Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2018 Mar 22;12:56. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00056

Aggression, Social Stress, and the Immune System in Humans and Animal Models

Aki Takahashi 1,2,3,*, Meghan E Flanigan 2, Bruce S McEwen 3, Scott J Russo 2
PMCID: PMC5874490  PMID: 29623033

Abstract

Social stress can lead to the development of psychological problems ranging from exaggerated anxiety and depression to antisocial and violence-related behaviors. Increasing evidence suggests that the immune system is involved in responses to social stress in adulthood. For example, human studies show that individuals with high aggression traits display heightened inflammatory cytokine levels and dysregulated immune responses such as slower wound healing. Similar findings have been observed in patients with depression, and comorbidity of depression and aggression was correlated with stronger immune dysregulation. Therefore, dysregulation of the immune system may be one of the mediators of social stress that produces aggression and/or depression. Similar to humans, aggressive animals also show increased levels of several proinflammatory cytokines, however, unlike humans these animals are more protected from infectious organisms and have faster wound healing than animals with low aggression. On the other hand, subordinate animals that receive repeated social defeat stress have been shown to develop escalated and dysregulated immune responses such as glucocorticoid insensitivity in monocytes. In this review we synthesize the current evidence in humans, non-human primates, and rodents to show a role for the immune system in responses to social stress leading to psychiatric problems such as aggression or depression. We argue that while depression and aggression represent two fundamentally different behavioral and physiological responses to social stress, it is possible that some overlapped, as well as distinct, pattern of immune signaling may underlie both of them. We also argue the necessity of studying animal models of maladaptive aggression induced by social stress (i.e., social isolation) for understanding neuro-immune mechanism of aggression, which may be relevant to human aggression.

Keywords: aggression, social stress, immune system, depression, humans, animal models

Introduction: interplay between immune system and central nervous system

The immune system is the body's primary active defense against physical injury and pathogens. These insults cause activation of leukocytes that produce cytokines to promote multiple kinds of inflammatory responses. Cytokines are known to produce an array of sickness behaviors such as reductions in activity, food intake, and social interaction, along with increased sleep and anhedonia (Larson and Dunn, 2001; Dantzer et al., 2008). Psychological stress can trigger cytokine release, and growing evidence has shown an important role for the immune system in regulating negative emotional states as well as personality (Black, 2003; Zalcman and Siegel, 2006; Dantzer et al., 2008; Koolhaas, 2008; Maes et al., 2009; Réus et al., 2015).

To produce deleterious behavioral effects in response to stress, peripheral cytokines must enter and act upon brain circuitry controlling mood and emotion (Menard et al., 2017b). There are two main pathways for peripheral cytokines to affect the central nervous system (CNS): the neural pathway via vagus nerve and the humoral pathway via crossing of the blood brain barrier (for more detail, see review from Hodes et al., 2015; Pfau and Russo, 2015). Within the CNS, activated microglial cells, astrocytes, neurons, and endothelial cells have all been shown to produce several cytokines and express many cytokine receptors (Hopkins and Rothwell, 1995; Allan et al., 2005). Thus, brain cytokines have important roles beyond inflammatory processes and can act as neuromodulators to regulate neuronal transmission and plasticity. For example, one of the major proinflammatory cytokines, interleukin-1β (IL-1β), has been shown to increase metabolism of norepinephrine and serotonin (5-HT) (Dunn, 1992; Linthorst et al., 1994, 1995; Zalcman et al., 1994; Brebner et al., 2000; Anisman et al., 2008). In addition, IL-1β increases the production of corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) from the hypothalamus and therefore activates the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) stress axis (Berkenbosch et al., 1987; Linthorst et al., 1994, 1995; Angeli et al., 1999). IL-1 activates the nuclear factor kappa B (NF-κB) signaling pathway (Osborn et al., 1989), which is well-established to regulate synaptic plasticity (Schneider et al., 1998; Russo et al., 2009; Boersma et al., 2011; Christoffel et al., 2011).

The immune system can also be modulated by the CNS via top-down mechanisms involving nervous and endocrine systems. For example, stress or negative emotions such as anger activates both the HPA axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary (SAM) axis to induce the release of pituitary and adrenal hormones such as adrenocorticotropic hormone, glucocorticoids, prolactin, growth hormone, noradrenaline, and adrenaline. These hormones directly modulate the activity of many immune cells, which express a variety of hormone receptors (for review see Glaser and Kiecolt-Glaser, 2005). For example, glucocorticoids strongly suppress immune cells, and they are widely used in the treatment of inflammatory and autoimmune diseases (Boumpas et al., 1993). Glucocorticoids inhibit the production of pro-inflammatory cytokines by acting directly on glucocorticoid receptors on leukocytes (Lew et al., 1988; Angeli et al., 1999; Dhabhar and McEwen, 1999) (Box 1). The sympathetic nervous system, which regulates physical responses to fight/flight situations, also innervates the hematopoietic stem cell niche located within lymphoid organs and bone marrow to modulate leukocyte differentiation and release through a β-adrenergic receptor mechanism (Elenkov et al., 2000; Bierhaus et al., 2003; Tan et al., 2007) (Box 1).

Box 1. Biphasic effects of stress on immune response.

Experiences leading to activation of the autonomic nervous system and HPA axis, as well as other mediators, have biphasic effects on immune responses. One example of this is seen with delayed type hypersensitivity (DTH), an antigen-specific cell-mediated immune response, where acute stress enhances the DTH response while chronic stress has an immunosuppressive effect on the DTH response (Dhabhar and McEwen, 1999). Immune cells “move to their battle stations” and traffic via the circulation to places in the body where they can fight an infection or heal a wound (Dhabhar et al., 2012). Besides adrenaline and cortisol as mediators of this trafficking, IFN-γ, and chemokines are involved (Dhabhar et al., 2000, 2010). IFN-γ plays a role in short-term stress induced immune-enhancement of cell-mediated immunity (Dhabhar et al., 2000), the primary acquisition of immune memory (Dhabhar and Viswanathan, 2005), and in anti-tumor immunity (Dhabhar et al., 2010). However, the actions of IFN-γ may largely come into play after leukocytes have been trafficked to potential sites of immune activation by corticosterone, epinephrine, and norepinephrine (Dhabhar et al., 2012). Parasympathetic and sympathetic activation have synergistic and somewhat opposing effects on immune activation; while activation of the sympathetic response increases inflammatory cytokine production, activation of the parasympathetic response has anti-inflammatory effects (Borovikova et al., 2000; Bierhaus et al., 2003; Matteoli et al., 2014).

Aggression and the immune system in humans

Aggressive behavior in humans is complex in its expression (i.e., physical and verbal) as well as its causes (i.e., provoked emotion) and consequences on victims (i.e., trauma induced psychopathology) (Box 2). Several mental disorders, such as schizophrenia, psychosis, antisocial personality disorder, impulse control disorder, depression, attention deficit disorder, and autism spectrum disorders accompany escalated forms of aggression toward others (violence) or one's self (self-mutilation) (Connor et al., 2002; Raine et al., 2002; Volavka et al., 2005; Comai et al., 2012; Matson and Jang, 2014; Das et al., 2016).

Box 2. Aggression study in human and animal models.

Human

Human aggression can be largely categorized into reactive aggression (impulsive and hostile) and premediated (instrumental) aggression. Quantification of aggression in human is often conducted by psychometric inventories. Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI, Buss and Durkee, 1957) or its modified Buss and Perry Aggression Questionnaire (BPAQ, Buss and Perry, 1992) contains subscales for hostility (cognitive aspect of aggression including feelings of ill-will and injustice), anger (affective aspect of aggression including physiological arousal and preparation for aggression), and physical and verbal aggression (behavioral aspect of aggression including instrumental or motor components of behavior). The Cook-Medley Hostility (Ho) Scale is another commonly used questionnaire (Cook and Medley, 1954) that measures cognitive (cynicism and hostile attribute), affective (hostile affect), and behavioral (aggressive responding) aspects of aggression. Other studies use methods to provoke anger in experimental settings and quantify participants' aggressive responses toward fictitious competitors (for review, see Miczek et al., 2002).

Animal models

Aggressive behavior in animals is species-typical behavior and it differs depending upon the social system of the species, and includes factors such as territorial aggression, dominance-related aggression, and maternal aggression (Miczek and Fish, 2006). Moreover, aggressive behavior in animals has been traditionally separated into offensive vs. defensive forms of aggression. Offensive aggression is motived by resource control and threat to those resources, whereas defensive aggression is motived by danger of harm to the individual itself (Blanchard and Blanchard, 2006). In the rodent model, aggressive behavior is often quantified by using the resident-intruder test. This test is conducted in the home-cage of a resident male (or sometimes resident female or dam) where an intruder male is introduced. Latency to first attack behavior (often a bite) is measured and used as an index of readiness to initiate aggressive behavior. Animals with shorter attack latency are considered to have higher aggression. In addition, the overall frequency and duration of aggressive acts are measured. Importantly, while dominance is a trait often associated aggression, for the purpose of this review it is important to highlight some distinctions. Dominance-related aggression typically occurs more often in species with defined social hierarchies, such as non-human primates, and is measured by ethological observation to record animals' interactive behaviors with other members in their habitat (either captivity or the field). Importantly, social dominance does not simply reflect trait aggressiveness (Buwalda et al., 2017) but can result from other factors such as ability to mobilize support or anxiety in monkeys and rats (de Waal, 1998; van der Kooij et al., 2017).

Increasing evidence shows that there is an important relationship between aggression traits and the immune system. For example, it has been reported that immunotherapy to treat patients with hepatitis C by chronic administration of interferon alpha (IFN-α) increases irritability and anger/hostility in some patients (McHutchison et al., 1998; Kraus et al., 2003; Lotrich et al., 2013). Also, hostile marital relationships are associated with slower wound healing and dysregulated cytokine production at wound sites (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2005). Pathological levels of aggression in intermittent explosive disorder (IED) or psychosis appear to be linked to heightened proinflammatory cytokines (Coccaro et al., 2014; Das et al., 2016). A similar relationship is observed in healthy individuals with high aggression as a personality trait. As summarized in Table 1, these individuals consistently showed higher circulating cytokines such as interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α), as well as C-reactive protein (CRP) than non-aggressive individuals (Box 3). However, these correlations varied depending on the subtype of aggression scale used. In one recent study, it was shown that the behavioral, but not cognitive nor affective, subscale of hostility positively correlated with IL-6 and CRP (Marsland et al., 2008). Therefore, elevations in basal levels of circulating proinflammatory cytokines, i.e., IL-6 or CRP, appear to be related to the behavioral aspect of aggression traits. Whether this relationship is direct or indirect must be addressed using animal models.

Table 1.

Relationship between aggression trait or state and immune responses in human studies.

Condition Subjects N Age Sample Stimulation Scale* Cytokines Relationship** Reference
Basal Intermittent Explosive Disorder (IED) males and females 197 32–35.8 Plasma LHA CRP +
(r = 0.41, p < 0.001)
Coccaro et al., 2014
IL-6 +
(r = 0.36, p < 0.001)
Basal Healthy males and females 855 20–54 Plasma Ho, BPAQ (Behavioral hostility) IL-6 +
(β = 0.09, p = 0.04)
Marsland et al., 2008
CRP +
(β = 0.08, p = 0.04)
Basal Healthy males and females 6814 45–84 Serum Ho CRP +
Ranjit et al., 2007
IL-6 +
fibrinogen +
Basal Older males and females (caregiver) 214 69.1 Plasma Ho CRP +
(β = 0.18, p < 0.05)
Graham et al., 2006
IL-6 No
Basal Healthy non-smoking males 62 18–50 Monocyte LPS BPAQ TNF-α +
(r = 0.35, p = 0.007)
Suarez et al., 2002
Basal Healthy females 44 23–49 Monocyte LPS Ho IL-1α +
(β = 0.03, p = 0.02)
Suarez et al., 2004
IL-1β +
(β = 0.02, p = 0.06)
IL-8 +
(β = 0.05, p = 0.01)
Basal Healthy males (with military records) 304 30.7 T cells, B cells, and NK-cells T-cell mitogen Ho IL-6, MCP-1 etc (β = −0.29), p < 0.001 Mommersteeg et al., 2008
IL-4, IL-5, IL-10 +
(β = 0.20, p < 0.01)
IL-2, TNF-α, IFN-γ +
(β = 0.24, p < 0.001)
Basal Healthy males (with military records) 4415 30–48 Whole blood Aggressive behavior scale (DSM-III antisocial personality disorder) T cell (CD4) number +
(β = 0.17, p < 0.001)
Granger et al., 2000
T cell (CD8) number No
B cell number +
(β = 0.20, p < 0.001)
Basal Healthy males and females 38 32.5 CSF Composite aggression score (LHA +
BDHI)
IL-6 No Coccaro et al., 2015
Soluble IL-1 receptor II +
(r = 0.35, p = 0.03)
State (2 h before rugby match) Male rugby athletes 20 27.2 Serum State-Trait Anger expression Inventory-2 IL-1β +
(β = 0.60–0.72, p < 0.05)
Pesce et al., 2013
State (Conflict/social support session) Healthy marital couples 42 22–77 Plasma The Rapid Marital Interaction Coding System IL-6 +
Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2005
TNF-α +
State (Anger recall interview) Healthy non-smoking males 58 18–65 Plasma LPS Total negative score IL-1β No Suarez et al., 2006
IL-6 +
(males with high insulin resistance)
TNF-α
(males with low insulin resistance)
State (Stressful event self-discloser) Male college undergraduate students 43 20 NK cell activity Ho NK cytotoxicity +
(males with high hostility)
Christensen et al., 1996
State (Discussion of marital problem) Married couples 41 30.9–31.9 NK cell activity Ho NK cytotoxicity +
(r = 0.71, p < 0.01 in high hostile males)

(r = −0.63, p < 0.01 in low hostile males)
Miller et al., 1999
*

LHA, life history of aggression; Ho, Cook-Medley Hostility Sale; BPAQ, Buss and Perry Aggression Questionnaire; BDHI, Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory. Total negative score includes "anxious, depressed, upset, tense, angry, frustrated, agitated, nervous, irritated, and sad.”

**

Significant relationship [either positive (+) or negative (–) direction] between aggression scores and cytokines. r, correlation coefficients; β, beta coefficients from the regression analysis; B, unstandardized regression coefficient from the Structural equation modeling (SEM). No statistic values were indicated if the study used other types of analysis.

Box 3. IL-1, IL-6, CRP, TNF-α.

IL-1 (interleukin-1) is a potent pro-inflammatory cytokine first identified as an endogenous pyrogen due to its ability to affect the hypothalamic thermoregulatory center. Currently, there are 11 cytokines in the IL-1 super family (for review, see Allan et al., 2005). Two major subtypes of IL-1 ligands, IL-1α and IL-1β, bind to IL-1 receptors (IL-1R) to activate intracellular cascades such as NF-κB and mitogen-activated protein kinases (MAPKs), and trigger the transcription of multiple inflammation-associated genes including IL-6 and TNF-α. There is also a ligand known as IL-1RA that antagonizes IL-1R to inhibit downstream signaling. Many types of cells in both the peripheral and central immune system produce IL-1 and express IL-1 receptors, including leukocytes, endothelial cells, adipocytes, fibroblasts, neurons, and glial cells.

IL-6 (interleukin-6) is a cytokine that can exhibit either anti-inflammatory or pro-inflammatory properties depending on whether the IL-6 receptor and glycoprotein 130 (gp130) signal transducer are soluble or membrane bound. As is the case with IL-1, IL-6 is produced in many cell types. It was originally identified as B-cell differentiation factor, but it also has a variety of additional functions outside of B cells such as production of acute-phase proteins from liver, angiogenesis, T-cell differentiation, bone metabolism, and neuronal growth (for review, see Hodes et al., 2016).

CRP (C-reactive protein) is one of the acute-phase proteins from the liver activated in by pro-inflammatory cytokines as early a response to inflammation. CRP acts as a pattern recognition molecule that binds to the surface of several microbes and dead cells, and it has been used as a sensitive but non-specific marker of inflammation and infection (Pepys and Hirschfield, 2003).

TNF-α (tumor necrosis factor alpha) is a pro-inflammatory cytokine that was originally identified as a cytotoxic factor produced by lymphocytes and macrophages. More recently TNF-α has been shown to trigger the induction of an array of pro-inflammatory cytokines to regulate cell proliferation, differentiation, and cell death (Aggarwal et al., 2012).

High states of anger (acute episodes of anger) also induce proinflammatory cytokine release. Marital couples show increases in plasma IL-6 and TNF-α after conflict interactions compared to after supportive interactions, and these increases in cytokines were larger in couples who showed higher hostile behaviors during their conflict interactions (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2005). Interestingly, the expectation of an aggressive encounter can also increase circulating proinflamatory cytokine levels as well. Rugby athletes displayed increased IL-1β levels in their blood 2 h before a match, when state anger is high, compared to basal levels (Pesce et al., 2013). Furthermore, IL-1β levels 2 h before the match were positively correlated to anger score. Thus, both an aggressive experience and an expectation of an aggressive event are accompanied by state-related increases in inflammatory cytokines. It is possible that the readiness to provoke anger and aggressive behavior depends on these individual differences in cytokine production. But also, since noradrenaline triggers activation of monocytes to produce inflammatory cytokines (Bierhaus et al., 2003), sympathetic activation by aggressive events or their expectation might be the cause of this increased cytokine production.

Immune responses are largely divided into two categories: a rapid general immune response (innate immunity) and an acquired delayed immune response (adaptive immunity). Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) is an endotoxin that composes the outer surface membrane of gram-negative bacteria, acts as a pathogen-associated molecular pattern (PAMPs), and stimulates the innate immune response. Studies examining the role of the innate immune response on aggression show that there is a positive correlation between LPS-stimulated monocyte TNF-α expression and aggression (in hostility and behavioral, but not anger, subscales) in healthy males (Suarez et al., 2002). Similarly, monocytes isolated from females with high hostility released more IL-1 and IL-8 than those isolated from low hostility females after LPS-stimulation (Suarez et al., 2004). There have also been reports of increased natural killer (NK) cell cytotoxicity in highly hostile individuals (Christensen et al., 1996; Miller et al., 1999). Thus, individuals with high aggression traits tend to have high innate immune responses, though it is still unclear whether these are causally linked to aggression.

Only a few studies have examined the adaptive immune system in anger/hostility. Of note, one study found that there was a significant positive correlation between the frequency of T and B lymphocyte numbers and past aggressive acts; however, this relationship was only clear in individuals with moderate aggression, but not in highly aggressive individuals (Granger et al., 2000). Another study found that hostility is positively correlated with the release of both pro-inflammatory (TNF-α and IL-2) and anti-inflammatory (IL-4 and IL-10) cytokines from isolated T-cells (Mommersteeg et al., 2008). T-cell driven IL-6, however, was negatively correlated with hostility in the aforementioned study, which opposes the results observed in studies where cytokines were measured from whole serum/plasma or directly in monocytes. This discrepancy suggests that the relationship between aggression traits and inflammatory response is different depending on the leukocyte cell types studied.

Studies measuring cytokines from cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) contrast with those measuring them from peripheral blood. There is no observed correlation between IL-6 levels in the CSF and aggression (Coccaro et al., 2015). Instead, there is a positive correlation between levels of soluble IL-1 receptor II (sIL-1R2) in the CSF and aggression history (Coccaro et al., 2015). IL-1R2 and its soluble form sIL-1R2 act as decoy receptors for IL-1 and inhibit IL-1 mediated signal transduction (Allan et al., 2005). The sIL-1R2 binds to IL-1β with high affinity, and thus the level of IL-1R2 was used as an indirect measurement of IL-1β in the CNS in their study.

These studies in humans highlight the correlational relationship between aggression and the immune system. In the later section, we discuss findings from animal models where more causal relationships between the immune system and aggressive behaviors are beginning to be examined.

Link between aggression, depression, and the immune system in human studies

Similar to the findings from aggression studies, increased circulating IL-6 has been observed in humans suffering from major depression (Maes et al., 1997; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2003; Hodes et al., 2014; Kiraly et al., 2017). Thus, increased circulating IL-6 seems to be one of the important endophenotypes in depressive-like behaviors as well. Although aggression (violence) and depression are phenotypically very different behavioral outputs, both aggression and depression are triggered by social stress. In fact, suicidal behavior, the most problematic consequence of depression, can be considered as a form of escalated aggression toward the self, and a high comorbidity of suicide and aggression has been observed in human patients (McCloskey and Ammerman, 2018). Thus, it is possible that aggression and depression share certain biological mechanisms.

Repeated immunotherapy to treat patients with acquired immune deficiency syndrome, autoimmune disease, or hepatitis C increases depression as well as anger/hostility (McHutchison et al., 1998; Kraus et al., 2003). In addition, epidemiological evidence has shown that individuals with psychological traits of either depression or hostility have a greater risk of developing coronary heart disease (CHD), which is a known inflammatory disease (Rozanski et al., 1999; Smith and Ruiz, 2002; Betensky and Contrada, 2010). Both male and female individuals with high hostility displayed higher plasma IL-6 levels than non-hostile controls only when they concurrently suffered high depression symptoms (Suarez, 2003). This positive correlation between hostility and IL-6 levels was absent among individuals with low depression symptoms. A similar pattern was observed in another study, where the authors found a significant interaction between hostility and depression symptoms for serum IL-6 and CRP (Stewart et al., 2008). A much larger study of both males and females confirmed a positive correlation between hostility and CRP in highly depressed individuals, but also found no relationship between these measures in people with low levels of depression (Brummett et al., 2010). In contrast, other studies observed a positive correlation between hostility and IL-6 or TNF-α only in individuals with low levels of depression (Miller et al., 2003). This study reported that all individuals with high levels of depression showed high levels of IL-6 regardless of hostility, and thus the relationship between hostility and circulating cytokine levels was not observed. In other experimental designs, the relationship between hostility and cytokines remained even after correcting for depression phenotypes (Marsland et al., 2008). Given the inconsistent results from these correlational studies in humans, far more work is needed to elucidate whether disruptions within the immune system are a common endophenotype for both depression and anger/hostility traits in humans.

Animal models of aggression and the immune system

In contrast to human aggression research, which mainly focuses on examining anger and hostility as negative emotional states with pathological aspects that could be the matter of clinical concern, aggression research in animals must consider its ethological and evolutionary importance. Aggression has an adaptive significance for most animal species and is critical for acquiring and protecting territory, food, reproductive mates, and offspring. In animals with hierarchical societies, aggressive behavior is thought to help individuals gain and maintain higher social status (Box 2). It has been shown that aggressive behavior, especially the experience of winning, has rewarding properties in animals and repeated aggressive experience may lead to compulsive, pathological aggression that is highly reinforcing (Fish et al., 2002; Falkner et al., 2016; Golden et al., 2016, 2017). Since aggressive behavior poses a strong risk of injury, it is reasonable to assume that animals with high levels of aggression would have stronger immune responses in order to actively recover from injury and to protect themselves from infection.

As with humans, differences in peripheral immune function have been observed between high and low aggressive non-human primates and rodents. For example, baboons with higher hierarchical status within the group showed faster wound healing than subordinate individuals in the wild (Archie et al., 2012). Male cynomolgus monkeys with the lowest social status had rates of infection by adenovirus five times greater than monkeys with higher social rank (Cohen et al., 1997). Also, more aggressive cynomolgus monkeys had higher lymphocyte numbers than less aggressive monkeys when they were infected by herpes B virus (Line et al., 1996). In mice, female BALB/c mice with high aggressive behavior were less vulnerable to tumor induction by murine sarcoma virus than low aggression BALB/c females (Amkraut and Solomon, 1972). In agreement with this, highly aggressive C57BL/6 and CBA male mice show stronger immune response (increase in plaque- and rosette-forming cell numbers) toward immunization with protein antigen than submissive males (Devoino et al., 1993). There was also a positive correlation between aggression traits and experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis (EAE) response in a wild rat population whereby aggressive male rats were more susceptible to experimentally-induced autoimmune disease (Kavelaars et al., 1999), suggesting that aggressive individuals have highly activated immune systems (Table 2). These data are in some conflict with data obtained from humans, as highly aggressive humans tend to have higher concentrations of proinflammatory cytokines and slower wound healing than less aggressive humans (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2005). It is possible that activation of the immune system is adaptive in aggressive or dominant individuals but can become maladaptive in extreme cases of pathological aggression, which make up most of the cases in human studies. This hypothesis needs further testing.

Table 2.

Relationship between aggression and peripheral immune responses in rodent models.

Condition Animal model Strain Sex Stimulation Sample Cytokines etc Effect Reference
Basal Aggression selection line NC900 and NC100 mouse (Originated from ICR) Male 3-methylcholanthrene Animal Number of animals with tumors NC900 < NC100 Petitto et al., 1993
Splenocytes NK cytotoxicity NC900 > NC100
Basal Aggression selection line NC900 and NC100 mouse (Originated from ICR) Male T cell mitogen concanavalin A (ConA) Splenocytes T cell proliferation NC900 > NC100 Petitto et al., 1994
Splenocytes IL-2 NC900 > NC100
Splenocytes IFN-γ NC900 > NC100
LPS Splenocytes B cell proliferation
Basal Spontaneous fighting female and non-fighting female BALB/c mouse Female Murine sarcoma virus Animal Tumor size Fighter < Non fighter Amkraut and Solomon, 1972
Basal Resident-intruder test Wildtype rat Male Experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis (EAE) Animal Disease score Short attack latency > Long attack latency Kavelaars et al., 1999
3 days confrontations Aggressive and submissive males in resident-intruder test C57BL/6 mouse Male B16F10 melanoma injection Animal Number of metastasis in the lung Aggressive < Submissive Sá-Rocha et al., 2006
Staphylococcus aureus Blood neutrophils Basal oxidative burst Aggressive > Submissive
Blood monocytes Basal oxidative burst Aggressive > Submissive
Blood lymphocytes NK cell cytotoxicity Aggressive > Submissive
Spleen lymphocytes NK cell cytotoxicity Aggressive > Submissive
10 days confrontations Sensory contact CBA/Lac mouse Male Immunization with Sheep red blood cells Splenocytes Plaque forming cells Aggressive > control = Submissive Devoino et al., 1993
Rosette-forming cells Aggressive > control = Submissive
C57BL/6J mouse Plaque forming cells Aggressive = control > Submissive
Rosette-forming cells Aggressive = control > Submissive
10 and 20 days confrontations Sensory contact C57B/6J mouse Male Splenocytes IL-2 20 day > 10 day aggression > control Idova et al., 2016
TNF-α
ConA IL-2 20 day = 10 day aggression > control
LPS TNF-α 20 day = 10 day aggression < control
20 days confrontations Aggressive and submissive males in 15 min confrontations C57BL/6J mouse Male Serum IL-1a Stewart et al., 2015
IL-2
IL-4
IL-6 Aggressive = control (<) Submissive
IL-7 Aggressive = control < Submissive
IL-10 Aggressive > control = Submissive
IL-15 Aggressive = control < Submissive
MCP-1
VEGF Aggressive = control < Submissive

Individual differences in peripheral immune responses are also reported in forward genetic models of aggression in which animals are selectively bred for aggressive behavior. High aggression NC900 mice and low aggression NC100 mice have been selected over generations from the ICR outbred founder population (Cairns et al., 1983; Gariepy et al., 1996). Interestingly, the NC900 line showed reduced vulnerability to tumor development after calcinogen treatment than the low aggression NC100 line (Petitto et al., 1993). Furthermore, splenic NK cytotoxic activity was also higher in NC900 mice, and exposure to a T cell mitogen caused greater splenic T cell proliferation and increased production of proinflammatory cytokines IL-2 and intereferon-gamma (IFN-γ) in NC900 mice (Petitto et al., 1993, 1994). Thus, aggressive NC900 mice have stronger NK cell and T cell immune responses than non-aggressive NC100 mice. These differences were observed without having any aggressive experience, suggesting that they may reflect trait-like immunity differences.

Studies in constitutive gene knockout mice support the involvement of the immune system in aggressive behaviors. Deletion of TNF receptors, TNF-R1 and TNF-R2, reduced the duration of aggressive behaviors in the resident-intruder test in male mice (Patel et al., 2010). This is in line with findings from human studies in which TNF-α is increased in highly aggressive individuals (Table 1). On the other hand, IL-6 knockout mice showed shorter attack latency and increased frequency of aggressive behaviors in the resident-intruder test (Alleva et al., 1998). This same study showed that overexpression of IL-6 had no effect on inter-male aggression, but increased non-agonistic social interaction behaviors such as anogenital sniffing. Although interpretation of the results from these knockout mice is complex because of possible compensatory changes in other cytokines throughout the developmental period, these results strongly indicate functional involvement of the immune system in aggressive behaviors.

Although there are relatively few studies that have investigated the role of brain cytokine signaling in aggression, a handful of studies performed in a cat defensive rage model suggest that it functionally promotes defensive aggression (Zalcman and Siegel, 2006). In this model, electrical activation of either the periaqueductal gray (PAG) or medial preoptic area/hypothalamus causes cats to express a range of defensive aggressive behaviors in response to threat such as hissing, pupillary dilatation, retraction of the ear, as well as increases in blood pressure and heart rate (Siegel et al., 1999). It has been shown that IL-1β, IL-2, and their receptors are localized in a variety brain regions including the PAG and the medial hypothalamus (Bhatt et al., 2005; Hassanain et al., 2005). The local administration of IL-1β into the medial hypothalamus caused an enhancement of the defensive rage response (reduction of attack latency) after PAG stimulation (Hassanain et al., 2003). This pro-aggressive effect of IL-1β injection into the medial hypothalamus was blocked by the 5-HT2 receptor antagonist LY-53857. Importantly, strong co-localization of 5-HT2C receptors and IL-1 type 1 receptors (IL-1RI) in the medial hypothalamus may reflect the fact that IL-1β and 5-HT are activating the same population of neurons in this region to enhance defensive rage responses (Hassanain et al., 2005). In contrast to IL-1β, microinjection of IL-2 into the medial hypothalamus suppressed defensive rage behavior (Bhatt et al., 2005). The suppressive effect of IL-2 was blocked by pretreatment with a GABAA receptor antagonist into the medial hypothalamus, suggesting that the effect of IL-2 in the medial hypothalamus is mediated through GABAA receptors (Bhatt et al., 2005). However, IL-2 also facilitated defensive rage behavior when it was microinjected into the PAG (Bhatt and Siegel, 2006), and thus the function of cytokines for defensive rage depends heavily on the brain area in which it is expressed. Also, the function of cytokines might be different depending upon the type of aggression. In mouse territorial aggression models using a resident-intruder test, it has been shown that systemic injection of IL-1β causes a strong increase in attack latency concomitant with a reduction in the total duration of aggressive behaviors (Cirulli et al., 1998), indicating that systemic IL-1β has a suppressive effect on intermale offensive aggression. However, this study used only systemic treatment of IL-1β and its effect in the brain has to be studied in offensive aggression.

In summary, both peripheral cytokines and cytokines in the brain have important modulatory roles in both offensive and defensive aggression. Further studies to examine the complex neural circuitry in which cytokines act to affect aggressive behavior will be necessary to understand the extent of neuro-immune interactions in aggression.

Animal model of depression and the immune system

A large body of evidence shows that both chronic and repeated exposure to social defeat stress, which leads to depression-like behaviors in stress susceptible individuals, has a significant effect on the immune system. Currently, we understand far more about the detailed biological mechanisms underlying the link between social defeat stress and immune activation than we do between aggression and immune activation (for reviews, see Hodes et al., 2015; Pfau and Russo, 2015; Ménard et al., 2017a; Weber et al., 2017). Chronic social defeat stress has been used as an animal model of depression with high ethological and face validity (Miczek et al., 2008; Golden et al., 2011). Disruption of established social hierarchy in the home cage by repeated intrusions of a large dominant male has been shown to cause intensive stress and increases circulating corticosterone in male C57BL/6 mice (Avitsur et al., 2001, 2002). Despite the known immuno-suppressive effect of corticosterone, animals who underwent this social disruption procedure displayed an increased number of splenic monocytes and elevations in IL-6, IL-1β, and TNF-α release after endotoxin LPS stimulation compared to unstressed controls (Stark et al., 2001; Avitsur et al., 2003, 2005; Bailey et al., 2009). Interestingly, these studies also found that social stress caused glucocorticoid receptor (GR) desensitization in splenic monocytes, making them insensitive to inhibition by glucocorticoids and further exacerbating the pro-inflammatory effects of stress (Stark et al., 2001; Avitsur et al., 2002; Jung et al., 2015). Furthermore, monocytes expressing GR lost the ability to efficiently translocate GR into the nucleus, and thus were unable to suppress NF-κB activity (Quan et al., 2003). These changes in the properties of spleen monocytes in socially disrupted animals were mediated by increased norepinephrine and epinephrine release in the blood and spleen (Hanke et al., 2012). These results suggest that repeated social stress results in abnormal activation of the immune system through a loss of negative feedback signaling via corticosterone.

The peripheral immune system has also been implicated in determining individual vulnerability to social stress. For example, Hodes et al. (2014) examined the levels of circulating cytokines in susceptible and resilient C57BL/6 male mice after 10 days of repeated social defeat stress. Susceptible animals that developed social avoidance after repeated defeat experiences, exhibited higher IL-6 in their serum compared to stress-resilient mice as well as non-defeated control mice. Transplantation of bone marrow from susceptible donor males into host control males caused an increased social aversion following acute social stress, indicating that leukocytes are at least partly responsible for stress susceptibility. Interestingly, there was a preexisting difference in leukocytes between susceptible and resilient males such that susceptible animals had more circulating leukocytes and produced more IL-6 after LPS stimulation than resilient animals (Hodes et al., 2014). Increased peripheral IL-6 levels were also observed in susceptible female mice in a newly developed female social defeat stress model, indicating that IL-6 is a common mechanism mediating social stress susceptibility among the sexes (Takahashi et al., 2017). In addition to exhibiting differences in the immune response to stress, we also show that permeability of the blood brain barrier is different between resilient and susceptible male mice (Menard et al., 2017b). Stress susceptible mice display damaged and leaky blood vessels in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) due to loss of Claudin 5 expression, a molecule that helps form tight junctions between endothelial cells that make up the blood brain barrier (BBB). This impairment in BBB permeability allows more blood IL-6 to enter the brain and promotes depression-like behaviors following social defeat. Future work is needed to understand at the molecular level how social stress primes the neuro-immune axis to open the BBB and promotes a permissive environment for immune-CNS interactions.

Biological mechanisms linking aggression, depression, and immune function

Broadly, the studies described above support the notion that both aggressive and socially-defeated animals display increased production of peripheral cytokines. However, mounting evidence suggests that this simple view does not capture the complex dynamics of cytokine levels in socially stressed animals. For example, repeated aggressive encounters over 20 days caused increases of serum proinflammatory cytokines IL-6, IL-7, and IL-15 in loser, but not winner, male C57BL/7 mice (Stewart et al., 2015). Importantly, there was an increase in the anti-inflammatory cytokine IL-10 only in winner males. Further studies are required to understand whether these changes in cytokines observed in winners and losers represent common responses to social stress, or if there are specific immune responses that differ between winners and losers (such as specific activation of anti-inflammatory IL-10 in winners) that function to alter subsequent behavioral outputs. One hypothesis is that dominant individuals have well-balanced activation of both pro-inflammatory and anti-inflammatory cytokines to actively cope with injury or infections caused by aggressive interactions. In contrast, the experience of repeated social defeat stress, which causes a depression-like phenotype, may induce dysregulation of the immune system leading to a pro-inflammatory state of the animal.

The stress hormone corticosterone increases in both dominant and subordinate animals during an aggressive encounter (Covington and Miczek, 2005), but only subordinate animals show over-activation of HPA axis and long-term hypercortisolism after continuous subordination (Ely and Henry, 1978; Sapolsky, 1989). For example, dominant baboons showed normal activation of cortisol secretion by corticosterone-releasing factor (CRF) and suppression by glucocorticoid negative feedback. By contrast, continuously subordinated baboons displayed hypercortisolism, a blunted response to CRF, and resistance to glucocorticoid-induced negative immune feedback (Sapolsky, 1990). Similarly, in human air traffic controllers with high competence and satisfaction showed a positive correlation between plasma cortisol and amount of workload (number of airplanes under their control), but individuals with low competence showed blunted or disregulated cortisol response (Rose et al., 1982). However, recent work suggests that both the stability of the hierarchy as well as the species under investigation influences findings of whether subordinate status is associated with the highest rate of physical and psychological stressors (Abbott et al., 2003). The immune system also responds to agonistic encounters (acute social stress) in both aggressive dominant and submissive defeated individuals. However, as we have discussed in former sections, dominant animals mount strong adaptive immune responses that protect them from infection or enhance their recovery from injury, while defeated animals develop more prolonged dysregulation of the immune system that leads to pathological physiology and behavioral phenotypes. While causal data linking corticosterone to immune-mediated aggression is not yet available, it is one of the key mediators of brain-immune interactions (Spencer et al., 1991; Dhabhar and McEwen, 1999; Marques-Deak et al., 2005; also see Box 1) making it an attractive candidate for further study.

5-HT is one of the most well-studied neurotransmitters involved in the pathophysiology of both depression and escalated aggression (Olivier, 2015; Manchia et al., 2017). Serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), the most widely used pharmacological treatment for depressed patients, suppress escalated forms of aggression in rodent models (Pinna et al., 2003; Caldwell and Miczek, 2008; Mikics et al., 2017) and owner-directed aggression in dogs (Dodman et al., 1996). It has been shown in a variety of studies that peripheral cytokines are capable of altering central 5-HT neurotransmission. For example, endotoxin LPS administration activated catecholamine metabolism and increased tryptophan in the brain of mice and rats (Mefford and Heyes, 1990; Dunn, 1992; Linthorst et al., 1995). Furthermore, peripheral IL-1β administration increased levels of 5-HT or its metabolite 5-HTIAA in several brain areas and increased 5-HT1B and 5-HT2C receptor expression in the hippocampus (Gemma et al., 1997; Connor et al., 1998; Dunn, 2006; Anisman et al., 2008), whereas IL-1 receptor antagonists reduced extracellular 5-HT in the hypothalamus (El-Haj et al., 2002). Conversely, in the periphery, 5-HT has immunoregulatory functions via its actions on 5-HT receptors expressed on immune cells (Mössner and Lesch, 1998; Herr et al., 2017). It has been shown that antidepressants, such as imipramine and fluoxetine, have anti-inflammatory effects and reduce the production of the pro-inflammatory cytokine IFN-γ while increasing production of the anti-inflammatory cytokine IL-10 (Maes et al., 1999; Kubera et al., 2001; Ramirez et al., 2015; Köhler et al., 2017). However, these findings need to be interpreted with caution, given that other studies have reported that SSRI treatment increases several pro-inflammatory cytokines in the brain, and that this elevation is necessary to produce the anti-depressant effects of SSRIs (Warner-Schmidt et al., 2011).

In most species, the expression of aggression is generally not pathological but adaptive. Therefore, the link between immune function, aggression, and depression that is observed in humans cannot be simply studied in ethologically relevant animal models of aggression. Rather, it will be appropriate for future studies to utilize animal models of escalated or pathological aggression when investigating the specific role of the immune system in aggression that is relevant to human psychiatric disease. For example, social isolation stress has been shown to induce escalated aggression as well as depressive-like behaviors in rodents (Haller et al., 2014). There are other animal models of escalated aggression considered to be relevant to the study of human aggression (Miczek et al., 2013). These animal models should be used in future studies aimed at understanding the immunobiology of aggression and its relation to depression.

Conclusion and future perspectives

Findings from studies using animal models of social stress have uncovered numerous neurobiological mechanisms of immune—brain interactions that have opened up new avenues for anti-depressant drug discovery. In contrast, studies to define the neurobiological mechanism of immune—brain interactions in pathological aggression have lagged far behind, which some have argued reflects the relative paucity in novel drug targets for the treatment of psychiatric conditions with high aggression. Thus, we need a much better understanding of the neural circuits affected by the immune system involved in aggressive behavior. The use of animal models of escalated pathological aggression will help us understand relevant immunobiological mechanisms driving aggressive behavior and provide insight into the possible causes of pathological human aggression. Additionally, it is important to better define how the immune system interfaces with brain circuitry to control aggression. Future studies will have to consider whether individual differences in the immune system are causally linked with aggression, or whether the differences in immune function are simply a consequence of differing amounts of aggressive behaviors.

We currently have a very limited understanding of the neuro-immune interactions mediating aggression in females. In human studies, positive correlations between aggression and peripheral cytokine levels are observed in both males and females, yet rodent studies have not tested causal immunological mechanisms of aggression in females. Future studies will be required to clarify the degree of overlap in the immunological mechanisms that mediate male and female aggression. Given that that there are significant basal sex differences in the immune system (Klein and Flanagan, 2016), it is hypothesized that the immunological mechanisms driving aggression in males and females are different and highly dependent upon gonadal hormone status.

Author contributions

AT, MF, BM, and SR discussed about the content of this review and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. This research was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 17H04766 and 15K12773 (AT), US National Institutes of Mental Health (NIMH) grants T32 MH096678, F31 MH111108-01 (MF), R01 MH114882, R01 MH104559, and R01 MH090264 (SR), and National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health grants P50 MH096890 and P50 AT008661 (SR).

References

  1. Abbott D. H., Keverne E. B., Bercovitch F. B., Shively C. A., Mendoza S. P., Saltzman W., et al. (2003). Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Horm. Behav. 43, 67–82. 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00037-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aggarwal B. B., Gupta S. C., Kim J. H. (2012). Historical perspectives on tumor necrosis factor and its superfamily: 25 years later, a golden journey. Blood 119, 651–665. 10.1182/blood-2011-04-325225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allan S. M., Tyrrell P. J., Rothwell N. J. (2005). Interleukin-1 and neuronal injury. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 5, 629–640. 10.1038/nri1664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alleva E., Cirulli F., Bianchi M., Bondiolotti G. P., Chiarotti F., De Acetis L., et al. (1998). Behavioural characterization of interleukin-6 overexpressing or deficient mice during agonistic encounters. Eur. J. Neurosci. 10, 3664–3672. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00377.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amkraut A., Solomon G. F. (1972). Stress and murine sarcoma virus (Moloney)-induced tumors. Cancer Res. 32, 1428–1433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Angeli A., Masera R. G., Sartori M. L., Fortunati N., Racca S., Dovio A., et al. (1999). Modulation by cytokines of glucocorticoid action. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 876, 210–220. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb07641.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Anisman H., Gibb J., Hayley S. (2008). Influence of continuous infusion of interleukin-1β on depression-related processes in mice: corticosterone, circulating cytokines, brain monoamines, and cytokine mRNA expression. Psychopharmacology 199, 231–244. 10.1007/s00213-008-1166-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Archie E. A., Altmann J., Alberts S. C. (2012). Social status predicts wound healing in wild baboons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 9017–9022. 10.1073/pnas.1206391109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Avitsur R., Kavelaars A., Heijnen C., Sheridan J. F. (2005). Social stress and the regulation of tumor necrosis factor-α secretion. Brain Behav. Immun. 19, 311–317. 10.1016/j.bbi.2004.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Avitsur R., Padgett D. A., Dhabhar F. S., Stark J. L., Kramer K. A., Engler H., et al. (2003). Expression of glucocorticoid resistance following social stress requires a second signal. J. Leukoc. Biol. 74, 507–513. 10.1189/jlb.0303090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Avitsur R., Stark J. L., Sheridan J. F. (2001). Social stress induces glucocorticoid resistance in subordinate animals. Horm. Behav. 39, 247–257. 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Avitsur R., Stark J. L., Dhabhar F. S., Padgett D. A., Sheridan J. F. (2002). Social disruption-induced glucocorticoid resistance: kinetics and site specificity. J. Neuroimmunol. 124, 54–61. 10.1016/S0165-5728(02)00010-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bailey M. T., Kinsey S. G., Padgett D. A., Sheridan J. F., Leblebicioglu B. (2009). Social stress enhances IL-1β and TNF-α production by Porphyromonas gingivalis lipopolysaccharide-stimulated CD11b+ cells. Physiol. Behav. 98, 351–358. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.06.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Berkenbosch F., van Oers J., del Rey A., Tilders F., Besedovsky H. (1987). Corticotropin-releasing factor-producing neurons in the rat activated by interleukin-1. Science 238, 524–526. 10.1126/science.2443979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Betensky J. D., Contrada R. J. (2010). Depressive symptoms, trait aggression, and cardiovascular reactivity to a laboratory stressor. Ann. Behav. Med. 39, 184–191. 10.1007/s12160-010-9176-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bhatt S., Siegel A. (2006). Potentiating role of interleukin 2 (IL-2) receptors in the midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG) upon defensive rage behavior in the cat: role of neurokinin NK(1) receptors. Behav. Brain Res. 167, 251–260. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bhatt S., Zalcman S., Hassanain M., Siegel A. (2005). Cytokine modulation of defensive rage behavior in the cat: role of GABAa and interleukin-2 receptors in the medial hypothalamus. Neuroscience 133, 17–28. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bierhaus A., Wolf J., Andrassy M., Rohleder N., Humpert P. M., Petrov D., et al. (2003). A mechanism converting psychosocial stress into mononuclear cell activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 1920–1925. 10.1073/pnas.0438019100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Black P. H. (2003). The inflammatory response is an integral part of the stress response: implications for atherosclerosis, insulin resistance, type II diabetes and metabolic syndrome X. Brain Behav. Immun. 17, 350–364. 10.1016/S0889-1591(03)00048-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Blanchard C. D., Blanchard R. J. (2006). Stress and aggressive behavior, in Biology of Aggression, ed. Nelson R. J. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 275–294. [Google Scholar]
  21. Boersma M. C., Dresselhaus E. C., De Biase L. M., Mihalas A. B., Bergles D. E., Meffert M. K. (2011). A requirement for nuclear factor-kappaB in developmental and plasticity-associated synaptogenesis. J. Neurosci. 31, 5414–5425. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2456-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Borovikova L. V., Ivanova S., Zhang M., Yang H., Botchkina G. I., Watkins L. R., et al. (2000). Vagus nerve stimulation attenuates the systemic inflammatory response to endotoxin. Nature 405, 458–462. 10.1038/35013070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Boumpas D. T., Chrousos G. P., Wilder R. L., Cupps T. R., Balow J. E. (1993). Glucocorticoid therapy for immune-mediated diseases: basic and clinical correlates. Ann. Intern. Med. 119, 1198–1208. 10.7326/0003-4819-119-12-199312150-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Brebner K., Hayley S., Zacharko R., Merali Z., Anisman H. (2000). Synergistic effects of interleukin-1beta, interleukin-6, and tumor necrosis factor-alpha: central monoamine, corticosterone, and behavioral variations. Neuropsychopharmacology 22, 566–580. 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00166-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Brummett B. H., Boyle S. H., Ortel T. L., Becker R. C., Siegler I. C., Williams R. B. (2010). Associations of depressive symptoms, trait hostility, and gender with C-reactive protein and interleukin-6 response after emotion recall. Psychosom. Med. 72, 333–339. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d2f104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Buss A. H., Durkee A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. J. Consult. Psychol. 21, 343–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Buss A. H., Perry M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 452–459. 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Buwalda B., Koolhaas J. M., de Boer S. F. (2017). Trait aggressiveness does not predict social dominance of rats in the Visible Burrow System. Physiol. Behav. 178, 134–143. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cairns R. B., MacCombie D. J., Hood K. E. (1983). A developmental-genetic analysis of aggressive behavior in mice: I. Behavioral outcomes. J. Comp. Psychol. 97, 69–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Caldwell E. E., Miczek K. A. (2008). Long-term citalopram maintenance in mice: selective reduction of alcohol-heightened aggression. Psychopharmacology 196, 407–416. 10.1007/s00213-007-0972-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Christensen A. J., Edwards D. L., Wiebe J. S., Benotsch E. G., McKelvey L., Andrews M., et al. (1996). Effect of verbal self-disclosure on natural killer cell activity: moderating influence of cynical hostility. Psychosom. Med. 58, 150–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Christoffel D. J., Golden S. A., Dumitriu D., Robison A. J., Janssen W. G., Ahn H. F., et al. (2011). IκB kinase regulates social defeat stress-induced synaptic and behavioral plasticity. J. Neurosci. 31, 314–321. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4763-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Cirulli F., De Acetis L., Alleva E. (1998). Behavioral effects of peripheral interleukin-1 administration in adult CD-1 mice: specific inhibition of the offensive components of intermale agonistic behavior. Brain Res. 791, 308–312. 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00137-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Coccaro E. F., Lee R., Coussons-Read M. (2014). Elevated plasma inflammatory markers in individuals with intermittent explosive disorder and correlation with aggression in humans. JAMA Psychiatry 71:158. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Coccaro E. F., Lee R., Coussons-Read M. (2015). Cerebrospinal fluid inflammatory cytokines and aggression in personality disordered subjects. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18:pyv001. 10.1093/ijnp/pyv001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cohen S., Line S., Manuck S. B., Rabin B. S., Heise E. R., Kaplan J. R. (1997). Chronic social stress, social status, and susceptibility to upper respiratory infections in nonhuman primates. Psychosom. Med. 59, 213–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Comai S., Tau M., Pavlovic Z., Gobbi G. (2012). The psychopharmacology of aggressive behavior: a translational approach: part 2: clinical studies using atypical antipsychotics, anticonvulsants, and lithium. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 32, 237–260. 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31824929d6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Connor D. F., Glatt S. J., Lopez I. D., Jackson D., Melloni R. H. (2002). Psychopharmacology and aggression. I: a meta-analysis of stimulant effects on overt/covert aggression–related behaviors in ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 253–261. 10.1097/00004583-200203000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Connor T. J., Song C., Leonard B. E., Merali Z., Anisman H. (1998). An assessment of the effects of central interleukin-1beta,−2,−6, and tumor necrosis factor-alpha administration on some behavioural, neurochemical, endocrine and immune parameters in the rat. Neuroscience 84, 923–933. 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00533-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cook W. W., Medley D. M. (1954). Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. J. Appl. Psychol. 38, 414–418. 10.1037/h0060667 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Covington H. E., III., Miczek K. A. (2005). Intense cocaine self-administration after episodic social defeat stress, but not after aggressive behavior: dissociation from corticosterone activation. Psychopharmacology 183, 331–340. 10.1007/s00213-005-0190-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dantzer R., O'Connor J. C., Freund G. G., Johnson R. W., Kelley K. W. (2008). From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 46–56. 10.1038/nrn2297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Das S., Deuri S. K., Sarmah A., Pathak K., Baruah A., Sengupta S., et al. (2016). Aggression as an independent entity even in psychosis- the role of inflammatory cytokines. J. Neuroimmunol. 292, 45–51. 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2016.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. de Waal F. B. M. (1998). Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes, Revised Edn. (Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Univ. Press; ). [Google Scholar]
  45. Devoino L., Alperina E., Kudryavtseva N., Popova N. (1993). Immune responses in male mice with aggressive and submissive behavior patterns: strain differences. Brain Behav. Immun. 7, 91–96. 10.1006/brbi.1993.1009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Dhabhar F. S., McEwen B. S. (1999). Enhancing versus suppressive effects of stress hormones on skin immune function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 1059–1064. 10.1073/pnas.96.3.1059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Dhabhar F. S., Viswanathan K. (2005). Short-term stress experienced at time of immunization induces a long-lasting increase in immunologic memory. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 289, R738–R744. 10.1152/ajpregu.00145.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Dhabhar F. S., Malarkey W. B., Neri E., McEwen B. S. (2012). Stress-induced redistribution of immune cells–from barracks to boulevards to battlefields: a tale of three hormones–Curt Richter Award winner. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1345–1368. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.05.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Dhabhar F. S., Satoskar A. R., Bluethmann H., David J. R., McEwen B. S. (2000). Stress-induced enhancement of skin immune function: a role for gamma interferon. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 2846–2851. 10.1073/pnas.050569397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Dhabhar F. S., Saul A. N., Daugherty C., Holmes T. H., Bouley D. M., Oberyszyn T. M. (2010). Short-term stress enhances cellular immunity and increases early resistance to squamous cell carcinoma. Brain Behav. Immun. 24, 127–137. 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Dodman N. H., Donnelly R., Shuster L., Mertens P., Rand W., Miczek K. (1996). Use of fluoxetine to treat dominance aggression in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 209, 1585–1587. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Dunn A. J. (1992). Endotoxin-induced activation of cerebral catecholamine and serotonin metabolism: comparison with interleukin-1. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 261, 964–969. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Dunn A. J. (2006). Effects of cytokines and infections on brain neurochemistry. Clin. Neurosci. Res. 6, 52–68. 10.1016/j.cnr.2006.04.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Elenkov I. J., Wilder R. L., Chrousos G. P., Vizi E. S. (2000). The sympathetic nerve–an integrative interface between two supersystems: the brain and the immune system. Pharmacol. Rev. 52, 595–638. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. El-Haj T., Poole S., Farthing M. J., Ballinger A. B. (2002). Anorexia in a rat model of colitis: interaction of interleukin-1 and hypothalamic serotonin. Brain Res. 927, 1–7. 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03305-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ely D. L., Henry J. P. (1978). Neuroendocrine response patterns in dominant and subordinate mice. Horm. Behav. 10, 156–169. 10.1016/0018-506X(78)90005-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Falkner A. L., Grosenick L., Davidson T. J., Deisseroth K., Lin D. (2016). Hypothalamic control of male aggression-seeking behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 596–604. 10.1038/nn.4264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Fish E. W., De Bold J. F., Miczek K. A. (2002). Aggressive behavior as a reinforcer in mice: activation by allopregnanolone. Psychopharmacology 163, 459–466. 10.1007/s00213-002-1211-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gariepy J. L., Lewis M. H., Cairns R. B. (1996). Neurobiology and aggression, in Aggression and Violence: Genetic, Neurobiological and Biosocial Perspectives, eds Stoff D. M., Cairns R. B. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; ), 41–63. [Google Scholar]
  60. Gemma C., Imeri L., de Simoni M. G., Mancia M. (1997). Interleukin-1 induces changes in sleep, brain temperature, and serotonergic metabolism. Am. J. Physiol. 272, R601–R606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Glaser R., Kiecolt-Glaser J. K. (2005). Stress-induced immune dysfunction: implications for health. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 5, 243–251. 10.1038/nri1571 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Golden S. A., Covington H. E., Berton O., Russo S. J. (2011). A standardized protocol for repeated social defeat stress in mice. Nat. Protoc. 6, 1183–1191. 10.1038/nprot.2011.361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Golden S. A., Heins C., Venniro M., Caprioli D., Zhang M., Epstein D. H., et al. (2017). Compulsive addiction-like aggressive behavior in mice. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 239–248. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Golden S. A., Heshmati M., Flanigan M., Christoffel D. J., Guise K., Pfau M. L., et al. (2016). Basal forebrain projections to the lateral habenula modulate aggression reward. Nature 534, 688–692. 10.1038/nature18601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Graham J. E., Robles T. F., Kiecolt-Glaser J. K., Malarkey W. B., Bissell M. G., Glaser R. (2006). Hostility and pain are related to inflammation in older adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 20, 389–400. 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Granger D. A., Booth A., Johnson D. R. (2000). Human aggression and enumerative measures of immunity. Psychosom. Med. 62, 583–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Haller J., Harold G., Sandi C., Neumann I. D. (2014). Effects of adverse early-life events on aggression and anti-social behaviours in animals and humans. J. Neuroendocrinol. 26, 724–738. 10.1111/jne.12182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hanke M. L., Powell N. D., Stiner L. M., Bailey M. T., Sheridan J. F. (2012). Beta adrenergic blockade decreases the immunomodulatory effects of social disruption stress. Brain Behav. Immun. 26, 1150–1159. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hassanain M., Bhatt S., Zalcman S., Siegel A. (2005). Potentiating role of interleukin-1beta (IL-1beta) and IL-1beta type 1 receptors in the medial hypothalamus in defensive rage behavior in the cat. Brain Res. 1048, 1–11. 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.04.086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hassanain M., Zalcman S., Bhatt S., Siegel A. (2003). Interleukin-1 beta in the hypothalamus potentiates feline defensive rage: role of serotonin-2 receptors. Neuroscience 120, 227–233. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00264-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Herr N., Bode C., Duerschmied D. (2017). The effects of serotonin in immune cells. Front. Cardiovasc. Med. 4:48. 10.3389/fcvm.2017.00048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hodes G. E., Kana V., Menard C., Merad M., Russo S. J. (2015). Neuroimmune mechanisms of depression. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1386–1393. 10.1038/nn.4113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Hodes G. E., Ménard C., Russo S. J. (2016). Integrating Interleukin-6 into depression diagnosis and treatment. Neurobiol. Stress 4, 15–22. 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Hodes G. E., Pfau M. L., Leboeuf M., Golden S. A., Christoffel D. J., Bregman D., et al. (2014). Individual differences in the peripheral immune system promote resilience versus susceptibility to social stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 16136–16141. 10.1073/pnas.1415191111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hopkins S. J., Rothwell N. J. (1995). Cytokines and the nervous system. I: expression and recognition. Trends Neurosci. 18, 83–88. 10.1016/0166-2236(95)80029-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Idova G. V., Markova E. V., Gevorgyan M. M., Alperina E. L., Zhukova E. N. (2016). Changes in production of cytokines by C57Bl/6J mouse spleen during aggression provoked by social stress. Bull. Exp. Biol. Med. 160, 679–682. 10.1007/s10517-016-3248-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Jung S. H., Wang Y., Kim T., Tarr A., Reader B., Powell N., et al. (2015). Molecular mechanisms of repeated social defeat-induced glucocorticoid resistance: role of microRNA. Brain Behav. Immun. 44, 195–206. 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.09.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kavelaars A., Heijnen C. J., Tennekes R., Bruggink J. E., Koolhaas J. M. (1999). Individual behavioral characteristics of wild-type rats predict susceptibility to experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. Brain Behav. Immun. 13, 279–286. 10.1006/brbi.1998.0534 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kiecolt-Glaser J. K., Loving T. J., Stowell J. R., Malarkey W. B., Lemeshow S., Dickinson S. L., et al. (2005). Hostile marital interactions, proinflammatory cytokine production, and wound healing. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62:1377. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kiecolt-Glaser J. K., Preacher K. J., MacCallum R. C., Atkinson C., Malarkey W. B., Glaser R. (2003). Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 9090–9095. 10.1073/pnas.1531903100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kiraly D. D., Horn S. R., Van Dam N. T., Costi S., Schwartz J., Kim-Schulze S., et al. (2017). Altered peripheral immune profiles in treatment-resistant depression: response to ketamine and prediction of treatment outcome. Transl. Psychiatry 7:e1065. 10.1038/tp.2017.31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Klein S. L., Flanagan K. L. (2016). Sex differences in immune responses. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 16, 626–638. 10.1038/nri.2016.90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Köhler C. A., Freitas T. H., Stubbs B., Maes M., Solmi M., Veronese N., et al. (2017). Peripheral alterations in cytokine and chemokine levels after antidepressant drug treatment for major depressive disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. Mol. Neurobiol. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1007/s12035-017-0632-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Koolhaas J. M. (2008). Coping style and immunity in animals: making sense of individual variation. Brain Behav. Immun. 22, 662–667. 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kraus M. R., Schäfer A., Faller H., Csef H., Scheurlen M. (2003). Psychiatric symptoms in patients with chronic hepatitis C receiving interferon alfa-2b therapy. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64, 708–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kubera M., Lin A. H., Kenis G., Bosmans E., van Bockstaele D., Maes M. (2001). Anti-Inflammatory effects of antidepressants through suppression of the interferon-gamma/interleukin-10 production ratio. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 21, 199–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Larson S. J., Dunn A. J. (2001). Behavioral effects of cytokines. Brain Behav. Immun. 15, 371–387. 10.1006/brbi.2001.0643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lew W., Oppenheim J. J., Matsushima K. (1988). Analysis of the suppression of IL-1 alpha and IL-1 beta production in human peripheral blood mononuclear adherent cells by a glucocorticoid hormone. J. Immunol. 140, 1895–1902. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Line S. W., Kaplan J. R., Heise E. R., Hilliard J. K., Cohen S., Rabin B. S., et al. (1996). Effects of social reorganization on cellular immunity in male cynomolgus monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 39, 235–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Linthorst A. C., Flachskamm C., Holsboer F., Reul J. M. (1994). Local administration of recombinant human interleukin-1 beta in the rat hippocampus increases serotonergic neurotransmission, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis activity, and body temperature. Endocrinology 135, 520–532. 10.1210/endo.135.2.7518383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Linthorst A. C., Flachskamm C., Müller-Preuss P., Holsboer F., Reul J. M. (1995). Effect of bacterial endotoxin and interleukin-1 beta on hippocampal serotonergic neurotransmission, behavioral activity, and free corticosterone levels: an in vivo microdialysis study. J. Neurosci. 15, 2920–2934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Lotrich F. E., Sears B., McNamara R. K. (2013). Anger induced by interferon-alpha is moderated by ratio of arachidonic acid to omega-3 fatty acids. J. Psychosom. Res. 75, 475–483. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Maes M., Bosmans E., De Jongh R., Kenis G., Vandoolaeghe E., Neels H. (1997). Increased serum IL-6 and IL-1 receptor antagonist concentrations in major depression and treatment resistant depression. Cytokine 9, 853–858. 10.1006/cyto.1997.0238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Maes M., Song C., Lin A. H., Bonaccorso S., Kenis G., De Jongh R., et al. (1999). Negative immunoregulatory effects of antidepressants inhibition of interferon-γ and stimulation of interleukin-10 secretion. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 370–379. 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00088-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Maes M., Yirmyia R., Noraberg J., Brene S., Hibbeln J., Perini G., et al. (2009). The inflammatory & neurodegenerative (I&ND) hypothesis of depression: leads for future research and new drug developments in depression. Metab. Brain Dis. 24, 27–53. 10.1007/s11011-008-9118-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Manchia M., Carpiniello B., Valtorta F., Comai S. (2017). Serotonin dysfunction, aggressive behavior, and mental illness: exploring the link using a dimensional approach. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 8, 961–972. 10.1021/acschemneuro.6b00427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Marques-Deak A., Cizza G., Sternberg E. (2005). Brain-immune interactions and disease susceptibility. Mol. Psychiatry 10, 239–250. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Marsland A. L., Prather A. A., Petersen K. L., Cohen S., Manuck S. B. (2008). Antagonistic characteristics are positively associated with inflammatory markers independently of trait negative emotionality. Brain Behav. Immun. 22, 753–761. 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Matson J. L., Jang J. (2014). Treating aggression in persons with autism spectrum disorders: a review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 35, 3386–3391. 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Matteoli G., Gomez-Pinilla P. J., Nemethova A., Di Giovangiulio M., Cailotto C., van Bree S. H., et al. (2014). A distinct vagal anti-inflammatory pathway modulates intestinal muscularis resident macrophages independent of the spleen. Gut 63, 938–948. 10.1136/gutjnl-2013-304676 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. McCloskey M. S., Ammerman B. A. (2018). Suicidal behavior and aggression-related disorders. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 22, 54–58. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. McHutchison J. G., Gordon S. C., Schiff E. R., Shiffman M. L., Lee W. M., Rustgi V. K., et al. (1998). Interferon Alfa-2b alone or in combination with ribavirin as initial treatment for chronic hepatitis C. N. Engl. J. Med. 339, 1485–1492. 10.1056/NEJM199811193392101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mefford I. N., Heyes M. P. (1990). Increased biogenic amine release in mouse hypothalamus following immunological challenge: antagonism by indomethacin. J. Neuroimmunol. 27, 55–61. 10.1016/0165-5728(90)90136-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Ménard C., Pfau M. L., Hodes G. E., Russo S. J. (2017a). Immune and neuroendocrine mechanisms of stress vulnerability and resilience. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 62–80. 10.1038/npp.2016.90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Menard C., Pfau M. L., Hodes G. E., Kana V., Wang V. X., Bouchard S., et al. (2017b). Social stress induces neurovascular pathology promoting depression. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1752–1760. 10.1038/s41593-017-0010-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Miczek K. A., Fish E. W. (2006). Monoamines, GABA, Glutamate, and Aggression, in Biology of Aggression, eds Nelson R. J. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 114–149. [Google Scholar]
  107. Miczek K. A., de Boer S. F., Haller J. (2013). Excessive aggression as model of violence: a critical evaluation of current preclinical methods. Psychopharmacology 226, 445–458. 10.1007/s00213-013-3008-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Miczek K. A., Fish E. W., De Bold J. F., De Almeida R. M. (2002). Social and neural determinants of aggressive behavior: pharmacotherapeutic targets at serotonin, dopamine and gamma-aminobutyric acid systems. Psychopharmacology 163, 434–458. 10.1007/s00213-002-1139-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Miczek K. A., Yap J. J., Covington H. E. (2008). Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol. Ther. 120, 102–128. 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2008.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Mikics É., Guirado R., Umemori J., Tóth M., Biró L., Miskolczi C., et al. (2017). Social learning requires plasticity enhanced by fluoxetine through prefrontal Bdnf-TrkB signaling to limit aggression induced by post-weaning social isolation. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 235–245. 10.1038/npp.2017.142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Miller G. E., Dopp J. M., Myers H. F., Stevens S. Y., Fahey J. L. (1999). Psychosocial predictors of natural killer cell mobilization during marital conflict. Health Psychol. 18, 262–271. 10.1037/0278-6133.18.3.262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Miller G. E., Freedland K. E., Carney R. M., Stetler C. A., Banks W. A. (2003). Cynical hostility, depressive symptoms, and the expression of inflammatory risk markers for coronary heart disease. J. Behav. Med. 26, 501–515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Mommersteeg P. M. C., Vermetten E., Kavelaars A., Geuze E., Heijnen C. J. (2008). Hostility is related to clusters of T-cell cytokines and chemokines in healthy men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 1041–1050. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Mössner R., Lesch K. P. (1998). Role of serotonin in the immune system and in neuroimmune interactions. Brain Behav. Immun. 12, 249–271. 10.1006/brbi.1998.0532 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Olivier B. (2015). Serotonin: a never-ending story. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 753, 2–18. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.10.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Osborn L., Kunkel S., Nabel G. J. (1989). Tumor necrosis factor alpha and interleukin 1 stimulate the human immunodeficiency virus enhancer by activation of the nuclear factor kappa B. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 2336–2340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Patel A., Siegel A., Zalcman S. S. (2010). Lack of aggression and anxiolytic-like behavior in TNF receptor (TNF-R1 and TNF-R2) deficient mice. Brain Behav. Immun. 24, 1276–1280. 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Pepys M. B., Hirschfield G. M. (2003). C-reactive protein: a critical update. J. Clin. Invest. 111, 1805–1812. 10.1172/JCI18921 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Pesce M., Speranza L., Franceschelli S., Ialenti V., Iezzi I., Patruno A., et al. (2013). Positive correlation between serum interleukin-1β and state anger in rugby athletes. Aggress. Behav. 39, 141–148. 10.1002/ab.21457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Petitto J. M., Lysle D. T., Gariepy J. L., Clubb P. H., Cairns R. B., Lewis M. H. (1993). Genetic differences in social behavior: relation to natural killer cell function and susceptibility to tumor development. Neuropsychopharmacology 8, 35–43. 10.1038/npp.1993.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Petitto J. M., Lysle D. T., Gariepy J. L., Lewis M. H. (1994). Association of genetic differences in social behavior and cellular immune responsiveness: effects of social experience. Brain Behav. Immun. 8, 111–122. 10.1006/brbi.1994.1011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Pfau M. L., Russo S. J. (2015). Peripheral and central mechanisms of stress resilience. Neurobiol. Stress 1, 66–79. 10.1016/j.ynstr.2014.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Pinna G., Dong E., Matsumoto K., Costa E., Guidotti A. (2003). In socially isolated mice, the reversal of brain allopregnanolone down-regulation mediates the anti-aggressive action of fluoxetine. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 2035–2040. 10.1073/pnas.0337642100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Quan N., Avitsur R., Stark J. L., He L., Lai W., Dhabhar F., et al. (2003). Molecular mechanisms of glucocorticoid resistance in splenocytes of socially stressed male mice. J. Neuroimmunol. 137, 51–58. 10.1016/S0165-5728(03)00042-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Raine A., Lencz T., Yaralian P., Bihrle S., LaCasse L., Ventura J., et al. (2002). Prefrontal structural and functional deficits in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr. Bull. 28, 501–513. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Ramirez K., Shea D. T., McKim D. B., Reader B. F., Sheridan J. F. (2015). Imipramine attenuates neuroinflammatory signaling and reverses stress-induced social avoidance. Brain Behav. Immun. 46, 212–220. 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.01.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Ranjit N., Diez-Roux A. V., Shea S., Cushman M., Seeman T., Jackson S. A., et al. (2007). Psychosocial factors and inflammation in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Arch. Intern. Med. 167:174. 10.1001/archinte.167.2.174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Réus G. Z., Fries G. R., Stertz L., Badawy M., Passos I. C., Barichello T., et al. (2015). The role of inflammation and microglial activation in the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Neuroscience 300, 141–154. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Rose R. M., Jenkins C. D., Hurst M., Kreger B. E., Barrett J., Hall R. P. (1982). Endocrine activity in air traffic controllers at work. III. Relationship to physical and psychiatric morbidity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 7, 125–134. 10.1016/0306-4530(82)90004-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Rozanski A., Blumenthal J. A., Kaplan J. (1999). Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation 99, 2192–2217. 10.1161/01.CIR.99.16.2192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Russo S. J., Wilkinson M. B., Mazei-Robison M. S., Dietz D. M., Maze I., et al. (2009). Nuclear factor kappa B signaling regulates neuronal morphology and cocaine reward. J. Neurosci. 29, 3529–3537. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6173-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Sá-Rocha V. M., Sá-Rocha L. C., Palermo-Neto J. (2006). Variations in behavior, innate immunity and host resistance to B16F10 melanoma growth in mice that present social stable hierarchical ranks. Physiol. Behav. 88, 108–115. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Sapolsky R. M. (1989). Hypercortisolism among socially subordinate wild baboons originates at the CNS level. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 46, 1047–1051. 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110089012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sapolsky R. M. (1990). Adrenocortical function, social rank, and personality among wild baboons. Biol. Psychiatry 28, 862–878. 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90568-M [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Schneider H., Pitossi F., Balschun D., Wagner A., del Rey A., Besedovsky H. O. (1998). A neuromodulatory role of interleukin-1beta in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 7778–7783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Siegel A., Roeling T. A., Gregg T. R., Kruk M. R. (1999). Neuropharmacology of brain-stimulation-evoked aggression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 359–389. 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00040-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Smith T. W., Ruiz J. M. (2002). Psychosocial influences on the development and course of coronary heart disease: current status and implications for research and practice. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 70, 548–568. 10.1037/0022-006X.70.3.548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Spencer R. L., Miller A. H., Stein M., McEwen B. S. (1991). Corticosterone regulation of type I and type II adrenal steroid receptors in brain, pituitary, and immune tissue. Brain Res. 549, 236–246. 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90463-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Stark J. L., Avitsur R., Padgett D. A., Campbell K. A., Beck F. M., Sheridan J. F. (2001). Social stress induces glucocorticoid resistance in macrophages. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 280, R1799–R1805. 10.1152/ajpregu.2001.280.6.R1799 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Stewart A. M., Roy S., Wong K., Gaikwad S., Chung K. M., et al. (2015). Cytokine and endocrine parameters in mouse chronic social defeat: implications for translational ‘cross-domain’ modeling of stress-related brain disorders. Behav. Brain Res. 276, 84–91. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Stewart J. C., Janicki-Deverts D., Muldoon M. F., Kamarck T. W. (2008). Depressive symptoms moderate the influence of hostility on serum interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein. Psychosom. Med. 70, 197–204. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181642a0b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Suarez E. C. (2003). Joint effect of hostility and severity of depressive symptoms on plasma interleukin-6 concentration. Psychosom. Med. 65, 523–527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Suarez E. C., Boyle S. H., Lewis J. G., Hall R. P., Young K. H. (2006). Increases in stimulated secretion of proinflammatory cytokines by blood monocytes following arousal of negative affect: the role of insulin resistance as moderator. Brain Behav. Immun. 20, 331–338. 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Suarez E. C., Lewis J. G., Kuhn C. (2002). The relation of aggression, hostility, and anger to lipopolysaccharide-stimulated tumor necrosis factor (TNF)-alpha by blood monocytes from normal men. Brain Behav. Immun. 16, 675–684. 10.1016/S0889-1591(02)00019-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Suarez E. C., Lewis J. G., Krishnan R. R., Young K. H. (2004). Enhanced expression of cytokines and chemokines by blood monocytes to in vitro lipopolysaccharide stimulation are associated with hostility and severity of depressive symptoms in healthy women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 1119–1128. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Takahashi A., Chung J. R., Zhang S., Zhang H., Grossman Y., Aleyasin H., et al. (2017). Establishment of a repeated social defeat stress model in female mice. Sci. Rep. 7:12838. 10.1038/s41598-017-12811-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Tan K. S., Nackley A. G., Satterfield K., Maixner W., Diatchenko L., Flood P. M. (2007). β2 adrenergic receptor activation stimulates pro-inflammatory cytokine production in macrophages via PKA- and NF-κB-independent mechanisms. Cell. Signal. 19, 251–260. 10.1016/j.cellsig.2006.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. van der Kooij M. A., Hollis F., Lozano L., Zalachoras I., Abad S., et al. (2017). Diazepam actions in the VTA enhance social dominance and mitochondrial function in the nucleus accumbens by activation of dopamine D1 receptors. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 569–578. 10.1038/mp.2017.135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Volavka J., Czobor P., Citrome L., McQuade R. D., Carson W. H., Kostic D., et al. (2005). Efficacy of aripiprazole against hostility in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder: data from 5 double-blind studies. J. Clin. Psychiatry 66, 1362–1366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Warner-Schmidt J. L., Vanover K. E., Chen E. Y., Marshall J. J., Greengard P. (2011). Antidepressant effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are attenuated by antiinflammatory drugs in mice and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 9262–9267. 10.1073/pnas.1104836108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Weber M. D., Godbout J. P., Sheridan J. F. (2017). Repeated social defeat, neuroinflammation, and behavior: monocytes carry the signal. Neuropsychopharmacology 42, 46–61. 10.1038/npp.2016.102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Zalcman S. S., Siegel A. (2006). The neurobiology of aggression and rage: role of cytokines. Brain Behav. Immun. 20, 507–514. 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Zalcman S., Green-Johnson J. M., Murray L., Nance D. M., Dyck D., Anisman H., et al. (1994). Cytokine-specific central monoamine alterations induced by interleukin-1,−2 and−6. Brain Res. 643, 40–49. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90006-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES