Abstract
After psychological trauma, recurrent intrusive visual memories may be distressing and disruptive. Preventive interventions post trauma are lacking. Here we test a behavioural intervention after real-life trauma derived from cognitive neuroscience. We hypothesized that intrusive memories would be significantly reduced in number by an intervention involving a computer game with high visuospatial demands (Tetris), via disrupting consolidation of sensory elements of trauma memory. The Tetris-based intervention (trauma memory reminder cue plus c. 20 min game play) vs attention-placebo control (written activity log for same duration) were both delivered in an emergency department within 6 h of a motor vehicle accident. The randomized controlled trial compared the impact on the number of intrusive trauma memories in the subsequent week (primary outcome). Results vindicated the efficacy of the Tetris-based intervention compared with the control condition: there were fewer intrusive memories overall, and time-series analyses showed that intrusion incidence declined more quickly. There were convergent findings on a measure of clinical post-trauma intrusion symptoms at 1 week, but not on other symptom clusters or at 1 month. Results of this proof-of-concept study suggest that a larger trial, powered to detect differences at 1 month, is warranted. Participants found the intervention easy, helpful and minimally distressing. By translating emerging neuroscientific insights and experimental research into the real world, we offer a promising new low-intensity psychiatric intervention that could prevent debilitating intrusive memories following trauma.
Introduction
After psychological trauma, sensory memories can recurrently spring to mind unbidden,1 bringing back sights and sounds of the events, evoking strong emotion, hijacking attention and profoundly disrupting current activities. Intrusive memories comprise a core clinical feature2 of acute stress disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).1 In the first days post trauma, intrusive memories (among other symptoms) have been associated with a diagnosis of PTSD at 1 year,3 and early intrusion symptoms with a non-remitting PTSD trajectory over 15 months.4 Intrusive memories also occur across a range of other mental disorders from depression5 to complicated grief,6 comprising an important transdiagnostic target7 for preventive psychiatric interventions. However, currently preventive interventions after trauma targeting the full syndrome of PTSD are either ineffective8, 9 or unappealing/inaccessible10, 11 to most people. New approaches are needed—we suggest targeting preventive efforts on a focal symptom—here, intrusive memories of the trauma.
We have called for the development of mechanistically driven behavioural interventions,12 preferably low-intensity and deliverable by non-specialists to allow for scalability.13 A potential treatment opportunity as a cognitive 'therapeutic vaccine' (ref 14, p 1315) delivered post trauma15 to limit the reoccurrence of intrusive memories is offered by a combination of insights from neuroscience. Memory consolidation theory suggests a time window of several hours post trauma during which trauma memory is malleable and vulnerable to disruption.16, 17 Animal research examining molecular and cellular processes in memory consolidation demonstrates that it is possible to interfere with fear memory stabilization soon after learning, for example, using the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin.18, 19, 20 In humans, however, protein synthesis inhibitors are toxic: studies using the β-blocker propranolol as a consolidation blockade have not yet shown success,21, 22 and electroconvulsive therapy, while effective in disrupting memory,23 cannot be used readily in the aftermath of trauma. Techniques are needed to target the intrusive nature of memories specifically as this is associated with clinical impairment. Cognitive science predicts that cognitive tasks with high visuospatial demands will selectively disrupt sensory (predominantly visual) aspects of memory (i.e. those that underpin intrusions) via competition for limited cognitive resources24, 25, 26 when that memory is labile. Combined, these insights suggest that engaging in cognitive tasks with high visuospatial demands during the time window of trauma memory consolidation may reduce the occurrence of subsequent intrusive visual memories of trauma.
Using an experimental analogue of a traumatic event (film footage) under controlled laboratory settings, we have tested the relative efficacy of various cognitive tasks in reducing intrusive memories. In line with the hypotheses, the results show that visuospatial tasks (e.g. complex pattern tapping) during or soon after the event consistently lead to a reduction in the number of subsequent intrusive memories,15, 27, 28, 29, 30 whereas some verbal tasks (e.g. backwards counting; the verbal computer game Pub Quiz) do not and can even increase intrusions indicating possible harmful effects15, 27, 31, 32 (though see Van den Hout and Engelhard33). We suggest that visuospatial cognitive tasks act not merely via distraction, but by modality-specific interference with sensory (visual) aspects of intrusive memory. Critical to future translation to real trauma, we have shown that a popular and widely available visually absorbing computer game (playing 'Tetris')34 is also effective as a component of a behavioural intervention compared with control conditions (a verbal 'Pub Quiz' game or no task).15, 29, 35
In a parallel randomized controlled trial, we tested the hypothesis that a behavioural intervention (playing the visually absorbing computer game Tetris after a reminder cue), compared with an attention-placebo (ref 36, p 191) control condition (a written activity log), would reduce the number of intrusive memories of trauma over the subsequent week. The control condition was selected for nonspecific factors including time, contact with the researcher, location of treatment procedure and engagement in a structured task. Unlike the intervention condition, the control condition contained no reminder cue for the traumatic event. Study information and materials referred to 'simple activities' in both conditions for credibility (Supplementary Information). The primary outcome was the number of intrusive memories 1 week post accident (as logged in a daily diary); secondary outcomes were post-trauma symptomatology, anxiety and depression (1 week/1 month). Participant feedback was assessed at 1 month.
Materials and methods
Participants and procedure
Participants were 71 patients presenting to the emergency department of the John Radcliffe Hospital (Oxford, UK) (Figure 1): 34 were men and 37 were women, and the mean age was 39.66 years (s.d. 16.32). Inclusion criteria were: age ⩾18 years; experienced/witnessed a motor vehicle accident (as a driver, passenger, motorcyclist or pedestrian); met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edition (DSM-IV) PTSD criterion A1 for a traumatic event ('experienced, witnessed or was confronted with an event or events that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury'); seen in emergency department within 6 h of leaving scene of the accident; reported memory of the accident; fluent in written and spoken English; alert and orientated, Glasgow Coma Scale37 score=15; and sufficient physical mobility to play a computer game on the intervention platform (Nintendo DS) at the point of taking informed consent. Exclusion criteria were loss of consciousness for >5 min, reported history of severe mental illness, current intoxication, substance abuse or neurological condition, or currently suicidal. The study was approved by the local National Research Ethics Service Research Ethics Committee (Oxford C: 12/SC/0485).
Potential participants were identified by emergency department staff. Eligibility was assessed by a clinical psychologist (LI) using information from medical records and face-to-face interview. After receiving a description of the study, all participants provided written informed consent before completing baseline measures. Participants were then randomly allocated in a 1:1 ratio to two parallel treatment conditions using a web-based randomization system, provided by the Oxford Cognitive Health and Neurosciences Clinical Trials Unit and verified by independent statisticians. Randomization was used to balance groups on known and unknown baseline predictors of outcome,38 and carried out using minimization39 based on gender, age and perceived life threat to self, with an additional random component to ensure allocations remained unpredictable. The randomization system was accessed by the researcher (LI) from a separate office after baseline measures had been completed. Participants were not informed as to condition allocation. The researcher delivering the procedures (LI) was not blind to participant allocation, as the need to provide verbal instructions precluded such blinding.
Outcome assessment was scheduled for 1 week and 1 month after the accident. After 1-month follow-up, participants were contacted by telephone and debriefed. They were offered a £30 GBP ($45 USD) store voucher to compensate them for their time. Recruitment occurred between March 2014 and January 2015. The last follow-up assessment was completed in February 2015. The trial ended once the planned sample size had been achieved.
Assessments
Traumatic event and emergency department treatment characteristics
Details of participants’ traumatic event, treatment in the emergency department and previous emergency department attendances were collected from medical records. Severity of physical injury, indicated by the Injury Severity Score (range 0–75), was rated using the Abbreviated Injury Scale.40 Agreement for injury codings between raters (LI and a research nurse) was 100%. Participants rated perceived life threat during the accident: 'to what extent did you feel your life was in danger?' and 'to what extent did you feel that someone else’s life was in danger?' from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely).41 Dissociative symptoms were assessed with the Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire-Self Report.42 Emotional responses were assessed with the Peritraumatic Distress Inventory.43 Participants reported experience of prior psychological trauma, current and past mental illness, and family history of mental illness.
Primary outcome
The total number of intrusive memories in the week after the traumatic event was assessed using a daily pen-and-paper diary (adapted from refs 15, 29, 35). Participants recorded the occurrence of intrusive memories in everyday life by ticking a box for the day and time period (morning/afternoon/evening) when the intrusive memory occurred, or marked 'zero' if they experienced none. Intrusive memories were described as: 'image-based memories of the accident that pop into your mind without warning. They often take the form of visual pictures in your mind’s eye, for example, like a snapshot image or a film clip. They can also include other senses, for example, sounds and smells'. Participants were not to record memories recalled deliberately or general verbal thoughts. For examples of intrusive memories see Supplementary Table 2. The diary started on the day of the accident ('Day 1') and was completed for seven days. Daily reminders to complete the diary were sent via SMS. Upon completion, participants rated 'how accurately do you think you completed the diary?' from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). Participants returned the diary by post. The number of intrusive memories was selected for several reasons: it allows comparison with preclinical work; was readily understood by traumatized patients without the need for explanation from an expert; and is directly relevant to the clinical goal of reducing the number of times the memory intruded (not just reducing its intensity).
Secondary outcomes
Post-trauma distress was assessed using the Impact of Event Scale—Revised,40, 45 which has subscales for intrusion, avoidance and hyperarousal symptoms. PTSD symptom severity was assessed with the Post-traumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa46). Anxiety and depression symptoms were assessed with the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.47 To minimize assessor bias and participant burden, measures were completed remotely (online using the secure web-based software 'Qualtrics'48 or by post).
Participant feedback
A 13-item feedback questionnaire assessed participants’ experience of the study. Items included ratings of how easy, helpful and distressing/burdensome participants found playing Tetris on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 9 (extremely), expectancy ratings for their condition on number of intrusive memories on a 21-point scale from −10 (extreme decrease), 0 (no effect) to +10 (extreme increase)35 and open questions.
Treatment conditions
Participants in both treatment conditions received usual care in the emergency department (e.g. assessments and medical treatment). The condition-specific procedures below were delivered around usual care by a clinical psychologist (LI). Both condition-specific procedures were standardized and structured.
Intervention
The intervention procedure involved two key components: a reminder cue for the traumatic event followed by playing the computer game Tetris.29 For the memory reminder cue, participants were asked to think back to the accident and briefly tell the researcher the worst moments that came to mind.49 Following instructions and practice, participants played Tetris on a Nintendo DS XL. Participants were required to undertake minimum Tetris game play for at least one uninterrupted period of 10 min and for ~20 min in total.
Control
In the control procedure participants filled in a simple activity log to note down each activity they had already engaged in during their time in the emergency department. They wrote brief entries in a list in a column (e.g., reading, talking, receiving treatment, crossword, texting) and recorded each activity’s duration (in minutes) in a second column. Following instructions of how to complete the log, participants completed it using pen-and-paper on a clipboard for ~20 min (a similar duration to the intervention condition).
Time spent playing Tetris and filling the activity log was equivalent; and total time spent in the emergency department, during which other activities were unrestricted, was equivalent in the two conditions (Supplementary Information).
Data analysis
Power analysis
Using a conservative estimated effect size of Cohen’s d=0.7, based on a previous laboratory study,29 a total sample size of n=66 was required to provide 80% power at α=0.05, two-tailed. Recruitment of at least 70 participants was planned to allow for attrition.
Main efficacy analyses
Analyses were intention-to-treat, including all randomized participants. Missing values were estimated using multiple imputation. Five data sets were generated for each missing value at 1 week follow-up, and 10 data sets for each missing value at 1-month follow-up, in line with the recommendation that the number of data sets approximates the percentage of missing data.50 Treatment group, age, gender and perceived life threat to self were included as auxiliary variables.51 Estimates were pooled in line with guidelines for multiple imputation.52 As all between-group comparisons in continuous outcomes at 1 week and 1 month were planned a priori, differences were tested using two-tailed t-tests at α=0.05 (ref 53, p 372). Cohen’s d effect sizes were calculated as t[Sqrt(1/n1+1/n2)], and 95% confidence intervals for the effect size were calculated using ESCI software.54 Percentage of participants with a symptom profile on the PDS consistent with a DSM-IV diagnosis of PTSD was compared between conditions using logistic regression (2 × 2 χ2 test) at α=0.05, two-tailed. Efficacy analyses were conducted in SPSS version 2255 by the first author and verified by a statistician (PCW) blind to condition. Before analysis, 100% of raw data for the primary efficacy analysis, and a randomly selected 10% of raw data for the secondary efficacy analyses, were checked for accuracy by a researcher not involved in data collection and blind to condition. Analyses were also conducted on a 'per-protocol' population (Supplementary Table 3).
Exploratory analyses
To investigate the time course of intrusive memories reported in the daily diary over the first seven days after the accident, frequency scattergraphs showing the distribution of the number of intrusive memories on each day per condition were plotted. A nonlinear time-series analysis was used to produce a nonparametric line of best fit, summarizing the distribution of the number of intrusive memories on each day, smoothed from day to day over the 7-day period, by accounting for the number of intrusive memories at nearby time points (autocorrelation). This was achieved by fitting counts of the number of intrusive memories for each participant (Y) through time (t) with a generalized additive model:56
where u is a random variable of time and s(t, 4) is the smoother with four effective degrees of freedom (as in James et al.35). Expected Poisson distributions at day 2 and day 7 were generated. Time-series analyses were undertaken in R using data provided by participants who returned the diary (n=67).
Code availability
The computer code used to generate the results can be accessed via Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/e4hc7.
Results
Participants
Figure 1 shows the participant flow diagram. Table 1 provides sample, traumatic event and emergency department treatment characteristics for the intervention and control conditions, with no significant differences between conditions.
Table 1. Sample, traumatic event and emergency department treatment characteristics of study participants by treatment condition.
Sample characteristics |
Intervention (n=37) |
Control (n=34) |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | S.d. | Mean | S.d. | |
Age (years) | 38.9 | 16.1 | 40.5 | 16.8 |
Years in education | 15.9 | 3.3 | 15.2 | 3.2 |
n | % | n | % | |
Gender | ||||
Female | 20 | 54.1 | 17 | 50.0 |
Male | 17 | 45.9 | 17 | 50.0 |
Ethnicity group | ||||
White British | 28 | 75.7 | 28 | 82.4 |
Ethnic minority | 9 | 24.3 | 6 | 17.6 |
Marital status | ||||
Single | 18 | 48.6 | 17 | 50.0 |
Married or cohabiting | 17 | 45.9 | 14 | 41.2 |
Divorced | 2 | 5.4 | 2 | 5.9 |
Widowed | 0 | 0 | 1 | 2.9 |
Employment status | ||||
Employed | 26 | 70.3 | 24 | 70.6 |
Unemployed | 0 | 0 | 1 | 2.9 |
Student | 7 | 18.9 | 5 | 14.7 |
Retired | 4 | 10.8 | 4 | 11.8 |
n | % | n | % | |
Traumatic event | ||||
DSM-IV PTSD criterion A1 | 37 | 100 | 34 | 100 |
Experienced event | 37 | 100 | 34 | 100 |
Witnessed event | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Brought in by ambulance | 29 | 78.4 | 25 | 73.5 |
Type of motor vehicle accident | ||||
Car/van/bus driver | 19 | 51.4 | 13 | 38.2 |
Car/van passenger | 0 | 0 | 4 | 11.8 |
Motorcyclist | 6 | 16.2 | 5 | 14.7 |
Cyclist | 12 | 32.4 | 8 | 23.5 |
Pedestrian | 0 | 0 | 4 | 11.8 |
Perceived life threat to self (score>0) | 31 | 83.8 | 31 | 91.2 |
Perceived life threat to other (score>0) | 16 | 43.2 | 19 | 55.9 |
Mean | S.d. | Mean | S.d. | |
Perceived life threat to self | 5.19 | 3.20 | 5.56 | 3.23 |
Perceived life threat to someone else | 2.22 | 3.25 | 3.56 | 3.99 |
Time since traumatic event (min) | 192 | 69 | 211 | 67 |
Injury Severity Score | 1.46 | 2.34 | 1.97 | 2.10 |
PDEQ score | 19.86 | 8.02 | 19.18 | 8.40 |
PDI score | 18.70 | 10.36 | 16.59 | 10.34 |
n | % | n | % | |
Treatment in emergency department | ||||
Location in emergency department | ||||
Resuscitation | 8 | 21.6 | 6 | 17.6 |
Majors | 11 | 27.9 | 15 | 44.1 |
Minors/other | 18 | 48.6 | 13 | 38.2 |
Admitted as in-patient | 10 | 27.0 | 10 | 29.4 |
Received opiate medication | 8 | 21.6 | 9 | 26.5 |
n | % | n | % | |
History of trauma and mental illness | ||||
Prior psychological trauma | 28 | 77.8 | 24 | 70.6 |
Current/past mental illness | 6 | 16.2 | 6 | 17.6 |
Family history of mental illness | 10 | 27.8 | 7 | 20.6 |
Number of previous emergency department attendances in last year | ||||
0 | 31 | 83.8 | 26 | 76.5 |
1–4 | 6 | 16.2 | 8 | 23.5 |
Abbreviations: DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition; PDEQ, Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire; PDI, Peritraumatic Distress Inventory; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.
Treatment adherence, attrition and adverse effects
All participants allocated to the intervention condition completed the memory reminder cue, and only one participant did not play Tetris for the minimum required duration of 10 min uninterrupted (they were moved by staff to a different bay). All participants allocated to the control condition completed the activity log. Self-report accuracy ratings for completion of the daily diary were high and did not differ between conditions. Attrition (whole sample) was 6% at 1 week and 13% at 1 month. Further details regarding diary completion accuracy ratings and attrition are provided in Supplementary Information. No adverse effects (i.e., negative reactions to the treatment procedures, such as significantly increased distress or suicidality) were recorded in either condition.
Main efficacy analyses
Continuous outcomes were transformed to correct for skewness using the natural logarithmic function with the exception of the 1-week Impact of Event Scale—Revised intrusion subscale score (which approximated a normal distribution). Variances were similar between conditions on all outcomes. Table 2 shows intention-to-treat analyses for primary and secondary outcomes. Per-protocol analyses showed the same pattern of results (Supplementary Table 3).
Table 2. Intention-to-treat results for primary and secondary outcomes in the trial.
Continuous outcome |
Intervention (n=37) |
Control (n=34) |
Analysis |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | S.d. | Mean | S.d. | ta | d | 95% CI for d | |
Primary outcome, 1 week | |||||||
Number of intrusive memories of traumatic event | 8.73 | 11.55 | 23.26 | 32.99 | 2.80** | 0.67 | 0.18, 1.14 |
Secondary outcomes, 1 week | |||||||
Impact of Event Scale—Revised | |||||||
Intrusion subscale | 7.27 | 5.27 | 10.70 | 7.29 | 2.25* | 0.54 | 0.06, 1.01 |
Avoidance subscale | 7.69 | 8.11 | 8.07 | 7.90 | 0.26 | 0.06 | −0.41, 0.53 |
Hyperarousal subscale | 5.26 | 5.79 | 6.98 | 7.42 | 0.96 | 0.23 | −0.24, 0.70 |
Total | 20.85 | 19.92 | 25.73 | 21.21 | 1.11 | 0.26 | −0.21, 0.73 |
Post-traumatic Diagnostic Scale | 11.38 | 8.55 | 14.28 | 11.94 | 0.83 | 0.20 | −0.27, 0.67 |
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale | 7.96 | 6.27 | 9.83 | 8.43 | 0.61 | 0.15 | −0.32, 0.61 |
Secondary outcomes, 1 month | |||||||
Impact of Event Scale—Revised | |||||||
Intrusion subscale | 5.21 | 5.09 | 7.01 | 6.90 | 0.93 | 0.22 | −0.25, 0.69 |
Avoidance subscale | 4.80 | 6.21 | 4.87 | 6.64 | 0.01 | 0.00 | −0.47, 0.47 |
Hyperarousal subscale | 4.29 | 6.47 | 5.28 | 6.45 | 0.59 | 0.14 | −0.33, 0.61 |
Total | 14.47 | 15.09 | 17.32 | 20.39 | 0.46 | 0.11 | −0.36, 0.58 |
PDS | 9.54 | 9.20 | 10.21 | 11.26 | 0.29 | 0.07 | −0.40, 0.54 |
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale | 7.44 | 7.20 | 8.12 | 8.20 | 0.18 | 0.04 | −0.42, 0.51 |
Categorical outcome, 1 month | n | % | n | % | βb | OR | 95% CI for OR |
PDS symptoms consistent with PTSD criteria | 4 | 12.9 | 3 | 9.7 | 0.34 | 1.4 | 0.28, 7.09 |
Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio; PDS, Post-traumatic Diagnostic Scale; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.
*P<0.05; **P<0.01.
d.f.=69.
Logistic regression, d.f.=1.
Primary outcome
Participants in the intervention condition recorded significantly fewer intrusive memories in the week after the accident than participants in the control condition, with a medium effect size57 (M=8.73 vs M=23.26, t(69)=2.80, P=0.005, d=0.67, 95% CI: 0.18,1.14) (Figure 2a).
Secondary outcomes
At 1 week follow-up, participants in the intervention condition reported less distress from intrusion symptoms (Impact of Event Scale—Revised intrusion subscale) than participants in the control condition, with a medium effect size (Table 2). Effect sizes for all other measures at one week, and all measures at one month, were small to negligible (Table 2).
Exploratory analyses
From nonlinear time-series analysis, the generalized additive model summarizing the distribution of the number of intrusive memories on each of the first seven days after the accident indicated fewer intrusive memories across the seven days, as well as a more marked decline over the seven days, in the intervention compared with the control condition (Figure 2b). Poisson distributions revealed that the expected probability of having zero intrusive memories showed a greater increase over time for participants in the intervention condition (from 13.8% on day 2 to 59.3% on day 7) compared with participants in the control condition (from 1.5% on day 2 to 5.6% at day 7) (Figure 3).
Participant feedback
Feedback ratings indicated that participants in the intervention condition found playing Tetris very easy (median=7), very helpful (median=7) and minimally distressing/burdensome (median=1).
Examples of participants’ experience of the intervention follow:
One woman in her 20s described having repeated intrusive memories of her motor vehicle accident trauma: 'the picture of falling on the street with my head kept popping up in my head' and 'seeing blood dripping'. She engaged well with playing Tetris in the emergency department and found it fun. 'I think it helped a lot to distract my mind after [the] accident by playing Tetris'.
A woman in her 60s who had never played Tetris before, nor used a Nintendo DS, described playing the game as 'good, really fun'. She asked to continue playing for a little longer, even when the 20-min intervention period was over. At the end of the study, she commented: 'It certainly took my mind off of it at a time when I probably would have sat brooding and feeling very sorry for myself… when you're running the whole thing through your mind and you're on your own at a vulnerable time after the ambulance crew have left you'.
One man in his late 30s described intrusive images of seeing the tree just before the moment of collision, followed by the white flash of the airbag. He wrote at the end of the study: 'I think that playing Tetris helped focus my mind and bring some 'normality' back to my head. I didn't dwell on the accident too much while I was in hospital. Playing Tetris seemed a bit strange at the time, but looking back it has been a help. Thank you'.
Discussion
An experimental science-driven intervention to reduce intrusive memories of real-world trauma, in patients presenting to a hospital emergency department after a traumatic motor vehicle accident involving directly experienced actual or threatened death or serious injury, was effective compared with an attention-placebo control. This hypothesis-driven work is at the interface between preclinical and clinical research in psychiatry, psychology and post-traumatic reactions. Its principles are derived from molecular, cellular and neural research on memory consolidation16, 18, 19, 20 alongside the neuroscience of visual memory,58 deploying a behavioural technique to modify emotional intrusive memory. Results provide a critical and compelling translation of previous laboratory findings with experimental trauma15, 29, 35 to the ‘real world’. The behavioural intervention with traumatized individuals comprised two steps: (i) a trauma memory reminder cue, followed by (ii) engaging for at least 10 min in a computer game with high visuospatial demands (Tetris)—hypothesized to compete with consolidation of visual memories of trauma. The intervention reduced the number of intrusive memories by 62% in the subsequent week compared with control.
This result with patients compares favourably to previous laboratory-based trauma simulation studies, which found, for example, a relative reduction in intrusive memory count by 58% over the same time period.29 Time-series analyses revealed an accelerated recovery from intrusive memories over the first week in the intervention condition, mirroring laboratory findings.35 There were convergent findings on a measure of clinical post-trauma intrusion symptoms (Impact of Event Scale—Revised intrusion subscale) at 1 week, but not on other symptom clusters or at 1 month. The current study was designed to detect an effect on the primary outcome measure at 1 week; results suggest that a larger trial, powered to detect differences at 1 month, is warranted.
This brief 'therapist-free' technological intervention was found to be feasible and acceptable—with 48% of patients approached agreeing to participate (compared with 10% in a psychotherapy trial11 and 8% in a pharmacological trial,21 both also in the emergency department), and with intervention completion at 97%. No adverse effects were observed or reported. Comparison with the wider population of motor vehicle accident survivors seen in the emergency department during the period of data collection suggested our sample had a greater severity of physical trauma (Supplementary Table 4). Patients were reached within a short time post trauma (~3 h after their motor vehicle accident), that is, within a memory consolidation time window as planned.
Our experimental medicine approach targets one core clinical feature—intrusive memories of trauma—rather than a whole psychiatric syndrome (ref 59, p 22). We found effects specific to this subset of symptoms (cf. Soeter and Kindt60), that is, those related to the hypothesized mechanisms of the intervention. After a comprehensive therapy for full PTSD such as trauma-focussed cognitive therapy, symptom clusters change together;61 that our data suggests the possibility that specific symptom subsets may be targeted raises intriguing questions about the disorder's heterogeneity and inner structure.62 The distinction between intrusive vs voluntarily recalled memories is important for treatment development. The critical difference between clinical and non-clinical memories of a traumatic event is that the former spring to mind unbidden (i.e. are intrusive), and not that a memory of the traumatic event exists per se. Indeed, we have argued that 'erasure' of the whole trauma memory could be undesirable,63 as patients may need to deliberately remember events, for example, for legal testimony.
Preventive interventions post trauma are currently lacking. Motor vehicle accidents are common traumatic events,64 and after-effects in terms of mental disorder well established.65, 66 In the emergency department, patients typically wait up to 4 h or more in the United Kingdom,67 providing an opportunity to reach patients within a few hours of a traumatic event, that is, within the putative timeframe for memory consolidation. The feasibility of implementing the intervention68 is promising due to its brevity, low cost, simplicity for training and delivery, and flexibility of administration (Supplementary Information).
A brief, science-driven intervention offers a low-intensity means that could substantially improve the mental health of those who have experienced psychological trauma—and for we believe the first time offers a cognitive 'therapeutic vaccine' (ref 14, p 1315) that could be administered soon after a traumatic event (cf. rabies vaccine after a dog bite) to prevent intrusive memories of trauma in the subsequent week. It will be important to build on the findings of this study to understand which intervention aspects are critical. For example, our intervention comprised both a trauma memory reminder and Tetris game play, in line with our aim of translating to a clinical setting our earlier laboratory findings during a putative time window for reconsolidation35 and consolidation.15, 29 The choice of including a reminder cue during memory consolidation may be surprising, but recent experimental work suggests that even then a cue may be critical.69 We reasoned that reminder cues serve additional functions beyond reactivation and rendering memory labile—such as orienting the interference procedure to more targeted elements within the memory trace. This idea is relevant for complex memories,70, 71 here visual elements of complex real-world trauma scenes. In contrast, in animal studies in which pharmacological agents block the protein synthesis necessary for memory consolidation,19 such blockade is likely to affect multiple elements of a freshly encoded experience, rather than one specific aspect. Further work is needed to unpick this mechanism. We hypothesize that not only Tetris but any task with high visuospatial demands is likely to be useful within the procedure (e.g. games such as Candy Crush, drawing) unlike predominantly verbally distracting tasks (e.g. reading, crosswords). However, either element of the intervention in isolation (memory reminder/visuospatial task) is unlikely to be effective.35
This is an early-phase or explanatory trial to establish the efficacy of the intervention (ref 72, p 23), and the activity log was selected to control for as many nonspecific confounding factors as possible, while minimizing the potential for harmful effects.15, 31 There is no comparator preventive treatment in the immediate aftermath of trauma8, 9, 73 to use as a control. Future mechanism-focussed research should seek improved controls: this requires innovation, as in comparison with drug trials, for psychological interventions it is challenging to create an 'inactive' control that resembles the active treatment.74 While it is not possible to blind participants in psychological trials, in the current study expectations do not appear to be associated with performance on the primary outcome measure (Supplementary Information).
Preventive mental health interventions are needed post trauma. Here we find a positive effect of the Tetris-based intervention delivered soon after trauma in the emergency department on the primary outcome measure—intrusive memories of trauma over 1 week. Future studies are needed, designed to test whether effects extend to 1 month or longer. Only one 'dose' of the 20 min intervention was given, and the opportunity to give multiple doses and/or longer durations should be explored. The clinical utility to patients of a brief intervention that reduces their symptoms even if limited to the first week post trauma should also be explored. Critically, as yet successful translations of contemporary neuroscience into mental health treatments have been lacking. This study illuminates how combining clinical (trauma), neuroscientific (memory consolidation) and cognitive (cognitive task competition) theory can lead to a novel and effective clinical technique—opening the way for other mechanistically driven behavioural treatment innovations.
Acknowledgments
LI was supported by a National Institute for Health Research Doctoral Research Fellowship (DRF-2011-04-076). EAH and SEB were supported by the Medical Research Council (United Kingdom) intramural programme (MRC-A060-5PR50). EAH was also supported by Karolinska Institutet. RMS was supported by a Medical Research Council Clinician Scientist Fellowship (G0802821). ACN was supported by a Wellcome Trust Senior Investigator Award (104571/Z/14/Z). JRG is an NIHR Senior Investigator. The authors thank the emergency department research and nursing team at the John Radcliffe Hospital Oxford, the Oxford Cognitive Health and Neuroscience Clinical Trials Unit and the patients who participated in the trial. Funding to pay the Open Access publication charges for this article was provided by the United Kingdom Medical Research Council (MC-A060-5PR50). Data and Materials will be made available via Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/e4hc7/, or are otherwise available from the authors on request (with the exception of questionnaire measures subject to third-party copyright or potentially identifying patient information). Protocols for study procedures are subject to a Material Transfer Agreement.
Footnotes
Supplementary Information accompanies the paper on the Molecular Psychiatry website (http://www.nature.com/mp)
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
References
- American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Kupfer DJ, Regier DA. Neuroscience, clinical evidence, and the future of psychiatric classification in DSM-5. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168: 172–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creamer M, O'Donnell ML, Pattison P. The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in severely injured trauma survivors. Behav Res Ther 2004; 42: 315–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galatzer-Levy IR, Karstoft K-I, Statnikov A, Shalev AY. Quantitative forecasting of PTSD from early trauma responses: a machine learning application. J Psychiatr Res 2014; 59: 68–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birrer E, Michael T, Munsch S. Intrusive images in PTSD and in traumatised and non-traumatised depressed patients: a cross-sectional clinical study. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45: 2053–2065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant RA, Kenny L, Joscelyne A, Rawson N, Maccallum F, Cahill C et al. Treating prolonged grief disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71: 1332–1339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a 'transdiagnostic' theory and treatment. Behav Res Ther 2003; 41: 509–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts NP, Kitchiner NJ, Kenardy J, Bisson JI. Multiple session early psychological interventions for the prevention of post-traumatic stress disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2009; 3: CD006869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sijbrandij M, Kleiboer A, Bisson JI, Barbui C, Cuijpers P. Pharmacological prevention of post-traumatic stress disorder and acute stress disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2015; 2: 413–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kornor H, Winje D, Ekeberg O, Weisaeth L, Kirkehei I, Johansen K et al. Early trauma-focused cognitive-behavioural therapy to prevent chronic post-traumatic stress disorder and related symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 2008; 8: 81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbaum BO, Kearns MC, Price M, Malcoun E, Davis M, Ressler KJ et al. Early intervention may prevent the development of posttraumatic stress disorder: a randomized pilot civilian study with modified prolonged exposure. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 72: 957–963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes EA, Craske MG, Graybiel AM. Psychological treatments: a call for mental-health science. Clinicians and neuroscientists must work together to understand and improve psychological treatments [comment]. Nature 2014; 511: 287–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazdin AE, Blase SL. Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice to reduce the burden of mental illness. Perspect Psychol Sci 2011; 6: 21–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poland GA, Murray D, Bonilla-Guerrero R. New vaccine development. BMJ 2002; 324: 1315–1319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes EA, James EL, Kilford EJ, Deeprose C. Key steps in developing a cognitive vaccine against traumatic flashbacks: visuospatial Tetris versus verbal Pub Quiz. PLoS ONE 2010; 5: e13706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL. Memory—a century of consolidation. Science 2000; 287: 248–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL. Time-dependent processes in memory storage. Science 1966; 153: 1351–1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nader K, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. The labile nature of consolidation theory. Nature 2000; 1: 216–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation of auditory pavlovian fear conditioning requires protein synthesis and protein kinase A in the amygdala. J Neurosci 2000; 20: RC96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafe GE, Nader K, Blair HT, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: a cellular and molecular perspective. Trends Neurosci 2001; 24: 540–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein MB, Kerridge C, Dimsdale JE, Hoyt DB. Pharmacotherapy to prevent PTSD: results from a randomized controlled proof-of-concept trial in physically injured patients. J Traumatic Stress 2007; 20: 923–932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitman RK, Sanders KM, Zusman RM, Healy AR, Cheema F, Lasko NB et al. Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 51: 189–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroes MCW, Tendolka I, van Wingen GA, van Waarde JA, Strange BA, Fernandez G. An electroconvulsive therapy procedure impairs reconsolidation of episodic memories in humans. Nat Neurosci 2014; 17: 204–2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD, Andrade J. Working memory and the vividness of imagery. J Exp Psychol 2000; 129: 126–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kavanagh DJ, Freese S, Andrade J, May J. Effects of visuospatial tasks on desensitization to emotive memories. Br J Clin Psychol 2001; 40: 267–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engelhard IM, van den Hout MA, Smeets MAM. Taxing working memory reduces vividness and emotional intensity of images about the Queen's Day tragedy. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2011; 42: 32–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes EA, Brewin CR, Hennessy RG. Trauma films, information processing, and intrusive memory development. J Exp Psychol 2004; 133: 3–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuart ADP, Holmes EA, Brewin CR. The influence of a visuospatial grounding task on intrusive images of a traumatic film. Behav Res Ther 2006; 44: 611–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes EA, James EL, Coode-Bate T, Deeprose C. Can playing the computer game 'Tetris' reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PLoS ONE 2009; 4: e4153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deeprose C, Zhang S, Dejong H, Dalgleish T, Holmes EA. Imagery in the aftermath of viewing a traumatic film: using cognitive tasks to modulate the development of involuntary memory. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2012; 43: 758–764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourne C, Frasquilho F, Roth AD, Holmes EA. Is it mere distraction? Peri-traumatic verbal tasks can increase analogue flashbacks but reduce voluntary memory performance. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2010; 41: 316–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewin CR. Episodic memory, perceptual memory and their interaction: foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Bull 2014; 140: 69–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Hout MA, Engelhard IM. How does EMDR work? J Exp Psychopathol 2012; 3: 724–738. [Google Scholar]
- Tetris Zone.Tetris, Version 1.2.1. Blue Planet Software: Honolulu, HI, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- James EL, Bonsall MB, Hoppitt L, Tunbridge EM, Geddes JR, Milton AL, Holmes EA. Computer game play reduces intrusive memories of experimental trauma via reconsolidation update mechanisms. Psychol Sci 2015; 26: 1201–2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazdin A. Research Design in Clinical Psychology, 4th edn. Pearson: London, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet 1974; 304: 81–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moher D, Hopewell S, Schulz KF, Montori V, Gotzsche PC, Devereaux PJ et al. CONSORT 2010 Explanation and Elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ 2010; 340: c869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pocock SJ, Simon R. Sequential treatment assignment with balancing for prognostic factors in the controlled clinical trial. Biometrics 1975; 31: 103–115. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Association for the Advancement of Automotive MedicineThe Abbreviated Injury Scale 2005 Update 2008. Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine: Barrington, IL, USA, 2008. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard EB, Hickling EJ, Taylor AE, Loos W. Psychiatric morbidity associated with motor vehicle accidents. J Nerv Ment Dis 1995; 183: 495–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marmar CR, Weiss DS, Metzler TJ. The Peritraumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire. In: Wilson JP, Keane TM (eds). Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997, pp 412–428. [Google Scholar]
- Brunet A, Weiss DS, Metzler TJ, Best SR, Neylan TC, Rogers C et al. The Peritraumatic Distress Inventory: a proposed measure of PTSD criterion A2. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158: 1480–1485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss DS, Marmer CR. The Impact of Event Scale—Revised. In: Wilson JP, Keane TM (eds). Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD: A Handbook for Practitioners. Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997, pp 399–411. [Google Scholar]
- Creamer M, Bell R, Failla S. Psychometric properties of the Impact of Event Scale—Revised. Behav Res Ther 2003; 41: 1489–1496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB. The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS) Manual. National Computer Systems: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qualtrics. Qualtrics. Provo, UT, USA, 2015.
- Lilley SA, Andrade J, Turpin G, Sabin-Farrell R, Holmes EA. Visuospatial working memory interference with recollections of trauma. Br J Clin Psychol 2009; 48: 309–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodner TE. What improves with increased missing data imputations? Struct Equ Modelling 2008; 15: 651–675. [Google Scholar]
- Sterne JAC, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG et al. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ 2009; 338: b2393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Howell DC. Statistical Methods for Psychology. Wadsworth Group: Pacific Grove, CA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Cumming G. Exploratory Software for Confidence Intervals, ESCI. La Trobe University: Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0IBM Corp.: Armonk, New York, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Hastie TJ, Tibshirani RJ. Generalized Additive Models. Chapmann & Hall/CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JW. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Pearson J, Naselaris T, Holmes EA, Kosslyn SM. Mental imagery: functional mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends Cogn Sci 2015; 19: 590–602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Advisory Mental Health Council WorkgroupFrom Discovery to Cure: Accelerating the Development of New And Personalized Interventions for Mental Illnesses. Report of the National Advisor Mental Health Council Workgroup. National Institute of Mental Health: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Soeter M, Kindt M. Erasing fear for an imagined threat event. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2012; 37: 1769–1779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehlers A, Hackmann A, Grey N, Wild J, Liness S, Albert I et al. A randomized controlled trial of 7- day intensive and standard weekly cognitive therapy for PTSD and emotion-focused supportive therapy. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171: 294–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel TR, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K et al. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 748–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes EA, Sandberg A, Iyadurai L. Erasing trauma memories [letter to editor]. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norris FH. Epidemiology of trauma: frequency and impact of different potentially traumatic events on different demographic groups. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992; 60: 409–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Initial posttraumatic stress responses following motor vehicle accidents. J Traumatic Stress 1996; 9: 223–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehlers A, Mayou RA, Bryant B. Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. J Abnorm Psychol 1998; 107: 508–519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Department of HealthThe Handbook to The NHS Constitution for England 27 July 2015. Department of Health: Whitehall, London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Bird VJ, Le Boutillier C, Leamy M, Williams J, Bradstreet S, Slade M. Evaluating the feasibility of complex interventions in mental health services: standardised measure and reporting guidelines. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 204: 316–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau-Zhu A. Intrusive emotional memories: a special form of memory? (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit & University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK, 2017.
- Schlichting ML, Preston AR. Memory integration: neural mechanisms and implications for behavior. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2015; 1: 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark IA, Holmes EA, Woolrich MW, Mackay CE. Intrusive memories to traumatic footage: the neural basis of their encoding and involuntary recall. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 505–518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BS, Wessely S. Clinical Trials in Psychiatry. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Rose SC, Bisson J, Churchill R, Wessely S. Psychological debriefing for preventing post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2002; CD000560: 1–49. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Button KS, Munafo MR. Addressing risk of bias in trials of cognitive behavioral therapy. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry 2015; 27: 144–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.