Skip to main content
GigaScience logoLink to GigaScience
. 2018 Feb 8;7(3):giy004. doi: 10.1093/gigascience/giy004

Advanced lesion symptom mapping analyses and implementation as BCBtoolkit

Chris Foulon 1,2,3,, Leonardo Cerliani 1,2,3, Serge Kinkingnéhun 1, Richard Levy 2, Charlotte Rosso 3,4,5, Marika Urbanski 1,2,6, Emmanuelle Volle 1,2,3, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC5863218  PMID: 29432527

Abstract

Background

Patients with brain lesions provide a unique opportunity to understand the functioning of the human mind. However, even when focal, brain lesions have local and remote effects that impact functionally and structurally connected circuits. Similarly, function emerges from the interaction between brain areas rather than their sole activity. For instance, category fluency requires the associations between executive, semantic, and language production functions.

Findings

Here, we provide, for the first time, a set of complementary solutions for measuring the impact of a given lesion on the neuronal circuits. Our methods, which were applied to 37 patients with a focal frontal brain lesions, revealed a large set of directly and indirectly disconnected brain regions that had significantly impacted category fluency performance. The directly disconnected regions corresponded to areas that are classically considered as functionally engaged in verbal fluency and categorization tasks. These regions were also organized into larger directly and indirectly disconnected functional networks, including the left ventral fronto-parietal network, whose cortical thickness correlated with performance on category fluency.

Conclusions

The combination of structural and functional connectivity together with cortical thickness estimates reveal the remote effects of brain lesions, provide for the identification of the affected networks, and strengthen our understanding of their relationship with cognitive and behavioral measures. The methods presented are available and freely accessible in the BCBtoolkit as supplementary software [1].

Keywords: Brain, MRI, Lesion, Statistics, Software, Open source, Connectivity, Disconnection, Behaviour


Recent advances in neuroimaging techniques have allowed for further examination of the structural and functional organization of the human brain. While diffusion weighted imaging (DWI) tractography [2] depicts how brain areas are connected together, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures the activity within and interaction between brain areas [3]. These methods have been successfully applied to the healthy human brain; however, they remain underused in patients with brain lesions.

Patients with brain lesions provide a unique opportunity to understand the functioning of the human mind. Lesion symptom mapping analyses traditionally assume that visible and directly damaged areas are responsible for a patient's symptoms [4–7]. Following this logic, the areas that are the most frequently damaged by the lesion are considered as the neuronal substrate for the function. Previous studies that used this method have identified critical areas dedicated to, for example, language production [8], comprehension [9], spatial awareness [10–13], and other high-level cognitive functions [14–17]. However, anatomical disconnections between regions are also important considerations for the exploration of cognitive deficits [18, 19]. The dysfunction of distant areas that are connected to the lesioned tissue has also been reported in fMRI studies. These studies have shown that the networks are disrupted even by distant lesions through disconnection and diaschisis mechanisms [20–22].

Nonlocal effects of lesions have previously been explored using various forms of atlas-based analyses of tract damage [23–32], lesion-driven tractography [32–34], disconnectome mapping [35–39], and lesion-driven resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) connectivity [34, 40]. However, determining what these methods actually measure and identifying how to properly combine them are not always clear to the scientific community. Furthermore, there is an extremely limited availability of free, open-source software that applies methods to measure the nonlocal effects of lesions. These resources and scientific tools remain very much inaccessible and present a potential threat to reproducible science [41].

Disconnections and diaschisis can have an impact on distant regions in several respects through maladaptive responses and pathological spread [42]. When disconnected from its inputs and outputs, a region can no longer contribute to the elaboration of the supported function. This phenomenon is called diaschisis [20, 21, 43]. Once deprived from its inputs and/or outputs, transneuronal degeneration in the region will occur [42], dendrite and synapse density will decrease in number, myelin content will be altered, and neurons will reduce in size or die through a mechanism called apoptosis, a programmed cell death [44–46]. Hence, a white matter disconnection leads to both functional and anatomical changes that extend well beyond the visible damage. New approaches are therefore required to capture the long-range effects that follow brain disconnections. For instance, cortical thickness [see, 47] and other volumetric [eg, voxel-based morphometry 48] analyses have been used to study the structural changes associated with brain lesions but have not been applied in the context of brain disconnection.

In response to this need, here, we provide a set of complementary solutions to measure both the circuit and the subsequent changes within the circuit that are caused by a lesion. We applied these methods to 37 patients with focal brain lesions following a stroke or surgical resection. We first assessed the risk of disconnection in well-known white matter tracts and tested their relationship with category fluency performance. Category fluency is an appropriate test to explore disconnection since it requires the associations between executive, semantic, and language production functions [49, 50]. We then developed a tractography-based approach in order to produce maps of the areas that are directly disconnected by the lesion and tested their relationship with category fluency performance. We additionally calculated the rs-fMRI connectivity of these areas to reveal the whole network of directly and indirectly disconnected regions that participate in category fluency. Finally, we explored potential microstructural changes in the latter disconnected regions by estimating neuronal loss or local connectivity degeneration derived from magnetic resonance-based measures of cortical thickness and resting-state fMRI entropy.

Methods

Participants and category fluency task

Thirty-seven right-handed patients (French native speakers; 19 females; mean age 48 ±14.2 years, age ranging from 23 to 75 years) who presented with a frontal lobe lesion at the chronic stage (>3 months) were included in this study (see Table 1 for demographics). These patients were recruited from the stroke unit and the neuroradiology department at Salpêtrière Hospital, the neurological unit at Saint-Antoine Hospital, and the neuroradiology department at Lariboisière Hospital in Paris. Patients with a history of psychiatric or neurological disease, drug abuse, or MRI contraindications were not included. Additionally, we gathered behavioral data from 54 healthy participants (French native speakers; 27 females; mean age 45.8 ±14.4 years, age ranging from 22 to 71 years) in order to constitute a normative group.

Table 1:

Demographical and clinical data

ID Age (years) Education (years) Gender Lesion side Lesion volume (mm3) Lesion delay (months) Etiology
P01 56 17 F Right 255 7 Stroke
P02 55 19 M Left 34374 76 Hematoma
P03 46 17 F Left 14847 126 Stroke
P04 50 11 F Left 110145 137 Surgery
P05 64 14 M Right 59048 119 Stroke
P06 32 16 F Right 15946 129 epilepsy
P07 51 11 M Bilateral 113170 54 Stroke
P08 70 5 F Left 51530 85 Surgery
P09 47 11 M Right 7809 115 Hematoma
P10 62 13 F Bilateral 21295 14 Hematoma
P11 41 16 M Right 55848 29 Surgery
P12 46 12 M Bilateral 2542 51 Hematoma
P13 67 15 M Left 4102 133 Stroke
P14 49 9 M Bilateral 14929 19 Hematoma
P15 36 14 F Right 40854 82 Surgery
P16 40 22 F Left 24829 56 Hematoma
P17 40 14 M Bilateral 14364 7 Hematoma
P18 23 16 F Right 21681 47 Surgery
P19 54 22 M Right 51897 48 Stroke
P20 71 17 M Left 25779 91 Hematoma
P21 23 15 F Right 29513 36 Surgery
P22 27 9 F Left 12986 30 Surgery
P23 26 13 F Left 2640 19 Surgery
P24 32 14 F Left 12653 4 Surgery
P25 59 16 F Left 97 9 Hematoma
P26 26 13 F Left 26928 32 Stroke
P27 58 12 M Left 1026 3 Stroke
P29 75 12 F Left 14938 16 Hematoma
P30 52 13 F Right 11978 20 Surgery
P31 58 12 M Right 13263 21 Surgery
P32 62 5 M Right 20281 9 Surgery
P33 41 17 M Left 7463 29 Surgery
P34 42 17 M Left 24319 6 Infection
P35 60 12 M Right 41897 24 Surgery
P36 51 14 F Right 39213 17 Surgery
P37 51 12 F Right 8133 48 Surgery
P38 33 17 M Right 140947 48 Surgery

All participants performed a category fluency task [51] in French. They were instructed to enumerate as many animals as possible during a timed period of 120 seconds. A clinical neuropsychologist (M. U.) recorded the results. Repetition and declination of the same animal were not taken into account in the final category fluency score.

The local ethics committee (Comités de protection des personnes, CPP Ile de France VI, Groupe hospitalier Pitie Salpetriere, reference project number 16-10) approved the experiment. All participants provided written, informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants also received a small indemnity for their participation.

Magnetic resonance imaging

An axial 3-dimensional magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo dataset covering the entire head was acquired for each participant (176 slices, voxel resolution = 1 × 1 × 1 mm, echo time = 3 msec, repetition time = 2300 msec, flip angle = 9°).

Additionally, the same participants underwent an fMRI session of resting state. During the resting-state session, participants were instructed to relax, keep their eyes closed, and avoid falling asleep. Functional images were obtained using T2-weighted echo-planar imaging with blood oxygenation level–dependent contrast using SENSitivity Encoding (SENSE) imaging, an echo time of 26 msec, and a repetition time of 3000 msec. Each dataset comprised 32 axial slices acquired continuously in ascending order covering the entire cerebrum with a voxel resolution of 2 × 2 × 3 mm; 200 volumes were acquired using these parameters for a total acquisition time of 10 minutes.

Finally, DWI was also acquired for 54 participants of the normative group (French native speakers; 27 females; mean age 45.8 ±14.4 years, age ranging from 22 to 71 years) and consisted of 70 near-axial slices acquired using a fully optimized acquisition sequence for the tractography of DWI, which provided isotropic (2 × 2 × 2 mm) resolution and coverage of the entire head with a posterior–anterior phase of acquisition. The acquisition was peripherally gated to the cardiac cycle [52], with an echo time = 85 msec. We used a repetition time equivalent to 24 RR (ie, interval of time between 2 heart beat waves). At each slice location, 6 images were acquired with no diffusion gradient applied. Additionally, 60 diffusion-weighted images were acquired in which gradient directions were uniformly distributed on the hemisphere with electrostatic repulsion. The diffusion weighting was equal to a b-value of 1500 s/mm2.

Stereotaxic space registration

As spatial normalization can be affected by the presence of a brain lesion, additional processing was required before the normalization could be calculated. For instance, in the case of bilateral lesions, the registration was weighted as previously reported [53]. For unilateral lesions, the first step was to produce an enantiomorphic filling of the damaged area [54]. Each patient's lesion (or signal abnormalities due to the lesion) was manually segmented (using FSLview; [55]). Unilateral lesions were replaced symmetrically by the healthy tissue of the contralateral hemisphere. Enantiomorphic T1 images were fed into FMRIB's Automated Segmentation Tool (FAST) [56] for estimation of the bias field and subsequent correction of radiofrequency field inhomogeneity. This improved the quality of the automated skull stripping performed using a brain extraction tool (BET) [57] and the registration to the MNI152 using affine and diffeomorphic deformations [58]. The original T1 images (non enantiomorphic) were registered to the MNI152 space using the same affine and diffeomorphic deformations as calculated above. Subsequently, lesions were segmented again in the MNI152 space under the supervision of an expert neurologist (E. V.). This method has been made freely available as the tool normalisation as part of BCBtoolkit [1].

The following sections are hypotheses driven and outlined in Supplementary Fig. 1.

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

Category fluency performance (mean performance with 95% confidence intervals [CIs]) for patients with (dark gray) or without (light gray) disconnection of each tract of interest. The green intervals indicate the range of controls’ performance corresponding to 95% CIs. * P < 0.05.

White matter tracts disconnection

Each patient's lesion was compared with an atlas of white matter tracts [59], indicating for each voxel, the probability of finding a white matter tract such as the arcuate fasciculus, the frontal aslant tract, or the uncinate fasciculus in the MNI152 coordinate system. We considered a tract to be involved when the likelihood of a tract being present in a given voxel was estimated above 50% [23]. This method is freely available as tractotron in BCBtoolkit [1]. We focused on frontal lobe tracts with a potential effect on executive, semantic, and language functions since all of the patients had a frontal lesion. These tracts included the cingulum, the frontal aslant, and the frontal superior and inferior longitudinal tracts for the executive functions [60]; the uncinate and the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi for the semantic access [61, 62]; and the anterior and long segment of the arcuate fasciculi for the phonemic system [63, 64]. A Kruskal-Wallis test was used to compare performance on the category fluency test for each tract between both preserved and disconnected patients and control participants. Subsequently, for each significant tract between patients, Mann-Whitney post hoc comparisons were performed (Fig. 1).

Direct disconnection of brain areas: structural connectivity network

This approach used the DWI datasets of 10 participants in the normative group to track fibers that passed through each lesion.

For each participant, tractography was estimated as indicated in [65].

Patients’ lesions in the MNI152 space were registered to each control native space using affine and diffeomorphic deformations [58] and, subsequently, used as seed for the tractography in Trackvis [66]. Tractography from the lesions were transformed in visitation maps [67, 68], binarized, and brought to the MNI152 using the inverse of precedent deformations. Finally, we produced a percentage overlap map by summing at each point in the MNI space the normalized visitation map of each healthy patient. Hence, in the resulting disconnectome map, the value in each voxel took into account the interindividual variability of tract reconstructions in controls and indicated a probability of disconnection from 50% to 100% for a given lesion (ie, thus explaining more than 50% of the variance in disconnection and corresponding to a large effect size). This procedure was repeated for all lesions, allowing the construction of a disconnectome map for each patient/lesion. These steps were automatized in the tool disconnectome map as part of the BCBtoolkit. Note that sample size and age effects are carefully explored and reported in the Supplementary Material. Overall, 10 patients are sufficient to produce a good enough disconnectome map that matches the overall population (more than 70% of shared variance). We also demonstrate in the Supplementary Material that disconnectome maps show a very high anatomical similarity between decades and no decrease of this similarity with age.

Thereafter, we used AnaCOM2, which is available within the BCBtoolkit, in order to identify the disconnections that are associated with a given deficit, that is, connections that are critical for a given function (Fig. 2). AnaCOM2 is comparable to AnaCOM [69] but has been reprogrammed and optimized to work on any Linux or Macintosh operating systems.

Figure 2:

Figure 2:

Areas directly disconnected by the lesion that significantly contributed to a decreased score on category fluency task (referred to as “disconnected areas” in the manuscript). A) Representative slices from disconnectome maps computed for category fluency performance; blue clusters indicate group average low performance and red clusters indicate high performance. B) Brain areas contributing significantly after correction for multiple comparisons. C) Category fluency performance (mean performance with 95% confidence intervals [CIs]) for patients with (dark gray) or without (light gray) disconnection of each of the examined cortical regions. The green interval indicates performance in matched controls with 95% CIs. Abbreviations: A, anterior group of thalamic nuclei; LH, left hemisphere; IPs, intraparietal sulcus; MFg, middle frontal gyrus; pars Op., frontal pars opercularis; PreSMA, presupplementary motor area; RH, right hemisphere; VA, ventral anterior; VLa, ventrolateral anterior; VLp, ventrolateral posterior. * P < 0.05 Bonferroni-Holm corrected for multiple comparisons.

Initially, AnaCOM is a cluster-based lesion symptom mapping approach that identifies clusters of brain lesions that are associated with a given deficit, that is, the regions that are critical for a given function. In the context of this article, AnaCOM2 used disconnectome maps instead of lesion masks to identify clusters of disconnection that are associated with category fluency deficits, that is, the connections that are critical for a given function. Compared to standard voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM) [8], AnaCOM2 regroups voxels with the same distribution of neuropsychological scores into clusters of voxels. Then, for each cluster larger than 8 mm3, AnaCOM2 will perform a Kruskal-Wallis test between patients with a disconnection, patients spared of disconnection, and controls. Resulting P values are Bonferroni-Holm corrected for multiple comparisons. Subsequently, significant clusters (P value < 0.05) are used to perform a post hoc Mann-Whitney comparison between 2 subgroups of interest (ie, disconnected patients and healthy participants). Post hoc results are Bonferroni-Holm corrected for multiple comparisons (statistical tests and corrections are computed using R language [70]).

Patient–control comparisons have been chosen as a first step in order to avoid drastic reduction of statistical power when 2 or more nonoverlapping areas are responsible for patients’ reduced performance [69]. Nonparametric statistics have been chosen, as it is fair to consider that some clusters will not show a Gaussian distribution. AnaCOM2 resulted in a statistical map that reveals, for each cluster, the significance of a deficit in patients who undertake a given task as compared to controls.

In the following sections, the term “clusters” systematically refers to the result of the post hoc Mann-Whitney comparison between disconnected patients and healthy participants who survived Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple comparisons.

fMRI meta-analyses

A method described by Yarkoni et al. [71, 72] was used to identify the functional networks involved in category fluency. We searched for brain regions that are consistently activated in studies that load highly on the following 2 features: “fluency” (120 studies, 4214 activations) and “category” (287 studies, 10179 activations). The results were superimposed on the 3D reconstruction of the MNI152 images (Fig. 3).

Figure 3:

Figure 3:

Areas classically activated with functional magnetic resonance imaging (P < 0.01 corrected for False Discovery Rate) during fluency (pink) and categorization (cyan) tasks. Areas involved in both fluency and categorization are highlighted in dark blue. Abbreviations: A, anterior group of thalamic nuclei; IPs, intraparietal sulcus; MFg, middle frontal gyrus; PreSMA, presupplementary motor area; VA, ventral anterior; VLa, ventrolateral anterior; VLp, ventrolateral posterior.

Indirect disconnection of brain areas: functional connectivity network

Rs-fMRI images were first motion corrected using MCFLIRT [73], then corrected for slice timing, smoothed with a full half width maximum equal to 1.5 times the largest voxel dimension, and finally filtered for low temporal frequencies using a Gaussian-weighted local fit to a straight line. These steps are available in Feat as part of the FSL package [74].

Rs-fMRI images were linearly registered to the enantiomorphic T1 images and, subsequently, to the MNI152 template (2 mm) using affine transformations. Confounding signals were discarded from rs-fMRI by regressing out a confound matrix from the functional data. The confound matrix included the estimated motion parameters obtained from the previously performed motion correction, the first eigenvariate of the white matter and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) as well as their first derivative. Eigenvariates can easily be extracted using fslmeants combined with the –eig option. White matter and CSF eigenvariates were extracted using masks based on the T1-derived 3-classes segmentation thresholded to a probability value of 0.9, registered to the rs-fMRI images, and binarized. Finally, the first derivative of the motion parameters, white matter, and CSF signal was calculated by linear convolution between their time course and a [−1 0 1] vector.

For each control participant, we extracted the time course that corresponded to each significant cluster, which was identified by the statistical analyses of the disconnectome maps. These time courses were subsequently correlated to the rest of the brain so as to extract seed-based resting-state networks. In order to obtain the most representative networks at the group level, for each seed-based resting-state network, we calculated the median network across the group. The median network resulting from a seed contains, in each voxel, the median of functional connectivity across all the controls. Medians were chosen instead of average as they are less sensitive to outliers and are more representative of the group-level data [75]. The calculation of the functional connectivity was automatized and made available inside the funcon tool as part of BCBtoolkit. Medians were calculated using the function fslmaths.

Visual inspection revealed that several of these resting-state networks shared a very similar distribution of activations. Therefore, an “activation” matrix was derived from the seed-based resting-state networks. This matrix consisted of columns that indicated each seed-based resting-state network and rows that represented the level of activation for each voxel in the cortex. This activation matrix was entered into a principal component analysis in SPSS (Chicago, Illinois) using a covariance matrix and varimax rotation (with a maximum of 50 iterations for convergence) in order to estimate the number of principal components to extract for each function. Components were plotted according to their eigenvalue (y; lower left panel in Fig. 4); we applied a scree test to separate the principal from residual components. This analysis revealed that 3 factors were enough to explain 82% of the variance of the calculated seed-based resting-state networks. This means that 3 factors are good enough to summarize most of the seed-based resting-state network results. Finally, brain regions that had a statistically significant relationship with the 3 components (ie, factor-networks) were detected using a linear regression with 5.000 permutations, in which the eigenvalues of the 3 components represented the independent variable and the seed-based resting-state networks represented the dependent variable. Results were Family Wise Error–corrected for multiple comparisons and projected onto the average 3D rendering of the MNI152 template in the top panel of Fig. 4. In the following sections, the term “factor-networks” systematically refers to brain regions that have a statistically significant relationship with the 3 components.

Figure 4:

Figure 4:

Functional networks involving the identified disconnected areas, as defined by resting state functional connectivity. Top panel, Main cortical networks involving the disconnected areas revealed by a principal component analysis. Bottom left panel, Principal component analysis of the raw functional connectivity result. Bottom right panel, Strength of the functional connectivity for patients with (dark gray) or without (light gray) involvement of the functional network. Abbreviations: CO, cingulo-opercular network; CS, cortico-striatal network; VFP, ventral fronto-parietal network. * P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01.

Additionally, for each patient, we extracted the time course that corresponded to each factor-network. These time courses were subsequently correlated to the rest of the brain so as to extract seed-based factor-networks in each patient. FSLstats was used to extract the strength of factor-networks functional connectivity and, subsequently, to compare patients according to their disconnection status. Note that a patient disconnected in a factor-network is a patient who has a disconnection in at least 1 of the clusters that contributed significantly to the factor-network.

Structural changes in disconnected regions

A distant lesion can affect cortical macro- and microstructures remotely. Conscious of this, we attempted to estimate these structural changes and their relationship with category fluency within each functional factor-network. To this aim, we explored the properties of each functional network using the following 2 complementary measures: T1w-based cortical thickness to identify fine local volumetric changes and the Shannon entropy of rs-fMRI as a surrogate for the local complexity of the neural networks [76]. Each original functional network seeded from each cluster was thresholded and binarized at r > 0.3 and used as a mask to extract cortical thickness and entropy. Patients’ lesions were masked out for these analyses.

For the cortical thickness, a registration-based method (Diffeomorphic Registration based Cortical Thickness, DiReCT) was used [77] from the T1-weighted imaging dataset. The first step, as for the normalization, was to produce an enantiomorphic filling of the damaged area in order to prevent the analysis from being contaminated by the lesioned tissue. The second step of this method consisted of creating two 2-voxel thick sheets, 1 laying just between the gray matter and the white matter and the second laying between the gray matter and the CSF. The gray–white interface was then expanded to the gray–CSF interface using diffeomorphic deformation estimated with Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs). The registration produced a correspondence field that allowed an estimate of the distance between the gray–white and the gray–CSF interfaces and thus corresponded to an estimation of cortical thickness. Voxels that belonged to the lesion were subsequently removed from the cortical thickness maps (see Supplementary Fig. S2). This approach has good scan–rescan repeatability and good neurobiological validity as it can predict with a high statistical power the age and gender of the participants [78] as well as atrophy following brain lesions [79]. Note that the striatum and thalamus were excluded from the cortical thickness analysis since they do not have a cortical ribbon.

Shannon entropy is an information theory–derived measure that estimates signal complexity [80, 81]. In the context of rs-fMRI, the entropy measures the local complexity of the blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) signal as a surrogate of the complexity of the spontaneous neuronal activity [82, 83]. Since “cells that fire together wire together” [84], for each gray matter voxel, Shannon entropy of rs-fMRI can be considered as a surrogate for the complexity of the connections within this voxel and between this voxel and the rest of the brain. Shannon entropy was extracted from the previously preprocessed rs-fMRI using the following formula: − sum (p*log(p)), where p indicates the probability of the intensity in the voxel [76].

FSLstats was used to extract the average cortical thickness and resting state fMRI entropy for each cluster and factor-network. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS software. In our analysis, Gaussian distribution of the data was not confirmed for the cortical thickness and the entropy measures using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Therefore, nonparametric statistics were chosen to compare cortical thickness and entropy levels between patients disconnected, patients spared, and controls in each cluster and factor-network. Additionally, bivariate Spearman rank correlation coefficient analyses were performed between the cortical thickness or entropy measurement of each functional network and each patient's category fluency performance. Correlation significant at P < 0.0041 survives Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (12 networks).

Results

White matter tracts disconnection

Patients’ lesions were compared to an atlas of white matter connections in order to identify the probability of tract disconnections [59]. A Kruskal-Wallis test indicated that for each tract, patients (ie, connected and disconnected) and control participants showed a significantly different performance on the category fluency test (all P < 0.001; full statistics reported in Table 2). Between patients, post hoc comparisons revealed that disconnections of the left frontal aslant (U = 90.0; P = 0. 0389), frontal inferior longitudinal (U = 69.0; P = 0. 0216) and frontal superior longitudinal (U = 75.0; P = 0. 0352) tracts, and the anterior (U = 28.5; P = 0. 0116) and long segment (U = 31.5; P = 0.0059) of the arcuate fasciculus were associated with a poorer performance in category fluency (Fig. 1). However, these post hoc comparisons did not survive Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple comparisons.

Table 2:

White matter tracts disconnection relationship with category fluency statistical report

3 groups comparison Patients disconnected and connected Patients disconnected and controls Patients connected and controls n1a n2b
Tract K P value U P value U P value U P value
Cingulum left 19 0.0001 141 0.2035 189 0.0003 277 0.0003 16 21
Cingulum right 19 0.0001 161 0.5 280 0.0001 187 0.0019 23 14
Uncinate left 19 0.0001 148 0.3994 176 0.0027 291 0.0001 13 24
Uncinate right 19 0.0001 167 0.4635 209 0.0004 258 0.0003 17 20
Arcuate anterior segment left 22 0.0000 29 0.0116 12 0.0004 454 0.0001 5 32
Arcuate anterior segment right 19 0.0001 126 0.3855 118 0.0025 348 0.0001 16 21
Arcuate long segment left 23 0.0000 32 0.0059 13 0.0001 453 0.0002 6 31
Arcuate Long segment right 19 0.0001 107 0.2559 117 0.0068 349 0.0001 9 28
Inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus left 19 0.0001 165 0.5 196 0.0011 271 0.0001 15 22
Inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus right 19 0.0001 157 0.3457 199 0.0002 268 0.0004 17 20
Frontal aslant tract left 21 0.0000 90 0.0389 90 0.0001 377 0.0004 11 26
Frontal aslant tract right 19 0.0001 155 0.3131 194 0.0001 272 0.0012 18 19
Frontal inferior longitudinal left 21 0.0000 69 0.0216 54 0.0001 413 0.0004 9 28
Frontal inferior longitudinal right 19 0.0001 140 0.3051 171 0.0022 295 0.0001 13 34
Frontal superior longitudinal left 20 0.0000 75 0.0352 73 0.0004 393 0.0002 9 28
Frontal superior longitudinal right 19 0.0001 129 0.1992 120 0.0001 346 0.0005 13 34

Results are not corrected for multiple comparisons.

aNumber of disconnected patients.

bNumber of spared patients.

These results indicate that poor performance measured in patients with brain damage can be associated to some extent with white matter tract disconnections.

Direct disconnection of brain areas: structural connectivity network

As different white matter atlases exist for the interpretation of the white matter tract disconnection [85] and atlas-based approaches cannot assess the disconnection of the subportion of tracts nor the involvement of multiple tracts by a lesion, data-driven maps of disconnection or “disconnectomes” were produced. Using tractography in a group of 10 healthy controls, the registered lesions were used as a seed to reveal white matter tracts that passed through the injured area so as to produce maps of disconnections, later referred to as disconnectome maps. Category fluency scores were attributed to each patient's disconnectome map (see Fig. 2A). A Kruskal-Wallis test indicated that for several clusters, patients (ie., connected and disconnected) and control participants showed a significantly different performance on the category fluency test (all P < 0.001; full statistics reported in Table 3).

Table 3:

Direct disconnection of brain areas relationship with category fluency statistical report

Fluency score 3 groups comparison Patients disconnected and connected (uncorrected) Patients disconnected and controls Patients connected and controls n1a n2b
Disconnected areas Kruskal-Wallis P value U P value U P value U P value
Left hemisphere PreSMA 21.34128456 0.0059 212 0.0456 88.5 0.0248 377.5 0.0329 12 25
IPs 23.35102548 0.0023 172 0.0098 18 0.0295 448 0.0324 7 30
Cingulate ant 18.6697471 0.0125 160.5 0.7452 304 0.0248 162 0.0329 25 12
Cingulate mid 21.52289636 0.0054 219 0.0464 95.5 0.0141 370.5 0.0329 13 24
MFg 23.12826675 0.0026 237 0.0103 81.5 0.0054 384.5 0.0329 13 24
Opercularis, striatum, thalamus 23.99647373 0.0017 179 0.0043 16 0.0249 450 0.0324 7 30
Right hemisphere PreSMA 22.92724537 0.0028 208 0.0130 50.5 0.0138 415.5 0.0329 10 27
Cingulate ant 18.8698263 0.0125 185.5 0.6470 225 0.0330 241 0.0329 20 17
Cingulate mid 18.62681983 0.0125 137.5 0.6966 317 0.0415 149 0.0329 25 12
MFg 22.06856039 0.0042 196 0.0382 54 0.0180 412 0.0329 10 27
Striatum 18.604408 0.0125 154.5 0.8966 310 0.0313 156 0.0329 14 23
Thalamus 19.117101 0.0125 192 0.5326 202 0.0248 264 0.0329 14 23

Unless specified, P values are Bonferroni-Holms corrected for multiple comparisons.

Abbreviation: MFg, middle frontal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area.

aNumber of disconnected patients.

bNumber of connected patients.

Results were further statistically assessed using Mann-Whitney post hoc comparisons in order to identify areas that, when deafferented due to a disconnection mechanism, lead to a significant decrease in performance in category fluency when compared to controls.

The following results are Bonferroni-Holm corrected for multiple comparisons. Main cortical areas in the left hemisphere included the pre supplementary motor area (preSMA; cluster size = 1449; Mann Whitney U = 88.5; P = 0.025), the anterior portion of the intraparietal sulcus (IPs; cluster size = 1143; U = 18; P = 0.030), anterior cingulate gyrus (cluster size = 837; U = 304; P = 0.025) and middle cingulate gyrus (cluster size = 898; U = 95.5; P = 0.014), the middle frontal gyrus (MFg; cluster size = 829; U = 81.5; P = 0.005), and the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus (cluster size = 5314; U = 16; P = 0.025).

In the right hemisphere, the preSMA (cluster size = 1050; U = 50.5; P = 0.014), the MFg (cluster size = 552; U = 54; P = 0.018), the anterior cingulate gyrus (cluster size = 572; U = 44.5; P = 0.009), and the middle cingulate gyrus (cluster size = 817; U = 317; P = 0.041) were also involved (Fig. 2B).

Subcortical areas in the left hemisphere involved the caudate, the putamen, and several ventral thalamic nuclei including the ventral anterior, the ventrolateral anterior, and the ventrolateral posterior as a part of the same cluster (cluster size = 5314; U = 16; P = 0.025).

In the right hemisphere, the striatum (cluster size = 527; U = 310; P = 0.031) and the ventral thalamic nuclei (cluster size = 935; U = 202.0; P = 0.025) were also involved (Fig. 2B).

Additionally, between-patient (ie, connected and disconnected, uncorrected for multiple comparisons) comparisons confirmed the critical involvement of the preSMA (U = 212; P = 0.0456), the MFg (U = 237; P = 0.01), the pars opercularis (U = 179; P = 0.004), and the IPs (U = 172; P = 0.01) in the left hemisphere. The preSMA (U = 208; P = 0.01) and the MFg (U = 196; P = 0.038) were also involved in the right hemisphere (Fig. 2C). Full statistics are reported in Table 3.

fMRI Meta-analyses

We further examined whether the disconnected areas in patients with poor performance are functionally engaged in tasks related to fluency and categorization using a meta-analysis approach [71, 72].

The result indicates that disconnected areas reported as significantly contributing to category fluency performance in patients are classically activated by functional MRI tasks that require either fluency or categorization in healthy controls (Fig. 3).

Indirect disconnection of brain areas: functional connectivity network

As the disconnectome mapping method cannot measure the indirect disconnection produced by a lesion (ie, it fails to measure the disconnection in a region that is not directly anatomically connected to a damaged area but that nonetheless remains a part of the same large network of functionally connected areas), we used functional connectivity in healthy controls. This allowed us to reveal the entire network of regions that are functionally connected to the areas that were reported as contributing significantly to the category fluency performance when directly disconnected. When compared to tractography, functional connectivity has the added advantage of revealing the areas that contribute to the network through both direct and indirect structural connections.

Principal component analysis indicated that the significant areas that contributed to category fluency performance belonged to 3 main functional networks (ie, factor-networks) (Fig. 4), which accounted for more than 80% of the total variance of the functional connectivity results.

The left cingulate clusters (anterior and middle), the right anterior cingulate, the middle frontal gyrus, the thalamus, and the operculum all belonged to the cingulo-opercular network (CO) [86], including the right preSMA, posterior cingulate, and rostral portion of the middle frontal gyrus.

The middle of the cingulate gyrus and the striatum in the right hemisphere both belonged to a cortico-striatal network (CS) [87] that involves the right thalamus and striatum.

Finally, the left MFg, preSMA, IPs, pars opercularis, thalamus, and striatum were all involved in a larger, left ventral fronto-parietal network (VFP), which also included other areas such as the right preSMA, the frontal eye field, and the temporo-parietal junction [88].

Additional analyses revealed the differences in the functional connectivity of these factor-networks relative to the disconnected status of areas involved in category fluency. Between-patient (ie, connected and disconnected) comparisons revealed significantly lower functional connectivity in the left VFP network (U = 54.0; P = 0.006) and in the CS network (U = 63.0; P = 0.027) when anatomically disconnected. The CO network, however, did not show any significant difference (U = 40.0; P = 0.213). Overall, the strength of the functional connectivity for each patient did not correlate significantly with the fluency performance.

Structural changes in disconnected regions

Additional exploratory analyses revealed structural changes related to the disconnections. We estimated these changes using the following 2 complementary measures: T1w-based cortical thickness to identify fine local volumetric changes and the Shannon entropy of rs-fMRI as a surrogate for the local complexity of the neural networks [76].

When compared to controls, patients showed a reduced cortical thickness in the left pars opercularis (H = 13; P = 0.0012), MFg (H = 8; P = 0.0143), preSMA (H = 8; P = 0.0224), IPs (H = 9; P = 0.0131), and right anterior (H = 7; P = 0.0296) and middle cingulate gyrus (H = 23; P = 0.000). When compared to patients with no disconnection, only the right middle cingulate gyrus survived the Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple comparisons (U = 67; P = 0.004). When compared to controls, disconnected patients showed reduced entropy for all regions (all P < 0.05, except for the right middle frontal gyrus). However, when compared to patients with no disconnection, none of the comparisons survived the Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple comparisons. Uncorrected P values are reported as an indication in Table 4 and the bar chart in Supplementary Fig. S3.

Table 4:

Cortical thickness and functional magnetic resonance imaging entropy measures in disconnected areas

Cortical thickness 3 groups comparison Patients disconnected and connected Patients disconnected and controls Patients connected and controls n1a n2 b
Disconnected areas Kruskal-Wallis P value U P value U P value U P value
Left hemisphere PreSMA 8 0.0224 109 0.0944 168 0.0057 514 0.0565 12 25
IPs 9 0.0131 54 0.0251 59 0.0019 667 0.1134 7 30
Cingulate ant 5 0.0822 110 0.1000 465 0.0175 269 0.2061 25 12
Cingulate mid 5 0.0759 139 0.2998 278 0.1436 435 0.0137 13 24
MFg 8 0.0143 109 0.0695 172 0.0028 502 0.0710 13 24
Opercularis 13 0.0012 40 0.0061 44 0.0006 583 0.0225 7 30
Right hemisphere PreSMA 4 0.1328 134 0.4931 214 0.1711 523 0.0254 10 27
Cingulate ant 7 0.0296 167 0.4696 362 0.0191 295 0.0169 20 17
Cingulate mid 23 0.0000 61 0.0020 223 0.1414 254 0.1415 25 12
MFg 6 0.0587 116 0.2634 163 0.0359 538 0.0359 10 27
Shannon entropy
Left hemisphere PreSMA 24 0.0000 86 0.2171 85 0.0004 210 0.0000 12 25
IPs 27 0.0000 40 0.0422 18 0.0002 260 0.0000 7 30
Cingulate ant 44 0.0000 84 0.1158 109 0.0000 3 0.0000 25 12
Cingulate mid 36 0.0000 97 0.3029 45 0.0000 127 0.0000 13 24
MFg 16 0.0004 100 0.4246 125 0.0043 272 0.0003 13 24
Opercularis, striatum, thalamus 17 0.0002 65 0.3680 75 0.0181 287 0.0001 7 30
Right hemisphere PreSMA 8 0.0177 82 0.2364 117 0.0078 413 0.0243 10 27
Cingulate ant 55 0.0000 81 0.0640 16 0.0000 4 0.0000 20 17
Cingulate mid 22 0.0000 111 0.4596 203 0.0001 114 0.0003 25 12
MFg 22 0.0000 55 0.0497 136 0.0533 209 0.0000 10 27
Striatum 23 0.0000 110 0.4436 202 0.0001 100 0.0001 14 23
Thalamus 58 0.0000 67 0.0204 0 0.0000 6 0.0000 14 23

Results are not corrected for multiple comparisons.

Abbreviation: IPs, intraparietal sulcus; MFg, middle frontal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area.

aNumber of disconnected patients.

bNumber of spared patients.

None of these measures correlated significantly with the fluency performance.

In order to further assess the integrity of the whole network of regions that were functionally connected to the areas reported as having significantly contributed to category fluency performance, we also extracted the cortical thickness and entropy from the regions that were functionally connected to the disconnected areas. Correlation analyses indicated that a thinner cortex in the ventral fronto-parietal network seeded from the left MFg (Spearman Rho = .464 ± 0.341; P = .004), IPs (Rho = .475 ± 0.341; P = .003), and left pars opercularis/striatum/thalamus (Rho = .512 ± 0.341; P = .001) corresponded to a reduced performance in category fluency (Fig. 5). Additionally, a thinner cortical thickness in the left preSMA functional network (Rho = .376 ± 0.341; P = .024) and a higher rs-fMRI entropy (Rho = −.420 ± 0.370; P = .019) in the mid cingulate gyrus functional network were associated with poorer performance in category fluency. These last 2 results, however, did not survive Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple comparisons.

Figure 5:

Figure 5:

Dimensional relationship between cortical thickness measured in rs-fMRI disconnected networks and category fluency. Note that regression lines (in black) and intervals (mean confidence intervals in red) are for illustrative purposes since we performed a rank-order correlation. Abbreviations: IPs, intraparietal sulcus; MFg, middle frontal gyrus.

The same analyses were repeated controlling for age and lesion size and confirmed the results for the ventral fronto-parietal network seeded from the left MFg (Spearman Rho = .423; P = .01), IPs (Rho = .538; P = .001), and left opercularis (Rho = .590 ± 0.341; P < .001) and corresponded to a reduced performance in category fluency (Fig. 5). Additionally, a thinner cortical thickness in the left preSMA functional network (Rho = .439; P = .007) and a higher rs-fMRI entropy (Rho = −.420 ± 0.370; P = .019) in the mid cingulate gyrus functional network were associated with poorer performance in category fluency.

Discussion

A large set of complementary methods can capture the impact of lesions on distant regions and expose the subsequent consequences on patients’ neuropsychological performance. Several of these methods are built directly into our freely available software package BCBtoolkit. This package can be used to measure the pathophysiological mechanisms that cause cognitive deficits and to assess the relationship between these mechanisms and their consequential effects. Here, we evaluated the risk of disconnection of classically defined white matter tracts and tested their relationship with category fluency performance. We then used a tractography-based approach to reveal regions that were structurally disconnected by the lesion and to assess their relationship with category fluency performance as compared to controls and other patients. Functional connectivity from the disconnected regions revealed large networks of interconnected areas. Within these regions/networks, measures of cortical thickness and entropy of the rs-fMRI images were correlated to fluency performance, suggesting that some structural changes that occurred within these networks were due to the remote effect of a lesion that led to cognitive impairments. Consequently, the BCBtoolkit provided investigators with an ability to quantify the effect of brain damage on the whole brain and to explore its relationship to behavioral and cognitive abilities.

The investigation into the contribution of white matter tract disconnection is an approach that is more than a century old and postulates an interruption in the course of white matter tracts in single case patients [89, 90]. Our method provides an anatomical rationale and puts forth a statistical methodology that enables it to be extended to group-level studies. In the case of category fluency performance, this analysis particularly revealed a significant involvement of the anterior and long segments of the arcuate fasciculus, which are implicated in the language network [90–92]. However, these tracts have been defined, for convenience, by their shape (eg, uncinate for hook-shaped connections and arcuate for arched-shaped connections) and should not be considered as a single unit, as, ultimately, subportions could contribute differently to the elaboration of the cognition and behavior.

Data-driven maps of disconnection, or disconnectomes, were consequently produced in order to identify the subportion of disconnected tracts and reveal the pattern of cortico-subcortical areas that were disconnected by the lesion. For the first time, we exemplify that this method goes beyond assessing only lesions and can be used to assess the relationship between disconnected areas and the patient's neuropsychological performance. Here, this approach revealed that category fluency performance significantly decreased when several cortical and subcortical clusters were directly disconnected. The observed areas are consistent with previous lesion studies on fluency tasks [93]. Furthermore, each area identified as significantly involved in this analysis corresponded, almost systematically, to activation loci derived from fMRI studies in healthy controls who performed fluency and/or categorization tasks. This result suggests that the method appropriately identified altered functional networks that contribute to the category fluency test. Nonetheless, one might argue that a cascade of polysynaptic events can influence behavior and that dysfunctional, disconnected areas will also impact other indirectly connected areas.

In order to explore this additional dimension, we calculated the functional connectivity of the previously identified disconnected regions (ie, clusters). In the case of the present analysis on category fluency performance, we revealed that the disconnected areas belonged to the following 3 large functional networks (ie, factor-networks): a left-dominant ventral fronto-parietal network; a mirror of the right-lateralized ventral attention network [94], which links key language territories [88] and is associated with executive functions [95, 96]. In addition, we showed the involvement of the cingulo-opercular network, a network that interacts with the fronto-parietal control network for the control of goal-directed behaviors [97], which together with the cortico-striatal network may also be linked to a reduced performance in fluency tasks [98]. The cingulo-opercular and cortico-striatal networks may also have contributed to performance through the global inertia or the ability of participants to allocate and coordinate resources during the task [99]. Finally, disconnection was associated with a significant reduction of functional connectivity in 2 of the 3 factor-networks investigated. This is an important result, as functional connectivity appeared to be less significantly impaired in bilateral networks, suggesting that the proportion of the preserved functional network in both of the intact hemispheres may contribute to the strength of functional connectivity.

Changes in connectivity should induce changes in the microstructure of the areas of projection and provoke cognitive or behavioural consequences. Measures of the cortical thickness revealed a significant thinning for some, but not all, directly disconnected areas. This result may reflect a potential transneuronal degeneration mechanism [42]. However, current limitations in spatial resolution and MRI signal might have biased this measure in some regions due to changes in myelination in the lower layers of the cortex [100]. Cortical thickness analyses revealed that the left dominant ventral fronto-parietal network, whether it is seeded from MFg, IPs, or subcortical structures in the left hemisphere, had a reduced cortical thickness associated with the category fluency performance. This result indicates a strong and encouraging relationship between the integrity of a network derived from measures of cortical thickness and behavioral performances. Future research can benefit from this approach to stratify patient populations and predict potential recovery.

Additionally, we explored whether structural changes such as other neural (eg, synaptic plasticity) or nonneural factors (eg, altered properties of the vasculature) could also be captured by measures of rs-fMRI entropy. Our results replicated recently published results that showed a strong decrease of entropy in both hemispheres when patients were compared to controls [101]. This indicates a large-scale effect of brain lesion on the overall BOLD dynamic of the brain. Finally, the result between patients (connected and disconnected) did not survive the correction for multiple comparisons, suggesting that, although promising, Shannon entropy measures of BOLD may be too noisy of a measure to capture very fine microstructural events with high enough statistical power.

Previous reports indicated that AnaCOM suffers from lower specificity than VLSM (Rorden et al. [102]. AnaCOM compares performance of patients with that of controls, an approach that has previously been criticized [102]. In the context of our study, classic VLSM did not reveal any significant area involved with category fluency. In classic VLSM approaches, nonoverlapping lesions are competing for statistical significance, fundamentally assuming that a single region is responsible for the symptoms. In the present study, we followed Associationist principles [19, 18], assuming that several interconnected regions will contribute to the elaboration of the behavior. By comparing the performance between patients and a control population using AnaCOM2, several nonoverlapping regions can reach significance without competing for it. Hence, our results differ theoretically and methodologically from previous approaches. Perhaps more importantly, the network of disconnected areas revealed by AnaCOM2 is typically considered as functionally engaged during fluency and categorization tasks in healthy controls.

Newer multivariate methods have also been shown to provide superior performance compared to traditional VLSM (ie, support vector regression lesion-symptom mapping) [7, 103]. For instance, such approaches have been used to model the statistical relationship between damaged voxels in order to reduce false positives. In the disconnectome maps, this relationship has been preestablished using an anatomical prior derived from tractography in healthy controls. Therefore, it is not recommended to use multivariate approaches with the disconnectome maps, as they might come into conflict with the prebuilt anatomical association between the voxels. Additionally, these approaches require a much larger database of patients than the current study. Future research that uses large lesion databases will be required to explore the effect of multivariate statistical analysis on disconnectome maps.

Multivariate approaches also elegantly demonstrated that false positives can be driven by the vascular architecture [7]. This is an important limitation concerning any voxel and vascular lesion symptom mapping. Here, the group of patients explored had stroke and surgical lesions. Although we cannot exclude the participation of the vascular architecture in the present findings, the heterogeneity of the lesions included in our analyses may have limited this factor. Additionally, the statistical interaction between vascular architecture and the disconnectome map results remain to be explored in large databases of lesions.

Methods used to estimate cortical thickness have previously been reported to perform poorly in peri-infarct regions, and the quality of the tissue segmentation may be particularly poor for stroke patients [79]. Here, we followed previously published recommendations for applying DiReCT [77] to the data from stroke patients; the lesion was masked out, the tissue segmentations were visually inspected, and manual boundary correction was performed when necessary (see Supplementary Fig. S2 for an example).

Finally, we applied our methods to the neural basis of category fluency as a proof of concept. The anatomy of category fluency should be, ideally, replicated in a larger sample of patients that includes adequate lesion coverage of the entire brain to provide a more comprehensive understanding of category fluency deficit after a brain lesion. While gathering such a large dataset of patients with brain lesions would have been impossible to achieve before, it might soon become possible thanks to collaborative initiatives such as the Enigma Consortium stroke recovery initiative [104, 105].

Conclusion

Overall, using BCBtoolkit, researchers and clinicians can measure distant effects of brain lesions and associate these effects with neuropsychological outcomes. However, our methods require the manual delineation of lesion masks, automatization remaining a big challenge, especially on T1 images [105]. Taken together, these neuroimaging measures help discern the natural history of events that occur in the brain after a lesion, as well as assist in the localization of functions. These methods, gathered in the BCBtoolkit, are freely available as supplementary software [1, 107].

Availability of supporting data

Patients’ lesions registered to the reference map MNI152 are available as supplementary material via the BCBlab website [106] and via the GigaScience database GigaDB [107]. However, we are not able to fully share the actual clinical sample data because sharing of the clinical raw data is not covered by the participants’ consent. A copy of the consent form as signed by the participants is available via GigaDB.

Availability of supporting source code and requirements

  • Project name: BCBtoolkit

  • Project home page: http://toolkit.bcblab.com

  • Operating system(s): Linux, MacOS

  • Programming language: Java, Bash, R

  • Other requirements: FSL, R, Python 2.7, Numpy

  • License: BSD 3-Clause

An archival copy of the supporting source code is also available via GigaDB [107].

Additional material

Figure S1: Step by step, hypotheses-driven analyses with BCBtoolkit.

Figure S2: Native T1, enantiomorphic deformation and derived cortical thickness of 3 representative patients.

Figure S3: Cortical thickness and Shannon entropy measures (mean with 95% confidence intervals) in patients with (dark gray) or without (light gray) disconnection for each of the disconnected areas. The green interval indicates performance in matched controls with 95% confidence intervals.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author contributions

C.F. implemented the methods inside the BCBtoolkit, performed the analyses, and wrote the manuscript. L.C. created the pipeline for the preprocessing of the resting state and for the functional correlation and revised the manuscript. S.K. conceived and helped to upgrade the statistical analyses. C.R. collected the neuroimaging data. M.U. and E.V recruited the participants; collected and built the database of patients; matched healthy controls, including the neuropsychological and neuroimaging data; and revised the manuscript. E.V. also participated in the conception of the lesion study and provided funding for the database acquisition. R.L. provided funding for the study and revised the manuscript. M.T.d.S. wrote the manuscript, provided funding, conceived and coordinated the study, and reviewed and collected neuroimaging data.

Supplementary Material

GIGA-D-17-00149_Original-Submission.pdf
GIGA-D-17-00149_Revision-1.pdf
GIGA-D-17-00149_Revision-2.pdf
Response-to-Reviewer-Comments_Original-Submission.pdf
Response-to-Reviewer-Comments_Revision-1.pdf
Reviewer-1-Report_Original-Submission -- Aaron Boes

07 Aug 2017 Reviewed

Reviewer-1-Report_Revision-1 -- Aaron Boes

28 Nov 2017 Reviewed

Reviewer-2-Report_Original_Submission -- Muthuraman Muthuraman

15 Aug 2017 Reviewed

Reviewer-2-Report_Revision-1 -- Muthuraman Muthuraman

29 Nov 2017 Reviewed

Supplement Materials

Acknowledgments

We thank Lauren Sakuma, Roberto Toro, Jean Daunizeau, Emmanuel Mandonnet, Beatrice Garcin, Stephanie J. Forkel, and the BCBlab and Brainhack for useful discussions. We also thank Mozilla spaces for their support, the participants of this study as well as Prof. Claude Adam, Dr. Carole Azuar, Dr. Marie-Laure Bréchemier, Dr. Dorian Chauvet, Dr. Frédéric Clarençon, Dr. Vincent Degos, Prof. Sophie Dupont, Prof. Damien Galanaud, Dr. Béatrice Garcin, Dr. Florence Laigle, Dr. Marc-Antoine Labeyrie, Dr. Anne Leger, Prof. Vincent Navarro, Prof. Pascale Pradat-Diehl, and Prof. Michel Wager for their help in recruiting the patients. The research leading to these results was funded by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (grants ANR-09-RPDOC-004-01 and ANR-13- JSV4-0001-01) and from the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale. Additional financial support was provided by the program Investissements d’avenir (ANR-10-IAIHU-06).

References

  • 1. Brain Connectivity Behaviour Toolkit (BCBtoolkit), http://toolkit.bcblab.com/. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Jones DK, Simmons A, Williams SC et al. . Non-invasive assessment of axonal fiber connectivity in the human brain via diffusion tensor MRI. Magn Reson Med 1999;42(1):37–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Logothetis N. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 2008;453(7197):869–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Broca P. Perte de la parole, ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lobe antérieur gauche du cerveau. Bull Soc Anthropol 1861;2:235–8, 301–321. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Damasio H, Damasio A. Lesion analysis in Neuropsychology, ed. O.U. Press, New York, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Rorden C, Karnath HO, Bonilha L. Improving lesion-symptom mapping. J Cogn Neurosci 2007;19(7):1081–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Mah YH, Husain M, Rees G et al. . Human brain lesion-deficit inference remapped. Brain 2014;137(9):2522–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Bates E, Wilson SM, Saygin AP et al. . Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nat Neurosci 2003;6(5):448–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RD Jr et al. . Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 2004;92(1-2):145–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Karnath HO, Ferber S, Himmelbach M. Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature 2001;411(6840):950–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Bird CM, Malhotra P, Parton A et al. . Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 2006;77(9):1008–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Husain M, Kennard C. Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction. J Neurol 1996;243(9):652–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK et al. . The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain 2003;126(9):1986–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Coulthard EJ, Nachev P, Husain M. Control over conflict during movement preparation: role of posterior parietal cortex. Neuron 2008;58(1):144–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Volle E, Kinkingnéhun S, Pochon JB et al. . The functional architecture of the left posterior and lateral prefrontal cortex in humans. Cereb Cortex 2008;18(10):2460–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Volle E, Levy R, Burgess PW. A new era for lesion-behavior mapping of prefrontal functions, in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, Stuss DT, Knight RT, Eds. 2013, p. 500–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Badre D, Hoffman J, Cooney JW et al. . Hierarchical cognitive control deficits following damage to the human frontal lobe. Nat Neurosci 2009;12(4):515–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Geschwind N. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man - Part I. Brain 1965;88(2):237–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Geschwind N. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man - Part II. Brain 1965;88(3):585–644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Carrera E, Tononi G. Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain 2014;137(9):2408–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Finger S, Koehler PJ, Jagella C. The Monakow concept of diaschisis. Arch Neurol 2004;61(2):283–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C et al. . Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nat Neurosci 2005;8(11):1603–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Tomaiuolo F, Aiello M et al. . Damage to white matter pathways in subacute and chronic spatial neglect: a group study and 2 single-case studies with complete virtual “in vivo” tractography dissection. Cereb Cortex 2014;24(3):691–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Kinkingnéhun S, Delmaire C et al. . Visualization of disconnection syndromes in humans. Cortex 2008;44(8):1097–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Urbanski M, Bréchemier ML, Garcin B et al. . Reasoning by analogy requires the left frontal pole: lesion-deficit mapping and clinical implications. Brain 2016;139(6):1783–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Cazzoli D, Hopfner S, Preisig B et al. . The influence of naturalistic, directionally non-specific motion on the spatial deployment of visual attention in right-hemispheric stroke. Neuropsychologia 2016;92:181–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Piai V, Meyer L, Dronkers NF et al. . Neuroplasticity of language in left-hemisphere stroke: evidence linking subsecond electrophysiology and structural connections. Hum Brain Mapp 2017;38(6):3151–3162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Rudrauf D, Mehta S, Grabowski TJ. Disconnection's renaissance takes shape: formal incorporation in group-level lesion studies. Cortex 2008;44(8):1084–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Fridriksson J, Dazhou G, Paul F et al. . Damage to the anterior arcuate fasciculus predicts non-fluent speech production in aphasia. Brain 2013;136(11):3451–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Hope TM, Seghier ML, Prejawa S et al. . Distinguishing the effect of lesion load from tract disconnection in the arcuate and uncinate fasciculi. Neuroimage 2016;125:1169–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Corbetta M, Ramsey L, Callejas A et al. . Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron 2015;85(5):927–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Griffis JC, Nenert R, Allendorfer JB et al. . Damage to white matter bottlenecks contributes to language impairments after left hemispheric stroke. NeuroImage: Clinical 2017;14:552–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. He BJ, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL et al. . Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 2007;53(6):905–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Turken AU, Dronkers NF. The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Front Syst Neurosci 2011;5:1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Bonilha L, Jensen JH, Baker N et al. . The brain connectome as a personalized biomarker of seizure outcomes after temporal lobectomy. Neurology 2015;84(18):1846–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Ratiu P et al. . From Phineas Gage and Monsieur Leborgne to H.M.: revisiting disconnection syndromes. Cereb Cortex 2015;25(12):4812–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Kuceyeski A, Navi BB, Kamel H et al. . Structural connectome disruption at baseline predicts 6-months post-stroke outcome. Hum Brain Mapp 2016;37(7):2587–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Yourganov G, Fridriksson J, Rorden C et al. . Multivariate connectome-based symptom mapping in post-stroke patients: networks supporting language and speech. J Neurosci 2016;36(25):6668–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Kuceyeski A, Maruta J, Relkin N et al. . The Network Modification (NeMo) Tool: elucidating the effect of white matter integrity changes on cortical and subcortical structural connectivity. Brain Connectivity 2013;3(5):451–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Boes AD, Prasad S, Liu H et al. . Network localization of neurological symptoms from focal brain lesions. Brain 2015;138(10):3061–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Munafo M. Metascience: reproducibility blues. Nature 2017;543(7647):619–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M. The connectomics of brain disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015;16(3):159–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Feeney DM, Baron JC. Diaschisis. Stroke 1986;17(5):817–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Cowan W. Contemporary Research Methods in Neuroanatomy. Springer, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Bredesen DE. Neural apoptosis. Ann Neurol 1995;38(6):839–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Capurso SA, Calhoun ME, Sukhov RR et al. . Deafferentation causes apoptosis in cortical sensory neurons in the adult rat. J Neurosci 1997;17(19):7372–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Schaechter JD, Moore CI, Connell BD et al. . Structural and functional plasticity in the somatosensory cortex of chronic stroke patients. Brain 2006;129(10):2722–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Xing S, Lacey EH, Skipper-Kallal LM et al. . Right hemisphere grey matter structure and language outcomes in chronic left hemisphere stroke. Brain 2016;139(1):227–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Gladsjo JA, Schuman CC, Evans JD et al. . Norms for letter and category fluency: demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment 1999;6(2):147–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. MacPherson SE, Della Sala S. Handbook of Frontal Lobe Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Jones DK, Griffin LD, Alexander DC et al. . Spatial normalization and averaging of diffusion tensor MRI data sets. Neuroimage 2002;17(2):592–617. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Brett M, Leff AP, Rorden C et al. . Spatial normalization of brain images with focal lesions using cost function masking. Neuroimage 2001;14(2):486–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Nachev P, Coulthard E, Jäger HR et al. . Enantiomorphic normalization of focally lesioned brains. Neuroimage 2008;39(3):1215–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk.
  • 56. Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith SM. segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 2001;201:45–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 2002;17(3):143–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G et al. . A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 2011;54(3):2033–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Rojkova K, Volle E, Urbanski M et al. . Atlasing the frontal lobe connections and their variability due to age and education: a spherical deconvolution tractography study. Brain Struct Funct 2016;221(3):1751–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Catani M, Dell’acqua F, Vergani F et al. . Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain. Cortex 2012;48(2):273–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Von Der Heide RJ, Skipper LM, Klobusicky E et al. . Dissecting the uncinate fasciculus: disorders, controversies and a hypothesis. Brain 2013;136(6):1692–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Duffau H, Gatignol P, Mandonnet E et al. . New insights into the anatomo-functional connectivity of the semantic system: a study using cortico-subcortical electrostimulations. Brain 2005;128(4):797–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Catani M, Jones DK, Ffytche DH. Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Ann Neurol 2005;57(1):8–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Catani M, Bambini V. A model for social communication and language evolution and development (SCALED). Curr Opin Neurobiol 2014;28:165–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Forkel SJ et al. . A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat Neurosci 2011;14(10):1245–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Trackvis, http://trackvis.org/.
  • 67. Ciccarelli O, Toosy AT, Parker GJ et al. . Diffusion tractography based group mapping of major white-matter pathways in the human brain. Neuroimage 2003;19(4):1545–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Ffytche DH, Bizzi A et al. . Atlasing location, asymmetry and inter-subject variability of white matter tracts in the human brain with MR diffusion tractography. Neuroimage 2011;54(1):49–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Kinkingnehun S, Volle E, Pélégrini-Issac M et al. . A novel approach to clinical-radiological correlations: Anatomo-Clinical Overlapping Maps (AnaCOM): Method and validation. Neuroimage 2007;37(4):1237–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. R Core Team 2016, https://www.r-project.org/.
  • 71. Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE et al. . Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods 2011;8(8):665–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Neurosynth, http://neurosynth.org/.
  • 73. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M et al. . Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 2002;17(2):825–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B et al. . Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 2009;45(1):S173–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Kenney J. Mathematics of Statistics. London: Chapman & Hall, 1939. [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Tononi G, Edelman GM, Sporns O. Complexity and coherency: integrating information in the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 1998;2(12):474–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Das SR, Avants BB, Grossman M et al. . Registration based cortical thickness measurement. Neuroimage 2009;45(3):867–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Tustison NJ, Cook PA, Klein A et al. . Large-scale evaluation of ANTs and FreeSurfer cortical thickness measurements. Neuroimage 2014;99:166–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Li Q, Pardoe H, Lichter R et al. . Cortical thickness estimation in longitudinal stroke studies: a comparison of 3 measurement methods. NeuroImage: Clinical 2015;8:526–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Shannon CE. The mathematical theory of communication. 1963; MD Comput, 1997;14(4):306–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Gray R. Entropy and Information Theory. Springer US, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Ogawa S, Lee TM, Kay AR et al. . Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation.. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1990;87(24):9868–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM et al. . Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 1995;34(4):537–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Hebb DO. The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York: Wiley and Sons, 1949. [Google Scholar]
  • 85. de Haan B, Karnath HO. ‘Whose atlas I use, his song I sing?’ - The impact of anatomical atlases on fiber tract contributions to cognitive deficits after stroke. Neuroimage 2017;163:301–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Sadaghiani S, D’Esposito M. Functional characterization of the cingulo-opercular network in the maintenance of tonic alertness. Cereb Cortex 2015;25(9):2763–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Voorn P, Vanderschuren LJ, Groenewegen HJ et al. . Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends Neurosci 2004;27(8):468–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL et al. . Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2009;106(31):13040–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Lichtheim L. On aphasia. Brain 1885;74:433–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Catani M, Ffytche DH. The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. Brain 2005;128(10):2224–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Dronkers NF, Plaisant O, Iba-Zizen MT et al. . Paul Broca's historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain 2007;130(5):1432–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Forkel SJ, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F et al. . Anatomical predictors of aphasia recovery: a tractography study of bilateral perisylvian language networks. Brain 2014;137(7):2027–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. MacPherson SE, Sala SD. Handbook of Frontal Lobe Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ et al. . Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2006;103(26):10046–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Parlatini V, Radua J, Dell’Acqua F et al. . Functional segregation and integration within fronto-parietal networks. Neuroimage 2017;146:367–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Power JD, Petersen SE. Control-related systems in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2013;23(2):223–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Gratton C, Neta M, Sun H et al. . Distinct stages of moment-to-moment processing in the cinguloopercular and frontoparietal networks. Cereb Cortex 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98. Chouiter L, Holmberg J, Manuel AL et al. . Partly segregated cortico-subcortical pathways support phonologic and semantic verbal fluency: A lesion study. Neuroscience 2016;329:275–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99. Bonnelle V, Ham TE, Leech R et al. . Salience network integrity predicts default mode network function after traumatic brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2012;109(12):4690–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Wagstyl K, Ronana L, Goodyer IM et al. . Cortical thickness gradients in structural hierarchies. Neuroimage 2015;111:241–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Saenger VM, Ponce-Alvarez A, Adhikari M et al. . Linking entropy at rest with the underlying structural connectivity in the healthy and lesioned brain. Cereb Cortex 2017;(in press)(2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102. Rorden C, Fridriksson J, Karnath HO. An evaluation of traditional and novel tools for lesion behavior mapping. Neuroimage 2009;44(4):1355–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103. Zhang Y, Kimberg DY, Coslett HB et al. . Multivariate lesion-symptom mapping using support vector regression. Hum Brain Mapp 2014;35(12):5861–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104. http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/ongoing/enigma-stroke-recovery/.
  • 105. Liew S-L, et al. A large, open source dataset of stroke anatomical brain images and manual lesion segmentations. 10.1101/1796142017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106. Open Database Brain Connectivity Behaviour Laboratory (BCBlab), http://opendata.bcblab.com/. [Google Scholar]
  • 107. Foulon C, Cerliani L, Kinkingnéhun S et al. . Supporting data for “advanced lesion symptom mapping analyses and implementation as BCBtoolkit”. GigaScience Database 2018; 10.5524/100399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

GIGA-D-17-00149_Original-Submission.pdf
GIGA-D-17-00149_Revision-1.pdf
GIGA-D-17-00149_Revision-2.pdf
Response-to-Reviewer-Comments_Original-Submission.pdf
Response-to-Reviewer-Comments_Revision-1.pdf
Reviewer-1-Report_Original-Submission -- Aaron Boes

07 Aug 2017 Reviewed

Reviewer-1-Report_Revision-1 -- Aaron Boes

28 Nov 2017 Reviewed

Reviewer-2-Report_Original_Submission -- Muthuraman Muthuraman

15 Aug 2017 Reviewed

Reviewer-2-Report_Revision-1 -- Muthuraman Muthuraman

29 Nov 2017 Reviewed

Supplement Materials

Articles from GigaScience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES