Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Mar 6;7:e31730. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31730

Individual crop loads provide local control for collective food intake in ant colonies

Efrat Esther Greenwald 1,, Lior Baltiansky 1,, Ofer Feinerman 1,
Editor: Iain D Couzin2
PMCID: PMC5862530  PMID: 29506650

Abstract

Nutritional regulation by ants emerges from a distributed process: food is collected by a small fraction of workers, stored within the crops of individuals, and spread via local ant-to-ant interactions. The precise individual-level underpinnings of this collective regulation have remained unclear mainly due to difficulties in measuring food within ants’ crops. Here we image fluorescent liquid food in individually tagged Camponotus sanctus ants and track the real-time food flow from foragers to their gradually satiating colonies. We show how the feedback between colony satiation level and food inflow is mediated by individual crop loads; specifically, the crop loads of recipient ants control food flow rates, while those of foragers regulate the frequency of foraging-trips. Interestingly, these effects do not rise from pure physical limitations of crop capacity. Our findings suggest that the emergence of food intake regulation does not require individual foragers to assess the global state of the colony.

Research organism: Other

eLife digest

In an ant society, a small group of workers, called foragers, feeds the rest of the colony. Each forager goes out of the nest to find food; any liquid food she collects is stored in her ‘crop’, a pouch located just upstream of her stomach. When a forager goes back to the nest, she unloads this liquid by mouth-to-mouth contact into the crops of other ants.

The foragers need to adjust how often they go on foraging trips based on the amount of food the other ants require at any given time. However, it is still unclear how foragers can assess the changing needs of the colony. For example, it had been assumed that a forager would fully feed the individuals she encounters in the nest and then go for another foraging trip when her crop is empty. Yet, scientists had not managed to track food transfer at the level of the individual insect to confirm if this is the case.

Greenwald, Baltiansky and Feinerman have now used laboratory ant colonies and fluorescently labeled food to monitor in real time how food is transferred between individual ants. Contrary to previous hypotheses, when a forager comes back to the nest, she gives small portions of food to many different ants. The insects in the colony are therefore being nourished through these repetitive interactions. As the experiments show, when a forager meets other ants in the nest, the fullness of their crops reliably represents how full the colony is as a whole. Moreover, the portion that the forager gives is, on average, proportional to the space available in the receiver’s crop: the emptier the crop, the more food is given. The amount of food in the crops of the receiving ants therefore controls how much food enters the colony, and the rate at which a forager unloads its crop.

A possible mechanism for regulating foraging frequency is that a forager considers whether or not to go on a foraging trip only after she senses a substantial change in the amount of food in her crop. In this case, her decision is based on the fullness of her own crop: the smaller the amount of food left in her pouch, the more likely she is to decide to leave the nest to bring in more food. Because the rate at which the foragers’ crop empties is tied to the amount of food in the receiving ants’ crops, how often the forager goes for food changes with the hunger level of the whole colony, with more trips when the ants are hungrier.

These experiments show that the amount of food in the crops of the receiving and foraging ants helps foragers adapt their behavior to the colony’s needs. This mechanism means the insects can achieve a common goal without explicitly knowing it. However, it remains to be explained how exactly the mechanical changes in the fullness of foragers’ crop underpin this decision-making process.

Introduction

Eusocial insects stand out in their ability to achieve collective regulation with no central control. Nutritional management in bees and ants is a compelling example. On the one hand, the colony as a whole displays high levels of collective regulation on the amount of food collected (Howard and Tschinkel, 1980; Sorensen et al., 1985; Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999), on its nutritional composition (Dussutour and Simpson, 2009; Cook et al., 2010; Bazazi et al., 2016), and on its dissemination within the colony (Anderson and Ratnieks, 1999; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Greenwald et al., 2015). On the other hand, this regulation is achieved by individuals that react to their local environment. Food dissemination often relies on local trophallactic interactions in which liquid food, not fully digested, is regurgitated from the crop of one individual and passed mouth-to-mouth to another ((Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990) chapter 7, page 291 and SI of (Greenwald et al., 2015)). Such distributed processes are characterized by intricate interaction networks that include significant random aspects (Fewell, 2003; Pinter-Wollman et al., 2011; Mersch et al., 2013; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010) which may hinder global coordination. How colonies manage to achieve tight nutritional regulation despite the difficulties that are inherent to a distributed process is not fully understood.

An essential component in the nutritional regulation of any living system is the adjustment of incoming food rates to the current level of satiation (Parks, 2012; Simpson and Raubenheimer, 1993; Josens and Roces, 2000). To experimentally approach the principles behind such adjustments it is useful to observe the global process in which food accumulates in the system. Indeed, the dynamics of food accumulation in ant colonies have been a subject of interest for many years (Wilson and Eisner, 1957; Markin, 1970; Howard and Tschinkel, 1980; Buffin et al., 2009; Buffin et al., 2012; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Greenwald et al., 2015). These studies show that when introduced to a new food source, the levels of food stored within the colony display logistic dynamics. The logistic growth in the amount of accumulated food supports the notion that total food inflow is regulated by the amount of food already stored within the colony. The local origins of this global regulation are still not fully understood.

To understand how global food flow regulation emerges from single ant behaviors one should consider the forager ants. These ants, which typically constitute only a small fraction of all workers, are the ones responsible for bringing food into the nest (Oster and Wilson, 1978; Traniello, 1977). Therefore, any change in the global inflow of food to the colony must be manifested in the rate at which foragers collect and deliver food. Accordingly, colonies can regulate the inflow of food by modulating foraging effort: for example, by varying the number of active foragers through recruitment (Gordon, 2002). Indeed, many studies on the regulation of foraging have focused on recruitment behavior and have shown that it correlates with the colony’s nutritional state (Traniello, 1977; Seeley, 1989; Tenczar et al., 2014; Cassill, 2003). In this work, we explore a less studied aspect of food flow regulation, namely, changes in the behavior of already active foragers. Active foragers engage in repeated trips between the food source and the nest (Traniello, 1977; Tenczar et al., 2014), where they use trophallaxis to deliver their food load to multiple recipients (Seeley, 1989; Gregson et al., 2003; Huang and Seeley, 2003; Traniello, 1977). The rate at which a forager leaves the nest for her next trip as well as the amount of food that she manages to unload per trip provide potential regulators of the collective foraging effort. These regulators may be tied to the colony’s nutritional state through the experience of returning foragers when they unload in the nest. In this vein, it was shown that honeybee foragers experience longer waiting times between subsequent unloading interactions if the colony is satiated (Seeley, 1989), and it was suggested that they use this information to adjust their recruitment behavior.

Most previous studies of individual forager behavior did not make direct connections between single ant rules and the global dynamics of food accumulation (Seeley, 1989; Huang and Seeley, 2003; Gregson et al., 2003). Nonetheless, the observations and interpretations they present are consistent with a simple intuition for the origins of the observed logistic dynamics in the accumulation of food: Initially, when a scout from a hungry colony encounters food she commences a recruitment process in which the number of active foragers increases (Greene and Gordon, 2007). This positive feedback is followed by a delayed negative feedback that results from the increased difficulty of foragers to locate available recipients as the colony satiates (Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994; Buffin et al., 2009; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010). A simple prediction follows: if foragers wait to unload their entire crop contents before leaving for their next foraging trip (Gregson et al., 2003; Traniello, 1977) then the frequency at which a forager exits the nest should gradually decrease as the colony satiates (Buffin et al., 2009).

Although the above intuition may seem complete, it has only little empirical support. Until recently, microscopic measurements of real-time individual crop loads and food-flows in single interactions were unavailable. As a result, existing explanations for different aspects of the foraging process rely (either explicitly or implicitly) on various assumptions. The foragers were assumed to unload their entire crop contents before leaving the nest (Traniello, 1977; Gregson et al., 2003; Buffin et al., 2009) and use local experience to assess the colony’s nutritional state (Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994; Huang and Seeley, 2003). The recipients were assumed to be either empty or full (Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994) and fill upon a single interaction with a forager (Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994). As for the pattern of interactions between foragers and their recipients, it was assumed that in the nest a forager has a constant probability per unit time to interact with potential recipients (Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994), that there is a formation of queues of returning foragers and available receivers (Seeley, 1989), and that interaction patterns are random (Seeley and Tovey, 1994; Buffin et al., 2009; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010).

Relying on individual-level assumptions may be deceiving since multiple sets of microscopic rules can lead to similar macroscopic outcomes. For example, the slowing down of foragers’ unloading rates may stem from reduced rates of trophallactic interactions but can also be the result of smaller amounts of food transferred per interaction. Both will affect the global outcome similarly. To uniquely identify the micro-scale mechanisms of food inflow regulation and examine the assumptions outlined above, we tracked fluorescently-labeled food in crops of individually tagged ants (Greenwald et al., 2015). This technology allowed for a non-intrusive study of the dynamics of food accumulation in ant colonies with a spatial resolution of single-ant crop loads and a temporal resolution sufficient to capture single trophallactic events. We thus present the missing experimental data on the crop contents of encountered ants, the amount of food transferred per interaction, the dynamics of forager unloading at different satiety states of the colony, and the amount of food in the foragers’ crops when they exit the nest.

In the following sections we use these highly resolved measurements to quantitatively link the microscopic and macroscopic scales of food accumulation dynamics in ant colonies. We demonstrate how the global dynamics and the regulation of individual foraging effort rely on individual crop loads. Specifically, we delineate how individual crop loads affect a forager’s unloading rate as well as her decision to exit the nest for the next food collection trip. Our findings suggest a distributed regulation mechanism which does not require individual foragers to assess global, colony-scale variables.

Results

Dynamics of food accumulation

Food accumulation dynamics were studied by introducing starved colonies of Camponotus sanctus ants to fluorescently-labeled food. Food was supplied ad-libitum to isolate the effects of colony satiation from the effect of resource availability on the inflow of food. As the colony replenished, we followed the traffic and storage of the food within the crops of individual ants using real-time fluorescent imaging (Figure 1a, Video 1 and Video 2, for details see Materials and methods).

Figure 1. Dynamics of food accumulation in a starved colony.

(a) A single frame from a video of a colony in the course of food accumulation. Ant identity is presented as a unique number next to her tag, and the fluorescent food is presented in red. The right side of the image is an IR-covered nest, and the left side is a neighboring open yard that includes a food source and a water source. In this frame a forager can be seen feeding from the food source (ant 207), and a trophallactic event between ants 203 and 421 is magnified. (b) Global food accumulation (normalized fluorescence), F(t), is plotted in black. The accumulated food brought by each forager, fi(t) is plotted in a unique color. The dashed line is the predicted colony state according to Equation 3 for m=0.6×10-4, as estimated from Figure 1c. (c) The time-averaged global inflow, dFdt, as a function of F(t), is plotted in the black solid line. Time-averaged flows through individual foragers, dfidt, are plotted in unique colors (same as in panel b). Flows were calculated by differentiating the colony state and the contributions of each forager (the curves from Figure 1b) with respect to time (see Methods, Data Analysis). Colored circles on the global plot depict each forager’s first return from the food source. The black dashed line represents Equation 2, where m was calculated as follows: the flow through each forager was fit with an equation of the form dfidt=mi(1-F(t)), and m was taken to be the average of all mi. Results from all three experimental colonies can be found in Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2. Source files for panels b and c are available in Figure 1—source datas 1 and 2.

Figure 1—source data 1. Trophallactic interactions.
This data also relates to Figures 2, 3 and 6.
elife-31730-fig1-data1.xlsx (121.7KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.006
Figure 1—source data 2. Temporal data.
This data also relates to Figures 2 and 6.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.007

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Food accumulation dynamics in all three experimental colonies.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Food accumulation (top row) and food inflow (bottom row) of each experimental colony (columns). The results of Colony A are presented in Figure 1 in the main text, where a detailed explanation can be found in the caption. The smoothed data of accumulated food, which is referred to as ‘colony state’ in all other analyses, and from which the food inflow was derived (see Methods, Food flow), is presented in the red dotted curve. The blue dotted curve is the integral of the obtained food flow. This was plotted to validate the method of differentiation that was used to derive the food inflow (see Methods, Food flow).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Food flows through individual foragers and their average.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

All food flows through individual foragers from all three observation experiments (gray solid lines) were normalized such that dfidt(t=0)=1, to account for scale differences between experiments. The average of all normalized flows (black dashed line) is highly linear with the colony state. Most foragers, despite starting to forage at different times, do not greatly deviate from this straight line.

Video 1. A trophallactic event.

Download video file (1.1MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.008

Two Camponotus sanctus ants engaged in trophallaxis of fluorescent liquid food (presented in purple).

Video 2. Food accumulation within a colony of Camponotus sanctus.

Download video file (73MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.009

A starved colony replenishes on fluorescent liquid food (presented in red), brought in by few consistent foragers. Ant Identity is presented as a unique number next to her barcode tag.

We found that the total amount of food in the colony gradually accumulated until saturation (Figure 1b, (Buffin et al., 2009)). The level at which food saturated was defined post-hoc as the colony’s intake volume target. We define the ‘colony state’ at time t, denoted F(t), as the total amount of food in the colony at time t divided by the colony’s intake target. The ‘colony state’ is thus a normalized measure of the colony’s satiety level, starting from F=0 when the colony is starved and gradually approaching F=1 as the colony approaches its target (Figure 1b, black line).

To enable tracing of the food flow process on the single-ant level, all ants were individually tagged (Figure 1a, and see Methods, Experimental setup). As could be expected (Gordon, 1989; Tenczar et al., 2014), this labeling showed that a few consistent foragers were accountable for the transfer of food from the source to the ants in the nest (Figure 1b). This allowed us to study the dynamics of food accumulation by expressing colony state as the sum of the contributions of individual foragers:

F(t)=i=1Nfi(t) (1)

where fi(t) is the portion of the colony state contributed by forager i by time t, and N is the number of foragers.

Feedback on the individual forager scale

Collective food inflow (dFdt), as well as the flow of food through each individual forager (dfidt) were derived by differentiating the measured colony state and the individual contributions with respect to time (Figure 1c, Figure 1—figure supplement 2, and Materials and methods, Data Analysis). This revealed that flows of food through each individual forager declined with increasing colony satiation state, F, and were roughly proportional to the available space in the colony, 1-F:

i,dfidtm(1-F(t)) (2)

where m is a constant (Figures 1c and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). This linear relationship holds for each forager, regardless of when she began foraging and is thus incompatible with feed-forward control in which a forager slows down as a function of her own history. Rather, it supports a mechanism by which the colony state feeds back on the food transfer rate of each individual forager.

Breaking down the total inflow of food into individual forager contributions and using Equation 2, we obtain:

dFdt=i=1n(t)dfidtn(t)m(1-F(t)) (3)

where n(t) is the number of foragers that have begun foraging by time t. This formulation provides simple intuition for the non-monotonicity of food flow as apparent in Figure 1c. Specifically, an initial rise in collective inflow occurred when the number of foragers grew at a rate that overcame the rate of individual flow decay. Once the number of active foragers stabilized total flow rates declined linearly with colony state.

Equation 3 describes a feedback process in which the rate of change in the colony state depends on the colony state itself, and more specifically - on 1-F, the space left to fill until the colony reaches its target. This is a direct consequence of individual foragers that deliver food at slower rates as the colony fills (Equation 2). However, the satiation state of the colony is a global factor that is, most likely, not directly available to individual ants. In the next section, we demonstrate how the observed feedback emerges from pairwise trophallactic interactions.

Global feedback from local interactions

The average food flow through a single forager (dfidt) can be estimated by the product of two macroscopic parameters that may depend on the colony state, F: her average interaction rate, r(F), and the average volume transferred per interaction, v(F).

dfidtr(F)v(F) (4)

While both the interaction rate and the interaction volume declined with increasing colony state, the change in the interaction volume was more prominent (Figure 2a, Figure 2—figure supplements 1 and 2). In fact, interaction volumes were nearly sufficient to account for the inflow dynamics, while the interaction rate introduced a minor second-order correction (Figure 2b and Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Importantly, interaction rates alone did not suffice to account for the inflow dynamics (Figure 2b).

Figure 2. Interaction volume is the dominant component of unloading rate.

(a) Interaction rate (blue) and interaction volumes (red) both decline with increasing colony state. Binned data is presented by mean ± SEM. Interaction rate was calculated as the inverse of all intervals between interactions. The intervals were binned according to the colony state at which they occurred (n = 49, 79, 101, 170, 240 for bins 1–5, respectively. See Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for raw data and Figure 2—source data 1 for binning sensitivity analysis). Blue line depicts a linear fit r(F)=0.032-0.017F, R2=0.96. Interaction volumes were measured in units of pixel intensities, normalized between experiments (see Methods, Data Analysis), and binned into equally-spaced colony state bins (n = 84, 137, 165, 274, 496 for bins 1–5, respectively. See S4). Red line represents the predicted relationship between the mean interaction volume and the colony state from Equation 9, with C0=1.14 and 1λ0=0.14 as obtained from the fit in Figure 3a. (b) Foragers’ unloading rates at each visit in the nest were binned according to colony state (black, mean ± SEM for each bin, n = 26,26,28,39,57 for bins 1–5, respectively). Mean unloading rate values were fitted by three functions: the blue line represents a model which includes the effect of interaction rates only (unloadingrate0.032-0.017F, function obtained from fit in panel a, R2=0.52), the red line represents a model which includes the effect of interaction volumes only (unloadingrate0.2-0.2F, function obtained from fit in Figure 2—figure supplement 2, R2=0.96), and the black dashed line represents a model that incorporates the combined effects of interaction volumes and interaction rates (unloadingrate(0.032-0.071F)(0.2-0.2F), R2=0.99). All panels in this figure represent pooled data from all three observation experiments. For raw data see Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Source file is available in Figure 1—source data 1.

Figure 2—source data 1. Sensitivity analysis for binning interaction rate data.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.014

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Raw (gray) and binned (black) data of interaction rates.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Each data point is the inverse of an interval between two consecutive interactions of a single forager. Binned data and linear fit are as presented in Figure 2a. The figure relates to the pooled data from all three observation experiments.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Raw data pooled from all three observation experiments (gray) and binned (black) data of interaction volumes.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Negative values represent interactions in which the forager was the receiver. Binned data and linear fit are as presented in Figure 2a. Linear fits to the raw data (red, y=0.185-0.178x, R2=0.10), and to the binned data (blue, y=0.200-0.201x, R2=0.90), are similar to each other and close to the prediction (black, y=0.163-0.163x, R2=0.73).
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Raw (gray) and binned (black) data of unloading rates.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

Each data point is the unloading rate of a forager in a single visit in the nest, calculated as the amount of food she transferred in that visit (in normalized units, see Methods: Data Analysis), divided by the duration of that visit. Binned data and fits are as presented in Figure 2b. The figure relates to the pooled data from all three observation experiments.

Therefore, we turned to explore the local determinants that affect interaction volumes, under the assumption that interaction volumes are set locally depending on the states of the interacting individuals. The maximal potential volume of any given interaction is constrained by both the donor’s crop load and the available space in the recipient’s crop. To inspect the impact of each of these two local factors, we examined the distribution of all interaction volumes (v) from foragers to non-forager recipients for different ranges of crop loads, either of the recipient (crecipient, Figure 3—figure supplement 1) or of the forager (cforager, Figure 3—figure supplement 2). We found that these distributions all follow an exponential probability density function (PDF) of the form:

p(v|c)=λce-λcv (5)

where p(v|c) is the conditional PDF of interaction volumes, v, given a crop load c, and c is either cforager or crecipient. We found that while the recipient’s crop load affected the distribution of interaction volumes (Figure 3a), that of the forager had little effect, if any (Figure 3b). Specifically, the distribution of interaction volumes scaled with the space left to fill in the recipient’s crop but was effectively independent of the forager’s crop load (hence, hereafter we take c=crecipient):

λc=λ0C0-c (6)

Figure 3. Microscopic food flow.

(a) The distributions of interaction volumes from foragers to non-forager recipients, at seven ranges of recipient crop loads, follow an exponential probability density function of the form p(v|c)=λce-λcv (Equation 5, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Here the seven rate parameters of the distributions (λc) are plotted as a function of the recipient’s crop load (crecipient). Mean ± STD of λc are over different binnings of the histograms to which the exponential distribution function was fit (17 histogram binwidths uniformly covering the range [0.01–0.09]). Curve represents a fit of the function λc=λ0C0-c (Equation 6): λ0=7.01, C0=1.14, R2=0.93. (b) Similarly to panel a, the rate parameters of the exponential distributions of interaction volumes were obtained for seven ranges of forager crop loads (Figure 3—figure supplement 2) and plotted as a function of the forager’s crop load, cforager. Dashed curve represents a fit of the form λc=constcforager, similar to Equation 6, but instead of a fraction of the recipient’s empty crop space, λc is assumed to be a fraction of the forager’s crop load. R2 was negative, indicating that this function is no better fit to the data than a constant (solid line). (c) The distributions of ‘relative interaction volumes’, v~, wherein each interaction volume was normalized to the available space in the receiver’s crop (v~=vC0-c). The distributions collapse onto a single exponential function p(v~)=1λe-1λv~, λ=0.12, R2=0.96. (d) The crop loads of non-foragers at interactions with foragers (blue, n as in b) compared to crop loads of all non-foragers in the colony (red, n = 202 per colony state bin). The distributions of crop loads at each colony state are plotted as a violin plot, and the mean ± SEM are plotted in solid lines. All panels in this figure represent pooled data from all three observation experiments. Source file is available in the Figure 1—source data 1.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Interaction volume distributions given recipient crop load.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Each plot depicts a PDF of interaction volumes for a different range of recipient crop loads (indicated in the titles). Data was extracted from pooled data from all three observation experiments. All distributions were fit with exponentials as specified in the text (red curves). The fitted coefficients, R2 of each fit, and sample sizes are indicated on the plots. See Methods section for details on units of food volume. To ensure that this procedure was not sensitive to histogram binwidths, it was performed on a range of binwidths as specified in the caption of Figure 3a. Here we show the results for a binwidth of 0.04.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Interaction volume distributions given Forager’s crop load.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Each plot depicts a PDF of interaction volumes for a different range of forager crop loads (indicated in the titles). Data was extracted from all three observation experiments. All distributions were fit with exponentials as specified in the text (red curves). The fitted coefficients, R2 of each fit, and sample sizes are indicated on the plots. See Methods section for details on units of food volume. To ensure that this procedure was not sensitive to histogram binwidths, it was performed on a range of binwidths as specified in the caption of Figure 3a. Here we show the results for a binwidth of 0.04.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Mean interaction volumes as a function of the recipient’s crop load.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

Interaction volumes (gray, pooled data from all three observation experiments) were binned according to the recipient’s crop load (black, mean ± SEM, n = 172,44,43,59,64,70,85,100,112,123 for bins 1–10, respectively) and fit with a function of the form y=ax+b (red). Binned data and fit are magnified in the inset. The same fit was obtained for 5–10 bins, resulting in a=-0.12 and b=0.13 with R2=0.93,0.94,0.94,0.94,0.9,0.88, respectively.

 where λ0=7.01 and C0=1.14 (Figure 3a). C0 may be interpreted as the average crop load target to which recipients aim to ultimately fill (expected to be close to 1).

On average, in an interaction with a forager the recipient receives 1λ0=0.14 of the space left to fill in her crop. Consistently, a linear fit relating mean interaction volume to recipient crop load v(c)=ac+b (Figure 3—figure supplement 3) yields b-a0.131λ0 . This is to be expected if v(c)=1λ0(1-c) . Indeed, normalizing interaction volumes by the total amount of available space in the recipient’s crop, v~=vC0-c, we find that all trophallactic volume distributions collapse onto a single exponential function p(v~)=λe-λv~ with 1λ=0.121λ0 (Figure 3c). Simply put, the volume of an interaction can be estimated by a random exponentially distributed fraction (v~) of the available space in the recipient’s crop (C0-c).

We can now move forward to express mean interaction volume, v, in terms of the colony state. Defining p(v|F) to be the conditional probability for an interaction of volume v when the colony state is F, the mean interaction volume (at colony state F) can be calculated by:

v(F)=vvp(v|F)dv (7)

In light of our findings that interaction volumes change mainly with respect to the recipient’s crop load, we can decompose p(v|F)=cp(v|c)p(c|F)dc where p(c|F) is the probability density that the recipient will have a crop load of size c at colony state F. Equation 7 now becomes:

v(F)=cp(c|F)vvp(v|c)dvdc (8)

The probability (p(c|F)) changed as the colony satiated and individual ants approached their targets. Figure 3d shows that the ants that interact with a forager reliably represent the satiation level of the colony and that the accuracy of this representation increases as the colony satiates. Altogether, substituting the microscopic interaction rule described by Equations 5 and 6 into the global summation described by Equation 8 demonstrates how the average interaction volume changes in proportion to the empty space in the colony (1-F):

v=cp(c|F)vλ0C0-ce-λ0C0-cvvdvdc=cp(c|F)C0-cλ0dc=1λ0(C0-c(F))=C0λ0(1-F) (9)

where the following identities were used: C0=ctargetN, F=cctarget, p(c)dc=1 and p(c)cdc=c. For each ant, ctarget signifies her crop load at colony satiation. The value of the multiplicative factor C0λ00.16 stands in agreement with our experimental measurements (Figure 2—figure supplement 2).

The above analysis demonstrates how the global inflow is determined by interaction volumes that are locally controlled by the recipient’s crop load, which on average represents the colony state. The forager’s crop comes into play in a different aspect of food inflow. Its finite capacity requires foragers to repeatedly leave the nest to reload at the food source in order to supply food to the entire colony. However, leaving the nest encompasses inevitable risks and energetic costs. Therefore, it is interesting to study whether foraging effort is also regulated, and if so, how it is expressed in decisions of individual foragers to leave the nest.

Foraging effort is matched to the colony’s Needs

To check whether foragers adjust their activity to the changing colony state we examined their behavior with respect to the accumulating food in the colony. The behavior of individual foragers was typically of a cyclic nature, as they alternated between two phases: (1) an outdoor phase, in which they filled their crops at the food source, and (2) an indoor phase, in which they distributed their crop contents in trophallaxis to colony members inside the nest (Figure 4a).

Figure 4. Foraging cycles.

(a) The estimated crop load of a single forager during the first two hours of an experiment. As typical for a forager, her crop load oscillates as she alternates between feeding at the food source (pink areas) and unloading in trophallaxis (gray areas) in continuous back-and-forth trips. (b) The foraging frequency of individual foragers, calculated as the inverse of cycle times (the time interval between two consecutive feeding events of a single forager), grows linearly with the empty space in the colony, 1-F. Data points and error bars represent means and SEM of cycles. The pooled data from all three observation experiments is grouped into equally-spaced bins of colony state (n = 57,39,28,26,26, for bins 1–5, respectively, see Figure 4—figure supplement 1). A linear fit is presented in red: y=0.8 10-3+3.6 10-3(1-F), R2=0.98. (c) Forager cycle durations are composed of an indoor phase (green) and an outer phase (yellow), the former accounting for most of the rising trend. The pooled data from all three observation experiments was binned and averaged as in panel b (n = 26,26,28,39,57, for bins 1–5, respectively, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). (d) Food accumulation in a perturbation experiment. Food rises to an initial plateau, and rises again to a secondary plateau after new hungry ants are introduced (black line). (e) Durations of foragers in the nest in the manipulation experiment described in panel d. Durations grow longer, drop after new hungry ants are introduced (black line), and subsequently rise again. Data points and error bars represent means and standard errors of durations of cycles grouped into time bins (n = 28,36,21,14,9,28,27,19,5, for bins 1–9, respectively). Raw data and results from a second replication of the perturbation experiment are presented in Figure 4—figure supplement 2. (f) A schematic representation of the observed negative feedback between the colony state and the foraging frequency. Source file for panels b and c is available in the Figure 4—source data 1. Source file for panels d and e is available in the Figure 4—source data 2.

Figure 4—source data 1. Foraging cycles.
This data also relates to Figure 5.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.022
Figure 4—source data 2. Manipulation experiments.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.023

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Foraging cycle times.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Both panels relate to the pooled data from all three observation experiments. (a) Foraging frequency, calculated as the inverse of cycle times (the time interval between two consecutive feeding events of a single forager) grows linearly with 1-F. Raw data (gray) was binned into equally-spaced bins of colony state (n = 57,39,28,26,26, for bins 1–5, respectively, in black mean ± SEM.) Linear fits to the raw data (red) and the binned data (blue, hidden behind the red) yield similar lines: y=0.8 10-3+3.6 10-3(1-F), R2=0.35,0.98, respectively. (b) Forager cycle durations are composed of an indoor phase (green) and an outer phase (yellow), the former accounting for most of the rising trend. Data was binned and averaged as in panel a (n = 26,26,28,39,57, for bins 1–5, respectively).
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Perturbation experiments.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Food accumulation, represented by the total fluorescence (top row), and durations of foragers in the nest (bottom row), as a function of time in the two experimental colonies that underwent colony state manipulation (see Methods). Solid black line represents the time of introducing new hungry ants to the system. The results of colony M2 are presented in Figure 4d–e in the main text, where a detailed explanation may be found in the caption. Note that the plots depicting durations in the nest here differ from that in Figure 4e, in that here durations are plotted by two separate groups: those of foragers which were active during the whole experiment (M1: n = 47,13,15,13,12,16,17,14 for bins 1–8, respectively; M2: n = 28,36,21,14,5,15,18,10,4, for bins 1–9, respectively) and those of foragers that began foraging after the manipulation (M1: n = 6,3,2, for bins 1–3, respectively; M2: n = 4,13,9,9,1, for bins 1–5, respectively). In Figure 4e, all were pooled together. The observation that foragers of both groups displayed similar patterns after the manipulation highlights the causality of the effect of colony state on forager behavior as opposed to time or the forager’s history.

The frequency of these cycles (‘foraging frequency’) displayed a tight linear relationship with the available space in the colony (1-F), demonstrating that foraging effort is matched to the colony’s needs (Figure 4b and Figure 4—figure supplement 1a, y=0.8 10-3+3.6 10-3(1-F), R2=0.98). Additionally, the increase in cycle times was mainly attributed to the prolonged indoor phase of the cycle, rather than the relatively constant outdoor phase (Figure 4c and Figure 4—figure supplement 1b, Spearman’s correlation test, indoor phase: rs=0.77, p<0.001, outdoor phase: rs=0.13, p=0.08). This suggests that foraging frequency was regulated by the colony.

To test for a causal effect of colony state on foraging frequency, we elicited an external perturbation on the colony state. Indeed, in experiments where the colony state was actively dropped by introducing new hungry ants after others had reached satiation (see Methods, Perturbation Experiment), foragers’ durations in the nest sharply dropped as well (Figure 4e and Figure 4—figure supplement 1). This response generated a secondary rise in the amount of food in the colony, relaxing at a new value as durations in the nest gradually lengthened once again (Figure 4d–e and Figure 4—figure supplement 2). These experiments explicitly decoupled colony state from the time that passed since the initial introduction of food, and thus show that the colony state rather than forager history was the important factor that affects foraging frequency.

Overall, these findings portray the following negative feedback process: foragers raise the colony state by bringing in food, while the colony state, in turn, inhibits their foraging frequency (Figure 4f). A possible mechanism for this feedback might be that foragers do not exit the nest for their next foraging trip before they fully unload (Traniello, 1977; Gregson et al., 2003; Buffin et al., 2009). In this case unloading rates directly dictate the foraging frequency. This is consistent with the fact that both unloading rate and the foraging frequency are proportional to the total available space, 1-F. We explore this hypothesized mechanism in the next section.

Foragers’ crop loads upon leaving the nest

We find that foragers do not leave the nest only after they have fully unloaded (Figure 5a). Nevertheless, the average amount of food in their crops when they exit remains nearly constant over different colony states (Figure 5a, Spearman’s correlation test, rs=0.24, p=0.001). This constant averagesuffices in producing the observed relation between foraging and unloading rates as specified above.

Figure 5. Foragers’ crop loads at exit.

(a) Foragers’ crop loads at the moments of exit were only weakly dependent on colony state (Spearman’s correlation test, rs=0.24, p=0.001), the average remaining approximately constant with only a slight rise at high colony states. Gray: raw data, black: mean ± SEM of binned data (n = 11,15,13,13,15,13,17,22,27,30 for bins 1–10, respectively). (b) The wide distribution of foragers’ crop loads at the moments of exit (n = 176). Each color represents a different forager, revealing that the distribution of crop loads upon exit is wide within each forager and not due to inter-individual variability. (c) The number of interactions a forager has in a single visit to the nest rises as the colony satiates, mean ± SEM of binned data (n = 26,26,28,39,57 for bins 1–5, respectively, Figure 5—figure supplement 1a). (d) Foragers reach deeper locations in the nest as the colony satiates, mean ± SEM of binned data (n as in panel c, Figure 5—figure supplement 1b). All panels relate to the pooled data from all three observation experiments. Source file is available in the Figure 4—source data 1.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Number of interactions and maximal distance from entrance in a forager’s visit in the nest.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Both panels relate to the pooled data from all three observation experiments. (a) The number of interactions in which a forager participates in a single visit to the nest rises as the colony satiates. Raw data (gray) and mean ± SEM of binned data (black, n = 26,26,28,39,57 for bins 1–5, respectively). (b) Foragers reach deeper locations in the nest as the colony satiates. Raw data (gray) and mean ± SEM of binned data (Black, n as in panel c). Data seems separated into clusters (upper and lower clouds) due to differences in the location of most of the ants within the nest between experiments.

To maintain a relatively constant average crop state upon exit despite the declining unloading rates, foragers stayed longer in the nest (Figure 4c) and performed more interactions (Figure 5c, and Figure 5—figure supplement 1a). They also actively explored deeper into the nest (Figure 5d and Figure 5—figure supplement 1b). Surprisingly, even though the average crop load with which foragers exit the nest remains constant, this is not because the foragers unload a fixed amount in each visit. Rather, the crop loads with which foragers left the nest were highly variable (Figure 5b). This raises the questions of when foragers decide to exit and how these decisions lead to exits at variable crop contents that, nevertheless, maintain a constant average over different colony states. Do factors other than their own crop load affect their decisions to exit, and more specifically, do the foragers use high-level information regarding the colony state?

Forager exit times are determined by their crop load and their unloading rate

To gain insight on the role of individual versus collective information in foragers’ decisions to exit, we were interested in a forager’s probability to exit the nest as a function of her own crop state (crop) and the colony state (colony). We first estimated the probability for an individual forager to exit per time unit given her crop load and colony state R(exit|crop,colony) (hereinafter, Rexit). We found that this exit rate was strongly dependent on both the forager’s crop state and the colony state (Figure 6a, Table 1).

Figure 6. Forager exits.

Figure 6.

(a–c) Forager exit probabilities as a function of the colony state and her own crop state. All panels relate to the pooled data from all three observation experiments. Observations are plotted on a 2-dimensional space of the forager’s crop state and the colony state, as black and white dots (white - ‘stay’ observations, black - ‘exits’). An observation was classified as an ‘exit’ if the forager left the nest before the next considered observation. The colored surface represents the estimated local probability to exit on this space, calculated as the fraction of ‘exits’ out of all observations in each bin in the space: the color of each pixel on the surface represents the probability calculated based on the n closest data points, and the pixel’s location is the average location of these points (a: n=300, b-c: n=30). The three panels consider three possible decision rates: Constant decision rate (a) where all observations, taken every two seconds, were considered to be decision points (excluding observations during trophallaxis). Decision rate matched to interaction rate (b) where only observations at ends of interactions were considered to be decision points. Decision rate matched to unloading rate (c) where only observations taken each time a forager unloaded Δcrop food, were considered to be decision points (Δcrop=10% of the forager’s capacity). (dG(crop), a projection of the two-dimensional probability presented in panel c on the forager’s crop state axis. Since foragers’ crop loads rarely rose in the nest, their lowest crop observation in a visit was generally an ‘exit’, so the calculated probability to exit in the lowest crop interval was 1. To ensure that the crop state played a role beyond this extreme effect, the GLM fit did not include the lowest crop interval. The crop load effect was significant: βc=-1.93, p<0.001. Source file for panels a and c is available in the Figure 1—source data 2. Source file for panel b is available in the Figure 1—source data 1.

Table 1. Logistic fits for a forager’s probability to exit (P) as a function of her crop load and the colony state.

A two-dimensional logistic function of the form P=(1+e-(α+βcrop+γcolony))-1 was fit to each estimated probability to exit from Figure 6a–c. The effect of each factor is reflected in its fitted coefficient. Within each model, effects can be compared to one another because the values of crop and colony lay on the same scale between 0 and 1. In the constant decision rate model, all coefficients were comparable in value, indicating that crop and colony had similar meaningful effects on the probability to exit. In the model where decision rate was matched to interaction rate, the effect of colony was weaker than the effect of crop, but both were still meaningful. In the model where decision rate was matched to unloading rate, the effect of colony approached 0 and was very weak compared to the effect of crop.

Decision rate model Factor Coefficient 95% CI R2
Constant Intercept α=-1.914 -1.921 -1.906 0.81
Colony State β=-1.763 -1.778 -1.747
Forager’s Crop γ=-2.330 -2.347 -2.312
Interaction Rate Intercept α=2.701 2.686 2.716 0.92
Colony State β=-2.199 -2.218 -2.181
Forager’s Crop γ=-5.810 -5.837 -5.783
Unloading Rate Intercept α=-0.093 -0.105 -0.081 0.71
Colony State β=0.499 0.482 0.515
Forager’s Crop γ=-3.092 -3.115 -3.069

To understand which information the foragers require to generate this exit pattern we make the simplifying and common assumption that Rexit is an outcome of a Markovian decision process (Robinson et al., 2011; Sumpter et al., 2012), and can be treated as a product of two probabilities:

R(exit|crop,colony)=R(makeadecision)P(decision=exit|crop,colony) (10)

where R(makeadecision) is the probability of a forager to make a decision within a time unit (hereinafter, Rdecide), and P(decision=exit|crop,colony) is her probability to decide to exit given her crop load and colony state when a decision is made (hereinafter, P).

Since the precise timings of an ant’s decisions are beyond our experimental reach, we replaced Rdecide by three assumed decision rates: (1) a constant decision rate, (2) a decision rate that is matched to the forager’s interaction rate, and (3) a decision rate matched to the forager’s unloading rate. Figure 6 shows the corresponding P for each decision rate. For a constant decision rate, P is proportional to Rexit and depends on both the forager’s crop state and the colony state (Figure 6a, Table 1). For a decision rate that is matched to the forager’s interaction rate (e.g. a forager considers whether to exit only after an interaction has ended), P was calculated by considering only observations at ends of interactions as decision points. In this case, the effect of the colony state on P is present but smaller that the effect of the forager’s crop (Figure 6b, Table 1). Last, for a decision rate matched to the forager’s unloading rate, P was calculated by considering observations at fixed intervals of the forager’s crop load as decision points. For this case, the effect of the colony state on P approaches zero, such that P varies predominantly with the forager’s internal crop state (Figure 6c–d, Table 1).

Since the probability to decide to exit, P, was effectively independent of the colony state when the rate of decisions, Rdecide, was adjusted to the unloading rate, we learn that the rate of exits Rexit can be decomposed into two functions with a clear separation of variables:

R(exit|crop,colony)=U(colony)G(crop) (11)

where U(colony) is linear in the unloading rate (Equation 2) and G(crop) is a function of the crop that does not depend on the colony state (Figure 6d). Interpreting this result from the perspective of the individual forager suggests a simple biological mechanism that may underly this separation of variables: In the course of unloading, the forager considers whether to exit or not each time she senses a sufficiently large change in her crop load, and then decides to exit based on her crop load alone. Since the rate at which her crop load changes is mainly affected by the recipients (Figures 2 and 3), the rate of her decisions is controlled by the colony (U(colony)1-F); once the forager is triggered to make a decision, the decision itself depends on personal information alone (G(crop), Figure 6d).

Discussion

The total flow of food into an ant colony is the sum of many small trophallactic events between foragers and recipient ants. A reliable description of colony level food regulation therefore demands empirical data on the statistics and rules that guide these microscopic events. In this work, we provide the first quantitative multi-scale account of the dynamics of ant colony satiation that links the global state to local interaction statistics. While our global-scale measurements generally concur with previous studies, our microscopic observations revealed two significant deviations from prevalent individual-level assumptions. First, foragers do not necessarily deliver their entire crop load before exiting the nest. Second, recipients do not fill to their capacity in a single interaction. The next subsections discuss the ways in which our new individual-level observations agree with previous collective level measurements despite the aforementioned deviations. We further discuss how these new findings alter the current understanding of the food intake process.

Regulation of food flow

On the scale of the entire colony, and in agreement with previous studies (Buffin et al., 2009; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010), we find that food accumulation follows logistic dynamics (Figure 1b). Our individual-level measurements confirm that the logistic equation which describes the global dynamics can be interpreted as the product of two intuitive terms (Equation 3): the number of active foragers times the average unloading rate per forager. As previously speculated (Buffin et al., 2009), the initial rise in the global food inflow stems from gradually joining foragers while its subsequent decay is the result of a negative feedback process wherein colony satiation levels work to decrease the unloading rates of individual foragers (Figure 1c).

We traced the mechanisms of this large-scale negative feedback to the immediate experience of individual foragers and specifically to the crop-loads of the ants they interact with. When a forager enters the nest she interacts with a ‘representative sample’ of recipient ants, i.e. ants whose crop load is, on average, proportional to the total satiation state of the colony (Figure 3d). Further, in each such interaction the amount of food transferred is random but, on average, proportional to the available space in the recipient’s crop (Figure 3a,c). Together, these findings imply that unloading rates are determined by the colony and directly proportional to the total empty space in the crops of the entire colony.

Many sets of local rules could have yielded the same average flow, and thus would have been consistent with similar global dynamics. For example, previous studies have attributed the global negative feedback to the decreasing probability of a forager to encounter an accepting recipient, which delays the time until delivery (Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994; Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999). However, due to experimental limitations these studies relied on the implicit assumption that recipients are satisfied by a single interaction, while in fact recipients may very well be partially satiated (Huang and Seeley, 2003). Our measurements on the level of single crops show that recipients are typically partially loaded, and the effect of their crop loads on interaction sizes is more dominant than the minor decrease of interaction rates in generating the collective negative feedback.

Interestingly, partial crop loads do not affect the interaction volume merely by physical limitation: in most interactions the donor does not deliver her entire crop load, nor does the recipient fill up to her capacity. This finding contradicts the prevalent assumption used by those studies that did take partially loaded recipients into account (Huang and Seeley, 2003; Gregson et al., 2003). These studies supposed that the amount of food transferred in an interaction is the maximal possible amount, and partial crop loads result from discrepancies between foragers’ loads and recipients’ capacities. Here we introduce explicit measurements of interaction volumes that reveal that exponentially distributed interaction volumes lead to partially loaded ants. This volume distribution concurs with the global feedback as it is scaled to the available space in the recipient’s crop. Feedback based on interaction volumes that are not set by physical limitations, rather than ‘all-or-none’ interactions, potentially allows individual ants to fine-tune their intake and allow for combinations of several sources towards their desired nutritional target (Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999).

Regulation of foraging trips

Previous studies typically addressed the global feedback between colony state and the collective foraging effort (Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994; Cassill, 2003). Our work complements this by demonstrating how this feedback acts on individual foraging frequencies (Figure 4, see also [Tenczar et al., 2014] and [Rivera et al., 2016]). Furthermore, while previous studies suggested that foragers use local information, such as time delays, interaction rates or number of refused interactions, to infer the colony’s needs (Seeley, 1989; Seeley and Tovey, 1994; Cassill, 2003; Greene and Gordon, 2007; Gordon et al., 1993), we propose a mechanism that demonstrates how foragers could adjust their foraging frequency relying on their own crop load alone (Figure 6c,d) (Mayack and Naug, 2013). In brief, foragers could adjust their exit rates to colony needs by modulating their decision rate according to unloading rates, while the decision itself depends on their current crop load alone.

Interestingly, foragers usually do not exit completely empty (Figure 5a,b) as could be intuitively assumed (Gregson et al., 2003; Buffin et al., 2009). This provides further evidence that, similar to interaction volumes, foraging activity is not regulated by pure physical limitations (i.e. an empty crop). Rather, we have found that foragers exit with a wide range of crop loads. The lack of a well-defined exit threshold entails a potentially wasteful effect in which forager crop loads at exit increase with colony state: The difficulty of unloading at higher colony states means that foragers spend longer times with a relatively full crop. Since there is a probability to exit at any crop load this may lead to an upward drift in the crop loads of exiting foragers. Here we show this drift is minor (Figure 5a) and propose different options by which this may be achieved. For example, it could be the case that after each interaction a forager decides whether to exit the nest or, rather, wait for another opportunity to unload. This decision scheme holds an appealing simplicity as it implies that a forager’s decision rate is set externally and not by an internal clock or parameters. On the other hand, it demands that the forager makes complex decisions that integrate the state of her own crop with colony-level information (Figure 6b). Another possibility rids the foragers of the need to use colony level information. In this case, foragers effectively modulate their decisions to exit according to their unloading rates. We suggest a biologically appealing mechanism to achieve this, in which decisions occur at constant crop intervals (Figure 6c). Generally, any mechanism by which the forager’s trigger to leave the nest depends on her unloading rate could yield similar results.

Leaving the nest while partially loaded could hold some benefits: foragers may use the food in their crops as provisions to be consumed in their expedition (Rytter and Shik, 2016), waiting for full unloading in the nest may be time-consuming and limit exploration for other food types, and frequent visits to the food source may ensure its exploitation. This raises the question whether there exists an optimal crop load with which foragers should exit the nest, which could potentially depend on factors such as the abundance and quality of the food source, predation risk, and the demand for food in the nest (Dornhaus and Chittka, 2004).

In light of our findings on both the forager’s decision to exit and the distribution of interaction volumes, we hypothesize that an internal mechanism based on the mechanical tension of the crop’s walls is involved in trophallaxis. Considering the crop as an elastic organ that stretches as it fills, the relative change in the volume of the recipient’s crop may provide a mechanism for the scaling of interaction volumes with available crop space. Additionally, if ants could sense changes in the tension of their crop walls (Stoffolano and Haselton, 2013), then this would provide an anatomical basis for a model in which foragers adjust decision rates to unloading rates.

Materials and methods

Study species: Camponotus sanctus

Camponotus sanctus are omnivorous ants that are presumed to naturally live in monogynous colonies of tens to hundreds of individuals (projecting from Camponotus socius, [Tschinkel, 2005]), distributed from the near East to Iran and Afghanistan (Ionescu-Hirsch, 2009). Workers of this species are relatively large (0.8–1.6 cm) and characterized by translucent gasters, rendering them suitable for both barcode labeling and crop imaging. Our experiments were conducted on lab colonies of 50–100 workers, reared from single queens that were collected during nuptial flights in Neve Shalom and Rehovot, Israel. Table 2 contains further details on each experimental colony.

Table 2. Experimental colonies.

Experiment type Colony # Ants Starvation period # Major Workers # Foragers
Observation A 100 3 weeks 1 5
B 62 5 weeks 1 3
C 53 4 weeks 2 4
Manipulation M1 69 3 weeks 1 23
M2 95 3 weeks 2 16

Experimental setup

Fluorescent food imaging and 2D barcode identification (BugTag, Robiotec) were used to obtain a live visualization of the food flow through colonies of individually tagged ants. See (Greenwald et al., 2015) for a detailed description of the experimental setup. In short, an artificial nest was placed on a glass platform positioned between two cameras. A camera below the nest filmed through the platform, capturing the fluorescence emitted from the food inside the translucent ants. Meanwhile, a camera above the nest filmed through its infrared shelter, capturing the barcodes on the ants’ thoraxes, allowing identification of single ants inside the nest. Together, footages from both cameras enabled the association between each individual ant and her food load, throughout time and across trophallactic events. The two cameras were synchronously triggered at a fixed frame rate, (here 0.5 Hz., except for colony B which was recorded at 1 Hz.). We chose a temporal resolution that is sufficient to capture events of 2 s since shorter interactions barely involve food exchange (Greenwald et al., 2015).

Image processing

Top camera images were used to extract ant identities, coordinates and orientations using the BugTag software (Robiotec). Bottom camera images were used to detect fluorescence with a pixel intensity threshold, using the openCV library in Python. Gasters of fed ants appeared as bright ‘blobs’ and thus passed the image threshold (for details, see [Greenwald et al., 2015]).

In order to associate between the identity of an ant and her appropriate blob, the image from the upper camera was transformed to align with the fluorescent image. Then, for each identified tag, a small area extended from the back of the tag toward the ant’s abdomen was crossed with the thresholded fluorescent image. If a blob intercepted this area, it was assigned to the tag’s identity.

Thus, for each experiment a database was obtained, which included for every frame the coordinates, orientation, and measured fluorescence (in arbitrary units of pixel intensity) of each identified ant.

Interaction identification and crop load estimation

Even though the fluorescence emitted from an ant’s crop is reasonably indicative of the food volume, it is a noisy measurement mainly due to her highly variable postures. Therefore, assuming that an ant’s crop content remains constant during the intervals between trophallactic events, it is best evaluated as the maximal fluorescence measurement acquired in each such interval (Greenwald et al., 2015).

In order to precisely consider the relevant intervals for this estimation, the trophallactic interactions were manually identified from the video. Interactions were classified as trophallactic events whenever the mandibles of the participating ants came in contact and the mandibles of at least one of the ants were open. For forager ants, another situation in which their crop loads may change is when they directly feed from the food source. These feedings were also manually identified from the video, as times when a forager’s open mandibles touched the food source.

Ultimately, for each ant we obtained a ‘timeline’, describing at every instance whether she was engaged in trophallaxis (and if so, with whom), whether she was directly feeding from the food source, and the estimated food load in her crop. Figure 4a depicts an example of such individual-level data.

Observation experiment: Monitoring food flow as hungry colonies gradually satiate

Following a food-deprivation period of 3–5 weeks, ant colonies (queen, workers and brood) were manually barcoded and introduced to the experimental nest for an acclimatization period of at least 4 hr. The nest consisted of an IR-sheltered chamber (~100 cm2), neighboring an open area which served as a yard (Figure 1a). After the acclimatization period, the two cameras synchronously started to record. After 30 min, the fluorescent food (sucrose [80 g/l], Rhodamine B [0.08 g/l]) was introduced to the nest yard ad libitum, and the recording proceeded for at least four more hours - a duration sufficient for the colony to reach its desired food volume intake (Figure 1b and Figure 1).

Overall, we analyzed data from three such experiments, that included 12 foragers, who fed from the food source 139 times, and were engaged in 1227 trophallactic interactions.

Perturbation experiment: introducing hungry ants after initial satiation

To manipulatively examine the role of the colony’s satiety in the control of food inflow, we characterized the system’s response to a perturbation in the colony’s satiety level. This experiment was conducted as the observation experiment described above, except that it consisted of two phases:

Phase 1: The starved colony was segregated between two equally-sized chambers - one with access to the nest yard, and the other blocked behind a removable perspex wall. Thus, when the fluorescent food was introduced to the nest yard, only the ants with access to the yard gradually satiated while the others in the blocked chamber remained hungry. We reasoned that if foragers react to the colony’s satiety through their experience in the nest, they would perceive saturation of the accessible chamber as saturation of the colony, as they could only interact with ants of the accessible chamber.

Phase 2: After the first chamber satiated, we introduced the hungry ants of the blocked chamber by removing the wall, effectively dropping the perceived satiety level of the colony at once. Recording then proceeded for at least 90 more minutes, sufficient for the colony to reach secondary satiation (Figure 4d and Figure 4—figure supplement 2).

Segregating the colony into two chambers. In order to avoid artificial biases in the chambers’ populations, ants were initially introduced to the nest without the wall to freely settle within it. Only after a habituation period of at least 4 hr, the wall was gently inserted to divide the ants, that were then left to habituate for at least one more hour before recording started. The blocked chamber included the queen and brood in both perturbation colonies, and the number of ants in the accessible chamber was 33 and 31 in colonies M1 and M2, respectively.

Time of wall removal. Satiation of the first chamber was identified with semi-online approximative image analysis of the videos from the fluorescence camera, by summing the pixel intensities of each frame, which rose as food accumulated. Satiation was determined when this fluorescent signal ceased to rise for at least 1 hr, serving as our cue to remove the wall.

Overall, we analyzed data from two perturbation experiments, on colonies of 69 and 95 ants, including 23 and 16 foragers, respectively.

Data analysis

All data obtained after crop load estimation was analyzed using Matlab software. Four data files are available with this manuscript.

Foragers

Each experiment consisted of a few individuals who performed consistent foraging cycles between the food source and the nest. Those ants were considered as ‘foragers’. Some other individuals were occasionally observed at the food source but clearly did not display such foraging cycles. To our purposes they were not considered as foragers. These ants visited the food source no more than four times, while consistent foragers performed an average of 15.67 cycles and no less than 8. The data presented here is from the first return of a forger to the nest from the food source until the end of the experiment.

Food flow

The total accumulated food was calculated as the sum of all interaction volumes between foragers and non-foragers. The volume of an interaction was taken to be positive when food was transferred from the forager and negative when it was transferred to the forager. Each forager’s contribution is the sum of her own interaction volumes. Although food accumulated through discrete local events, we were mostly interested in the average dynamics of food flow, which are convenient to describe in a continuous manner. Therefore, the accumulated food was first smoothed with a moving average with a time window large enough to include several trophallactic events (2000 s). This window size was chosen by plotting the smoothed data on top of the raw data and assuring that small fluctuations were smoothed while the general shape was maintained. Food inflow was derived by differentiating the smoothed data. Since differentiation is a process highly sensitive to local noise, we differentiated the smoothed accumulated food with a window of 200–500 s, depending on the fluctuations of the experiment. This window size was chosen by verifying that the sum of the obtained inflow is indeed sufficiently close to the raw data of accumulated food.

Food load normalization

Our experimental method provided us with measurements of food volume in arbitrary units of fluorescent pixel intensity. Due to possible variations in lighting conditions between experiments, the obtained pixel intensities were incomparable. Therefore we used pixel intensities only for analyses perfomed within the same experiment (Figure 1b–c, Figure 4d–e and Figure 1—figure supplement 1). For all other purposes, food load was estimated in normalized units. In analyses where individual crop loads and interaction volumes were linked to the global dynamics (Figure 3), absolute loads were important. Therefore, food loads were normalized between experiments, by dividing each measurement by the 90th percentile crop load measurement of its experiment. In analyzing foragers’ responses to their own crop loads (Figure 6), the relative satiety state of each forager was of interest. Accordingly, food loads were normalized between foragers, by dividing the measurements of each forager by her own maximal measurement.

Exponential fits to interaction volume distributions

Since exponential distributions could be fit only to ‘positive’ interactions, i.e. where the forager was the donor, when we fit exponential distributions we neglected the negative interactions. Negative interactions constituted 216 out of 962, and accounted for 12% of the total food flow. The consequence of this approximation is that we effectively lose 12% accuracy in the modeled food flow. Despite this loss of accuracy, the results from this analysis were consistent with parameters obtained otherwise (without neglecting the negative interactions), ensuring that it was indeed sufficient to consider only the positive interactions.

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge E Segre, G Han, Y Burnishev for the technical help and Y Dover and R Harpaz for statistical advice. We also thank E Fonio, Y Heyman, A Gelblum, H Rajendran, A Le Boeuf and T Halperin for instructive comments on the manuscript. OF is the incumbent of the Shloimo and Michla Tomarin Career Development Chair, was supported by the Israeli Science Foundation grant 833/15 and would like to thank the Clore Foundation for their ongoing generosity. This research is further supported by a research grant from the Estate of Rachmiel Ramon Bloch and by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Unions Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (grant agreement No 648032).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Ofer Feinerman, Email: ofer.feinerman@weizmann.ac.il.

Iain D Couzin, Princeton University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Israel Science Foundation 833/15 to Ofer Feinerman.

  • European Research Council 648032 to Ofer Feinerman.

  • The Estate of Rachmiel Ramon Bloch to Ofer Feinerman.

  • Clore Duffield Foundation to Ofer Feinerman.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.029

References

  1. Anderson C, Ratnieks FL. Information Processing in Social Insects. 1999. Task partitioning in foraging: general principles, efficiency and information reliability of queueing delays; pp. 31–50. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bazazi S, Arganda S, Moreau M, Jeanson R, Dussutour A. Responses to nutritional challenges in ant colonies. Animal Behaviour. 2016;111:235–249. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Buffin A, Denis D, Van Simaeys G, Goldman S, Deneubourg JL. Feeding and stocking up: radio-labelled food reveals exchange patterns in ants. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5919. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Buffin A, Goldman S, Deneubourg JL. Collective regulatory stock management and spatiotemporal dynamics of the food flow in ants. The FASEB Journal. 2012;26:2725–2733. doi: 10.1096/fj.11-193698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cassill D, Tschinkel WR. Regulation of diet in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Journal of Insect Behavior. 1999;12:307–328. doi: 10.1023/A:1020835304713. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cassill D. Rules of supply and demand regulate recruitment to food in an ant society. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2003;54:441–450. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0639-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cook SC, Eubanks MD, Gold RE, Behmer ST. Colony-level macronutrient regulation in ants: mechanisms, hoarding and associated costs. Animal Behaviour. 2010;79:429–437. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dornhaus A, Chittka L. Information flow and regulation of foraging activity in bumble bees (Bombus spp.) Apidologie. 2004;35:183–192. doi: 10.1051/apido:2004002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dussutour A, Simpson SJ. Communal nutrition in ants. Current Biology. 2009;19:740–744. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fewell JH. Social insect networks. Science. 2003;301:1867–1870. doi: 10.1126/science.1088945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gordon DM, Paul RE, Thorpe K. What is the function of encounter patterns in ant colonies? Animal Behaviour. 1993;45:1083–1100. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1134. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gordon DM. Dynamics of task switching in harvester ants. Animal Behaviour. 1989;38:194–204. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80082-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gordon DM. The regulation of foraging activity in red harvester ant colonies. The American Naturalist. 2002;159:509–518. doi: 10.1086/339461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Greene MJ, Gordon DM. Interaction rate informs harvester ant task decisions. Behavioral Ecology. 2007;18:451–455. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arl105. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Greenwald E, Segre E, Feinerman O. Ant trophallactic networks: simultaneous measurement of interaction patterns and food dissemination. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:12496. doi: 10.1038/srep12496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gregson AM, Hart AG, Holcombe M, Ratnieks FL. Partial nectar loads as a cause of multiple nectar transfer in the honey bee (Apis mellifera): a simulation model. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2003;222:1–8. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(02)00487-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Howard DF, Tschinkel WR. The effect of colony size and starvation on food flow in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1980;7:293–300. doi: 10.1007/BF00300670. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. The Ants. Harvard University Press; 1990. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Huang MH, Seeley TD. Multiple unloadings by nectar foragers in honey bees: a matter of information improvement or crop fullness? Insectes Sociaux. 2003;50:330–339. doi: 10.1007/s00040-003-0682-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ionescu-Hirsch A. An annotated list of camponotus of israel (hymenoptera: formicidae), with a key and descriptions of new species. Israel Journal of Entomology. 2009;39:57–98. [Google Scholar]
  21. Josens RB, Roces F. Foraging in the ant Camponotus mus: nectar-intake rate and crop filling depend on colony starvation. Journal of Insect Physiology. 2000;46:1103–1110. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1910(99)00220-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Markin GP. Food distribution within laboratory colonies of the argentine ant,Tridomyrmex humilis (Mayr) Insectes Sociaux. 1970;17:127–157. doi: 10.1007/BF02223074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mayack C, Naug D. Individual energetic state can prevail over social regulation of foraging in honeybees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2013;67:929–936. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1517-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mersch DP, Crespi A, Keller L. Tracking individuals shows spatial fidelity is a key regulator of ant social organization. Science. 2013;340:1090–1093. doi: 10.1126/science.1234316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Oster GF, Wilson EO. Caste and ecology in the social insects. Monographs in Population Biology. 1978;12:1–352. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Parks JR. A Theory of Feeding and Growth of Animals. Vol. 11. Springer Science & Business Media; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  27. Pinter-Wollman N, Wollman R, Guetz A, Holmes S, Gordon DM. The effect of individual variation on the structure and function of interaction networks in harvester ants. Journal of The Royal Society Interface. 2011;8:1562–1573. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2011.0059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rivera MD, Donaldson-Matasci M, Dornhaus A. Quitting time: when do honey bee foragers decide to stop foraging on natural resources? Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2016;3 doi: 10.3389/fevo.2015.00050. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Robinson EJ, Franks NR, Ellis S, Okuda S, Marshall JA. A simple threshold rule is sufficient to explain sophisticated collective decision-making. PLoS One. 2011;6:e19981. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Rytter W, Shik JZ. Liquid foraging behaviour in leafcutting ants: the lunchbox hypothesis. Animal Behaviour. 2016;117:179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.04.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Seeley TD, Tovey CA. Why search time to find a food-storer bee accurately indicates the relative rates of nectar collecting and nectar processing in honey bee colonies. Animal Behaviour. 1994;47:311–316. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1994.1044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Seeley TD. Social foraging in honey bees: how nectar foragers assess their colony's nutritional status. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1989;24:181–199. doi: 10.1007/BF00292101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sendova-Franks AB, Hayward RK, Wulf B, Klimek T, James R, Planqué R, Britton NF, Franks NR. Emergency networking: famine relief in ant colonies. Animal Behaviour. 2010;79:473–485. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Simpson SJ, Raubenheimer D. A Multi-Level Analysis of Feeding Behaviour: The Geometry of Nutritional Decisions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1993;342:381–402. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1993.0166. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sorensen AA, Busch TM, Vinson SB. Control of food influx by temporal subcastes in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1985;17:191–198. doi: 10.1007/BF00300136. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Stoffolano JG, Haselton AT. The adult Dipteran crop: a unique and overlooked organ. Annual Review of Entomology. 2013;58:205–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-120811-153653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sumpter DJ, Mann RP, Perna A. The modelling cycle for collective animal behaviour. Interface Focus. 2012;2:764–773. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2012.0031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tenczar P, Lutz CC, Rao VD, Goldenfeld N, Robinson GE. Automated monitoring reveals extreme interindividual variation and plasticity in honeybee foraging activity levels. Animal Behaviour. 2014;95:41–48. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.06.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Traniello JFA. Recruitment behavior, orientation, and the organization of foraging in the carpenter ant Camponotus pennsylvanicus degeer (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1977;2:61–79. doi: 10.1007/BF00299289. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tschinkel WR. The nest architecture of the ant, Camponotus socius. Journal of Insect Science. 2005;5:9. doi: 10.1093/jis/5.1.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wilson EO, Eisner T. Quantitative studies of liquid food transmission in ants. Insectes Sociaux. 1957;4:157–166. doi: 10.1007/BF02224149. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Iain D Couzin1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Individual crop loads provide local control for collective food intake in ant colonies" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Ian Baldwin as the Senior Editor. One of the two reviewers, Danielle Mersch, has agreed to reveal her identity.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This paper employs a novel quantitative imaging technique to investigate the microscopic dynamics of trophallaxis dynamics within ant colonies. By integrating this method with a simple individual-level model the authors have demonstrated that there exist simple, but effective, feedback mechanisms whereby colony-level nutritional regulation can be mediated by individuals that react only to their own satiation levels.

Essential revisions:

1) This work needs to better take into account previous studies in this area. Notably the highly-relevant studies on ants (i.e. Traniello, 1977; Buffin et al., 2009) and honey bees (i.e. Seeley, 1989) must be cited, and the present findings discussed in the light to these previous works. You should clearly balance explanations of how your results – and the application of a new method – represent a significant advance in the field by describing precisely how their work builds upon the prior research cited above and significantly advances the field.

2) Regarding data binning in Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 2—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 1 - the data are binned without explanation being given regarding the number and size of bins that were chosen. In addition, the number of bins varies between plots based on the same variable; see e.g. Figure 2A, Figure 2B, Figure 2F all depict colony state, but Figure 2A and Figure 2F have 5 bins, while Figure 2B has 8 bins. Because data binning can alter the shape of lines and curves, the choice of binning gives the impression that it may have been adjusted to produce 'desired' results. Please conduct a sensitivity analysis of the binning methodology, and/or refrain from binning and plot all data points instead (like in Figure 2—figure supplement 4), ideally color-coding them by forager ID, and/or colony ID to provide a more comprehensive view to the reader.

3) It was highlighted that sometimes the reader could fail in grasping what is meant by some statements (e.g. Subsections “Global feedback from local interactions” and “Foraging Effort is Matched to the Colony's Need”). Care should be given to the clarity of writing and grammatical correctness in the revised manuscript to ensure that the messages come across clearly. Also, please do not rely on mathematical expressions alone to make a point; please spell out the messages in plain writing as well as providing the equations.

4) It is presently unclear to how the input data used in the analysis, and the associated scripts, will be made available. The database mentioned (Subsection “Image processing”) and all scripts used to run their analysis must be made available. Information on the details of the analyses are relatively limited and require searching through Figures and the Material and methods section. Too little detail is given on the criteria used to exclude data points, on the number of points/foragers excluded, on how the size of the smoothing windows were selected, or which algorithms were used, and why. Similarly, as noted above, there is no information on the logic behind the data binning, nor on how many data points were averaged for each point in their plots. Details on replicates are present but hidden in the material and methods section. The statistical methods are also too unclear, particularly with respect to whether tests were done per colony, and p-values combined, or whether colony ID was included as random factor, or completely ignored. Individual data are grouped as averages, and maybe even across colonies, so the reader cannot evaluate the extent and type of variability that exists within the data. These impact the statistical validity of the work. Furthermore, the reader must be able to replicate these analyses, and the appropriate information must be given in order to do so.

eLife. 2018 Mar 6;7:e31730. doi: 10.7554/eLife.31730.032

Author response


We would like to begin by thanking the referees for the comprehensive review. We have edited our manuscript accordingly. Major edits include an extensive revision of the Introduction, a better explanation of all mathematical progressions, and splitting Figure 2 into two figures (Figure 2 and Figure 3) such that the new Figure 2 (where new information was added) provides a direct comparison showing where our work diverges from previous works. We believe that this revision better places our paper in context of previous knowledge as well as significantly improves readability.

Essential revisions:

1) This work needs to better take into account previous studies in this area. Notably the highly-relevant studies on ants (i.e. Traniello, 1977; Buffin et al., 2009) and honey bees (i.e. Seeley, 1989) must be cited, and the present findings discussed in the light to these previous works. You should clearly balance explanations of how your results – and the application of a new method – represent a significant advance in the field by describing precisely how their work builds upon the prior research cited above and significantly advances the field.

This comment seems to imply that all three papers were not cited in the original version of this paper. Since eLife publishes these comments and their answers with the manuscript we would like to set the record straight and state that Buffin et al., 2009 and Seeley, 1989 were cited and, more than briefly, discussed in the first version of this manuscript. We thank the editor and referees for pointing us to Traniello’s impressive work from 1977 (more about this work in our answers to the specific points below).

We have now extensively revised the Introduction of this paper so it more clearly states the general framework, the specific study question, what was previously known, how our work builds on this knowledge and what are the caveats in the earlier knowledge that we attempt to narrow using our new technological tools. The Introduction relies on 30 citations of previous work. The new first paragraph of the Discussion section now explicitly states our two, main new micro-scale measurements that deviate from what was previously assumed. The following two subsections of the Discussion section discuss the implications of all our findings by comparing them with the existing knowledge in a detailed point by point manner (including the relevant citations) and discuss those aspects in which previous understanding of food accumulation in ant colonies must be expanded or revised.

The main contributions of this work are:

A) A fully quantitative model for collective inflow from local rules. Previous works of food flow regulation provided mostly qualitative intuition for the connections between the micro and the macro of this system. In this work, we provide a detailed quantitative model and quantitatively verify it by relying on microscopic scale measurements (detailed in the Introduction) that were never before performed. The difference between our quantitative model and older qualitative explanations is now expressed the Discussion section.

B) New empirical data on the microscopic scale and its implications. Our new microscopic-level data provides quantitative support for some previously assumed notions while quantitatively contradicting others. These contradictions differ in essence from previously assumed notions (Introduction), and lead to questions that have not been addressed or even raised before.

For example, a core contradiction that our data reveals, is that a typical trophallactic interaction does not fill a recipient up to her capacity, even if the forager has enough food in her crop to do so. This pulls the rug from under the individual-level reasoning that was previously used to explain various aspects of colony food intake, such as: a forager's unloading rate in the nest (Seeley, 1989, Buffin, 2009, Sendova-Franks, 2010), the number of trophallactic interactions in which she participates (Huang, 2003, Gregson, 2003), the distribution of crop loads in the colony (Gregson, 2003), and perhaps more. This finding should shift the attention from the factor that was previously focused on, the rate of trophallactic events, to the previously neglected factor, the amount of food transferred in interactions (see Figure 2B now expanded and moved from the Supplementary information to the main-text and modified to reflect this, subsection “Global feedback from local interactions” and subsection “Regulation of food flow”). While our work draws a quite comprehensive quantitative account for the links between the distribution of interaction volumes and the regulation of collective food flow, new questions are now called for: what causes the characteristic exponential distribution of interaction volumes? If it is not physical constraints of crop capacity that determine interaction volumes, what does? And why is it that these distributions appear when a simple all-or-nothing distribution could have yielded similar collective results? These new notions are discussed in subsection “Regulation of food flow” and subsection “Regulation of foraging trips”.

The second core contradiction we found was that foragers do not unload their entire crop contents before leaving the nest to reload at the food source. Similarly, to the above contradiction, this unintuitive finding means that previous reasoning regarding the number of trophallactic interactions of a forager (Gregson, 2003), the distribution of crop loads in the colony (Gregson, 2003), and the frequency of a forager's exits (Buffin, 2009), is wrong. The new questions that rise from this observation have to do with the timing of foragers' exits: If a forager's trigger to re-exit the nest is not the complete emptying of her crop (and not even an obvious crop load threshold), what is it that triggers a forager to leave? What kind of computational and sensory abilities are demanded from a forager in order to generate regulation of foraging frequency? How does the average crop load with which a forager exits remain constant at different colony state conditions, even though there is a positive probability to leave the nest at a wide range of crop loads, and different colony states provide different prior distributions of foragers' crop loads? These questions are addressed in our analyses (Figure 6), which introduce a suggestion for a simple biologically-plausible mechanism for regulation of foraging frequency with a wide range of crop loads and few computational demands, which has not been described before (Discussion section). A new question that follows is: why do foragers leave the nest with a wide range of crop loads even though it seems to be energetically wasteful and make it harder to regulate foraging frequencies? This notion is discussed in subsection “Regulation of foraging trips”.

C) Data sharing. The novel microscale data on real-time individual crop loads and trophallactic food transfer, which is made fully available as Source data files that accompany this submission, holds value on its own. It provides a quantitative glimpse for those interested in ant nutrition, distributed regulation, and social processes, into an otherwise unseen natural distributed process.

2) Regarding data binning in Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 2—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 1 - the data are binned without explanation being given regarding the number and size of bins that were chosen. In addition, the number of bins varies between plots based on the same variable; see e.g. Figure 2A, Figure 2B, Figure 2F all depict colony state, but Figure 2A and Figure 2F have 5 bins, while Figure 2B has 8 bins. Because data binning can alter the shape of lines and curves, the choice of binning gives the impression that it may have been adjusted to produce 'desired' results. Please conduct a sensitivity analysis of the binning methodology, and/or refrain from binning and plot all data points instead (like in Figure 2—figure supplement 4), ideally color-coding them by forager ID, and/or colony ID to provide a more comprehensive view to the reader.

We chose to present binned data for two reasons: First, for visual clarity – the trends are most pronounced in the average values, which we obtain by binning. The average values are also the system parameters in our analytical framework. Second, our dataset has an inherent bias, by which due to the dynamics of food accumulation, there are more data points in high colony states compared to low colony states. Binning the data balances the relative weight across colony states, despite the bias in the number of datapoints.

That being said, we acknowledge that binning may be a sensitive procedure, and that raw data is valuable on its own. We therefore supplement our binned plots with plots containing raw data. Additionally, results of sensitivity analysis on binning methodology are now added, where relevant, to the Supplementary material. More detailed information on the number of points averaged in each bin is now provided in the figure captions. Figure 5D is modified to include a color-coding scheme by forager ID.

Binned figures:

Figure 2A (interaction rate) – In addition to plotting the raw data (Figure 2—figure supplement 1), since this figure presents a fit to the averaged bins we made sure that different binning choices do not affect the obtained fit. To that end we performed a series of fits for a range of bin numbers from 4 to 10 bins. The results of each fit are presented in a table in the Supplementary Information (Table S1). Indeed, binning did not substantially affect the fit, as can be seen by the obtained values and their small STD compared to their mean. Originally, what we concluded from the fit was that the change in mean interaction rates is much less prominent than the change in mean interaction volumes. Importantly, this conclusion holds for any of the fits obtained from the series of varying bin numbers: in all interaction rate fits, the slope is close to 2-folds smaller than the intercept, as opposed to interaction volumes – for which both values are the same.

Figure 2A (interaction volume) – Here we present raw data alongside the binned data in the Supplementary information (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). The red line in the plot Figure 2A is not a fit, but a prediction from our analyses that has nothing to do with the specific choice of bins. Additionally, the prediction is about mean interaction volumes, such that the bin averages are the relevant parameter to compare to. To show that this representation is not misleading due to the specific choice of bin number, we fit a linear function to both raw data and binned data – in both cases the slope and the intercept were indeed extremely close to each other, and moreover – close to the predicted value. This data is now also shown in the caption of Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Figure 3A,B (previously Figure 2C,D) – These figures were meant to show that the recipient’s crop load affects the distribution of interaction volumes, while the forager’s crop load does not. This conclusion was obtained also by another method that is less bin-sensitive and which was previously included in the supplementary. To avoid presentation of visually misleading information, we chose to move the supplementary results to the main text instead of the previous plots. They now include the mean and the STD of the parameter 𝜆c as obtained from various fits of the same form that were applied to a series of 17 bin size choices uniformly covering the range [0.01-0.09]. This procedure is explained in the caption of Figure 3, and fit examples are Figure 2—figure supplement 4 and Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

Figure 3D (previously Figure 2F) – Apart from binned averages, this panel also includes distributions of the full data, which we believe is the best way to evaluate the variation while allowing to compare between recipients’ states and the general state in the colony. Therefore, we chose to leave this plot as is, while the raw data is provided in a data source file. We now specify in the caption the number of datapoints in each bin: The blue bins include n =84, 137, 165, 274, 496 data points for bins 1-5, respectively and the red bins include n=202 data points per colony state bin.

Figure 4B,C (previously Figure 3B,C) – Are now accompanied by the full data shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 3. To ensure that the fit in Figure 4B is not sensitive to the chosen bins, we performed it also on the raw data – both procedures yielded the same fitted line (Figure 3—figure supplement 3). This is explained in the caption of Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Since Figure 4C presents the same data as in Figure 4B, and since no quantitative fits were made to it, we did not separately evaluate its binning – we present its raw data in Figure 3—figure supplement 3. The number of data points in each bin is now specified in the caption of the Figure: n=26,26,28,39,57, for bins 1-5, respectively.

Figure 4E (previously Figure 3E) – No quantitative fit was made to the data presented in this figure, it just visually shows the response to the manipulation. Therefore, its sensitivity to binning is not quantified, but the raw data is presented in Figure 4—figure supplement 1. The number of data points within each bin are now specified in the Figure caption: n =28,36,21,14,9,28,27,19,5, for bins 1-9, respectively.

Figure 5A (previously Figure 4A) – This figure includes now the raw data, and the number of data points included in each bin is mentioned in the caption: n=11,15,13,13,15,13,17,22,27,30 for bins 1-10, respectively. The claim that the foragers’ crop loads at exit are not strongly dependent on the colony state is supported by Spearman’s correlation test that was performed on the raw data (subsection “Foragers' crop loads upon leaving the nest”), such that the specific choice of bins does not affect it.

Figure 5B (previously Figure 4B) – This Figure was modified to include a color-coding by forager ID. The data is available in the source data file.

Figure 5C,D (previously Figure 4C,D) – As in Figure 4E, no quantitative fit was made to the data presented in these plots, so their sensitivity to binning was not quantified, but raw data is shown in Figure 4—figure supplement 2. The number of data points within each bin is now specified in the figure caption: n=26,26,28,39,57 for bins 1-5, respectively.

Figure 6A,C (previously Figure 5A–C) – The statistical support for the results presented in Figure 6A–C was obtained with a method that is more appropriate to the structure of our data. The GLM analysis that was used in the previous version was done on raw binary data (exit vs. stay), which was not homogeneously distributed over our space parameters. Since GLM was shown to be sensitive to biases in data distribution, the fitted model could have been misleading. In this version of the manuscript we overcame this bias by fitting the same logistic function as used in the GLM to the binned data which evenly spreads the weight of the data on the parameter space (see Table S2). This method is not sensitive to the binning procedure, since bins are equally-sized and highly overlapping, with the binned value located at a location that takes into account the distribution of data-points within the bin.

Figure 6D (previously Figure 5D) – The GLM fit presented in this panel was obtained by using the binary data presented in panel 5C. The plotted data points represent fractions of “exit/stay” calculated from bins of the binary data, which were chosen by Matlab’s default ‘auto’ algorithm, which chooses a bin width to cover the data range and reveal the shape of the underlying distribution. Those points are for visualization only, they must come from binned data by nature, and they do not affect the GLM. Here the GLM does not suffer from the weakness explained above in Figure 6A–C, since this is a one-dimensional GLM for the data that is distributed only over the forager’s crop load factor. Over this dimension the distribution of the data is sufficient for a reliable GLM.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2 – see Figure 2A.

Supplementary figure 5A is now presented as Figure 3A in the main text. For details see Figure 3A above.

Supplementary figure 5B is now Figure 3—figure supplement 3 (‘Mean Interaction Volumes as a Function of the Recipient’s Crop Load’) in the new version of the manuscript. It includes the raw data. The same fit was obtained for different binnings (5-10 bins), such that we ensure that it is not sensitive to binning. This is explained in the figure caption. The number of data points in each bin (n=26,26,28,39,57 for bins 1-5, respectively) and the fitting result for different binning are specified in the caption.

Supplementary figure 6 is Figure 4—figure supplement 2 (‘Perturbation Experiments’) in the new version of the manuscript. It includes the raw data and the number of data points in each bin are specified in the figure caption (M1:

n=47,13,15,13,12,16,17,14 for bins 1-8, respectively; M2: n=28,36,21,14,5,15,18,10,4, for bins 1-9, respectively) and those of foragers that began foraging after the manipulation (M1: n=6,3,2, for bins 1-3, respectively; M2: n=4,13,9,9,1, for bins 1-5, respectively).

3) It was highlighted that sometimes the reader could fail in grasping what is meant by some statements (e.g. Subsections “Global feedback from local interactions” and “Foraging E_ort is Matched to the Colony's Need”). Care should be given to the clarity of writing and grammatical correctness in the revised manuscript to ensure that the messages come across clearly. Also, please do not rely on mathematical expressions alone to make a point; please spell out the messages in plain writing as well as providing the equations.

The entire manuscript was extensively edited for clarity and grammar. Changes were done in Results section so that all mathematical expressions are also explained in words. In general, the subsection titled “Global feedback from local interaction” has been significantly edited. We have broken down the mathematical reasoning into easy-to-read steps all of which are explained at length within the text.

4) It is presently unclear to how the input data used in the analysis, and the associated scripts, will be made available. The database mentioned (Subsection “Image processing”) and all scripts used to run their analysis must be made available.

We have now organized all the data in Excel files, and MATLAB executable. As these are not too large, we have attached these files as Source Data files along with the paper.

We note here that while doing this we have found a normalization error in the definition of the colony state of one of the three analyzed colonies. The error was small, and its correction had no significant impact other than small quantitative corrections from our previous version. This had no effect on any of our qualitative findings or conclusions. We apologize for this confusion.

Information on the details of the analyses are relatively limited and require searching through Figures and the Material and methods section.

The information about the analysis has now been elaborated. References to the relevant place in the Supplementary Information or the Materials and methods section were carefully made within each figure caption.

Too little detail is given on the criteria used to exclude data points, on the number of points/foragers excluded, on how the size of the smoothing windows were selected, or which algorithms were used, and why.

In this version we elaborate more on these issues.

Exclusion of data points:

The data presented here is from the first return of a forger to the nest from the food source until the end of the experiment. We generally did not exclude data points, unless otherwise specified as in Figure 6A,D (see the caption of Figure 6) and Figure 3A–C (see Supplementary Information 2.4 ‘Fitting an exponential function to the distribution of interaction volumes’).

In the subfigures of Figure 3A–C, only interactions in which the forager was the donor (81% of interactions, subsection’ Exponential fits to interaction volume distributions’) were considered, since we modeled the inflow of food into the colony. In addition, the exponential distributions of interaction volumes that we obtained, support that negative interactions (i.e. where the forager is the recipient), which cannot distribute exponentially, are not part of the process in interest.

In Figure 6A, observation points during trophallaxis were excluded, since being engaged in trophallaxis prevents the possibility to exit the nest in the next observation (after 2 seconds).

In Figure 6D, since foragers' crop loads rarely rose in the nest, their lowest crop observation in a visit was generally an `exit', so the calculated probability to exit in the lowest crop interval was 1. To ensure that the crop state played a role beyond this extreme effect, we fit a GLM excluding the lowest crop interval (Figure 6 caption).

Smoothing:

The single place where smoothing was performed was in the panel describing food accumulation data (Figure 1B and Figure 1—figure supplement 1). The lines presented here are smoothed versions of the raw data that use a simple moving average filter. Window size was chosen by comparing smoothed data to the raw data. We now include the smoothed data along with the raw data to show that the smoothed data indeed captures the original shape (Figure 1—figure supplement 1 in the SI and ‘Food flow’ subsection in the Materials and Methods section.

Similarly, as noted above, there is no information on the logic behind the data binning, nor on how many data points were averaged for each point in their plots.

This point was addressed in the answer to comment 2 above.

Details on replicates are present but hidden in the material and methods section.

Replicate information is now easier to find, aggregated in ‘Table 1: Experimental Colonies’.

The statistical methods are also too unclear, particularly with respect to whether tests were done per colony, and p-values combined, or whether colony ID was included as random factor, or completely ignored. Individual data are grouped as averages, and maybe even across colonies, so the reader cannot evaluate the extent and type of variability that exists within the data. These impact the statistical validity of the work.

All statistical analysis were performed on the pooled data from all colonies and foragers with no grouping. Neither colony ID nor forager ID were considered as random factors. Variability of the data can now be evaluated from the supplementary figures where the raw data is included. This information is now included in the captions of Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 6, Figure 1—figure supplement 2, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, Figure 2—figure supplement 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 3—figure supplement 2, Figure 3—figure supplement 3, Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

Furthermore, the reader must be able to replicate these analyses, and the appropriate information must be given in order to do so.

We agree with this comment and have made our explanations more detailed. As noted above this detailed information about the analyses is now present in the caption and supplementary information of the figures. The attachment of Source data files to this submission should also be of help in this respect.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Trophallactic interactions.

    This data also relates to Figures 2, 3 and 6.

    elife-31730-fig1-data1.xlsx (121.7KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.006
    Figure 1—source data 2. Temporal data.

    This data also relates to Figures 2 and 6.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.007
    Figure 2—source data 1. Sensitivity analysis for binning interaction rate data.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.014
    Figure 4—source data 1. Foraging cycles.

    This data also relates to Figure 5.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.022
    Figure 4—source data 2. Manipulation experiments.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.023
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.31730.029

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES