Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018 Jan 4;89:86–91. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.01.003

Parenting Predicts Strange Situation Cortisol Reactivity Among Children Adopted Internationally

Carrie E DePasquale a, K Lee Raby b, Julie Hoye c, Mary Dozier c
PMCID: PMC5878708  NIHMSID: NIHMS934699  PMID: 29334626

Abstract

The functioning of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis can be altered by adverse early experiences. Recent studies indicate that children who were adopted internationally after experiencing early institutional rearing and unstable caregiving exhibit blunted HPA reactivity to stressful situations. The present study examined whether caregiving experiences post-adoption further modulate children’s HPA responses to stress. Parental sensitivity during naturalistic parent-child play interactions was assessed for 66 children (M age= 17.3 months, SD = 4.6) within a year of being adopted internationally. Approximately 8 months later, children’s salivary cortisol levels were measured immediately before as well as 15 and 30 minutes after a series of brief separations from the mother in an unfamiliar laboratory setting. Latent growth curve modeling indicated that experiencing more parental sensitivity predicted increased cortisol reactivity to the stressor. Although half the families received an intervention designed to improve parental sensitivity, the intervention did not significantly alter children’s cortisol outcomes. These findings suggest that post-adoption parental sensitivity may help normalize the HPA response to stress among children adopted internationally.

Keywords: HPA axis, hypocortisolism, Strange Situation, parental sensitivity, international adoption


Since 1999, families in the United States have adopted over 250,000 children from other countries, and many were adopted before the age of 3 (US DOS, 2015). Prior to adoption, many internationally adopted (IA) children experience a combination of institutional care, which is characterized by rotating shifts of a large number of caregivers, and foster care, which often involves frequent changes in caregivers (Dozier, Zeanah, Wallin, & Shauffer, 2012; Gunnar, Bruce, & Grotevant, 2000). Because of these early adversities, IA children are at increased risk for problematic adaptation in a number of domains, including physiological stress responses (for a review, see Juffer et al., 2011). Adoption leads to a dramatic improvement in caregiving environment, which has the potential to promote recovery in development following early social deprivation (Gunnar et al., 2000). The present study examined the role of IA children’s post-adoption caregiving experiences in the recovery of their HPA axis regulation.

1.1 Early social regulation of the HPA axis

The HPA axis is one of the body’s main stress response systems. Its primary hormonal end-product, the glucocorticoid cortisol, has wide ranging impacts on the body and brain, including regulation of the circadian rhythm and mobilization of the body’s metabolic resources in response to real or imagined threat (Gunnar, Doom, & Esposito, 2015). In normative-risk populations, exposure to a stressor results in a moderate increase in circulating cortisol produced by the adrenal cortex (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). Negative feedback mechanisms serve to suppress the production of cortisol, which efficiently returns the system back to basal levels.

There are two main types of glucocorticoid receptors in the brain. Mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) have a higher affinity for glucocorticoids, and cortisol typically is bound to a portion of these receptors in the absence of stress. Glucocorticoid receptors (GRs) have a lower affinity for glucocorticoids and mostly bind when there are high levels of circulating cortisol (for example, in response to stress). When the ratio of bound MR/GR receptors is high (i.e., there is cortisol bound to a high number of MR receptors and a low number of GR receptors), learning and memory are enhanced (Finsterwald & Alberini, 2014; see also Schilling et al., 2013). Thus, a moderate increase in cortisol to a stressor or stimulus, which facilitates MR binding with little GR binding, is likely conducive to healthy HPA stress response activation and termination.

During early development, the HPA axis is especially plastic and is regulated by children’s experiences with caregivers (Gunnar & Cheatham, 2003). Caregivers typically function to buffer children’s cortisol reactivity, meaning that the presence of an emotionally supportive caregiver prevents excessive activation of the HPA axis in response to stressful situations; conversely, in the absence of caregiver support, children tend to exhibit elevated HPA activity in response to stressors (Hostinar, Sullivan, & Gunnar, 2014). When children experience chronic stress, such as the absence of typical early caregiving relationships, the HPA axis reorganizes and downregulates its cortisol production, resulting in blunted cortisol responses to stress (Doom, Cicchetti, & Rogosch, 2014; Fisher, Van Ryzin, & Gunnar, 2011; Fries, Hesse, Hellhammer, & Hellhammer, 2005; McLaughlin et al., 2015). Decreased reactivity of the HPA axis may be the result of altered MR/GR ratios, particularly through excessive expression of GR (Strüber, Strüber, & Roth, 2014). Although this is presumed to be an adaptive response to avoid dangerously high elevations of cortisol, exhibiting little to no HPA response to stressors may also have long-term consequences for behavioral adaptation and physical health, including increased risk for externalizing behaviors (Alink et al., 2008; Conradt et al., 2014; Koss et al., 2016; Ruttle et al., 2011), reduced executive function abilities (Blair, Granger, & Razza, 2005), problems with peer relations (Pitula, DePasquale, Mliner, & Gunnar, in press), and increased risk for PTSD (Fries et al., 2005).

Blunted cortisol reactivity is distinct from social buffering. Sensitive caregivers – caregivers that respond contingently and flexibly to their child’s cues – tend to reduce children’s HPA response to stress. Children who have not experienced chronic stress and are in the presence of a supportive caregiver display a moderate, though not excessive, HPA stress response (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). Among children from normative-risk backgrounds, more supportive parent-child relationships are associated with a dampened HPA response to stress (for a review, see Hunter, Minnis, & Wilson, 2011). This allows the body to activate the HPA axis and other systems in order to respond to the stimulus or stressor effectively, but not so much that it becomes overwhelming (Hostinar et al., 2014). This pattern of HPA functioning is not associated with changes in MR/GR expression or behavioral problems (Chrousos, 2009). Thus, there seems to be an inverted U-shaped curve association between HPA axis reactivity and developmental adaptation, such that both too much and too little cortisol production can have damaging effects on well-being (Gunnar et al., 2015; Gunnar & Vazquez, 2001)

1.2 HPA axis functioning for children adopted internationally

Researchers have examined the HPA responses of IA children as a way of better understanding whether early adversity results in dysregulation of children’s neurobiological stress response system. An initial study of only 18 IA children suggested that these children showed increased cortisol reactivity relative to non-adopted children (Fries, Shirtcliff, & Pollack, 2008). However, more recent studies with larger samples indicate that IA children exhibited a more blunted pattern of cortisol reactivity than non-adopted children. Specifically, Koss and colleagues (2016) examined the cortisol responses to laboratory stress for over 100 IA children and reported that IA children exhibited an attenuated HPA response. Similarly, children raised in Romanian orphanages exhibited more blunted cortisol reactivity during early adolescence than children randomly assigned to receive a foster care intervention (McLaughlin et al., 2015). Similar patterns have also been found for children in foster care who have experienced maltreatment (Fisher, Kim, Bruce, & Pears, 2012). Thus, the preponderance of the evidence suggests that disturbances in early caregiving relationships typically experienced by internationally adopted children are associated with hypocortisolism.

To our knowledge, no studies have examined the significance of IA children’s post-adoptive caregiving experiences for their cortisol stress responses. As a result, the consequences of IA children’s experiences with their adoptive parents for their neurobiological development are still unknown. However, there is growing evidence indicating that sensitive caregiving promotes healthier behavioral outcomes among IA children (e.g., Garvin, Tarullo, Van Ryzin, & Gunnar, 2012; Stams, Juffer, & van IJzendoorn, 2002). Thus, sensitive caregiving appears to facilitate the recovery of IA children’s developmental outcomes following earlier experiences of deprivation.

1.4 The current study

The purpose of the current study was to examine the role of IA children’s post-adoptive caregiving experiences for their HPA responses. Data for this study were drawn from a randomized controlled trial assessing the effectiveness of an attachment-based intervention for IA children’s biobehavioral development. However, the current study focuses on the predictive significance of IA parents’ sensitive caregiving, which was observed prior to receiving the intervention (see the Supplemental Materials for more information about the intervention). Approximately 8 months later children’s cortisol responses to the Strange Situation Procedure (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Walls, 1978) were collected. The Strange Situation is one of the most common tasks for assessing cortisol reactivity among young children (for a review, see Gunnar, Talge, & Herrera, 2009). Based on the previous evidence that IA children are at risk for blunted cortisol reactivity to stress (e.g., Koss et al., 2016; McLaughlin et al., 2015), we hypothesized that experiencing higher post-adoption parental sensitivity would be associated with normalization of the stress response, and thus increased cortisol reactivity compared to those with less sensitive adoptive parents.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

Participants included 66 children (33 female) who were adopted from orphanages or foster care systems in several countries, including China (38%), Russia (21%), Ethiopia (18%), South Korea (18%), Kazakhstan (3%), and Armenia (2%) into the Mid-Atlantic region of the United States. Participants were recruited through partnerships with local international adoption agencies, an international adoption clinic at a local children’s hospital, and international adoption support groups. Families were asked to participate if they had recently adopted a child internationally. Children were excluded if they lacked mobility or if they had severe physical or mental disabilities. Children involved in the study came into the primary care of their adoptive parents between 4 and 28 months of age (M = 13.9, SD = 4.8). Based on adoptive parents’ reports, 80% percent of the children had experienced institutional care prior to adoption. Those children who had experienced institutional care spent on average 82% of their pre-adoptive lives in institutions (range = 2 to 23 months, M = 11.6). Ninety-five percent of the adoptive parents who participated in the study were female, and 95% were White/non-Hispanic (2% were African-American, 2% were Asian-American, and one did not provide information). Ninety-two percent of the adoptive parents were married, and the remaining 8% were single. Eighty-two percent had at least a college degree, and the others had at least a high school degree or GED. Sixty-four percent of the families had an annual income of $100,000 or more; approximately 32% had an annual income of $60,000–$99,000, and approximately 5% had an annual income of $40,000–$59,000. There were two pairs of adoptive siblings in the sample. The results did not differ with the random exclusion of one sibling from each pair, so siblings were retained in the results presented below. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the [deleted for blind review]. Informed consent was obtained from the child’s primary caregiver prior to participation.

2.2 Procedure

The current study involved data collected at three time-points. First, parent-child interactions were videotaped during an initial home assessment when children were between 7 and 33 months old (M = 17.3, SD = 4.8). At that time, children had been in their adoptive families’ care for between 1 and 12 months (M = 3.4, SD = 2.0). Second, shortly after this initial assessment families were randomly assigned by a coin toss to receive either an intervention intended to promote parenting behaviors associated with children’s attachment quality and biological regulation (for a review, see Dozier, Peloso, Lewis, Laurenceau, & Levine, 2008) or an occupational therapy control intervention (see Supplemental Materials for more information). Third, after completion of the intervention, observations of parent-child interactions were collected during a second home assessment, and families participated in the Strange Situation during a laboratory visit. At the time of this laboratory visit, children ranged in age from 21 to 33 months old (M = 25.8, SD = 2.1) and had been in their adoptive families’ care for between 5.2 and 21.5 months (M = 11.9, SD = 4.1).

2.3 Measures

2.3.1 Parental sensitivity

Parents and children were video recorded in their homes during semi-structured interaction tasks. The tasks were selected to be developmentally appropriate based on the children’s ages at the time of the assessment. When children were under the age of 20 months, children were placed in a child seat and parents were provided with three toys: a rattle, a set of stacking cups, and a squeaking toy. When children were between 21 and 35 months, parents and children were provided with a set of building blocks. Regardless of the child age, parents were instructed to interact with their children as they normally would, and the interaction was filmed for approximately 10 minutes, including 7 minutes of free play and 2–3 minutes of clean-up.

Parents’ behaviors during the interactions were coded on the basis of parents’ sensitivity to the child’s non-distress signals using a 5-point sensitivity scale (with decimal points allowed) that was adapted from the NICHD Observational Record of the Caregiving Environment (NICHD ECCRN, 1996). A score of 5 was assigned to parents who responded contingently and appropriately to their children’s cues. A score of 1 was assigned to parents who followed their own agenda during the play interactions and/or ignored children’s signals. All videos were double-coded, and the average score was used for analyses (M = 2.9, SD = 0.8). The intraclass correlation (two-way mixed, average measures) was .62.

Observations of parent-child interactions were collected prior to the intervention and shortly after completion of the intervention. We decided to focus on the pre-intervention parental sensitivity assessment to establish the temporal precedence of predicting cortisol reactivity from earlier parenting. However, all decisions regarding statistical significance remained the same when post-intervention parental sensitivity was used (see Supplemental Materials for more information).

2.3.2 Strange Situation

The Strange Situation Procedure involves eight increasingly stressful episodes that include two separations and two reunions with the parent. The first separation episode typically lasts up to three minutes (shortened if the child is very distressed), and the second separation episode can last up to six minutes (3 minutes with the stranger, and 3 in room alone). The separations are followed by three minute reunions with the parent. Children’s behaviors during the procedure were video-recorded. The laboratory-based procedure was originally developed to assess the quality of children’s attachment to their parent. Two trained coders coded children’s attachment behaviors during the Strange Situation using the guidelines of Ainsworth et al. (1978). Specifically, coders assigned ratings for children’s proximity seeking, contact maintenance, attachment avoidance, and contact resistance during the two reunion episodes. Based on these ratings, coders classified children as having a secure (75%), insecure-avoidant (11%), or insecure-resistant (14%) attachment to their parent. In addition, the coders evaluated children’s attachment disorganization/disorientation using Main and Solomon’s (1990) 9-point scale (M = 3.39, SD = 2.05), and children were classified as disorganized if they received a score of 5 or higher (31%; weighted κ = .40). We focused on categorically-measured attachment disorganization given the previous findings regarding the associations between disorganization and cortisol reactivity (e.g., Bernard & Dozier, 2010). Importantly, results did not substantively differ when dimensionally-measured disorganization or the categorical or dimensional indices of attachment security were used in the analyses.

2.3.3 Salivary cortisol

To assess salivary cortisol concentration, saliva samples were collected upon the child’s arrival at the lab as well as 15 and 30 minutes following completion of the Strange Situation Procedure. The Strange Situation Procedure was completed primarily in the late morning (median start time = 10:16 AM, SD = 112 minutes). Children were not allowed to eat in between saliva samples so as not to affect cortisol levels or the quality of the saliva sample. The sample taken upon arrival represents each child’s baseline cortisol level before experiencing the Strange Situation Procedure. Post-stressor samples were taken 15 and 30 minutes following completion of the Strange Situation because peak concentrations in response to various stressors are expected to occur within that interval (Kirschbaum & Helhammer, 1994). After completing the Strange Situation Procedure, children and their parents engaged in free-play in the examination room for 30 minutes to allow for post-Strange Situation salivary cortisol collection. For each sample, parents placed a dental cotton roll in the child’s mouth until it was sufficiently wet with saliva. All samples were stored in a freezer at −20 °C in the laboratory prior to assaying. Samples were assayed in duplicate using the High Sensitivity Salivary Cortisol Enzyme Immunoassay Kit (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA). Samples with an intra-assay coefficient of variation greater than 10% were assayed a second time. All inter- and intra-assay coefficients of variation were below 10%. Two samples involved biologically implausible values (>2.0 µg/dL) and were therefore excluded from the analyses. Across the 66 participants, usable cortisol values were available for 191 saliva samples. Participants were required to have at least one usable saliva sample to be included in analyses: all three samples were usable for 92% of participants and at least two were usable for 97% of participants. To correct for positive skew, cortisol concentration values were log10 transformed prior to analyses.

2.4 Data Analytic Plan

Latent growth curve analyses using the ‘lavaan’ package in R (Rosseel, 2012) were conducted to evaluate the predictors of children’s baseline cortisol levels (intercept) and cortisol reactivity (linear slope) to the Strange Situation procedure. The covariance between the three cortisol samples was also modeled in each analysis. The focal predictor in these analyses was parents’ sensitivity during the pre-intervention parent-child interactions. Whether children were exhibiting a disorganized attachment pattern was also included as a measure of children’s behavior during the Strange Situation. Intervention condition was included as a predictor of both intercept and slope given previous evidence of associations with cortisol production during the Strange Situation (Dozier et al., 2008; see Supplemental Materials for more detailed results regarding the intervention).

Children’s biological sex, number of siblings, age at the time of the Strange Situation Procedure, time of day of the procedure, region of adoption, percent of pre-adoptive life spent in institutional care, age at adoption, duration of time since adoption, family income, parent age at adoption, parent education, parent marital status, and whether the parents had previously adopted children were explored as potential covariates. These variables were not significantly associated with either cortisol intercept or slope across Strange Situation in this sample, with the exception that time since adoption significantly predicted baseline cortisol levels (z = 2.26, p = .02). This variable was included as a predictor of both intercept and slope.

3. Results

3.1 Preliminary Analyses

See Table 1 for zero-order correlations among the focal variables. Parental sensitivity, attachment disorganization, and intervention condition were not significantly associated with any individual cortisol measure. Parents who were randomly assigned to receive the attachment-based intervention demonstrated higher parental sensitivity prior to the intervention than parents who received the control intervention (see Supplemental Materials for more information). All three cortisol variables were highly correlated with one another.

Table 1.

Zero-order correlations and descriptive statistics for main study variables and covariates

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Intervention status
2. Attachment disorganization −.13
3. Pre-intervention parental sensitivity .25* −.16
4. Percent time in institutional care .12 .24* −.12
5. Age at adoption .09 .06 .14 .04
6. Time since adoption −.17 .00 −.09 −.07 −.90***
7. Pre-Strange Situation cortisol −.21 −.05 −.24 −.13 −.14 .14
8. 15 min-post Strange Situation cortisol −.13 −.00 .00 −.03 −.10 .05 .70***
9. 30-min post Strange Situation cortisol −.08 .05 .05 −.05 −.05 .01 .61*** .77***

Mean (SD) 2.87 .66 13.91 11.90 −1.02 −1.02 −1.05
(.83) (.38) (4.80) (4.05) (.32) (.35) (.32)

Note. N = 66.

*

p < .05,

***

p < .001.

For intervention condition, attachment-based intervention = 1 (n = 34), control intervention = 0 (n = 32); for attachment disorganization, organized attachment = 0 (n = 44), disorganized attachment = 1 (n = 20). All values shown are Pearson correlations of continuous variables, except those with intervention status and attachment disorganization which are Spearman’s rank correlations. Descriptive statistics for cortisol are log10 transformed values due to positive skew.

3.2 Cortisol Reactivity to the Strange Situation

The baseline growth curve model with no predictors indicated that there was not a sample-level cortisol response to the Strange Situation Procedure (z = −0.54, p = .59). However, there was significant unexplained variation in intercepts (z = 4.36, p < .001) and trend-level variation in slopes (z = 1.66, p = .10). This suggests that, although there was no overall cortisol response, participants varied in their cortisol patterns.

3.3 Predicting Baseline Cortisol Levels

Results of the model predicting cortisol levels immediately before the Strange Situation are reported in Table 2. Neither parental sensitivity, attachment disorganization, nor intervention condition predicted basal cortisol.

Table 2.

Parental sensitivity, attachment disorganization, and intervention condition as predictors of baseline cortisol levels and cortisol reactivity during the Strange Situation

Intercept Slope


β z-value R2 β z-value R2
Constant −2.56 −3.12*** .13 −2.30 −1.89 .40
Parental sensitivity −.18 −1.29 .49 2.32*
Attachment disorganization −.18 −1.31 .33 1.60
Time since adoption .10 0.70 −.21 −1.02
Intervention condition −.22 −1.53 .14 0.67
Residual variance .87 4.20*** .60 1.11
Intercept-slope covariance −.02 −0.06

Note. N = 66.

*

p < .05,

***

p < .001.

For intervention condition, attachment-based intervention = 1, control intervention = 0; for attachment disorganization, organized attachment = 0, disorganized attachment = 1. β values represent standardized regression coefficients.

3.4 Predicting Cortisol Reactivity to the Strange Situation

Results of the model predicting cortisol reactivity during the Strange Situation are reported in Table 2. Parental sensitivity predicted increased cortisol slope. As displayed in Figure 1 using a median split of high versus low parental sensitivity, IA children who experienced more sensitive care exhibited greater cortisol reactivity than children who experienced less sensitive care. Intervention condition and attachment disorganization were not significantly associated with cortisol reactivity. Moreover, results regarding the significance of parental sensitivity did not change when intervention status and/or attachment disorganization were omitted from the models, or when only looking at the subset of participants who completed the control intervention (see Supplemental Materials for more information).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Predicted values of internationally adopted children’s cortisol response to the Strange Situation as a function of high (≥ 3) and low (< 3) post-adoptive parental sensitivity (ns = 36 and 30, respectively), calculated using a median split. Raw cortisol values are presented for visualization purposes.

4. Discussion

The present study examined the role of sensitive caregiving for regulating IA children’s HPA responses to stress during early childhood. To our knowledge, the current study represents the first investigation of the implications of IA children’s post-adoptive caregiving experiences for their HPA responses to stress. Within this sample, higher parental sensitivity was associated with increased cortisol reactivity during the Strange Situation. Given the evidence that IA children and children with histories of institutional caregiving are more likely to show blunted patterns of cortisol reactivity (Koss et al., 2016; McLaughlin et al., 2015), this finding may indicate that increased parental sensitivity facilitates recovery in IA children’s HPA responses to stressors. This interpretation is further supported by evidence that sensitive post-adoptive parenting promotes more competent forms of behavioral adaptation among IA children (e.g., Garvin et al., 2012; Stams et al., 2002).

There is an inverted U-shaped curve in the association between cortisol and cognitive and behavioral outcomes in adults, such that moderate amounts of cortisol are associated with the best learning and memory performance. This non-linear association is due to the ratio of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors that are bound to cortisol in the brain (a high MR/GR ratio; for a review, see Finsterwald & Alberini, 2014). In response to the type of chronic stress experienced by IA children prior to adoption, the HPA axis may reorganize via altered MR/GR expression in the brain in order to prevent consistently high levels of cortisol production (Gunnar & Vazquez, 2001; Strüber et al., 2014). This results in a pattern of HPA functioning characterized by little to no cortisol production in response to stressors. The findings from the current study indicate that post-adoption sensitive caregiving may facilitate recovery of the HPA axis and normalize the functioning of IA children’s stress response systems.

Given that this is one of the first studies of the contributions of the post-adoptive caregiving environment for IA children’s HPA responses to stress, it will be important to replicate these findings in a larger sample of IA children. In addition, future studies should directly assess whether a moderate cortisol response as seen in IA children who experienced more sensitive care is associated with recovery in behavioral development. Further studies should assess whether the association between sensitive caregiving and cortisol reactivity seen in this study generalizes to children who have experienced other types of early adversity and are at risk for hypocortisolism (e.g., children who have experienced maltreatment). Another future direction will be to evaluate whether these findings are specific to the use of the Strange Situation Procedure or if they generalize to other stressors. Although the Strange Situation Procedure is the most commonly used paradigm for eliciting a stress response at this age (see Gunnar et al., 2009 for a review), there was not a significant sample-level cortisol response to the Strange Situation Procedure in this sample. This may have weakened the likelihood of observing associations with cortisol reactivity. However, the slightly older age of the children at the time of the procedure as well as the fact that these IA children may have been at risk for hypocortisolism possibly contributed to a smaller sample-level cortisol response in this study.

Similarly, given the evidence that IA children and children with histories of institutionalization show blunted HPA reactivity during later childhood (Hostinar, Johnson, & Gunnar, 2015; McLaughlin et al., 2015), it is important to investigate whether sensitive caregiving shapes IA children’s cortisol responses during later developmental periods. Future studies should examine other aspects of IA children’s post-adoptive environments, including the adoptive parents’ previous parenting experiences, depressive symptoms, and levels of community support. Additionally, it will be important to explore the role of different dimensions of parenting on the normalization of HPA axis reactivity following early social deprivation. There are several other dimensions of parenting that may also play a role in the impact of parenting on HPA functioning; however, we chose to focus on parental sensitivity because there is a large body of evidence documenting its relevance for non-adopted children’s cortisol outcomes (Hostinar, Sullivan, & Gunnar, 2014). Thus, parental sensitivity may have a particularly strong role in the adoptive parent’s ability to facilitate recovery of the HPA stress response following international adoption.

In contrast to previous findings based on non-adopted children (Bernard & Dozier, 2010; Gunnar, Brodersen, Nachmias, Buss, & Rigatuso, 1996; Hertsgaard, Gunnar, Erickson, & Nachmias, 1995), IA children’s attachment patterns were not significantly associated with their cortisol responses during the Strange Situation. This may be due to the fact that IA children’s attachment patterns were changing as a result of the recent major transition in their caregiving environment. It will important to examine the associations between IA children’s attachment patterns and cortisol responses after children have spent a longer duration of time with their adoptive families and their attachment patterns are more consolidated (Shapiro, Shapiro, & Paret, 2001).

Furthermore, enrollment in the attachment-based intervention was not associated with differences in cortisol reactivity to the Strange Situation. This null finding may seem paradoxical given that receiving the attachment-based intervention promoted increases in parental sensitivity (Yarger et al., under review), and parental sensitivity is positively associated with cortisol reactivity. Intervention effects may not have been detected due to limited statistical power given the relatively modest sample size. Additionally, those assigned to the attachment-based intervention in this sample were already more sensitive than their control group counterparts pre-intervention (however, Yarger et al., under review shows this is not the case in the larger sample from which this data was taken). Another possibility is the length of time needed for an effect to emerge at the neurobiological level. The Strange Situation Procedure was conducted soon after the completion of the 10-week intervention, which may not have allowed sufficient time for the changes in parental behavior to effect children’s neurobiological responses to stress. Future studies should investigate this possibility by assessing potential intervention effects on cortisol reactivity over a longer period of time.

This study has a number of strengths. Namely, the inclusion of an observational measure of post-adoption parenting to examine associations with cortisol reactivity is a unique strength of the current study. There was also a wide range of sensitive caregiving in this sample. Typically, studies measuring parenting in internationally adopted populations include primarily parents with relatively high parenting quality (e.g., Garvin et al., 2012; Lawler, Koss, & Gunnar, 2017), which reduces their ability to make conclusions that generalize to parents who display average or lower parenting quality. This sample allows us to more accurately evaluate the impact of post-adoption parenting across a wider range of observed parental sensitivity than the variation limited to only the most sensitive parents.

Altogether, the current study supports the notion that the functioning of children’s HPA axes are especially plastic during early development and are regulated by their experiences with caregivers. In addition, these findings offer initial evidence that post-adoption parental sensitivity may help promote IA children’s stress-regulatory development following severe early life adversity. These results help advance our understanding of how the consequences of adverse early caregiving experiences for children’s stress-regulatory development can be offset by later experiences. Specifically, post-adoption parental sensitivity may be a vital resource through which IA children achieve positive developmental outcomes despite experiencing considerable early life adversity.

Supplementary Material

supplement

Highlights.

  • Predictors of internationally adopted children’s cortisol reactivity are examined.

  • Increased cortisol reactivity is associated with more sensitive caregiving.

  • The extent of pre-adoptive deprivation did not predict reduced cortisol reactivity.

  • Results suggest that post-adoptive caregiving may recalibrate HPA axis functioning.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the families who participated in this research project. Funding: This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health [R01 MH052135] to the final author.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Author Note: All authors contributed substantially in the creation of this manuscript. The first and second authors processed, analyzed, and interpreted data and wrote portions of the manuscript. The third author oversaw data collection and processing and provided feedback for the manuscript. The final author designed the study and provided input on data interpretation and manuscript writing.

Conflicts of interest: None.

References

  1. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Walls S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alink LR, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Mesman J, Juffer F, Koot HM. Cortisol and externalizing behavior in children and adolescents: Mixed meta-analytic evidence for the inverse relation of basal cortisol and cortisol reactivity with externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50:427–450. doi: 10.1002/dev.20300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernard K, Dozier M. Examining infants’ cortisol responses to laboratory tasks among children varying in attachment disorganization: Stress reactivity or return to baseline? Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:1771–1778. doi: 10.1037/a0020660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernard K, Frost A, Bennett CB, Lindhiem O. Maltreatment and diurnal cortisol regulation: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;78:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blair C, Granger D, Razza RP. Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development. 2005;76:554–567. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00863.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK, Liaw FR, Spiker D. Enhancing the development of low birthweight, premature infants: Changes in cognition and behavior over the first three years. Child Development. 1993;64:736–753. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02940.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Conradt E, Abar B, Lester BM, LaGasse LL, Shankaran S, Bada H, Bauer CR, Whitaker TM, Hammond JA. Cortisol reactivity to social stress as a mediator of early adversity on risk and adaptive outcomes. Child Development. 2014;85:2279–2298. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chrousos G. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews Endocrinology. 2009;5:374–381. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2009.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Doom JR, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Longitudinal patterns of cortisol regulation differ in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2014;53:1206–1215. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dozier M, Peloso E, Lewis E, Laurenceau J-PP, Levine S. Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:845–859. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dozier M, Zeanah CH, Wallin AR, Shauffer C. Institutional care for young children: Review of literature and policy implications. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2012;6:1–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01033.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Finsterwald C, Alberini CM. Stress and glucocorticoid receptor-dependent mechanisms in long-term memory: From adaptive responses to psychopathologies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2014;112:17–29. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fisher PA, Kim HK, Bruce J, Pears KC. Cumulative effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on foster children’s HPA-axis reactivity during a psychosocial stressor. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2012;36:29–35. doi: 10.1177/0165025411406863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fisher PA, Van Ryzin MJ, Gunnar MR. Mitigating HPA axis dysregulation associated with placement changes in foster care. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:531–539. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fries E, Hesse J, Hellhammer J, Hellhammer DH. A new view on hypocortisolism. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:1010–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fries A, Shirtcliff EA, Pollak SD. Neuroendocrine dysregulation following early social deprivation in children. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50:588–599. doi: 10.1002/dev.20319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Garvin MC, Tarullo AR, Van Ryzin M, Gunnar MR. Postadoption parenting and socioemotional development in postinstitutionalized children. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24:35–48. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gunnar MR, Brodersen L, Nachmias M, Buss K, Rigatuso J. Stress reactivity and attachment security. Developmental Psychobiology. 1996;29:191–204. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199604)29:3<191::AID-DEV1>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gunnar MR, Bruce J, Grotevant H. International adoption of institutionally reared children: Research and policy. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:677–693. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400004077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gunnar MR, Cheatham C. Brain and behavior interface: Stress and the developing brain. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2003;24:195–211. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10052. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gunnar MR, Doom JR, Esposito EA. Psychoneuroendocrinology of stress: Normative development and individual differences. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science. 2015;7:106–151. [Google Scholar]
  22. Gunnar MR, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:145–173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gunnar MR, Talge NM, Herrera A. Stressor paradigms in developmental studies: What does and does not work to produce mean increases in salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:953–967. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gunnar MR, Vazquez DM. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: Potential indices of risk in human development. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:515–538. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hertsgaard L, Gunnar MR, Erickson MF, Nachmias M. Adrenocortical responses to the Strange Situation in infants with disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships. Child Development. 1995;66:1100–1106. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hostinar CE, Johnson AE, Gunnar MR. Early social deprivation and the social buffering of cortisol stress responses in late childhood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology. 2015;51:1597–1608. doi: 10.1037/dev0000029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychological Bulletin. 2014;140 doi: 10.1037/a0032671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hunter AL, Minnis H, Wilson P. Altered stress responses in children exposed to early adversity: A systematic review of salivary cortisol studies. Stress. 2011;14:614–626. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.577848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Intercountry Adoption Statistics. United States: Department of State website; [Retrieved April 10,2017]. (n.d.) from. https://travel.state.gov/content/adoptionsabroad/en/about-us/statistics.html. [Google Scholar]
  30. Juffer F, Palacios J, Le Mare L, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Tieman W, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Vorria P, van IJzendoorn MH, Verhulst FC. II. Development of adopted children with histories of early adversity. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2011;76:31–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00627.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: Recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;19:313–333. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)90013-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Koss KJ, Mliner SB, Donzella B, Gunnar MR. Early adversity, hypocortisolism, and behavior problems at school entry: A study of internationally adopted children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;66:31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lawler J, Koss K, Gunnar M. Bidirectional effects of parenting and child behavior in internationally adopting families. Journal of Family Psychology. 2017 doi: 10.1037/fam0000309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. 1990;1:121–160. [Google Scholar]
  35. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Tibu F, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Causal effects of the early caregiving environment on development of stress response systems in children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2015;112:5637–5642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423363112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 1996;11:269–306. [Google Scholar]
  37. Pitula CE, DePasquale CE, Mliner SB, Gunnar MR. Peer problems among post-institutionalized, internationally adopted children: Relations to hypocortisolism, parenting quality, and ADHD symptoms. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12986. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ramey CT, Yeates KO, Short EJ. The plasticity of intellectual development: Insights from preventive intervention. Child Development. 1984:1913–1925. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5–12 (BETA) Ghent, Belgium: Ghent University; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ruttle PL, Shirtcliff EA, Serbin LA, Fisher DBD, Stack DM, Schwartzman AE. Disentangling psychobiological mechanisms underlying internalizing and externalizing behaviors in youth: Longitudinal and concurrent associations with cortisol. Hormones and Behavior. 2011;59:123–132. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Schilling TM, Kölsch M, Larra MF, Zech CM, Blumenthal TD, Frings C, Schächinger H. For whom the bell (curve) tolls: Cortisol rapidly affects memory retrieval by an inverted U-shaped dose–response relationship. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:1565–1572. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Shapiro V, Shapiro J, Paret I. International adoption and the formation of new family attachments. Smith College Studies in Social Work. 2001;71:389–418. [Google Scholar]
  43. Stams GJJ, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH. Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:806–821. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Strüber N, Strüber D, Roth G. Impact of early adversity on glucocorticoid regulation and later mental disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;38:17–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Van den Dries L, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Fostering security? A meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review. 2009;31:410–421. [Google Scholar]
  46. Yarger HA, Bernard K, Meade EB, Wallin A, Dozier M. Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up for parents adopting internationally: Enhancing parenting quality under review. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

supplement

RESOURCES