Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Feb 2.
Published in final edited form as: Clin Psychol Rev. 2016 Feb 4;44:125–139. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.02.001

Shared and unique mechanisms underlying binge eating disorder and addictive disorders

Erica M Schulte a,*, Carlos M Grilo b,c,d, Ashley N Gearhardt a
PMCID: PMC5796407  NIHMSID: NIHMS935858  PMID: 26879210

Abstract

Scientific interest in “food addiction” is growing, but the topic remains controversial. One critique of “food addiction” is its high degree of phenotypic overlap with binge eating disorder (BED). In order to examine associations between problematic eating behaviors, such as binge eating and “food addiction,” we propose the need to move past examining similarities and differences in symptomology. Instead, focusing on relevant mechanisms may more effectively determine whether “food addiction” contributes to disordered eating behavior for some individuals. This paper reviews the evidence for mechanisms that are shared (i.e., reward dysfunction, impulsivity) and unique for addiction (i.e., withdrawal, tolerance) and eating disorder (i.e., dietary restraint, shape/weight concern) frameworks. This review will provide a guiding framework to outline future areas of research needed to evaluate the validity of the “food addiction” model and to understand its potential contribution to disordered eating.

Keywords: Binge eating disorder, Food addiction, Eating disorders, Substance use disorders

1. Introduction

It has recently been proposed that “food addiction” may be a contributor to obesity and eating-related problems (Gearhardt, Corbin, & Brownell, 2009a,b). This hypothesis proposes that certain foods, such as those high in sugar and fat, may be capable of triggering an addictive response in individuals with vulnerable characteristics (e.g., reward dysfunction, impulsivity) (Gearhardt, Davis, Kuschner, & Brownell, 2011). Early evidence in animal and human studies suggests that high-fat, high-sugar foods may activate reward-related neural circuitry in a similar manner as drugs of abuse (Gearhardt et al., 2011a; Johnson & Kenny, 2010a). Additionally, behavioral indicators of substance-use disorders, such as loss of control and use despite negative consequences, have been observed in response to these foods (Gearhardt et al., 2009a; Ifland et al., 2009). Although many symptoms of addictive disorders are behavioral in nature (e.g., consuming more than intended, inability to cut down on consumption) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and behavioral circumstances may increase addictive potential (e.g., intermittent access, binge patterns of use) (Hwa et al., 2011; Koob & Le Moal, 2001), the “food addiction” perspective does not reflect a behavioral addiction or an “eating addiction.” Rather, akin to substance-use disorders, this framework posits an interaction between the addictive potential of high-fat, high-sugar foods, behavioral factors that may increase addictive responses (e.g., intermittent, binge consumption), and an individual’s propensity to develop an addiction (Ahmed, Guillem, & Vandaele, 2013; Davis & Carter, 2009; Gearhardt et al., 2009a; Gold, Frost-Pineda, & Jacobs, 2003; Ifland et al., 2015; Ifland et al., 2009).

The Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) is currently the only validated measure to assess symptoms of “food addiction” (Gearhardt et al., 2009b). The YFAS is a 25-item self-report questionnaire that applies the diagnostic criteria for substance-use disorders to consumption of certain foods (see Table 1). The YFAS provides two scoring options: a symptom count (a sum of the seven diagnostic criteria) and a diagnostic threshold that reflects the criteria for a substance dependence diagnosis (the presence of three or more symptoms plus clinically significant impairment or distress). The YFAS has good internal consistency ranging from α = .76–.92 (Meule & Gearhardt, 2014) and demonstrates convergent validity with measures of eating pathology (e.g., emotional eating, food craving) and incremental validity in predicting binge eating frequency above and beyond existing measures (for a review see Meule & Gearhardt, 2014) The YFAS has been used to assess “food addiction” in both community and treatment-seeking samples and has been translated to German, French, Spanish, and Italian (Granero et al., 2014; Meule & Gearhardt, 2014; Pursey, Stanwell, Gearhardt, Collins, & Burrows, 2014).

Table 1.

YFAS symptoms based on DSM-IV criteria for substance dependence.

  1. Substance taken in larger amount and for longer period than intended

  2. Persistent desire or repeated unsuccessful attempt to quit

  3. Much time/activity to obtain, use, recover

  4. Important social, occupational, or recreational activities given up or reduced

  5. Use continues despite knowledge of adverse consequences (e.g. failure to fulfill role obligation, use when physically hazardous)

  6. Tolerance (marked increase in amount; marked decrease in effect)

  7. Characteristic withdrawal symptoms; substance taken to relieve withdrawal

Though “food addiction” is receiving increased attention, the topic remains controversial (Avena, Gearhardt, Gold, Wang, & Potenza, 2012; Corsica & Pelchat, 2010; Corwin & Hayes, 2014; Ziauddeen & Fletcher, 2013). This model posits that certain foods are addictive akin to substance-use disorders; however, there have been few studies examining which foods or ingredients in foods may have addictive potential (Corwin & Hayes, 2014; Ziauddeen & Fletcher, 2013). While initial evidence in animals and humans suggest that high-fat, high-sugar foods are most associated with behavioral indicators of “food addiction” (Avena, Rada, & Hoebel, 2008; Johnson & Kenny, 2010a; Schulte, Avena, & Gearhardt, 2015), identifying the potentially addictive agent in these foods is a critical next step in this line of research. Additionally, it has been suggested that “food addiction” cannot account for obesity, as only a relatively small percentage of obese individuals meet for YFAS diagnosis (Corwin & Hayes, 2014; Ziauddeen, Farooqi, & Fletcher, 2012). Thus, there have been conflicting findings in neuroimaging studies examining whether neural circuits implicated in addiction are also relevant to obesity (Corwin & Hayes, 2014; Ziauddeen et al., 2012). However, obesity is a multi-faceted condition that can result from a complex combination of a number of potential genetic and environmental factors, including for example, physical inactivity, medication side effects, and sleep problems, in addition to excessive food intake (Grilo & Pogue-Geile, 1991; Keith et al., 2006; Marcus & Wildes, 2009; Wright & Aronne, 2012). Although “food addiction” is more prevalent in participants with obesity (Flint et al., 2014), it has been observed in a range of weight classes (Gearhardt et al., 2009b) and may explain a unique phenotype of problematic eating behavior. Thus, obesity should not be used as a proxy for “food addiction” in future behavioral and neuroimaging studies.

Another important critique is the substantial phenotypic overlap between binge eating disorder (BED) and definitions of “food addiction.” Both BED and addiction are marked by loss of control over consumption, continued excess use despite negative consequences, and repeated, failed attempts to cut down on consumption (Gold et al., 2003). As a result of these similarities, measures of binge eating and “food addiction” (YFAS) are often highly correlated, both reflecting and resulting in the difficulty of evaluating and disentangling potential shared and unique aspects of these different constructs. For example, data from these types of measures cannot readily be placed in the same statistical model due to multi-collinearity concerns (Gearhardt, Rizk, & Treat, 2014).1 YFAS “food addiction” and BED commonly co-occur, although these constructs do not completely overlap. In samples of individuals with BED, the frequency of “food addiction” ranges from 42% to 57% and “food addiction” symptoms predict the frequency of binge eating episodes above and beyond measures of eating pathology and depression (Gearhardt, White, Masheb, & Grilo, 2013; Gearhardt et al., 2012). Individuals who meet the criteria for both BED and “food addiction” exhibit more frequent binge eating episodes, intense cravings, and depressive symptoms than those with only BED (Davis & Carter, 2009; Gearhardt et al., 2012). Among individuals who meet the criteria for YFAS “food addiction,” the frequency of BED ranges from 27% to 30% (Davis et al., 2011; Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014). Notably, in community studies with diverse weight groups, individuals categorized with “food addiction,” but not BED, report significant levels of impairment and distress, such as depressive symptoms, impulsivity, and negative affect (Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014). However, by focusing primarily on the psychometric and phenotypic overlap of “food addiction” and BED, it is challenging to evaluate whether an addictive process uniquely contributes to some types of disordered eating or whether “food addiction” is BED slightly reframed.

We propose the need to move past the use of descriptive similarities and differences to understand possible relationships between disordered eating, such as binge eating, and “food addiction.” Examining potential mechanisms underlying problematic eating behaviors may more appropriately determine whether an addictive process contributes to disordered eating for some individuals. Thus, there are three goals of this review. First, we review the shared and unique mechanisms implicated in addictive and eating-related problems to elucidate the relationship between addictive disorders and BED. Second, we examine how mechanisms unique to substance-use disorders may relate to problematic eating behavior in order to evaluate whether a “food addiction” framework may offer a novel understanding of eating-related issues. Third, we identify gaps in the literature and provide a theoretical roadmap to guide future research needed to determine the validity and utility of “food addiction” as a substance-based addictive disorder.

2. Shared mechanisms in addictive disorders and BED

Addiction and eating disorder perspectives share a number of proposed mechanisms, which may contribute partly to the similarities observed between BED and “food addiction” measures (Gearhardt et al., 2014c). This section will focus on how reward dysfunction, craving, emotion dysregulation, and impulsivity are relevant contributors to both addictive disorders and BED.

2.1. Reward dysfunction

Dysfunction in reward-related processes is one area similarly implicated in theories of addiction and disordered eating. Addictive substances and high fat/sugar foods appear to similarly implicate the mesolimbic dopamine system, which has been associated with wanting and motivation for a substance, and the mu-opioid systems, which are more closely related to the liking of the sensory experience (Berridge, 2007, 2009; Robinson & Berridge, 1993). The role of reward in addiction has long been discussed beginning in the 1950s and 1960s (Beach, 1957; Eveson, 1962). Specifically, addiction is thought to occur, in part, because addictive substances trigger hedonically pleasurable effects (Everitt & Robbins, 2005) and cause changes in reward-related neural systems (Dackis & O’Brien, 2001; Koob & Le Moal, 1997).

Although there is a consensus that reward dysfunction contributes to the risk of developing an addiction (Blum et al., 2000; Koob & Le Moal, 2001), there is a debate about whether hypo- or hyper- response to reward is more problematic. One hypothesis suggests that individuals with addiction use highly rewarding drugs or behaviors2 as a way to compensate for a deficient reward system (Blum et al., 2000; Volkow, Fowler, & Wang, 1999). In other words, these individuals use drugs of abuse to feel a level of reward others experience with less potent rewards (e.g., social interactions). As a result, individuals with hypo-responsive reward systems may seek out drugs in order to experience pleasure not achievable through other means (Reuter et al., 2005). In support of the hypo-reward hypothesis, persons with addiction exhibit reduced dopamine D2/D3 receptor availability, have less mesolimbic dopaminergic response to non-drug rewarding stimuli (e.g., money), and are more likely to have the DRD2 receptor A1 allele (which is associated with less D2-type receptor availability) (Blum et al., 2000; Reuter et al., 2005; Volkow et al., 1999). However, excess consumption of drugs of abuse can alter the reward system in a manner that reduces reward responsivity and D2-type receptor availability (Di Chiara & Bassareo, 2007; Koob & Le Moal, 1997; Spanagel & Weiss, 1999). Thus, it is unclear whether hypo-responsivity in the reward system is a cause or a consequence of addictive behaviors.

In contrast, hyper-reward responses to substance-related cues (e.g., alcohol advertisements, cigarette packs) are consistently found in addiction (Di Chiara, 1998; Everitt & Wolf, 2002; Robinson & Berridge, 1993). In other words, the reward system may generally be hypo-responsive, but becomes hyper-responsive to addiction-relevant cues. Individuals with addictions exhibit elevated activation in reward-related neural circuitry, such as the dorsolateral anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), in response to substance-related cues (Engelmann et al., 2012; Goudriaan, de Ruiter, van den Brink, Oosterlaan, & Veltman, 2010; Yang et al., 2009). These cues can also become highly salient, as reflected by elevated attention biases for drug-related cues among individuals with addiction (Field & Cox, 2008; Hester, Dixon, & Garavan, 2006; Lubman, Peters, Mogg, Bradley, & Deakin, 2000). Greater reactivity to these cues predicts intensity of drug craving and probability of relapse during quit attempts (Carter & Tiffany, 1999; Janes et al., 2010; Marissen et al., 2006). Thus, elevated reward sensitivity to addiction-relevant cues appears to be a major factor underlying addictive disorders.

Preliminary evidence suggests that reward dysfunction appears to be implicated in “food addiction.” The only neuroimaging study that has directly examined “food addiction” by using the YFAS observed patterns in reward circuitry that are also seen in addictive disorders (Gearhardt et al., 2011a). Individuals endorsing three or more YFAS “food addiction” symptoms exhibited increased neural activity in reward-related regions when anticipating a highly processed food (i.e., chocolate milkshake), relative to those who only endorsed one YFAS symptom (Gearhardt et al., 2011a). This pattern is characteristic of individuals with addictive disorders with respect to the anticipation of drug rewards (Martinez et al., 2005; Volkow et al., 2006), which provides evidence that reward dysfunction may similarly contribute to traditional addictive disorders and “food addiction.”

Reward dysfunction may also be a relevant mechanism in BED. Since the 1980s, reward sensitivity and dysfunctional reward responses to highly palatable, highly processed foods have been proposed as one potential explanation for binge eating (Dum, Gramsch, & Herz, 1983; Fullerton, Getto, Swift, & Carlson, 1985). Individuals prone to overeating may be driven to eat outside of homeostatic hunger to experience the hedonic, pleasurable effects of consuming certain foods (Lowe & Butryn, 2007). Though it has been argued that foods consumed during binge eating episodes vary in nutritional composition (Goldfein, Walsh, LaChaussee, Kissileff, & Devlin, 1993; Walsh, 2011), some studies have observed that highly processed foods (e.g., chocolate, French fries) are most frequently consumed when bingeing (Allison & Timmerman, 2007; Marcus, Wing, & Hopkins, 1988). Additionally, rats exhibit binge-eating behavior in response to highly processed food, but not to regular chow (Hagan, Chandler, Wauford, Rybak, & Oswald, 2003). As with drugs of abuse, consumption of these calorie-dense, nutrient-poor foods appears to activate the mesolimbic dopamine and endogenous opiate systems, which may be associated with the reinforcing properties of highly processed foods (Davis et al., 2009; Volkow, Wang, Fowler, & Telang, 2008).

Akin to addiction, the role of hypo- versus hyper-reward processes in binge eating is also unclear. Individuals who binge eat may be hypo-responsive to rewards compared to healthy controls, resulting in these individuals binging on highly processed food to mediate this reward deficiency (Blum, Cull, Braverman, & Comings, 1996). In support of this hypothesis, obese individuals with BED exhibit less activation in limbic regions implicated in reward compared to obese individuals without BED when exposed to monetary rewards (Balodis et al., 2013a). The presence of DRD2 receptor A1 allele is also linked to compulsive overeating, which may suggest that reward hyposensitivity is a genetic risk factor for the development of problematic eating behavior (Blum et al., 2000; Davis et al., 2008). However, as with addictive disorders, it is unclear whether reward hyposensitivity is a cause or effect of BED because excess consumption of highly processed foods may result in changes to the dopamine system to decrease reward sensitivity (Bello, Lucas, & Hajnal, 2002; Berridge, 2009; Johnson & Kenny, 2010a).

As demonstrated in addiction, persons with BED appear hyper-responsive to food-related cues. Individuals with BED compared with overweight and healthy controls exhibit increased activation in neural regions associated with reward appraisal (such as the OFC) in response to food stimuli (Schienle, Schafer, Hermann, & Vaitl, 2009; Weygandt, Schaefer, Schienle, & Haynes, 2012). One study found that binge eating, but not body mass index (BMI), was associated with increased striatal dopamine release in response to food stimulation (Wang et al., 2011). Akin to addictive disorders, individuals with BED may exhibit attention biases to high-calorie foods (Svaldi, Tuschen-Caffier, Peyk, & Blechert, 2010). However, it may be challenging to measure reward-responsiveness to food, as the reinforcing properties may be greater when individuals who binge eat are in a binge setting compared to being instructed not to binge eat (Schebendach, Broft, Foltin, & Walsh, 2013). This suggests that hyper-responsiveness to food cues may vary based on state with increased responsiveness when bingeing is possible. Although there is limited literature on this topic, existing studies appear to support a hyper-reward response to food cues for individuals who binge eat, especially when in a binge setting. Thus, hyper-responsivity to cues appears to be a common mechanism in BED and addictive disorders.

2.2. Craving

As with reward dysfunction, craving is implicated in both addiction and BED. Craving is a central component of addiction (Potenza & Grilo, 2014) that has been added to the diagnostic criteria for addictive disorders in the DSM-5 and is defined as a “strong desire or urge to use” (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In addictive disorders, cravings can be triggered by environmental cues of the drug and its hedonic effects (“reward craving”) or by the internal experience of withdrawal symptoms (“relief craving”) (Kilts et al., 2001; Verheul, van den Brink, & Geerlings, 1999). Triggers of craving (e.g., drug cues) may produce a dopaminergic release akin to consuming an addictive substance, which can increase drug-seeking behavior (Volkow et al., 2006; Volkow et al., 2008a). Activation of reward-related brain regions such as the insula, amygdala, OFC, and hippocampus have been observed in response to cue-induced craving (Bonson et al., 2002; Childress et al., 1999; Schneider et al., 2001; Wang et al., 1999). Thus, cravings appear to contribute to problematic use and compulsive drug seeking and may be related to elevated relapse rates in addictive disorders (Bottlender & Soyka, 2004; Killen & Fortmann, 1997).

Craving has also been examined as a potential mechanism in “food addiction.” Food craving is frequently described as “an intense desire to consume a specific food that is difficult to resist” (White, Whisenhunt, Williamson, Greenway, & Netemeyer, 2002), and food and drug cravings appear to activate similar brain regions (Kelley & Berridge, 2002; Pelchat, 2002; Pelchat, Johnson, Chan, Valdez, & Ragland, 2004; Tang, Fellows, Small, & Dagher, 2012). Individuals who endorse greater YFAS symptoms of “food addiction” report elevated food cravings (Davis et al., 2011; Gearhardt et al., 2014c; Meule & Kubler, 2012), particularly for high-fat, high-sugar foods (Gearhardt et al., 2014c), but do not have an expectation to be positively reinforced by consuming the craved foods (Meule & Kubler, 2012). This may reflect incentive sensitization, a common process in addictive disorders, where individuals experience a strong wanting or craving for a drug/food reward, but do not necessarily experience increased liking or pleasure (Berridge, 2009; Robinson & Berridge, 1993). While research in this area is in its nascent stages, existing evidence suggests that craving may contribute to “food addiction” in a manner akin to substance-use disorders.

Similar to addictive disorders, craving may be a relevant mechanism in BED. Craving may differ across weight categories, and associations between cravings and other clinical measures may vary across specific groups (White & Grilo, 2005). Notably, obese persons with BED generally have greater cravings for food and exhibit increased craving prior to consumption, relative to obese individuals without BED or normal-weight controls (Greeno, Wing, & Shiffman, 2000; Innamorati et al., 2014; Ng & Davis, 2013; White & Grilo, 2005). Akin to substance-use disorders, craving in BED may occur in response to food cues (Sobik, Hutchison, & Craighead, 2005), which may be associated with overeating (Jansen et al., 2003) or binge eating episodes. For example, Ng and Davis (2013) observed that the level of craving before food exposure predicted overeating in individuals with BED. Another study found that craving, even in the absence of hunger, was the best predictor for the onset of bingeing (Greeno et al., 2000). However, literature in this topic remains limited and further research is needed to determine whether cue-elicited craving may produce a dopaminergic response and activate reward-related regions (e.g., insula, OFC), akin to addictive disorders. Though it has been suggested that sweets (White & Grilo, 2005) and/or fats (Yanovski et al., 1992) may be implicated in binge eating episodes, future studies should examine which foods and food attributes (e.g., high-fat, highly processed) are frequently craved. In summary, craving may be an underlying mechanism in both binge eating behavior and compulsive drug use.

2.3. Emotion dysregulation

Emotion dysregulation, or the ineffective modulation of negative affect, is another important mechanism in both addictive disorders and BED. Negative affect has been associated with elevated cravings for an addictive substance since the 1960s (Ikard, Green, & Horn, 1969; Ikard & Tomkins, 1973; Tomkins, 1966). Negative affect can occur in response to a stressor or physical or psychological withdrawal symptoms, and is linked to increased consumption of drugs of abuse (Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004; Kenford et al., 2002; Kenny, Chen, Kitamura, Markou, & Koob, 2006; Sinha, 2001; Sinha, Catapano, & O’Malley, 1999; Sinha, Fuse, Aubin, & O’Malley, 2000). For some individuals, the substance may be used to cope with negative affect, which may reinforce continued self-administration (Baker et al., 2004; Brandon, 1994; Li & Sinha, 2008). Neuroimaging studies have shown that individuals addicted to psychostimulants demonstrate diminished activation in emotion processing regions (e.g., anterior cingulate cortex) and increased activation in regions associated with craving (e.g., striatum) in response to stress (Sinha et al., 2005). This supports the idea that individuals with addictive disorders may crave drugs of abuse in order to regulate negative emotional states. Further, negative affect appears to be a predictor of relapse, especially for individuals using drugs to cope (Miller, Westerberg, Harris, & Tonigan, 1996; Myers & Brown, 1990; Sinha, Garcia, Paliwal, Kreek, & Rounsaville, 2006) and when the onset of negative affect is rapid (Shiffman & Waters, 2004).

Akin to substance-use disorders, emotion dysregulation may contribute to “food addiction.” As individuals with addiction may utilize drugs in response to strong emotional states (Fox, Hong, & Sinha, 2008; Li & Sinha, 2008), persons with “food addiction” may consume certain foods in an effort to regulate emotions. In support of this theory, YFAS symptoms have been associated with greater endorsement of emotion dysregulation on self-report questionnaires (Gearhardt et al., 2013b; Gearhardt et al., 2012; Pivarunas & Conner, 2015). Further, individuals with elevated YFAS symptoms report that they are often motivated to consume highly processed foods in an effort to cope with negative emotions (Joyner, Schulte, Wilt, & Gearhardt, 2015). This preliminary work suggests that emotion dysregulation may be a mechanism implicated in “food addiction.” Future research could utilize advanced methodologies as ecological momentary assessment to dissect the process of how certain emotional states may trigger consumption of highly processed foods among individuals reporting YFAS “food addiction” symptoms and whether these patterns parallel precipitants of drug use in persons with substance-use disorders.

Emotion dysregulation has been proposed as a relevant mechanism in BED since the 1990s (Greeno et al., 2000; Grilo, Shiffman, & Carter-Campbell, 1994; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; Mussell et al., 1996; Stice, Akutagawa, Gaggar, & Agras, 2000; Telch & Agras, 1996). The experience of negative affect may induce cravings for highly processed foods and often precedes overeating (Grilo, Shiffman, & Wing, 1989) and specifically binge-eating episodes (Berg et al., 2014; Goldschmidt et al., 2014; Greeno et al., 2000; Telch & Agras, 1996). Binge eating, as substance-use, has long been suggested as a coping mechanism for intense emotional states (McManus & Waller, 1995; Nasser, Gluck, & Geliebter, 2004). In support of this hypothesis, Ranzenhofer et al. (2014) observed that interpersonal problems predict loss-of-control eating episodes, a potential precursor to BED, in adolescent girls. Ansell, Grilo, and White (2012) reported that interpersonal problems are associated with binge eating in women and that these effects were statistically mediated by negative affect. Additionally, Whiteside et al. (2007) found that emotion regulation deficits were the strongest predictor of binge eating above gender, restriction, and shape and weight concerns. Research is needed to examine whether the neural correlates of negative affect are similar among individuals with BED and substance-use disorders. As seen in addictive disorders, it appears that binge eating may be an attempt to cope with negative affect.

2.4. Impulsivity

Impulsivity is another mechanism underlying both addictive disorders and BED. Impulsivity refers to responding to internal or external stimuli in an unplanned manner, with little regard for potentially negative consequences that may result (Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz, & Swann, 2001). The role of impulsivity in addictive disorders started receiving attention in the 1950s (Lolli, 1951; Zimmering, Toolan, Safrin, & Wortis, 1951). While impulsivity may be a risk-factor for whether an individual develops an addiction, chronic substance use may also lead to worsening executive control deficits throughout the course of the addiction (de Wit, 2009). Addicted individuals typically show dysfunction in brain regions associated with impulsivity during decision-making tasks (Hester & Garavan, 2004; Hester, Lubman, & Yucel, 2010) and are impaired at delaying gratification of rewards (Hoffman et al., 2008; Monterosso et al., 2007). Thus, impulsivity may explain maladaptive decision-making in addiction where individuals engage in behavior that is rewarding in the short-term (e.g., drug use), despite being detrimental to them in the long-term (e.g., health problems, interpersonal difficulties). Importantly, impulsivity has been linked to higher rates of relapse in addictive disorders (Doran, Spring, McChargue, Pergadia, & Richmond, 2004; Miller, 1991).

Impulsivity may also be implicated in “food addiction.” Previous studies have found that YFAS “food addiction” symptoms are associated with attention-based impulsivity, marked by a faster response time to food cues (Meule, Lutz, Vogele, & Kubler, 2012), negative urgency, reflecting the tendency to act impulsively while experiencing negative mood states (Murphy, Stojek, & MacKillop, 2014; Pivarunas & Conner, 2015), and a lack of perseverance in tasks (Murphy et al., 2014). In the only neuroimaging study of YFAS “food addiction,” individuals reporting three or more YFAS symptoms exhibited decreased activation in inhibitory neural systems when consuming a highly processed food reward (i.e., chocolate milkshake), relative to persons endorsing one or fewer symptoms (Gearhardt et al., 2011a). Thus, akin to substance-use disorders, impulsivity may be a relevant mechanism in “food addiction” and warrants further empirical examination.

Impulsivity has also been discussed as an important mechanism in BED beginning in the 1990s (McManus & Waller, 1995; Mitchell & Mussell, 1995). Previous studies have found that individuals who binge eat demonstrate deficiencies in impulsivity, have trouble focusing attention, make risky decisions, and are poor at utilizing feedback to guide future behavior (Mobbs, Iglesias, Golay, & Van der Linden, 2011; Svaldi, Brand, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2010). It has also been suggested that impulsivity plays a role on the initiation of binge cravings (Dawe & Loxton, 2004; McManus & Waller, 1995). Similar to addictive disorders, binge eaters are also less successful at delaying gratification of rewards (Davis, Patte, Curtis, & Reid, 2010; Manwaring, Green, Myerson, Strube, & Wilfley, 2011). Thus, as seen in addiction, a maladaptive decision-making process may occur in individuals with BED, where the short-term reward of highly processed food is chosen instead of long-term health benefits. Schag et al. (2013) observed that individuals with BED exhibited impulsive eye movements in response to food stimuli. Neuroimaging studies suggest that individuals with BED have greater activation of executive control regions during a cognitive-control task, suggesting a maladaptive decision-making process (Balodis et al., 2013b; Gearhardt, Boswell, & Potenza, 2014). Akin to addictive disorders, high levels of impulsivity may also have implications in treatment of BED and rates of relapse. Several studies have found that impulsive children are more likely to binge eat and are less successful in treatment programs (Nederkoorn, Braet, Van Eijs, Tanghe, & Jansen, 2006; Nederkoorn, Jansen, Mulkens, & Jansen, 2007). Therefore, impulsivity appears to be a mechanism implicated similarly in substance-use disorders and BED.

3. Summary of shared mechanisms

In summary, addiction and binge eating share a number of proposed mechanisms. For both of these disorders, individuals may exhibit elevated reward responsivity to relevant cues and may act compulsively in the presence of these cues to satisfy intense cravings. Cravings may also be triggered by negative mood states, in which the drug or food may be used to regulate emotions. Finally, impulsivity has been identified as a shared mechanism underlying both substance-use disorders and BED and may have implications for continued administration and relapse.

Collectively, these shared mechanisms likely contribute to the behavioral and biological similarities between binge eating and traditional addictive disorders (Cassin & von Ranson, 2007; Gearhardt, White, & Potenza, 2011; Gold et al., 2003). Notably, smokers with BED are more likely to have a substance-use disorder than non-smokers with BED (White & Grilo, 2006), which provides further evidence for similar processes underlying BED and addiction at least in some persons or subgroups. If addictive disorders and BED are both explained in part by shared mechanisms, we propose that it may be plausible that an addictive process may be contributing to binge eating for some individuals. In support, preliminary evidence suggests that reward dysfunction, craving, emotion dysregulation, and impulsivity may all also contribute to “food addiction,” as measured by the YFAS. However, it is challenging to disentangle BED and “food addiction” given the high degree of phenotypic overlap and these common mechanisms. There is evidence that individuals who experience “food addiction,” but who do not meet criteria for BED or other eating disorders, exhibit clinically significant levels of pathology and eating-related concerns (Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014). Further, individuals with an eating disorder diagnosis who also meet for YFAS “food addiction” have particularly severe levels of pathology (Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014). Thus, the assessment of “food addiction” appears to provide clinically relevant information to the study of disordered eating. In order to further evaluate whether an addictive process may explain problematic eating behavior in some individuals, it is essential to examine mechanisms unique to an addiction perspective (e.g., withdrawal) and traditional eating disorder approaches (e.g., shape and weight concerns).

4. Unique aspects of addiction perspectives

We now review several core mechanisms of substance-use disorders: the importance of the substance, withdrawal, and tolerance. We evaluate whether these processes may contribute to problematic eating behavior in a manner similar to addictions. If certain foods share features with drugs of abuse and may be capable of triggering addictive responses in some individuals, this would provide support for the validity and utility of a “food addiction” framework.

4.1. Importance of the substance

An addiction framework posits that an addictive substance (e.g., nicotine) or behavior (e.g., gambling) interacts with individual risk factors (e.g., genetic predisposition, personality characteristics) to trigger an addictive process. Without exposure to the addictive substance, an individual at risk will not develop an addiction. Thus, the addictive substance or behavior is an essential aspect of the problematic behavior. Drugs of abuse are altered from their natural state to contain an increased concentration (or dose) of the addictive agent and to increase the rate in which the addictive agent is absorbed into the system (Barnett, Hawks, & Resnick, 1981; Henningfield & Keenan, 1993). For example, coca leaves have little addictive potential when chewed (Hanna & Hornick, 1977), but when they are refined into cocaine and consumed by nasal insufflation, they become highly addictive (Verebey & Gold, 1988). Further, addictive substances may be capable of producing neuroplastic changes in brain regions responsible for processing rewards and making decisions (Koob, 1992; Leshner, 1997). These changes in the mesolimbic dopamine system and prefrontal cortex may perpetuate self-administration (Chang, Alicata, Ernst, & Volkow, 2007; Volkow & Fowler, 2000). In other words, an individual’s brain may become “hijacked” by the addictive substance, resulting in compulsive consumption despite negative consequences (Volkow & Wise, 2005).

Highly processed foods (e.g., pizza, chocolate, chips) may share characteristics with drugs of abuse, as they are altered from their natural state to include higher concentrations of fat and/or refined carbohydrates like sugar or white flour, and the rate in which the refined carbohydrates are absorbed into the system is rapid (Gearhardt, Davis et al., 2011). For example, a minimally processed food, such as a banana, contains natural sugars but would be expected to have little or no addictive potential compared to a candy bar, which is processed to contain increased levels of fat and rapidly absorbed sugars.

Additionally, highly processed foods may also be capable of causing neuroplastic changes in reward-related brain regions that contribute to behavioral indicators of “food addiction” (Bello et al., 2002; Berridge, 2009; Johnson & Kenny, 2010b), although research on this topic is in its nascent stages. Rats who consume a diet of highly processed foods, such as cheesecake, experience downregulation in the dopamine system, akin to using drugs of abuse (Johnson & Kenny, 2010b). Notably, binge-prone rats only exhibit addictive-like responses to foods high in fat and sugar (e.g., Oreo Double-Stuf cookies) and not chow (Boggiano et al., 2007; Klump, Racine, Hildebrandt, & Sisk, 2013). Animal models suggest that sugar may be most closely associated with “food addiction,” as rats with intermittent access to sugar exhibit binge eating, enhanced motivation to obtain the substance (which may model craving), cross-sensitization, and withdrawal when the sugar is removed (Avena et al., 2008b) (for a review see Avena, 2010). In humans, Stice, Burger, and Yokum (2013a, 2013b) observed that a high-sugar milkshake strongly activated reward-related regions. Further, pharmacology used to treat alcohol-related problems by blocking opioid response (i.e., naloxone) has also been associated with reduced consumption of ultra-processed foods in binge eaters (Drewnowski, Krahn, Demitrack, Nairn, & Gosnell, 1995). Additionally, in the only neuroimaging study of “food addiction,” individuals with elevated YFAS symptoms exhibit neural responses akin to other addictive disorders in response to cues and consumption of a highly processed food (Gearhardt et al., 2011a).

One study has examined which foods are most likely to be implicated in “food addiction” in humans (Schulte et al., 2015). Schulte et al. (2015) asked participants to complete the YFAS then rate how likely they were to experience YFAS symptoms with 35 nutritionally diverse foods. Highly processed foods, with added fat and refined carbohydrates were identified as the most problematic. Foods with a high glycemic load (which reflects a faster speed of absorption of carbohydrates and a greater blood sugar spike) were especially problematic for individuals endorsing “food addiction” symptoms on the YFAS. Thus, like with drugs of abuse, the rapid speed of absorption of rewarding ingredients may make highly processed foods particularly triggering for susceptible individuals. Laboratory studies with direct observations and measurements of eating behavior and reward response are needed to confirm and extend these preliminary findings. Though more research is needed, if highly processed foods cause similar biological and behavioral changes as drugs of abuse, this would support applying an addiction perspective to problematic eating behavior in some individuals.

If certain foods are capable of triggering addiction-like changes in the brain, it would challenge one core tenet of traditional eating disorder treatment models such as cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) (Fairburn, 2008; Grilo & Mitchell, 2011) which states that there are no “bad” or “forbidden” foods. CBT perspectives on eating disorders emphasize individual differences (e.g., shape and weight overconcern, excessive or rigid dietary restraint, mood regulation difficulties, poor self-esteem) as factors that serve to either lead to and/or maintain problematic eating behaviors (Fairburn, 1995, 2008; Grilo & Mitchell, 2011); in such models, how individuals under- or over-eat (in the case of BED) is a key aspect but that the types of food consumed less importance (Fairburn, 1995, 2008; Grilo & Mitchell, 2011) In contrast, an addiction perspective suggests that some foods (or how some foods are processed) may trigger the reward system in a manner that makes it more challenging for at-risk individuals to moderate their consumption (Gearhardt, Davis et al., 2011). Thus, it is possible that certain foods may not merely be “bad” foods per a common cognitive bias in CBT models but may actually possess chemical properties that make them more difficult to eat in a controlled manner for both cognitive and neurobiological reasons.

Importantly, exposure to addictive substances does not universally trigger compulsive consumption, and individual differences (e.g., genetics, patterns of consumption, personality factors) play an important role in who becomes addicted. For example, 90% of people consume alcohol (an addictive substance) during their lifetime, but only 5–10% of individuals develop alcohol dependence (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Grant, 1997). However, there are many individuals with subclinical addictive-like responses to alcohol, which emphasizes the broad public health implications of classifying alcohol as an addictive substance. Similarly, if some foods have an addictive potential, it is unlikely that all or most people would develop a clinical-level of “addiction.” It is also probable that other individuals would experience subclinical addictive-like responses to certain foods, which increases the scope of potential public health initiatives (Gearhardt, Grilo, DiLeone, Brownell, & Potenza, 2011). It is likely that individual differences associated with addiction risk (e.g., impulsivity, family history of addiction) will interact with the potentially addictive nature of certain foods to trigger “compulsive” eating in some, but not all, individuals. In summary, for some cases of BED, mechanisms associated with dietary restraint or shape and weight concerns may precipitate binge-eating behavior. For others, highly processed foods may result in reward-related, neuroplastic changes in the brain and trigger an addictive process for at-risk individuals. This highlights the necessity of a mechanism-focused approach to understanding problematic eating behavior and for whom each mechanism may be most applicable.

4.2. Withdrawal and tolerance

Withdrawal and tolerance are unique markers of an addictive process that may provide insight into craving and relapse. Withdrawal is defined in the DSM-5 as the development of physiological or psychological symptoms in response to abstinence or decreased consumption of a substance (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Though physical withdrawal symptoms were previously considered a core component of addiction, they are no longer a mandatory characteristic of an addictive disorder, as withdrawal from drugs such as cocaine and behavioral addictions such as gambling result in minimal physical symptoms (Brower & Paredes, 1987; Rosenthal & Lesieur, 1992; Weddington et al., 1990). Psychological withdrawal symptoms, such as preoccupation and anxiety, are considered more universal across addictive disorders and are predictive of relapse rates (Kenford et al., 2002; West, Hajek, & Belcher, 1989).

Animal-model researchers have observed opioid-like withdrawal symptoms following addictive-like consumption of sucrose (Avena, Rada, & Hoebel, 2009). When sucrose is removed from the diet, rats experience anxiety, teeth chattering, and more aggressive behavior (Avena, Bocarsly, Rada, Kim, & Hoebel, 2008; Galic & Persinger, 2002). No previous studies have examined withdrawal symptoms in humans for highly processed foods. However, individuals who cut back on high-sugar foods (e.g., dieters) may exhibit metabolic changes in response to food cues, such as a decrease in blood sugar, and this may trigger cravings (Herman & Mack, 1975). Similar physiological changes can occur in response to cues for individuals with addictive disorders, which also results in strong cravings (Gearhardt et al., 2009a). Further, individuals experiencing problematic eating behavior anecdotally report withdrawal-like symptoms to carbohydrates (Gearhardt et al., 2009a; Pelchat, 2002). Thus, it is plausible that some individuals may experience withdrawal symptoms to highly processed foods. We are unaware of any prior studies that have investigated whether individuals endorsing YFAS “food addiction” symptoms develop a withdrawal in response to cutting down on high-fat/high-sugar foods, and this is an important area that requires further examination.

Similar to withdrawal symptoms, tolerance is another unique component of an addiction perspective. Tolerance is characterized by 1) the need to consume increased amounts of a substance to experience the desired, hedonic effects or 2) the diminished effect of a constant dose over time (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Reductions in reward-related neural pathways over time in response to a constant dose of a substance may model the development of tolerance. This has been demonstrated in traditional addictive disorders (Nestler & Malenka, 2004; Volkow, Fowler, Wang, Baler, & Telang, 2009). Importantly, a recent study observed that frequent ice-cream consumption was associated with reduced striatal responsivity during the receipt of an ice-cream based milkshake, which is consistent with the development of tolerance (Burger & Stice, 2012). Behavioral indicators of tolerance have also been explored. Spring et al. (2008) observed that a beverage high in carbohydrates became less effective at reducing self-reported dysphoric mood in obese individuals after repeated administration. No studies to date have investigated whether individuals with YFAS “food addiction” symptomology exhibit greater tolerance to the consumption of high-fat/high-sugar foods. The lack of empirical examination of withdrawal and tolerance is a significant gap in understanding how addiction-specific mechanisms may contribute to eating-related problems and an essential next step for evaluating the validity of the “food addiction” concept.

In summary, exploring whether withdrawal and tolerance contribute to binge eating behavior is important for evaluating the “food addiction” hypothesis because these components are unique to an addiction framework. There is no existing research examining the association of “food addiction” or BED with these mechanisms. If symptoms of withdrawal and tolerance contribute to eating pathology this would provide evidence that an addiction framework has unique explanatory power for BED. It is likely that while addiction mechanisms may be relevant for certain individuals with BED, the unique aspects of the eating disorder framework may be important for others.

5. Unique aspects of eating disorder perspectives

We now describe mechanisms implicated in eating disorders like BED and discuss why these processes would not be considered causal factors in the “food addiction” model. If mechanisms unique to a traditional eating disorder perspective explain BED but not “food addiction,” this would suggest that these two phenotypes do not overlap completely.

5.1. Restraint

Eating disorder models have long held that inappropriate (e.g., extreme or rigid) dietary restraint is a causal antecedent to binge eating (Fairburn & Harrison, 2003; Herman & Polivy, 1990; Howard & Porzelius, 1999; Polivy & Herman, 1985; Telch & Agras, 1993). Restraint was initially thought to be an intentional attempt to reduce calories below a biological “set point” of consumption (Herman & Mack, 1975; Nisbett, 1972). However, individuals reporting high levels of restraint often do not appear to be biologically deprived of calories (Lowe, 1993), suggesting that restraint may be more cognitive in nature. The perspective of cognitive restraint posits that a high level of restraint is indicative of an individual forming rigid dietary rules due to concerns about shape or weight (Herman & Polivy, 1980). In support of cognitive restraint, measures of restraint (e.g., Restraint Scale) appear to be associated with the utilization of rigid dietary rules (Herman & Polivy, 1980) but have not been related to biological factors such as acute or long-term caloric intake (Stice, Cooper, Schoeller, Tappe, & Lowe, 2007; Stice, Fisher, & Lowe, 2004; Stice, Sysko, Roberto, & Allison, 2010). Further, restrained eaters may exhibit increased food consumption following challenges to their cognition, such as a high-calorie preload (Polivy, 1976; Spencer & Fremouw, 1979) or tasks that engage cognitive resources (Bellisle & Dalix, 2001; Lowe & Kral, 2006; Ward & Mann, 2000).

More recently, restraint has been re-conceptualized by some as a reflection of hedonic hunger, or a perceived, cognitive deprivation for highly palatable foods despite meeting biological caloric needs (Lowe & Butryn, 2007; Lowe & Levine, 2005; Markowitz, Butryn, & Lowe, 2008). Hedonic hunger, as measured by the Power of Food Scale (PFS), is related to increased food craving, and greater consumption of highly processed food (e.g., chocolate) (Lowe & Butryn, 2007). Scores on the PFS are higher in obese individuals with BED (relative to those without the diagnosis) and higher PFS scores are associated with binge eating severity in BN (Lowe & Butryn, 2007; Witt & Lowe, 2014). Although scores on various restraint measures do not appear to be related to objectively measured caloric deprivation (Stice et al., 2007; Stice et al., 2004; Stice et al., 2010), restraint is associated with increased hedonic hunger (Lowe et al., 2009). Thus, dietary restraint may reflect, in part, perceived deprivation for highly palatable foods associated with hedonic hunger.

Dieting may also influence eating behavior and be associated with disordered eating. While restraint appears to reflect a cognitive, perceived deprivation of highly palatable foods, dieting – in contrast - is associated with decreased caloric intake for weight loss (Allen, Byrne, & McLean, 2012). Dieting prospectively predicts future weight gain (Lowe et al., 2006; Lowe, Doshi, Katterman, & Feig, 2013; Stice, Cameron, Killen, Hayward, & Taylor, 1999) and may be a proxy for an individual’s vulnerability for problematic eating behavior and past problems with eating regulation (Lowe, 2015). Individuals who diet for weight loss tend to endorse high levels of restrained eating, but the behavior of restrained eaters may vary based on whether the individual is currently dieting (Lowe, 1993, 1995). For example, restrained dieters reduce their food intake following a high-calorie preload (Lowe, 1995), which may be a response to a direct threat to weight-loss efforts. This contrasts restrained non-dieters who overeat after a high-calorie preload, perhaps due to a violation of rigid dietary rules (Polivy, 1976; Spencer & Fremouw, 1979).

Dieting for weight loss appears to be a relevant factor in BED, and data from retrospective self-report suggest that individuals with BED have histories of frequent dieting attempts and weight fluctuations although importantly dieting appears to precede binge eating in only about 50% of those who develop BED (Grilo & Masheb, 2000; Hilbert et al., 2014; Mussell et al., 1995; Reas & Grilo, 2007; Spurrell, Wilfley, Tanofsky, & Brownell, 1997). Fasting seems to predict binge eating across diverse weight and disordered eating groups (Stice, Davis, Miller, & Marti, 2008) and caloric restriction is associated with increased brain activity in regions associated with attention and reward processing in response to food cues (Stice et al., 2013a, 2013b).

While dieting may be a risk factor for binge eating in some individuals, binge eating precedes dieting in others (Grilo & Masheb, 2000; Hilbert et al., 2014; Reas & Grilo, 2007; Spurrell et al., 1997). In BED, those who report bingeing before dieting may have an earlier onset of binge behaviors and endorse lower levels of restraint than those who diet first (Spurrell et al., 1997). Although retrospective/prospective studies and CBT models suggest that dieting and restraint increase the risk of binge eating and eating disorders for some individuals (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003; Grilo & Masheb, 2000; Herman & Polivy, 1990; Spurrell et al., 1997), controlled intervention studies have challenged this idea in some respects for certain patient groups. Specifically, behavioral weight loss treatments, which decrease caloric intake, have been shown to effectively reduce binge eating behavior and eating disorder pathology in obese patients with BED (Grilo, Masheb, Wilson, Gueorguieva, & White, 2011; Wilson, Wilfley, Agras, & Bryson, 2010). Further, a carefully designed study demonstrated that a low-calorie diet (e.g., 1000 kcal/day) coupled with behavioral therapy does not appear to induce increased binge eating behavior (Wadden et al., 2004). Thus, in at least some circumstances, behavioral weight loss and low-calorie diets do not appear to increase the frequency of binge eating episodes or worsen BED symptoms.

An alternative understanding of restraint and dieting behaviors is that they are strategies utilized to overcome one’s personal vulnerability for weight gain and obesity (Agras, 2010; Lowe, 2015; Lowe et al., 2013). Thus, some individuals may be vulnerable to shape and weight ideals and engage in dietary restraint to maintain their weight (Lowe & Levine, 2005). Consistent with this idea, weight concern/importance may prospectively predict the onset of dieting and disordered eating behavior (Loth, MacLehose, Bucchianeri, Crow, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2014). However, other individuals may be vulnerable to a food’s hedonic properties and employ dietary restraint to counteract the rewarding nature of the food (Lowe & Butryn, 2007). Notably, individuals with BED exhibit lower levels of restraint than persons with BN, but binge-eating frequency is similar (Grilo et al., 2009; Wilfley, Schwartz, Spurrell, & Fairburn, 2000), which suggests that are mechanisms other than restraint contributing to BED.

Multiple pathways likely interact to result in the development of binge eating. Consistent with this idea, one prospective study demonstrated that body dissatisfaction interacts differently with depressive symptoms and dieting to increase the risk of disordered eating (Stice, Marti, & Durant, 2011). Further, models of binge eating that include both dieting and affect-related difficulties as contributors to bingeing behavior seem to most appropriately represent potential pathways to the development of BED (Allen et al., 2012). Thus, dietary restraint may be one contributing component to binge eating in combination with multiple other risk factors. Addiction-related mechanisms may be another relevant factor that interacts with dietary restraint to increase risk for BED.

Though dietary restraint as a putative underlying, causal mechanism for BED is unique to eating disorder models, restraint is not absent in addiction. Notably, the restraint behaviors in BED are not successful (Howard & Porzelius, 1999), which parallels the substance dependence criteria of being unable to cut down or abstain from use, despite the desire to do so (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Repeated, unsuccessful attempts to cut back on consumption or quit occur as part of an addiction (Best, Ghufran, Day, Ray, & Loaring, 2008; Fagan et al., 2007), and substance-related restraint, or attempts to cut down, has been measured in addictive disorders (Collins & Lapp, 1992; Ruderman & McKirnan, 1984). Further, the abstinence violation effect, where an individual excessively uses a substance after experiencing a lapse in restraint, is often applied to BED (Grilo & Shiffman, 1994), but originated from the addiction literature (Marlatt, 1979). Thus, for some persons with BED, dietary restraint may be a consequence, rather than a cause, of binge eating behavior (Grilo & Masheb, 2000; Reas & Grilo, 2007). For some individuals, restraint may be the failed attempt to control addictive-like consumption of highly rewarding, calorie-dense foods.

Dietary restraint may be a strategy for overcoming one’s personal vulnerability for weight gain in an obesogenic environment (Lowe, 2015; Lowe et al., 2013). While some individuals may be vulnerable to shape and weight ideals, others may have a propensity to develop an addictive-like response to hedonically pleasurable, highly processed foods and engage in dietary restraint after bingeing in an effort to avoid weight gain. Since restraint may be associated with perceived deprivation of these foods (Lowe & Levine, 2005), individuals who find these foods especially rewarding or “addictive” may experience hedonic deprivation more acutely, which may increase their risk for engaging in extreme dietary restraint to combat binge eating and weight cycling. This pattern of alternating between periods of binge eating and dietary restraint may lead to addiction-like changes in the brain, such as sensitization of the dopamine system (Avena et al., 2008b; Hyman, Malenka, & Nestler, 2006; Robinson & Berridge, 1993). Thus, for some individuals, dietary restraint may be used as an attempt to overcome weight gain related to a propensity for developing addictive-like eating in response to highly processed foods.

Relatedly, YFAS symptoms of “food addiction” have been associated with greater weight cycling (e.g., repeated loss and regain of twenty pounds or more), earlier age of first diet, and time spent dieting (Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014). While some studies have found an association between YFAS symptomology and a measure of dietary restraint, others have not (Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014; Gearhardt et al., 2013b; Gearhardt et al., 2012). In some cases, “food addiction” appears to be related to dieting and restraint, though the nature of this relationship is poorly understood, as the existing studies are cross-sectional or retrospective. Thus, it will be necessary to examine whether dieting and restraint may play some causal role in the development of “food addiction” or whether dietary restraint/dieting might develop in response to an individual’s propensity to consume highly processed foods in an addictive manner. Further, future work should evaluate whether alternating between periods of restriction and excess consumption of highly processed foods may create an intermittent pattern that, akin to substance use disorders (Berridge & Robinson, 1995; Robinson & Berridge, 2001), exacerbates addictive-like responses.

In summary, restraint and dieting may be causal mechanisms for some individuals with BED, as posited by traditional eating disorder models. In others, restraint may be linked with binge eating, but it may reflect attempts to resist biologic drives towards obesity (Agras, 2010; Lowe, 2015; Lowe et al., 2013) and/or attempts to manage an addictive response to highly processed foods. Additional longitudinal research is needed to understand how an individual’s personal vulnerabilities (e.g., shape/weight concern, propensity for addiction) may interact with binge eating to result in disordered eating behavior. Future studies might also examine whether periods of intermittent restraint or fasting (followed by episodes of binging) may contribute to addiction-related neural sensitization in humans.

5.2. Shape and weight concerns

In addition to dietary restraint, shape and weight concerns may be antecedents or risk factors for BED (Fairburn et al., 1998; Killen et al., 1996). This refers to the overvaluation of an individual’s own shape and/or weight (Hrabosky, Masheb, White, & Grilo, 2007). Though not a DSM-5 diagnostic criterion for BED, it has been suggested that shape and weight concerns represent a core psychopathology of eating disorders and may play a key role in the maintenance of binge eating and associated eating pathology (Fairburn et al., 2003). Overvaluation of shape and weight may prospectively predict the onset of binge eating for some individuals (Allen, Byrne, McLean, & Davis, 2008; Killen et al., 1994; Loth et al., 2014; Sonneville et al., 2015; Stice, Presnell, & Spangler, 2002). Additionally, high reports of body dissatisfaction appear to greatly increase the risk for disordered eating behavior (Stice et al., 2011), especially in early adolescence (Rohde, Stice, & Marti, 2015). While shape and weight concerns may precede eating problems for some, others may experience elevated shape and weight concerns due to negative outcomes such as weight gain associated with bingeing. Binge eaters who report shape and weight overvaluation present with more severe eating psychopathology and depression than binge eaters without the characteristic (Grilo, 2013; Grilo et al., 2008; Grilo, Masheb, & White, 2010; Sonneville et al., 2015). This underscores the idea that shape and weight concern is an important contributor to the trajectory of BED, though, akin to restraint, it may be a causal mechanism for some individuals but a consequence of bingeing for others.

The idea that shape and weight concerns may precipitate and/or maintain problematic eating is not characteristic of an addiction perspective. If one were to attempt to conceptualize overvaluation of shape/weight from an addiction perspective, it might perhaps be speculated that such shape and weight concerns develop as problematic eating continues. For example, it is logical that smokers would have higher rates of concern about lung cancer because they are at higher risk due to their addiction. Similarly, “food addiction” has been associated with elevated shape and weight concerns in cross-sectional research (Gearhardt, Boswell, & White, 2014; Gearhardt et al., 2013b; Gearhardt et al., 2012; Schebendach et al., 2013), which may be related to the increased risk of weight gain linked to greater levels of highly processed food consumption. In a parallel manner, from such a perspective, binge eating might be associated with greater shape and weight concerns because binge eating increases risk of future weight gain or obesity. However, studies with BED (Grilo et al., 2008) and other eating disorder groups with binge-eating behaviors (Grilo et al., 2009), have consistently demonstrated that overvaluation is not associated with either BMI or obesity (i.e., it appears to reflect a subjective cognitive feature unrelated to body weight). Thus, shape and weight concerns appear to be related to both binge eating and YFAS “food addiction” symptomology, however, longitudinal and experimental studies are needed to develop an improved understanding of these associations.

6. Practical implications

If addiction mechanisms may contribute to problematic eating behavior for some individuals, this framework may inform new treatments or improve existing interventions. While research has identified specific psychological (Wilson, Grilo, & Vitousek, 2007) and pharmacological (Reas & Grilo, 2014, 2015) treatment interventions for BED, even with the best available treatments (Grilo et al., 2011; Wilson et al., 2010), a sizeable minority of patients do not achieve remission and most fail to lose clinically meaningful weight. (Brownley, Berkman, Sedway, Lohr, & Bulik, 2007; Grilo et al., 2011; Vocks et al., 2010; Wilson et al., 2010), Treatment can be improved further by an improved understanding of predictors/moderators (Grilo, Masheb, & Crosby, 2012) and of mediators (Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn, & Agras, 2002; Wilson et al., 2007). Research on mediators of BED treatments is lacking and represents a priority for research. Novel treatments that target underlying mechanisms may increase efficacy. For example, if an intervention designed to reduce smoking focused on changing the individual’s thoughts and beliefs about cigarettes, but did not address the cigarettes’ addictive nature, this therapeutic technique would likely have limited success.

Future research is warranted to determine whether interventions that target addictive-like mechanisms may be beneficial for some individuals with problematic eating behavior. Notably, CBT for BED and addictive disorders already share common features, such as emotion regulation, trigger identification, and craving management (Gearhardt et al., 2011d). The greatest point of contention between existing treatments tailored to “food addiction” (e.g., Overeaters Anonymous) relative to treatment addressing traditional eating disorder tenets (e.g., restraint) is the role of the food (Gearhardt et al., 2011d). In eating disorder models, there are no “bad” or “forbidden” foods (Fairburn et al., 2003). However, an addiction perspective would suggest that some foods have a greater “addictive potential” and for some individuals, these foods may be capable of “hijacking” neural circuitry (Gearhardt, Davis et al., 2011) and making it more difficult to regulate eating behaviors and patterns. If an addictive process contributes to problematic eating behavior for some individuals (or if highly processed foods create greater challenges to controlled intake for both cognitive and neurobiological reasons), then perhaps specific interventions utilized in the treatment of addictive disorders should be evaluated for their utility in addressing “food addiction.” The assessment of novel treatment approaches may be clinically useful for improving outcomes, though research is also needed to identify the potential harm of incorporating addiction intervention perspectives into the treatment of eating problems.

One major concern regarding treatments for “food addiction” is that the goal of abstinence from certain foods may be harmful and increase disordered eating behaviors, such as dietary restraint (Wilson, 1993). Although abstinence-based interventions for problematic eating behavior, such as Overeaters Anonymous and Food Addicts Anonymous, have been around since the 1960’s, there has been limited empirical examination of their efficacy (Schwartz & Brownell, 1995). Due to the potential harm of abstinence-based programs, future research is needed to examine the utility of this treatment approach as applied to “food addiction.” Until there is significant empirical evidence of positive treatment outcomes, the identification of potential contraindications, and the assessment of possible adverse consequences, abstinence-based treatments for eating should be approached with caution.

However, there are empirically supported, addiction-focused interventions that do not require abstinence, such as harm-reduction. Harm-reduction aims to minimize potential harm associated with addictive substances by focusing on using in moderation and reducing consumption in high-risk situations (Cheung, 2000; Marlatt, 1996; Marlatt & Tapert, 1993). For example, a harm-reduction intervention for individuals with alcohol-use disorders would aim to reduce binge drinking episodes through strategies such as alternating between alcoholic beverages and glasses of water and monitoring the quantity of alcoholic drinks consumed (Marlatt, Somers, & Tapert, 1993; Marlatt & Witkiewitz, 2002). Harm-reduction approaches also focus on tolerating craving and developing alternative coping strategies to regulate intense mood states (Marlatt, Larimer, & Witkiewitz, 2011). While harm-reduction strategies are empirically supported for reducing negative outcomes associated with substance-use disorders (Langendam, van Brussel, Coutinho, & van Ameijden, 2001; McBride, Farringdon, Midford, Meuleners, & Phillips, 2004; Monti et al., 1999), it is uncertain whether this intervention would be well-suited for addictive-like consumption of highly processed foods. Thus, future examination of a harm-reduction approach for treating “food addiction” may be warranted.

Another consideration for the utility of “food addiction” in a clinical context is whether the construct predicts treatment outcomes. We are unaware of previous studies that have examined if addiction-specific treatments like harm reduction or abstinence-based interventions are effective in reducing symptoms of “food addiction” or eating-related problems like binge eating and obesity. However, there have been mixed findings regarding whether an individual’s endorsement of “food addiction” symptoms predicts outcomes in treatments for obesity, with some studies finding that “food addiction” was related to less weight loss (Burmeister, Hinman, Koball, Hoffmann, & Carels, 2013) and others observing no association with weight loss or attrition (Lent, Eichen, Goldbacher, Wadden, & Foster, 2014).

The literature regarding the predictive significance of BED for obesity treatment outcomes is mixed and suffers from methodological limitations, consisting of mostly retrospective and non-randomized studies, limited further by self-report measures and varying ways of categorizing “binge eaters” (Wilson et al., 2007). Blaine and Rodman (2007), in a matched-study meta-analysis of obesity trials, reported that obese binge eaters, on average, lost significantly less weight than obese non-binge-eaters (1.3 kg vs 10.5 kg). Such aggregate findings across studies suggest that BED is a negative prognostic indicator for obesity treatment, although the comparison strategy and the methodological limitations of many of the studies suggests caution and need for further research. More recently, Grilo and White (2013), in the first controlled, prospective study of BED status on obesity outcome treatment, reported that BED significantly predicted worse eating and depression outcomes and significantly moderated the effects of anti-obesity medication (orlistat) on weight loss (i.e., medication added to behavioral therapy enhanced weight losses among non-binge-eaters but not BED).

Given the mixed findings in the literature, further examination is warranted for how constructs as “food addiction” and binge eating may predict intervention outcomes for obesity. However, obesity is not synonymous with “food addiction” or binge eating, and future research should also focus on identifying which treatments may be most effective for particular eating-related problems (e.g., harm reduction for the treatment of “food addiction”) and whether individual characteristics (e.g., impulsivity) may moderate treatment outcome.

7. Future directions

Given the unique mechanisms relevant to applying an addiction framework to food (e.g., withdrawal), there are six key research directions that are important to further evaluate the “food addiction” construct (Table 2). First, it is essential to identify the addictive agent in food to determine which foods or ingredient(s) have an addictive potential. Though animal models and the only existing study in humans support the role of highly processed foods in “food addiction” (Avena et al., 2008b; Johnson & Kenny, 2010b; Murray, Tulloch, Chen, & Avena, 2015; Schulte et al., 2015), the addictive agent(s) is unknown, as are the specific individual characteristics that may enhance one’s risk of developing “food addiction.” The absence of a defined addictive agent remains one of the most significant criticisms of the food addiction hypothesis (Hebebrand et al., 2014; Ziauddeen & Fletcher, 2013). The identification of the potentially addictive agent in food is fundamental to the proposal of an addictive process and should be prioritized in future studies. Second, it is important to determine whether mechanisms unique to addictive disorders, such as withdrawal and tolerance, may contribute to addictive-like consumption of certain foods. Third, longitudinal work is needed in order to understand the temporal relations between “food addiction,” obesity, binge eating, and BED. The YFAS-C, a measure of “food addiction” symptoms in children, is associated with elevated BMI (Gearhardt, Roberto, Seamans, Corbin, & Brownell, 2013), emphasizing the necessity of examining how “food addiction” develops and relates to other eating-related problems across the lifespan. Further, longitudinal methodologies used in other fields (Cain et al., 2012; Shea et al., 2004) may help to clarify further the nature of the associations among the constructs and especially whether the possible mechanisms described in this review are causal factors or contributors to the maintenance or course of eating-related problems. Fourth, treatment approaches for addictive disorders, such as abstinence-based interventions or harm-reduction, should be evaluated for the treatment of “food addiction.” This is especially important given the potential for certain addiction-focused treatment approaches to increase restrictive eating practices. Fifth, there is emerging evidence that YFAS scores may be associated with other forms of binge eating, such as bulimia nervosa (Meule, von Rezori, & Blechert, 2014) and anorexia nervosa- binge/purge subtype (Granero et al., 2014). As such, the role of addiction-specific mechanisms in problematic eating behavior should be explored across various forms of binge eating and disordered eating in future research. Sixth, BED and “food addiction” are both associated with obesity but do not occur exclusively in obese individuals (Davis et al., 2011; de Zwaan, 2001; Gearhardt et al., 2009b). However, many of the mechanisms implicated in addictive disorders and BED (e.g., impulsivity, reward dysfunction) may also contribute to obesity (Davis & Carter, 2009; Volkow et al., 2008b). Thus, it will be important to examine whether certain mechanisms are particularly relevant to individuals with both obesity and compulsive patterns of food consumption.

Table 2.

Future directions in food addiction research.

  1. Identify the addictive agent(s) in food to determine which foods or ingredient(s) have an addictive potential

  2. Determine whether mechanisms unique to addictive disorders, such as withdrawal and tolerance, may contribute to addictive-like consumption of certain foods

  3. Utilize longitudinal research approaches to a) examine temporal relations between “food addiction,” obesity, binge eating, and BED, and b determine whether the mechanisms described in this review may be causal factors or contributors to the maintenance of eating-related problems

  4. Evaluate whether treatment approaches for addictive disorders, such as abstinence-based interventions or harm-reduction, may be appropriate and efficacious for the treatment of “food addiction”

  5. Explore whether addiction-specific mechanisms may contribute to eating-related problems more broadly (e.g., binge eating, bulimia nervosa)

  6. Examine whether certain mechanisms implicated in BED and “food addiction” are particularly relevant to individuals with both obesity and compulsive patterns of food consumption

8. Concluding remarks

“Food addiction” and BED have significant phenotypic overlap, such as loss of control over consumption and continued use despite negative consequences. To evaluate whether an addictive process contributes to problematic eating, increased attention to underlying mechanisms is warranted. While many mechanisms appear to be shared among BED and addictive disorders, such as reward dysfunction, craving, emotion dysregulation, and impulsivity, there are also important differences. For some individuals, dietary restraint and shape or weight concerns may be causal mechanisms in the development and/or maintenance of binge eating behavior. However, for others, highly processed foods may be capable of triggering neuroplastic changes in the brain to result in an addictive-like process. In conclusion, though “food addiction” and BED share several underlying mechanisms, the utility of “food addiction” may be most appropriately understood by investigating whether mechanisms unique to an addiction perspective offer novel explanatory power for problematic eating in some individuals. Further, a mechanistic approach may contribute to the development of new intervention approaches to improve treatment outcomes.

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • Shared mechanisms may explain phenotypic overlap between “food addiction” and BED.

  • Mechanisms unique to addictive disorders may contribute to addictive-like eating.

  • Future research needed to examine the utility of “food addiction” is recommended.

  • Addiction perspectives may inform novel interventions for disordered eating.

Footnotes

1

For example, a recent study by (Gearhardt, Rizk, et al., 2014) found that YFAS and Binge Eating Scale scores were correlated at .751.

2

In this manuscript, we will use the terms “substance” or “consumption” for ease of writing, but in the case of gambling this can also be applied to behavior and engagement in the behavior.

References

  1. Agras WS. The Oxford handbook of eating disorders. Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahmed SH, Guillem K, Vandaele Y. Sugar addiction: Pushing the drug–sugar analogy to the limit. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care. 2013;16(4):434–439. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e328361c8b8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/MCO.0b013e328361c8b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen KL, Byrne SM, McLean NJ. The dual-pathway and cognitive–behavioural models of binge eating: Prospective evaluation and comparison. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;21(1):51–62. doi: 10.1007/s00787-011-0231-z. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-011-0231-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Allen KL, Byrne SM, McLean NJ, Davis EA. Overconcern with weight and shape is not the same as body dissatisfaction: Evidence from a prospective study of pre-adolescent boys and girls. Body Image. 2008;5(3):261–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.03.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Allison S, Timmerman GM. Anatomy of a binge: Food environment and characteristics of nonpurge binge episodes. Eating Behaviors. 2007;8(1):31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.01.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association, A. P. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  7. American Psychiatric Association, A. P. A. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 2013 from http://dsm.psychiatryonline.org/book.aspx?bookid=556.
  8. Ansell EB, Grilo CM, White MA. Examining the interpersonal model of binge eating and loss of control over eating in women. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2012;45(1):43–50. doi: 10.1002/eat.20897. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Avena NM. The study of food addiction using animal models of binge eating. Appetite. 2010;55(3):734–737. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Avena NM, Bocarsly ME, Rada P, Kim A, Hoebel BG. After daily bingeing on a sucrose solution, food deprivation induces anxiety and accumbens dopamine/ acetylcholine imbalance. Physiology & Behavior. 2008a;94(3):309–315. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Avena NM, Gearhardt AN, Gold MS, Wang GJ, Potenza MN. Tossing the baby out with the bathwater after a brief rinse? The potential downside of dismissing food addiction based on limited data. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13(7):514. doi: 10.1038/nrn3212-c1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3212-c1 (author reply 514) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008b;32(1):20–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Sugar and fat bingeing have notable differences in addictive-like behavior. The Journal of Nutrition. 2009;139(3):623–628. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.097584. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/jn.108.097584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review. 2004;111(1):33–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Balodis IM, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, White MA, Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, … Potenza MN. Monetary reward processing in obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2013a;73(9):877–886. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Balodis IM, Molina ND, Kober H, Worhunsky PD, White MA, Rajita S, … Potenza MN. Divergent neural substrates of inhibitory control in binge eating disorder relative to other manifestations of obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013b;21(2):367–377. doi: 10.1002/oby.20068. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/oby.20068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Barnett G, Hawks R, Resnick R. Cocaine pharmacokinetics in humans. Journal of Ethnopharmacology. 1981;3(2–3):353–366. doi: 10.1016/0378-8741(81)90063-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Beach HD. Morphine addiction in rats. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie. 1957;11(2):104. doi: 10.1037/h0083703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bellisle F, Dalix AM. Cognitive restraint can be offset by distraction, leading to increased meal intake in women. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2001;74(2):197–200. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/74.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bello NT, Lucas LR, Hajnal A. Repeated sucrose access influences dopamine D2 receptor density in the striatum. Neuroreport. 2002;13(12):1575–1578. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200208270-00017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Berg KC, Peterson CB, Crosby RD, Cao L, Crow SJ, Engel SG, Wonderlich SA. Relationship between daily affect and overeating-only, loss of control eating-only, and binge eating episodes in obese adults. Psychiatry Research. 2014;215(1):185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191(3):391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Berridge KC. ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: Brain substrates and roles in eating disorders. Physiology & Behavior. 2009;97(5):537–550. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.044. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. The mind of an addicted brain: neural sensitization of wanting versus liking. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1995;71–76 [Google Scholar]
  25. Best DW, Ghufran S, Day E, Ray R, Loaring J. Breaking the habit: A retrospective analysis of desistance factors among formerly problematic heroin users. Drug and Alcohol Review. 2008;27(6):619–624. doi: 10.1080/09595230802392808. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09595230802392808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Blaine B, Rodman J. Responses to weight loss treatment among obese individuals with and without BED: A matched-study meta-analysis. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2007;12(2):54–60. doi: 10.1007/BF03327579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Blum K, Braverman ER, Holder JM, Lubar JF, Monastra VJ, Miller D, … Comings DE. Reward deficiency syndrome: A biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 2000;32(Suppl i–iv):1–112. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2000.10736099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Blum K, Cull JG, Braverman ER, Comings DE. Reward deficiency syndrome. American Scientist. 1996;84(2):132–145. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/29775633. [Google Scholar]
  29. Boggiano MM, Artiga AI, Pritchett CE, Chandler-Laney PC, Smith ML, Eldridge AJ. High intake of palatable food predicts binge-eating independent of susceptibility to obesity: An animal model of lean vs obese binge-eating and obesity with and without binge-eating. International Journal of Obesity. 2007;31(9):1357–1367. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803614. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Bonson KR, Grant SJ, Contoreggi CS, Links JM, Metcalfe J, Weyl HL, … London ED. Neural systems and cue-induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26(3):376–386. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00371-2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00371-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Bottlender M, Soyka M. Impact of craving on alcohol relapse during, and 12 months following, outpatient treatment. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2004;39(4):357–361. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agh073. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agh073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Brandon TH. Negative affect as motivation to smoke. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1994:33–37. [Google Scholar]
  33. Brower KJ, Paredes A. Cocaine withdrawal. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1987;44(3):297–298. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800150121016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Brownley KA, Berkman ND, Sedway JA, Lohr KN, Bulik CM. Binge eating disorder treatment: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(4):337–348. doi: 10.1002/eat.20370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Burger K, Stice E. Frequent ice cream consumption is associated with reduced striatal response to receipt of an ice cream-based milkshake. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2012;95(4):810–817. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.027003. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.111.027003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Burmeister JM, Hinman N, Koball A, Hoffmann DA, Carels RA. Food addiction in adults seeking weight loss treatment. Implications for psychosocial health and weight loss. Appetite. 2013;60(1):103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Cain NM, Ansell EB, Wright AG, Hopwood CJ, Thomas KM, Pinto A, … Grilo CM. Interpersonal pathoplasticity in the course of major depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80(1):78–86. doi: 10.1037/a0026433. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Carter BL, Tiffany ST. Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999;94(3):327–340. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Cassin SE, von Ranson KM. Is binge eating experienced as an addiction? Appetite. 2007;49(3):687–690. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.06.012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Chang L, Alicata D, Ernst T, Volkow N. Structural and metabolic brain changes in the striatum associated with methamphetamine abuse. Addiction. 2007;102(Suppl 1):16–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01782.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01782.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Cheung YW. Substance abuse and developments in harm reduction. CMAJ. 2000;162(12):1697–1700. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O’Brien CP. Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156(1):11–18. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Collins RL, Lapp WM. The temptation and restraint Inventory for measuring drinking restraint. British Journal of Addiction. 1992;87(4):625–633. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1992.tb01964.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Corsica JA, Pelchat ML. Food addiction: True or false? Current Opinion in Gastroenterology. 2010;26(2):165–169. doi: 10.1097/MOG.0b013e328336528d. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/MOG.0b013e328336528d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Corwin RLW, Hayes JE. Fructose, high fructose corn syrup, sucrose and health. Springer; 2014. Are sugars addictive? Perspectives for practitioners; pp. 199–215. [Google Scholar]
  46. Dackis CA, O’Brien CP. Cocaine dependence: A disease of the brain’s reward centers. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2001;21(3):111–117. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(01)00192-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Davis C, Carter JC. Compulsive overeating as an addiction disorder. A review of theory and evidence. Appetite. 2009;53(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Davis C, Curtis C, Levitan RD, Carter JC, Kaplan AS, Kennedy JL. Evidence that ‘food addiction’ is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite. 2011;57(3):711–717. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Davis C, Levitan RD, Kaplan AS, Carter J, Reid C, Curtis C, … Kennedy JL. Reward sensitivity and the D2 dopamine receptor gene: A case–control study of binge eating disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2008;32(3):620–628. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.09.024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Davis C, Levitan RD, Reid C, Carter JC, Kaplan AS, Patte KA, … Kennedy JL. Dopamine for “wanting” and opioids for “liking”: A comparison of obese adults with and without binge eating. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2009;17(6):1220–1225. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Davis C, Patte K, Curtis C, Reid C. Immediate pleasures and future consequences. A neuropsychological study of binge eating and obesity. Appetite. 2010;54(1):208–213. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.11.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Dawe S, Loxton NJ. The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2004;28(3):343–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Di Chiara G. A motivational learning hypothesis of the role of mesolimbic dopamine in compulsive drug use. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 1998;12(1):54–67. doi: 10.1177/026988119801200108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Di Chiara G, Bassareo V. Reward system and addiction: what dopamine does and doesn’t do. Current Opinion in Pharmacology. 2007;7(1):69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2006.11.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2006.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Doran N, Spring B, McChargue D, Pergadia M, Richmond M. Impulsivity and smoking relapse. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2004;6(4):641–647. doi: 10.1080/14622200410001727939. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14622200410001727939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Drewnowski A, Krahn DD, Demitrack MA, Nairn K, Gosnell BA. Naloxone, an opiate blocker, reduces the consumption of sweet high-fat foods in obese and lean female binge eaters. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1995;61(6):1206–1212. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/61.6.1206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Dum J, Gramsch C, Herz A. Activation of hypothalamic β-endorphin pools by reward induced by highly palatable food. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1983;18(3):443–447. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(83)90467-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Engelmann JM, Versace F, Robinson JD, Minnix JA, Lam CY, Cui Y, … Cinciripini PM. Neural substrates of smoking cue reactivity: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies. NeuroImage. 2012;60(1):252–262. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(11):1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Everitt BJ, Wolf ME. Psychomotor stimulant addiction: a neural systems perspective. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(9):3312–3320. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03312.2002. 20026356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Eveson M. Drug addiction: an hypothesis for experimental test. Canadian J Corrections. 1962;5:110. [Google Scholar]
  62. Fagan P, Augustson E, Backinger CL, O’Connell ME, Vollinger RE, Jr, Kaufman A, Gibson JT. Quit attempts and intention to quit cigarette smoking among young adults in the United States. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(8):1412–1420. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.103697. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2006.103697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Fairburn CG. Overcoming binge eating. Guilford Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  64. Fairburn CG. Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. Guilford Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  65. Fairburn CG, Harrison PJ. Eating disorders. Lancet. 2003;361(9355):407–416. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12378-1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12378-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2003;41(5):509–528. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00088-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Fairburn CG, Doll HA, Welch SL, Hay PJ, Davies BA, O’Connor ME. Risk factors for binge eating disorder: A community-based, case–control study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55(5):425–432. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2008;97(1–2):1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Flint AJ, Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD, Field AE, Rimm EB. Food-addiction scale measurement in 2 cohorts of middle-aged and older women. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2014;99(3):578–586. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.068965. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.113.068965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Fox HC, Hong KA, Sinha R. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control in recently abstinent alcoholics compared with social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors. 2008;33(2):388–394. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Fullerton DT, Getto CJ, Swift WJ, Carlson IH. Sugar, opioids and binge eating. Brain Research Bulletin. 1985;14(6):673–680. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(85)90117-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Galic MA, Persinger MA. Voluminous sucrose consumption in female rats: increased ‘nippiness’ during periods of sucrose removal and possible oestrus periodicity. Psychological Reports. 2002;90(1):58–60. doi: 10.2466/pr0.2002.90.1.58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Gearhardt AN, Boswell RG, Potenza MN. Neuroimaging of eating disorders, substance use disorders, and addictions: Overlapping and unique systems. In: Brewerton TD, Baker Dennis A, editors. Eating disorders, addictions and substance use disorders. Springer; Berlin Heidelberg: 2014a. pp. 71–89. [Google Scholar]
  74. Gearhardt AN, Boswell RG, White MA. The association of “food addiction” with disordered eating and body mass index. Eating Behaviors. 2014b;15(3):427–433. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.05.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Gearhardt AN, Davis C, Kuschner R, Brownell KD. The addiction potential of hyperpalatable foods. Current Drug Abuse Reviews. 2011c;4(3):140–145. doi: 10.2174/1874473711104030140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Gearhardt AN, Grilo CM, DiLeone RJ, Brownell KD, Potenza MN. Can food be addictive? Public health and policy implications. Addiction. 2011b;106(7):1208–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03301.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03301.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Gearhardt AN, Roberto CA, Seamans MJ, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale for children. Eating Behaviors. 2013a;14(4):508–512. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.07.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Gearhardt AN, White MA, Potenza MN. Binge eating disorder and food addiction. Current Drug Abuse Reviews. 2011d;4(3):201–207. doi: 10.2174/1874473711104030201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Gearhardt AN, White MA, Masheb RM, Grilo CM. An examination of food addiction in a racially diverse sample of obese patients with binge eating disorder in primary care settings. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2013b;54(5):500–505. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Gearhardt AN, Yokum S, Orr PT, Stice E, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Neural correlates of food addiction. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011a;68(8):808–816. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Food addiction: An examination of the diagnostic criteria for dependence. Journal Addiction Medicine. 2009a;3(1):1–7. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e318193c993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ADM.0b013e318193c993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite. 2009b;52(2):430–436. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Gearhardt AN, Rizk MT, Treat TA. The association of food characteristics and individual differences with ratings of craving and liking. Appetite. 2014c;79:166–173. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Gearhardt AN, White MA, Masheb RM, Morgan PT, Crosby RD, Grilo CM. An examination of the food addiction construct in obese patients with binge eating disorder. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2012;45(5):657–663. doi: 10.1002/eat.20957. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Gold MS, Frost-Pineda K, Jacobs WS. Overeating, binge eating, and eating disorders as addictions. Psychiatric Annals 2003 [Google Scholar]
  86. Goldfein JA, Walsh BT, LaChaussee JL, Kissileff HR, Devlin MJ. Eating behavior in binge eating disorder. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1993;14(4):427–431. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199312)14:4<427::aid-eat2260140405>3.0.co;2-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Goldschmidt AB, Crosby RD, Cao L, Engel SG, Durkin N, Beach HM, … Peterson CB. Ecological momentary assessment of eating episodes in obese adults. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2014;76(9):747–752. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Goudriaan AE, de Ruiter MB, van den Brink W, Oosterlaan J, Veltman DJ. Brain activation patterns associated with cue reactivity and craving in abstinent problem gamblers, heavy smokers and healthy controls: An fMRI study. Addiction Biology. 2010;15(4):491–503. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00242.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00242.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Granero R, Hilker I, Aguera Z, Jimenez-Murcia S, Sauchelli S, Islam MA, … Fernandez-Aranda F. Food addiction in a Spanish sample of eating disorders: DSM-5 diagnostic subtype differentiation and validation data. European Eating Disorders Review. 2014;22(6):389–396. doi: 10.1002/erv.2311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.2311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Grant BF. Prevalence and correlates of alcohol use and DSM-IV alcohol dependence in the United States: Results of the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1997;58(5):464–473. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Greeno CG, Wing RR, Shiffman S. Binge antecedents in obese women with and without binge eating disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68(1):95–102. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Grilo CM. Why no cognitive body image feature such as overvaluation of shape/ weight in the binge eating disorder diagnosis? International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(3):208–211. doi: 10.1002/eat.22082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Grilo CM, Masheb RM. Onset of dieting vs binge eating in outpatients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders. 2000;24(4):404–409. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Grilo CM, Mitchell JE. The treatment of eating disorders: A clinical handbook. Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  95. Grilo CM, Pogue-Geile MF. The nature of environmental influences on weight and obesity: A behavior genetic analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110(3):520–537. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Grilo CM, Shiffman S. Longitudinal investigation of the abstinence violation effect in binge eaters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62(3):611–619. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.62.3.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Grilo CM, White MA. Orlistat with behavioral weight loss for obesity with versus without binge eating disorder: Randomized placebo-controlled trial at a community mental health center serving educationally and economically disadvantaged Latino/as. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2013;51(3):167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.01.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Grilo CM, Crosby RD, Masheb RM, White MA, Peterson CB, Wonderlich SA, … Mitchell JE. Overvaluation of shape and weight in binge eating disorder, bulimia nervosa, and sub-threshold bulimia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47(8):692–696. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Grilo CM, Hrabosky JI, White MA, Allison KC, Stunkard AJ, Masheb RM. Overvaluation of shape and weight in binge eating disorder and overweight controls: Refinement of a diagnostic construct. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117(2):414–419. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Grilo CM, Masheb RM, Crosby RD. Predictors and moderators of response to cognitive behavioral therapy and medication for the treatment of binge eating disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80(5):897–906. doi: 10.1037/a0027001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Grilo CM, Masheb RM, White MA. Significance of overvaluation of shape/weight in binge-eating disorder: Comparative study with overweight and bulimia nervosa. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2010;18(3):499–504. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Grilo CM, Masheb RM, Wilson GT, Gueorguieva R, White MA. Cognitive–behavioral therapy, behavioral weight loss, and sequential treatment for obese patients with binge-eating disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2011;79(5):675–685. doi: 10.1037/a0025049. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Grilo CM, Shiffman S, Carter-Campbell JT. Binge eating antecedents in normal-weight nonpurging females: Is there consistency? The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1994;16(3):239–249. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199411)16:3<239::aid-eat2260160304>3.0.co;2-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Grilo CM, Shiffman S, Wing RR. Relapse crises and coping among dieters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;57(4):488–495. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.4.488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Hagan MM, Chandler PC, Wauford PK, Rybak RJ, Oswald KD. The role of palatable food and hunger as trigger factors in an animal model of stress induced binge eating. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2003;34(2):183–197. doi: 10.1002/eat.10168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.10168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Hanna JM, Hornick CA. Use of coca leaf in southern Peru: adaptation or addiction. Bulletin on Narcotics. 1977;29(1):63–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Heatherton TF, Baumeister RF. Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110(1):86–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Hebebrand J, Albayrak O, Adan R, Antel J, Dieguez C, de Jong J, … Dickson SL. “Eating addiction”, rather than “food addiction”, better captures addictive-like eating behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;47:295–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Henningfield JE, Keenan RM. Nicotine delivery kinetics and abuse liability. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61(5):743–750. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.5.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Herman CP, Mack D. Restrained and unrestrained eating. Journal of Personality. 1975;43(4):647–660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00727.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Herman CP, Polivy J. Restrained eating. Obesity. 1980:208–225. [Google Scholar]
  112. Herman CP, Polivy J. From dietary restraint to binge eating: Attaching causes to effects. Appetite. 1990;14(2):123–125. doi: 10.1016/0195-6663(90)90009-w. (discussion 142–123) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Hester R, Garavan H. Executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingulate, and cerebellar activity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(49):11017–11022. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3321-04.2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3321-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Hester R, Dixon V, Garavan H. A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;81(3):251–257. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.07.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Hester R, Lubman DI, Yucel M. The role of executive control in human drug addiction. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 2010;3:301–318. doi: 10.1007/7854_2009_28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/7854_2009_28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Hilbert A, Pike KM, Goldschmidt AB, Wilfley DE, Fairburn CG, Dohm FA, … Striegel Weissman R. Risk factors across the eating disorders. Psychiatry Research. 2014;220(1–2):500–506. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.054. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Hoffman WF, Schwartz DL, Huckans MS, McFarland BH, Meiri G, Stevens AA, Mitchell SH. Cortical activation during delay discounting in abstinent methamphetamine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 2008;201(2):183–193. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1261-1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1261-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Howard CE, Porzelius LK. The role of dieting in binge eating disorder: Etiology and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review. 1999;19(1):25–44. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00009-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Hrabosky JI, Masheb RM, White MA, Grilo CM. Overvaluation of shape and weight in binge eating disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75(1):175–180. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Hwa LS, Chu A, Levinson SA, Kayyali TM, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Persistent escalation of alcohol drinking in C57BL/6J mice with intermittent access to 20% ethanol. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. 2011;35(11):1938–1947. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01545.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01545.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Hyman SE, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ. Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2006;29:565–598. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Ifland JR, Preuss HG, Marcus MT, Rourke KM, Taylor WC, Burau K, … Manso G. Refined food addiction: A classic substance use disorder. Medical Hypotheses. 2009;72(5):518–526. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2008.11.035. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2008.11.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Ifland JR, Preuss HG, Marcus MT, Rourke KM, Taylor W, Theresa Wright H. Clearing the confusion around processed food addiction. Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2015;34(3):240–243. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2015.1022466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07315724.2015.1022466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Ikard FF, Tomkins S. The experience of affect as a determinant of smoking behavior: A series of validity studies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1973;81(2):172. doi: 10.1037/h0034555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Ikard FF, Green DE, Horn D. A scale to differentiate between types of smoking as related to the management of affect. Substance Use & Misuse. 1969;4(4):649–659. [Google Scholar]
  126. Innamorati M, Imperatori C, Balsamo M, Tamburello S, Belvederi Murri M, Contardi A, … Fabbricatore M. Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait (FCQ-T) discriminates between obese and overweight patients with and without binge eating tendencies: the Italian version of the FCQ-T. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2014;96(6):632–639. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2014.909449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2014.909449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Janes AC, Pizzagalli DA, Richardt S, Chuzi d, BFB, Chuzi S, Pachas G, … Kaufman MJ. Brain reactivity to smoking cues prior to smoking cessation predicts ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67(8):722–729. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.034. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Jansen A, Theunissen N, Slechten K, Nederkoorn C, Boon B, Mulkens S, Roefs A. Overweight children overeat after exposure to food cues. Eating Behaviors. 2003;4(2):197–209. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00011-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00011-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nature Neuroscience. 2010a;13(5):635–641. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nature Neuroscience. 2010b;13(5):635–641. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Joyner MA, Schulte EM, Wilt AR, Gearhardt AN. Addictive-like eating mediates the association between eating motivations and elevated body mass index. Translational Issues in Psychological Science. 2015;1(3):217. [Google Scholar]
  132. Keith SW, Redden DT, Katzmarzyk PT, Boggiano MM, Hanlon EC, Benca RM, … Allison DB. Putative contributors to the secular increase in obesity: Exploring the roads less traveled. International Journal of Obesity. 2006;30(11):1585–1594. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: Relevance to addictive drugs. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(9):3306–3311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03306.2002. (doi: 20026361) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Kenford SL, Smith SS, Wetter DW, Jorenby DE, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Predicting relapse back to smoking: Contrasting affective and physical models of dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70(1):216–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Kenny PJ, Chen SA, Kitamura O, Markou A, Koob GF. Conditioned withdrawal drives heroin consumption and decreases reward sensitivity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(22):5894–5900. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0740-06.2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0740-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Killen JD, Fortmann SP. Craving is associated with smoking relapse: Findings from three prospective studies. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1997;5(2):137–142. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.5.2.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Killen JD, Taylor CB, Hayward C, Haydel KF, Wilson DM, Hammer L, … Strachowski D. Weight concerns influence the development of eating disorders: A 4-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64(5):936–940. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.5.936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Killen JD, Taylor CB, Hayward C, Wilson DM, Haydel KF, Hammer LD, et al. Pursuit of thinness and onset of eating disorder symptoms in a community sample of adolescent girls: A three-year prospective analysis. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1994;16(3):227–238. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199411)16:3<227::aid-eat2260160303>3.0.co;2-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, Muhammad F, … Drexler KP. Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58(4):334–341. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Klump KL, Racine S, Hildebrandt B, Sisk CL. Sex differences in binge eating patterns in male and female adult rats. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(7):729–736. doi: 10.1002/eat.22139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Koob GF. Drugs of abuse: Anatomy, pharmacology and function of reward pathways. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 1992;13(5):177–184. doi: 10.1016/0165-6147(92)90060-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997;278(5335):52–58. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24(2):97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Kraemer HC, Wilson GT, Fairburn CG, Agras WS. Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59(10):877–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Langendam MW, van Brussel GH, Coutinho RA, van Ameijden EJ. The impact of harm-reduction-based methadone treatment on mortality among heroin users. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91(5):774–780. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.5.774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Lent MR, Eichen DM, Goldbacher E, Wadden TA, Foster GD. Relationship of food addiction to weight loss and attrition during obesity treatment. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2014;22(1):52–55. doi: 10.1002/oby.20512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/oby.20512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Leshner AI. Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science. 1997;278(5335):45–47. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Li CS, Sinha R. Inhibitory control and emotional stress regulation: neuroimaging evidence for frontal–limbic dysfunction in psychostimulant addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32(3):581–597. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Lolli G. The addictive drinker. Pastoral Psychology. 1951;2(3):20–27. [Google Scholar]
  150. Loth KA, MacLehose R, Bucchianeri M, Crow S, Neumark-Sztainer D. Predictors of dieting and disordered eating behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2014;55(5):705–712. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.04.016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Lowe MR. The effects of dieting on eating behavior: A three-factor model. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114(1):100–121. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Lowe MR. Restrained eating and dieting: Replication of their divergent effects on eating regulation. Appetite. 1995;25(2):115–118. doi: 10.1006/appe.1995.0047. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1995.0047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Lowe MR. Dieting: Proxy or cause of future weight gain? Obesity Reviews. 2015;16(Suppl 1):19–24. doi: 10.1111/obr.12252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/obr.12252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Lowe MR, Butryn ML. Hedonic hunger: A new dimension of appetite? Physiology & Behavior. 2007;91(4):432–439. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Lowe MR, Kral TV. Stress-induced eating in restrained eaters may not be caused by stress or restraint. Appetite. 2006;46(1):16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.01.014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Lowe MR, Levine AS. Eating motives and the controversy over dieting: Eating less than needed versus less than wanted. Obesity Research. 2005;13(5):797–806. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2005.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Lowe MR, Annunziato RA, Markowitz JT, Didie E, Bellace DL, Riddell L, … Stice E. Multiple types of dieting prospectively predict weight gain during the freshman year of college. Appetite. 2006;47(1):83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.03.160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2006.03.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Lowe MR, Butryn ML, Didie ER, Annunziato RA, Thomas JG, Crerand CE, … Halford J. The power of food scale. A new measure of the psychological influence of the food environment. Appetite. 2009;53(1):114–118. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Lowe MR, Doshi SD, Katterman SN, Feig EH. Dieting and restrained eating as prospective predictors of weight gain. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Lubman DI, Peters LA, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Deakin JF. Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychological Medicine. 2000;30(1):169–175. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Manwaring JL, Green L, Myerson J, Strube MJ, Wilfley DE. Discounting of various types of rewards by women with and without binge eating disorder: Evidence for general rather than specific differences. Psychological Record. 2011;61(4):561–582. doi: 10.1007/bf03395777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Marcus MD, Wildes JE. Obesity: Is it a mental disorder? The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2009;42(8):739–753. doi: 10.1002/eat.20725. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Marcus MD, Wing RR, Hopkins J. Obese binge eaters: Affect, cognitions, and response to behavioural weight control. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56(3):433–439. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.3.433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Marissen MA, Franken IH, Waters AJ, Blanken P, van den Brink W, Hendriks VM. Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment. Addiction. 2006;101(9):1306–1312. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01498.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01498.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Markowitz JT, Butryn ML, Lowe MR. Perceived deprivation, restrained eating and susceptibility to weight gain. Appetite. 2008;51(3):720–722. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.03.017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Marlatt GA. A cognitive-behavioral model of the relapse process. NIDA Research Monograph. 1979;(25):191–200. doi: 10.1037/e497382006-015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Marlatt GA. Harm reduction: come as you are. Addictive Behaviors. 1996;21(6):779–788. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(96)00042-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Marlatt GA, Tapert SF. Harm reduction: Reducing the risks of addictive behaviors. 1993. [Google Scholar]
  169. Marlatt GA, Witkiewitz K. Harm reduction approaches to alcohol use: Health promotion, prevention, and treatment. Addictive Behaviors. 2002;27(6):867–886. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(02)00294-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Marlatt GA, Larimer ME, Witkiewitz K. Harm reduction: Pragmatic strategies for managing high-risk behaviors. Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  171. Marlatt GA, Somers JM, Tapert SF. Harm reduction: Application to alcohol abuse problems. NIDA Research Monograph. 1993;137:147–166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Martinez D, Gil R, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Perez A, … Abi-Dargham A. Alcohol dependence is associated with blunted dopamine transmission in the ventral striatum. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;58(10):779–786. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.044. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. McBride N, Farringdon F, Midford R, Meuleners L, Phillips M. Harm minimization in school drug education: final results of the School Health and Alcohol Harm Reduction Project (SHAHRP) Addiction. 2004;99(3):278–291. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00620.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00620.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. McManus F, Waller G. A functional analysis of binge-eating. Clinical Psychology Review. 1995;15(8):845–863. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(95)00042-9. [Google Scholar]
  175. Meule A, Gearhardt AN. Five years of the Yale Food Addiction Scale: Taking stock and moving forward. Current Addiction Reports. 2014;1(3):193–205. [Google Scholar]
  176. Meule A, Kubler A. Food cravings in food addiction: The distinct role of positive reinforcement. Eating Behaviors. 2012;13(3):252–255. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.02.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Meule A, Lutz A, Vogele C, Kubler A. Women with elevated food addiction symptoms show accelerated reactions, but no impaired inhibitory control, in response to pictures of high-calorie food-cues. Eating Behaviors. 2012;13(4):423–428. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.08.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Meule A, von Rezori V, Blechert J. Food addiction and bulimia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review. 2014;22(5):331–337. doi: 10.1002/erv.2306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.2306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Miller L. Predicting relapse and recovery in alcoholism and addiction: neuropsychology, personality, and cognitive style. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 1991;8(4):277–291. doi: 10.1016/0740-5472(91)90051-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Miller WR, Westerberg VS, Harris RJ, Tonigan JS. What predicts relapse? Prospective testing of antecedent models. Addiction. 1996;91(Suppl):S155–S172. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Mitchell JE, Mussell MP. Comorbidity and binge eating disorder. Addictive Behaviors. 1995;20(6):725–732. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(95)00095-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Mobbs O, Iglesias K, Golay A, Van der Linden M. Cognitive deficits in obese persons with and without binge eating disorder. Investigation using a mental flexibility task. Appetite. 2011;57(1):263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783–1793. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Monterosso JR, Ainslie G, Xu J, Cordova X, Domier CP, London ED. Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task. Human Brain Mapping. 2007;28(5):383–393. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Monti PM, Colby SM, Barnett NP, Spirito A, Rohsenow DJ, Myers M, … Lewander W. Brief intervention for harm reduction with alcohol-positive older adolescents in a hospital emergency department. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67(6):989–994. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Murphy CM, Stojek MK, MacKillop J. Interrelationships among impulsive personality traits, food addiction, and Body Mass Index. Appetite. 2014;73:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Murray SM, Tulloch AJ, Chen EY, Avena NM. Insights revealed by rodent models of sugar binge eating. CNS Spectrums. 2015;1–7 doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000656. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1092852915000656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Mussell MP, Mitchell JE, de Zwaan M, Crosby RD, Seim HC, Crow SJ. Clinical characteristics associated with binge eating in obese females: A descriptive study. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders. 1996;20(4):324–331. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Mussell MP, Mitchell JE, Weller CL, Raymond NC, Crow SJ, Crosby RD. Onset of binge eating, dieting, obesity, and mood disorders among subjects seeking treatment for binge eating disorder. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1995;17(4):395–401. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199505)17:4<395::aid-eat2260170412>3.0.co;2-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Myers MG, Brown SA. Coping responses and relapse among adolescent substance abusers. Journal of Substance Abuse. 1990;2(2):177–189. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(05)80054-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Nasser JA, Gluck ME, Geliebter A. Impulsivity and test meal intake in obese binge eating women. Appetite. 2004;43(3):303–307. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.04.006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Nederkoorn C, Braet C, Van Eijs Y, Tanghe A, Jansen A. Why obese children cannot resist food: The role of impulsivity. Eating Behaviors. 2006;7(4):315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Nederkoorn C, Jansen E, Mulkens S, Jansen A. Impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in obese children. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45(5):1071–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Nestler EJ, Malenka RC. The addicted brain. Scientific American. 2004;290(3):78–85. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0304-78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Ng L, Davis C. Cravings and food consumption in binge eating disorder. Eating Behaviors. 2013;14(4):472–475. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.08.011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Nisbett RE. Hunger, obesity, and the ventromedial hypothalamus. Psychological Review. 1972;79(6):433. doi: 10.1037/h0033519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Pelchat ML. Of human bondage: Food craving, obsession, compulsion, and addiction. Physiology & Behavior. 2002;76(3):347–352. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00757-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Pelchat ML, Johnson A, Chan R, Valdez J, Ragland JD. Images of desire: Food-craving activation during fMRI. NeuroImage. 2004;23(4):1486–1493. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Pivarunas B, Conner BT. Impulsivity and emotion dysregulation as predictors of food addiction. Eating Behaviors. 2015;19:9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.06.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Polivy J. Perception of calories and regulation of intake in restrained and unrestrained subjects. Addictive Behaviors. 1976;1(3):237–243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(76)90016-2. [Google Scholar]
  201. Polivy J, Herman CP. Dieting and binging. A causal analysis. The American Psychologist. 1985;40(2):193–201. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.40.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Potenza MN, Grilo CM. How relevant is food craving to obesity and its treatment? Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2014;5:164. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00164. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Pursey KM, Stanwell P, Gearhardt AN, Collins CE, Burrows TL. The prevalence of food addiction as assessed by the Yale Food Addiction Scale: A systematic review. Nutrients. 2014;6(10):4552–4590. doi: 10.3390/nu6104552. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/nu6104552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Ranzenhofer LM, Engel SG, Crosby RD, Anderson M, Vannucci A, Cohen LA, … Tanofsky-Kraff M. Using ecological momentary assessment to examine interpersonal and affective predictors of loss of control eating in adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2014;47(7):748–757. doi: 10.1002/eat.22333. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Reas DL, Grilo CM. Timing and sequence of the onset of overweight, dieting, and binge eating in overweight patients with binge eating disorder. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(2):165–170. doi: 10.1002/eat.20353. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Reas DL, Grilo CM. Current and emerging drug treatments for binge eating disorder. Expert Opinion on Emerging Drugs. 2014;19(1):99–142. doi: 10.1517/14728214.2014.879291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1517/14728214.2014.879291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Reas DL, Grilo CM. Pharmacological treatment of binge eating disorder: Update review and synthesis. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy. 2015;16(10):1463–1478. doi: 10.1517/14656566.2015.1053465. http://dx.doi.org/10.1517/14656566.2015.1053465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Reuter J, Raedler T, Rose M, Hand I, Glascher J, Buchel C. Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(2):147–148. doi: 10.1038/nn1378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Brain Research Reviews. 1993;18(3):247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction. 2001;96(1):103–114. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9611038.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09652140020016996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Rohde P, Stice E, Marti CN. Development and predictive effects of eating disorder risk factors during adolescence: Implications for prevention efforts. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2015;48(2):187–198. doi: 10.1002/eat.22270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Rosenthal RJ, Lesieur HR. Self-reported withdrawal symptoms and pathological gambling. The American Journal on Addictions. 1992;1(2):150–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1521-0391.1992.tb00020.x. [Google Scholar]
  213. Ruderman AJ, McKirnan DJ. The development of a restrained drinking scale: A test of the abstinence violation effect among alcohol users. Addictive Behaviors. 1984;9(4):365–371. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(84)90036-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Schag K, Teufel M, Junne F, Preissl H, Hautzinger M, Zipfel S, Giel KE. Impulsivity in binge eating disorder: Food cues elicit increased reward responses and disinhibition. PloS One. 2013;8(10):e76542. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Schebendach J, Broft A, Foltin RW, Walsh BT. Can the reinforcing value of food be measured in bulimia nervosa? Appetite. 2013;62:70–75. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Schienle A, Schafer A, Hermann A, Vaitl D. Binge-eating disorder: Reward sensitivity and brain activation to images of food. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65(8):654–661. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.028. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Schneider F, Habel U, Wagner M, Franke P, Salloum JB, Shah NJ, … Zilles K. Subcortical correlates of craving in recently abstinent alcoholic patients. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158(7):1075–1083. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Schulte EM, Avena NM, Gearhardt AN. Which foods may be addictive? The roles of processing, fat content, and glycemic load. PloS One. 2015;10(2):e0117959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Matching individuals to weight loss treatments: A survey of obesity experts. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1995;63(1):149–153. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.63.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Shea MT, Stout RL, Yen S, Pagano ME, Skodol AE, Morey LC, … Zanarini MC. Associations in the course of personality disorders and axis I disorders over time. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113(4):499–508. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Shiffman S, Waters AJ. Negative affect and smoking lapses: A prospective analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72(2):192–201. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology. 2001;158(4):343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Sinha R, Catapano D, O’Malley S. Stress-induced craving and stress response in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 1999;142(4):343–351. doi: 10.1007/s002130050898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Sinha R, Fuse T, Aubin LR, O’Malley SS. Psychological stress, drug-related cues and cocaine craving. Psychopharmacology. 2000;152(2):140–148. doi: 10.1007/s002130000499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Sinha R, Garcia M, Paliwal P, Kreek MJ, Rounsaville BJ. Stress-induced cocaine craving and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine relapse outcomes. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(3):324–331. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Sinha R, Lacadie C, Skudlarski P, Fulbright RK, Rounsaville BJ, Kosten TR, Wexler BE. Neural activity associated with stress-induced cocaine craving: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychopharmacology. 2005;183(2):171–180. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0147-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0147-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Sobik L, Hutchison K, Craighead L. Cue-elicited craving for food: A fresh approach to the study of binge eating. Appetite. 2005;44(3):253–262. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Sonneville KR, Grilo CM, Richmond TK, Thurston IB, Jernigan M, Gianini L, Field AE. Prospective association between overvaluation of weight and binge eating among overweight adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2015;56(1):25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Spanagel R, Weiss F. The dopamine hypothesis of reward: Past and current status. Trends in Neurosciences. 1999;22(11):521–527. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01447-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  230. Spencer JA, Fremouw WJ. Binge eating as a function of restraint and weight classification. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1979;88(3):262–267. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.88.3.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Spring B, Schneider K, Smith M, Kendzor D, Appelhans B, Hedeker D, Pagoto S. Abuse potential of carbohydrates for overweight carbohydrate cravers. Psychopharmacology. 2008;197(4):637–647. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1085-z. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-008-1085-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Spurrell EB, Wilfley DE, Tanofsky MB, Brownell KD. Age of onset for binge eating: Are there different pathways to binge eating? The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1997;21(1):55–65. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199701)21:1<55::aid-eat7>3.0.co;2-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Stice E, Burger K, Yokum S. Caloric deprivation increases responsivity of attention and reward brain regions to intake, anticipated intake, and images of palatable foods. NeuroImage. 2013a;67:322–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.028. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  234. Stice E, Burger KS, Yokum S. Relative ability of fat and sugar tastes to activate reward, gustatory, and somatosensory regions. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2013b;98(6):1377–1384. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.069443. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.113.069443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  235. Stice E, Akutagawa D, Gaggar A, Agras WS. Negative affect moderates the relation between dieting and binge eating. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2000;27(2):218–229. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(200003)27:2<218::aid-eat10>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  236. Stice E, Cameron RP, Killen JD, Hayward C, Taylor CB. Naturalistic weight-reduction efforts prospectively predict growth in relative weight and onset of obesity among female adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67(6):967–974. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  237. Stice E, Cooper JA, Schoeller DA, Tappe K, Lowe MR. Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of moderate- to long-term dietary restriction? Objective biological and behavioral data suggest not. Psychological Assessment. 2007;19(4):449–458. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  238. Stice E, Davis K, Miller NP, Marti CN. Fasting increases risk for onset of binge eating and bulimic pathology: A 5-year prospective study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117(4):941–946. doi: 10.1037/a0013644. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  239. Stice E, Fisher M, Lowe MR. Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of acute dietary restriction? Unobtrusive observational data suggest not. Psychological Assessment. 2004;16(1):51–59. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  240. Stice E, Marti CN, Durant S. Risk factors for onset of eating disorders: evidence of multiple risk pathways from an 8-year prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2011;49(10):622–627. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  241. Stice E, Presnell K, Spangler D. Risk factors for binge eating onset in adolescent girls: A 2-year prospective investigation. Health Psychology. 2002;21(2):131–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  242. Stice E, Sysko R, Roberto CA, Allison S. Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of dietary restriction? Additional objective behavioral and biological data suggest not. Appetite. 2010;54(2):331–339. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  243. Svaldi J, Brand M, Tuschen-Caffier B. Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder. Appetite. 2010b;54(1):84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  244. Svaldi J, Tuschen-Caffier B, Peyk P, Blechert J. Information processing of food pictures in binge eating disorder. Appetite. 2010a;55(3):685–694. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.10.002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  245. Tang D, Fellows L, Small D, Dagher A. Food and drug cues activate similar brain regions: A meta-analysis of functional MRI studies. Physiology & Behavior. 2012;106(3):317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  246. Telch CF, Agras WS. The effects of a very low calorie diet on binge eating. Behavior Therapy. 1993;24(2):177–193. [Google Scholar]
  247. Telch CF, Agras WS. Do emotional states influence binge eating in the obese? The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1996;20(3):271–279. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199611)20:3<271::AID-EAT6>3.0.CO;2-L. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199611)20:3<271::AID-EAT6>3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  248. Tomkins SS. Psychological model for smoking behavior. American Journal of Public Health and the Nations Health. 1966;56(12_Suppl):17–20. doi: 10.2105/ajph.56.12_suppl.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  249. Verebey K, Gold MS. From coca leaves to crack: The effects of dose and routes of administration in abuse liability. Psychiatric Annals. 1988;18(9):513–520. [Google Scholar]
  250. Verheul R, van den Brink W, Geerlings P. A three-pathway psychobiological model of craving for alcohol. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 1999;34(2):197–222. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/34.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  251. Vocks S, Tuschen-Caffier B, Pietrowsky R, Rustenbach SJ, Kersting A, Herpertz S. Meta-analysis of the effectiveness of psychological and pharmacological treatments for binge eating disorder. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2010;43(3):205–217. doi: 10.1002/eat.20696. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.20696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  252. Volkow ND, Fowler JS. Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: Involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10(3):318–325. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  253. Volkow ND, Wise RA. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8(5):555–560. doi: 10.1038/nn1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  254. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. Imaging studies on the role of dopamine in cocaine reinforcement and addiction in humans. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 1999;13(4):337–345. doi: 10.1177/026988119901300406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  255. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Baler R, Telang F. Imaging dopamine’s role in drug abuse and addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56(Suppl 1):3–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.05.022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  256. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: Evidence of systems pathology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2008b;363(1507):3191–3200. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  257. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, … Wong C. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: Mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(24):6583–6588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1544-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  258. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, … Wong C. Dopamine increases in striatum do not elicit craving in cocaine abusers unless they are coupled with cocaine cues. NeuroImage. 2008a;39(3):1266–1273. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.059. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  259. Wadden TA, Foster GD, Sarwer DB, Anderson DA, Gladis M, Sanderson RS, … Phelan S. Dieting and the development of eating disorders in obese women: Results of a randomized controlled trial. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2004;80(3):560–568. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/80.3.560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  260. Walsh BT. The importance of eating behavior in eating disorders. Physiology & Behavior. 2011;104(4):525–529. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  261. Wang GJ, Geliebter A, Volkow ND, Telang FW, Logan J, Jayne MC, … Fowler JS. Enhanced striatal dopamine release during food stimulation in binge eating disorder. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2011;19(8):1601–1608. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2011.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  262. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Cervany P, Hitzemann RJ, Pappas NR, … Felder C. Regional brain metabolic activation during craving elicited by recall of previous drug experiences. Life Sciences. 1999;64(9):775–784. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(98)00619-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  263. Ward A, Mann T. Don’t mind if I do: disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78(4):753. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.4.753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  264. Weddington WW, Brown BS, Haertzen CA, Cone EJ, Dax EM, Herning RI, Michaelson BS. Changes in mood, craving, and sleep during short-term abstinence reported by male cocaine addicts. A controlled, residential study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1990;47(9):861–868. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810210069010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  265. West RJ, Hajek P, Belcher M. Severity of withdrawal symptoms as a predictor of outcome of an attempt to quit smoking. Psychological Medicine. 1989;19(4):981–985. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700005705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  266. Weygandt M, Schaefer A, Schienle A, Haynes JD. Diagnosing different binge-eating disorders based on reward-related brain activation patterns. Human Brain Mapping. 2012;33(9):2135–2146. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  267. White MA, Grilo CM. Psychometric properties of the Food Craving Inventory among obese patients with binge eating disorder. Eating Behaviors. 2005;6(3):239–245. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.01.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  268. White MA, Grilo CM. Psychiatric comorbidity in binge-eating disorder as a function of smoking history. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67(4):594–599. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  269. White MA, Whisenhunt BL, Williamson DA, Greenway FL, Netemeyer RG. Development and validation of the food-craving inventory. Obesity Research. 2002;10(2):107–114. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2002.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  270. Whiteside U, Chen E, Neighbors C, Hunter D, Lo T, Larimer M. Difficulties regulating emotions: Do binge eaters have fewer strategies to modulate and tolerate negative affect? Eating Behaviors. 2007;8(2):162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.04.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  271. Wilfley DE, Schwartz MB, Spurrell EB, Fairburn CG. Using the eating disorder examination to identify the specific psychopathology of binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2000;27(3):259–269. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(200004)27:3<259::aid-eat2>3.0.co;2-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  272. Wilson GT. Relation of dieting and voluntary weight loss to psychological functioning and binge eating. Annals of Internal Medicine. 1993;119(7_Part_2):727–730. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-119-7_part_2-199310011-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  273. Wilson GT, Grilo CM, Vitousek KM. Psychological treatment of eating disorders. The American Psychologist. 2007;62(3):199–216. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  274. Wilson GT, Wilfley DE, Agras WS, Bryson SW. Psychological treatments of binge eating disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(1):94–101. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  275. de Wit H. Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: A review of underlying processes. Addiction Biology. 2009;14(1):22–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  276. Witt AA, Lowe MR. Hedonic hunger and binge eating among women with eating disorders. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2014;47(3):273–280. doi: 10.1002/eat.22171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  277. Wright SM, Aronne LJ. Causes of obesity. Abdominal Imaging. 2012;37(5):730–732. doi: 10.1007/s00261-012-9862-x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00261-012-9862-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  278. Yang Z, Xie J, Shao YC, Xie CM, Fu LP, Li DJ, … Li SJ. Dynamic neural responses to cue-reactivity paradigms in heroin-dependent users: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30(3):766–775. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  279. Yanovski SZ, Leet M, Yanovski JA, Flood M, Gold PW, Kissileff HR, Walsh BT. Food selection and intake of obese women with binge-eating disorder. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1992;56(6):975–980. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/56.6.975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  280. Ziauddeen H, Fletcher PC. Is food addiction a valid and useful concept? Obesity Reviews. 2013;14(1):19–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01046.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01046.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  281. Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: How convincing is the addiction model? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13(4):279–286. doi: 10.1038/nrn3212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  282. Zimmering P, Toolan J, Safrin R, Wortis SB. Heroin addiction in adolescent boys. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1951;114(1):19–34. doi: 10.1097/00005053-195107000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  283. de Zwaan M. Binge eating disorder and obesity. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders. 2001;25(Suppl 1):S51–S55. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801699. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0801699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES