, response errors; RP, perseverative responses; RPE, perseverative errors.

For the correlation analysis between ECN/DMN FC and neuropsychological test score, we found that FC between the ECN and DMN was significantly negatively correlated with perseverative RE (G1: p = 0.034, r = −0.501; G2: p = 0.006, r = −0.568; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = −0.727), RPE (G1: p = 0.013, r = −0.575; G2: p = 0.012, r = −0.527; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = −0.659), RP (G1: p = 0.049, r = −0.470; G2: p = 0.037, r = −0.447; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = −0.688) and had significant positive correlation with CC (G1: p = 0.034, r = 0.503; G2: p = 0.009, r = 0.543; all subjects: p = 0.000, r = 0.719). For HC analysis, no significant correlation was found between FC with WCST scores (p > 0.05; Figure 2).

Discussion

In this study, we found significantly decreased FC between the ECN and DMN in ECF-impaired TLE patients compared with TLE patients without ECF impairment and HCs. Furthermore, we found that FC between the ECN and DMN is significantly positively correlated with ECF. Although MoCA and IQ performance were different between patients with and without ECF impairment, no correlation was detected between FC and these test scores in either subgroup.

TLE with ECF Impairment

Abnormal FC between the ECN and DMN in TLE patients with ECF impairment is consistent with the point of view that it is related to brain network dysfunction (Bernhardt et al., 2011; DeSalvo et al., 2014; Vaughan et al., 2016). Our results were also similar to the hypothesis of previous structural studies that multiple brain regions were involved in TLE patients with cognitive impairment (Kim et al., 2007; Tuchscherer et al., 2010; Widjaja et al., 2013; Leyden et al., 2015). There was still lack of elaboration focusing on the relationship between abnormal structures associating with ECF impairment in previous researches. Furthermore, ECF dysfunction related networks had not been determined yet in TLE patients. In this study, we investigated FC alterations in RSNs between subgroups of TLE patients with and without ECF impairment, and we also evaluated the relationship between FC and ECF performance. We found no significant intra-network FC difference between patients with and without ECF impairment, the inter-network analysis revealed diminished FC between the ECN and DMN in patients with poor ECF performance. This may indicate that functional integration of the whole brain network may be more sensitive than functional changes in a single RSN in detecting ECF impairment from the full TLE patient group in this study (Petersen and Sporns, 2015), and abnormal FC between ECN and DMN may reflect the brain network characteristics of ECF impairment in TLE.

Our study identified two core areas of the DMN and ECN associated with ECF. The DMN is the most well-studied RSN and covers the precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex (PCC); medial PFC (MPFC); and medial, lateral, and inferior parietal cortex (IPC; Mantini et al., 2007; Brewer et al., 2011; Mohan et al., 2016). The DMN is important for cognitive performance including language ability (Oser et al., 2014), episodic memory capacity (McCormick et al., 2014) and attentional control (Jiang et al., 2017). A few studies have reported functional organization abnormalities of the DMN in TLE patients with ECF impairment (Bossong et al., 2013; Crittenden et al., 2015). The ECN is accepted as being closely involved in cognitive domains including memory, emotion, and behavior (Seeley et al., 2007; Smith et al., 2009). The DLPFC, as the core area of ECN, was considered to be more vital for ECF (Miller and Cohen, 2001). And, ECF impairment was demonstrated associating with ECN abnormal in different types of imaging studies (Takaya et al., 2006; Keller et al., 2009; Dong et al., 2015). In previous studies of TLE, ECN with both structural and functional impairments have been revealed in the patient group when compared with HCs. Such as, ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices and frontal operculum of ECN were demonstrated atrophy though voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis (Cataldi et al., 2013). Furthermore, PFC atrophy was recognized as a key factor in TLE with ECF deficits though VBM anaylsis either (Keller et al., 2009). Evidence from DTI research also supported this view, which found damaged white matter fiber connections between PFC and caudate had negative correlation with ECF performance (Kortte et al., 2002). FMRI study revealed FC decreased of ECN in TLE patients appearing with working memory impairment (Vlooswijk et al., 2011). In addition, PFC was also involved in TLE patients with cognitive impairment (Martin et al., 2000). These studies indicated that ECN played an important role in TLE with neuropsychological performance abnormalities. However, we did not find a significant difference in intra-network FC of the ECN and DMN among the three groups in our study. Unlike previous investigations, we divided patients into subgroups depending on ECF impairment, which allowed us to assess special FC changes associated with ECF impairment in TLE. Neuropsychological abnormalities of patients in our study were not only ECF dysfunction, but also included cognitive impairment as the result mentioned. It would be more conducive to remove other confounding factors potentially leading to alterations of RSNs, such as the impacts of the disease itself and other cognitive dysfuntion in exploring the ECF impairment specifically. Therefore, we made a comparison between patients with and without ECF impairment rather than that between patients and healthy volunteers. In addition, we found no significant difference of clinical features, such as age at seizure onset and seizure duration between G1 and G2 group. No significant intra-network FC difference was found between subgroups of patient that may due to the similar disruption patterns of each RSN in TLE with the similar clinical features. However, we only noted decreased inter-network FC between the ECN and DMN in patients with ECF impairment. We therefore inferred that ECF performance in TLE patients is also influenced by the functional integration of multiple networks (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Zamarian et al., 2011; Petersen and Sporns, 2015).

We employed the data-driven method of GIG-ICA to extract RSNs and calculate FC between different RSN pairs. Our findings were similar to a previous report of decreased connectivity between the DMN and ECN in left TLE patients (de Campos et al., 2016). The probable explanation was that left TLE patients are more susceptible to bilateral and diffuse functional organization changes due to both ictal and interictal activities (Voets et al., 2012; DeSalvo et al., 2014; Taylor et al., 2015), which may result in low interactions between large-scale networks and further lead to irreversible cognitive dysfunction (Hamberger et al., 2007; de Campos et al., 2016). Previous studies of FC between large-scale networks did not further subdivide the patients, which may have prevented them from noting differences. Our results show that FC between the ECN and DMN may help to identify TLE patients with ECF impairment. The DMN extracted from our data contained the bilateral MPFC and precuneus gyrus, and the MPFC is considered an especially important component (Broyd et al., 2009). The ECN from our data mainly covered the bilateral middle frontal gyrus and dorsolateral superior frontal gyrus, which were the major components of dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) that was recognized as a core structure of the ECN (Seeley et al., 2007). PFC is a well-established structure for diverse information exchange and convergence to accomplish its role of cognitive control (Miller and Cohen, 2001). Thus, abnormal information transmission within the PFC could affect the function of whole PFC systems that may lead to ECF impairment. The MPFC was demonstrated closely related to emotion-based decision-making (Watanabe, 2016). The DLPFC was recognized as a central component to controlling behavior (Seeley et al., 2007). Since the MPFC and DLPFC are closely related to ECF (Royall et al., 2002), we assumed that decreased interaction between them may play a key role in ECF impairment due to TLE. In addition, previous studies indicated that working memory was a common basis of different executive functions among ECF components such as updating, shifting and inhibition (Miyake et al., 2000; Baddeley, 2001; Collette and Van der Linden, 2002). Besides, episodic memory capacity was proved positively correlated with DMN activity (McCormick et al., 2014). Decreased FC between the DMN and ECN represent reduced interaction between the two regions, so we concluded that damage to extra-temporal connections between two core RSNs involved in ECF and memory capacity may be another important element influencing ECF impairment in TLE (Keller and Roberts, 2008; Putcha et al., 2015).

Besides, we also found that FC between the ECN and DMN was significantly negatively correlated with RP, RE and RPE and positively correlated with CC in all TLE patients, suggesting that this FC pattern was positively correlated with ECF performance. Abnormal WCST scores are indicative of frontal lobe function damage, possibly impaired interactions between the MPFC of the DMN and the DLPFC of the ECN (Miller and Cohen, 2001), but this requires further study. However, our results demonstrated that FC between the ECN and DMN is significantly correlated with ECF performance. This FC pattern may be an important imaging feature reflecting alterations of RSNs in TLE patients with ECF impairment.

Finally, FC between the DMN and ECN was not significantly correlated with MoCA or IQ test scores in either TLE group in our study, suggesting that the abnormal FC was due to the disease itself, rather than other common factors of the patients.

Limitations

Several limitations should be noted in our study. First, our design did not allow us to control for confounding effects of antiepileptic drugs, which may affect ECF performance. Second, we only included patient with left-hemisphere TLE, it would be more comprehensive research to include subjects with right-hemisphere TLE. Third, our study is based on group-level analysis, making it difficult to apply our findings to individual patients. The fourth, we did not use Bonferroni correction to control for multiple comparisons on evaluating FC with WSCT correlations. In the future, analyzing TLE patients with ECF on an individual level will be more helpful to guide clinical management.

Conclusion

Our results show that ECF performance in patients with unilateral TLE is associated with functional integration involving multiple brain regions beyond the diseased temporal lobe. Decreased FC between the DMN and ECN was uncovered in TLE with ECF impairment, suggesting that this alteration may be a specific feature of ECF impairment due to epileptic events. Disturbed interaction between the DMN and ECN may be an underlying pathological mechanism of ECF impairment in patients with intractable unilateral left TLE.

Author Contributions

CZ and HY: conception/design of the work; acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data for the work; drafting of the work; final approval of the version to be published and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work. WQ, ZQ and CL: data analysis, data for the work; drafting of the work; final approval of the version to be published and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work. NC and KL: design of the work; revision of the work; final approval of the version to be published and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (code 81471649), Beijing Municipal Administration of Hospitals Clinical Medicine Development of Special Funding Support (code ZYLX201609), the Capital Health Research and Development of Special Project (code 2016-1-2011), and also by Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (code Z171100000117001).

Glossary

Abbreviations

BOLD

blood oxygen level-dependent

CC

categories completed

DTI

diffusion tensor imaging

ECF

executive control function

ECN

executive control network

FC

functional connectivity

FD

framewise displacement

FDR

false discovery rate

FLAIR

fluid-attenuated inverse recovery

FPN

frontoparietal network

GIG-ICA

group-information-guided independent component analysis

GLM

general linear model

HC

healthy control

HS

hippocampal sclerosis

ICs

independent components

IQ

intelligence quotient

MoCA

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

NRPE

nonperseverative errors

PFC

prefrontal cortex

RE

response errors

RP

perseverative responses

RPE

perseverative errors

rs-fMRI

resting-state fMRI

RSN

resting-state network

SMN

sensorimotor-related network

SPM

statistical parametric mapping

TLE

temporal lobe epilepsy

VBM

voxel-based morphometry

VN

visual network

VSN

visuospatial network

WAIS

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

WCST

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

Footnotes

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00609/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Baddeley A. D. (2001). Is working memory still working? Am. Psychol. 56, 851–864. 10.1037/0003-066x.56.11.851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baggio H. C., Segura B., Junque C. (2015a). Resting-state functional brain networks in Parkinson’s disease. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 21, 793–801. 10.1111/cns.12417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baggio H. C., Segura B., Sala-Llonch R., Marti M. J., Valldeoriola F., Compta Y., et al. (2015b). Cognitive impairment and resting-state network connectivity in Parkinson’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 199–212. 10.1002/hbm.22622 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bell B., Lin J. J., Seidenberg M., Hermann B. (2011). The neurobiology of cognitive disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 7, 154–164. 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bernhardt B. C., Chen Z., He Y., Evans A. C., Bernasconi N. (2011). Graph-theoretical analysis reveals disrupted small-world organization of cortical thickness correlation networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2147–2157. 10.1093/cercor/bhq291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Biswal B., Yetkin F. Z., Haughton V. M., Hyde J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. 10.1002/mrm.1910340409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bossong M. G., Jansma J. M., van Hell H. H., Jager G., Kahn R. S., Ramsey N. F. (2013). Default mode network in the effects of ∆9-Tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) on human executive function. PLoS One 8:e70074. 10.1371/journal.pone.0070074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brewer J. A., Worhunsky P. D., Gray J. R., Tang Y. Y., Weber J., Kober H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 108, 20254–20259. 10.1073/pnas.1112029108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Browndyke J. N., Berger M., Harshbarger T. B., Smith P. J., White W., Bisanar T. L., et al. (2017). Resting-state functional connectivity and cognition after major cardiac surgery in older adults without preoperative cognitive impairment: preliminary findings. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 65, e6–e12. 10.1111/jgs.14534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broyd S. J., Demanuele C., Debener S., Helps S. K., James C. J., Sonuga-Barke E. J. (2009). Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 279–296. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cataldi M., Avoli M., de Villers-Sidani E. (2013). Resting state networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 54, 2048–2059. 10.1111/epi.12400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chai X. J., Castañón A. N., Ongür D., Whitfield-Gabrieli S. (2012). Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage 59, 1420–1428. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Collette F., Van der Linden M. (2002). Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 105–125. 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00063-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Corcoran R., Upton D. (1993). A role for the hippocampus in card sorting? Cortex 29, 293–304. 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80182-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crittenden B. M., Mitchell D. J., Duncan J. (2015). Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. Elife 4:e06481. 10.7554/eLife.06481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. de Campos B. M., Coan A. C., Lin Yasuda C., Casseb R. F., Cendes F. (2016). Large-scale brain networks are distinctly affected in right and left mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3137–3152. 10.1002/hbm.23231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. DeSalvo M. N., Douw L., Tanaka N., Reinsberger C., Stufflebeam S. M. (2014). Altered structural connectome in temporal lobe epilepsy. Radiology 270, 842–848. 10.1148/radiol.13131044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dong G., Lin X., Potenza M. N. (2015). Decreased functional connectivity in an executive control network is related to impaired executive function in Internet gaming disorder. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 76–85. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.10.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Du Y., Fan Y. (2013). Group information guided ICA for fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 69, 157–197. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Du Y., Pearlson G. D., Liu J., Sui J., Yu Q., He H., et al. (2015). A group ICA based framework for evaluating resting fMRI markers when disease categories are unclear: application to schizophrenia, bipolar, and schizoaffective disorders. Neuroimage 122, 272–280. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fazel S., Wolf A., Långström N., Newton C. R., Lichtenstein P. (2013). Premature mortality in epilepsy and the role of psychiatric comorbidity: a total population study. Lancet 382, 1646–1654. 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60899-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Giovagnoli A. R. (2001). Relation of sorting impairment to hippocampal damage in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia 39, 140–150. 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00104-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Grafman J., Litvan I. (1999). Importance of deficits in executive functions. Lancet 354, 1921–1923. 10.1016/s0140-6736(99)90438-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hamberger M. J., Palmese C. A., Scarmeas N., Weintraub D., Choi H., Hirsch L. J. (2007). A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil to improve memory in epilepsy. Epilepsia 48, 1283–1291. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01114.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ibrahim G. M., Morgan B. R., Lee W., Smith M. L., Donner E. J., Wang F., et al. (2014). Impaired development of intrinsic connectivity networks in children with medically intractable localization-related epilepsy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5686–5700. 10.1002/hbm.22580 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jiang Y., Xia J., Li S., Chen J., Wang P., Chen Q. (2017). Neural dynamics underlying varying attentional control facing invariant cognitive task upon invariant stimuli. Neuroscience 353, 133–146. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Keezer M. R., Sisodiya S. M., Sander J. W. (2016). Comorbidities of epilepsy: current concepts and future perspectives. Lancet Neurol. 15, 106–115. 10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00225-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Keller S. S., Baker G., Downes J. J., Roberts N. (2009). Quantitative MRI of the prefrontal cortex and executive function in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 15, 186–195. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Keller S. S., Roberts N. (2008). Voxel-based morphometry of temporal lobe epilepsy: an introduction and review of the literature. Epilepsia 49, 741–757. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01485.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kim C. H., Lee S. A., Yoo H. J., Kang J. K., Lee J. K. (2007). Executive performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Eur. Neurol. 57, 39–46. 10.1159/000097009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kortte K. B., Horner M. D., Windham W. K. (2002). The trail making test, part B: cognitive flexibility or ability to maintain set? Appl. Neuropsychol. 9, 106–109. 10.1207/s15324826an0902_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kucukboyaci N. E., Girard H. M., Hagler D. J., Jr., Kuperman J., Tecoma E. S., Iragui V. J., et al. (2012). Role of frontotemporal fiber tract integrity in task-switching performance of healthy controls and patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 18, 57–67. 10.1017/S1355617711001391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Leyden K. M., Kucukboyaci N. E., Puckett O. K., Lee D., Loi R. Q., Paul B., et al. (2015). What does diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tell us about cognitive networks in temporal lobe epilepsy? Quant. Imaging Med. Surg. 5, 247–263. 10.3978/j.issn.2223-4292.2015.02.01 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Liang X., Zou Q., He Y., Yang Y. (2013). Coupling of functional connectivity and regional cerebral blood flow reveals a physiological basis for network hubs of the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 1929–1934. 10.1073/pnas.1214900110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lin J. J., Mula M., Hermann B. P. (2012). Uncovering the neurobehavioural comorbidities of epilepsy over the lifespan. Lancet 380, 1180–1192. 10.1016/s0140-6736(12)61455-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Liu F., Guo W., Fouche J. P., Wang Y., Wang W., Ding J., et al. (2015). Multivariate classification of social anxiety disorder using whole brain functional connectivity. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 101–115. 10.1007/s00429-013-0641-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ma Y., Shaik M. A., Kozberg M. G., Kim S. H., Portes J. P., Timerman D., et al. (2016). Resting-state hemodynamics are spatiotemporally coupled to synchronized and symmetric neural activity in excitatory neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 113, E8463–e8471. 10.1073/pnas.1525369113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mantini D., Perrucci M. G., Del Gratta C., Romani G. L., Corbetta M. (2007). Electrophysiological signatures of resting state networks in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 13170–13175. 10.1073/pnas.0700668104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Martin R. C., Sawrie S. M., Gilliam F. G., Palmer C. A., Faught E., Morawetz R. B., et al. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting performance in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: clinical and neuroanatomical correlates. Epilepsia 41, 1626–1632. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2000.01626.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McCormick C., Protzner A. B., Barnett A. J., Cohn M., Valiante T. A., McAndrews M. P. (2014). Linking DMN connectivity to episodic memory capacity: what can we learn from patients with medial temporal lobe damage? Neuroimage Clin. 5, 188–196. 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Miller E. K., Cohen J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A., Wager T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mohan A., Roberto A. J., Mohan A., Lorenzo A., Jones K., Carney M. J., et al. (2016). The significance of the default mode network (DMN) in neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders: a review. Yale J. Biol. Med. 89, 49–57. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Oser N., Hubacher M., Specht K., Datta A. N., Weber P., Penner I. K. (2014). Default mode network alterations during language task performance in children with benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS). Epilepsy Behav. 33, 12–17. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Pereira L., Airan R. D., Fishman A., Pillai J. J., Kansal K., Onyike C. U., et al. (2017). Resting-state functional connectivity and cognitive dysfunction correlations in spinocerebelellar ataxia type 6 (SCA6). Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 3001–3010. 10.1002/hbm.23568 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Petersen S. E., Sporns O. (2015). Brain networks and cognitive architectures. Neuron 88, 207–219. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Power J. D., Schlaggar B. L., Petersen S. E. (2014). Studying brain organization via spontaneous fMRI signal. Neuron 84, 681–696. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Putcha D., Ross R. S., Cronin-Golomb A., Janes A. C., Stern C. E. (2015). Altered intrinsic functional coupling between core neurocognitive networks in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage Clin. 7, 449–455. 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.01.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Raichle M. E. (2015). The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 433–447. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Royall D. R., Lauterbach E. C., Cummings J. L., Reeve A., Rummans T. A., Kaufer D. I., et al. (2002). Executive control function: a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. A report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 14, 377–405. 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.14.4.377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Saad Z. S., Gotts S. J., Murphy K., Chen G., Jo H. J., Martin A., et al. (2012). Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect. 2, 25–32. 10.1089/brain.2012.0080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Scholvinck M. L., Maier A., Ye F. Q., Duyn J. H., Leopold D. A. (2010). Neural basis of global resting-state fMRI activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 10238–10243. 10.1073/pnas.0913110107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Seeley W. W., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F., Keller J., Glover G. H., Kenna H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sherman L. E., Rudie J. D., Pfeifer J. H., Masten C. L., McNealy K., Dapretto M. (2014). Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: a longitudinal study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 148–159. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Smith S. M., Fox P. T., Miller K. L., Glahn D. C., Fox P. M., Mackay C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 13040–13045. 10.1073/pnas.0905267106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Song J., Qin W., Liu Y., Duan Y., Liu J., He X., et al. (2013). Aberrant functional organization within and between resting-state networks in AD. PLoS One 8:e63727. 10.1371/journal.pone.0063727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stam C. J., de Haan W., Daffertshofer A., Jones B. F., Manshanden I., van Cappellen van Walsum A. M., et al. (2009). Graph theoretical analysis of magnetoencephalographic functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 132, 213–224. 10.1093/brain/awn262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Takaya S., Hanakawa T., Hashikawa K., Ikeda A., Sawamoto N., Nagamine T., et al. (2006). Prefrontal hypofunction in patients with intractable mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 67, 1674–1676. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000242628.26978.e2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Taylor P. N., Han C. E., Schoene-Bake J. C., Weber B., Kaiser M. (2015). Structural connectivity changes in temporal lobe epilepsy: spatial features contribute more than topological measures. Neuroimage Clin. 8, 322–328. 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tuchscherer V., Seidenberg M., Pulsipher D., Lancaster M., Guidotti L., Hermann B. (2010). Extrahippocampal integrity in temporal lobe epilepsy and cognition: thalamus and executive functioning. Epilepsy Behav. 17, 478–482. 10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.01.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Vaughan D. N., Rayner G., Tailby C., Jackson G. D. (2016). MRI-negative temporal lobe epilepsy: a network disorder of neocortical connectivity. Neurology 87, 1934–1942. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Vlooswijk M. C., Jansen J. F., Jeukens C. R., Majoie H. J., Hofman P. A., de Krom M. C., et al. (2011). Memory processes and prefrontal network dysfunction in cryptogenic epilepsy. Epilepsia 52, 1467–1475. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2011.03108.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Voets N. L., Beckmann C. F., Cole D. M., Hong S., Bernasconi A., Bernasconi N. (2012). Structural substrates for resting network disruption in temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 135, 2350–2357. 10.1093/brain/aws137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Vollmar C., O’Muircheartaigh J., Barker G. J., Symms M. R., Thompson P., Kumari V., et al. (2011). Motor system hyperconnectivity in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy: a cognitive functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain 134, 1710–1719. 10.1093/brain/awr098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wang X. Q., Iang S. Y., Lu H., Ma L., Mao Y. L., Yang F. (2007). Executive function impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: neuropsychological and diffusion-tensor imaging study. Zhonghua Yi Xue Za Zhi 87, 3183–3187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wang C., Qin W., Zhang J., Tian T., Li Y., Meng L., et al. (2014). Altered functional organization within and between resting-state networks in chronic subcortical infarction. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 34, 597–605. 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Watanabe M. (2016). Emotional and motivational functions of the prefrontal cortex. Brain Nerve 68, 1291–1299. 10.11477/mf.1416200593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Widjaja E., Skocic J., Go C., Snead O. C., Mabbott D., Smith M. L. (2013). Abnormal white matter correlates with neuropsychological impairment in children with localization-related epilepsy. Epilepsia 54, 1065–1073. 10.1111/epi.12208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wu J., Quinlan E. B., Dodakian L., McKenzie A., Kathuria N., Zhou R. J., et al. (2015). Connectivity measures are robust biomarkers of cortical function and plasticity after stroke. Brain 138, 2359–2369. 10.1093/brain/awv156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Yan C. G., Cheung B., Kelly C., Colcombe S., Craddock R. C., Di Martino A., et al. (2013). A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional connectomics. Neuroimage 76, 183–201. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yan C. G., Wang X. D., Zuo X. N., Zang Y. F. (2016). DPABI: data processing and analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics 14, 339–351. 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Yang G. J., Murray J. D., Repovs G., Cole M. W., Savic A., Glasser M. F., et al. (2014). Altered global brain signal in schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, 7438–7443. 10.1073/pnas.1405289111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Yeo B. T., Krienen F. M., Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M. R., Lashkari D., Hollinshead M., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Zamarian L., Trinka E., Bonatti E., Kuchukhidze G., Bodner T., Benke T., et al. (2011). Executive functions in chronic mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. Treat. 2011:596174. 10.1155/2011/596174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zhang Z., Lu G., Zhong Y., Tan Q., Liao W., Chen Z., et al. (2009). Impaired perceptual networks in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by resting fMRI. J. Neurol. 256, 1705–1713. 10.1007/s00415-009-5187-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES